Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Semantic verbal fluency analysis in highly educated older adults

ABSTRACT

Purpose

to compare the performance of a group of highly educated, healthy older adults in the quantitative and qualitative analysis of the Semantic Verbal Fluency (SVF) test in the Animals and Fruits categories, the performance of the subgroups of participants (with high [HE] and very high [VHE] educational level) in both categories, as well as the performance between men and women.

Methods

The study included 31 older adults (> 60 years old), with eight years or more of formal education, from the Federal District (DF-Brazil). All performed the 60-second test in both categories. The following items were analyzed: total words, clustering, switching, intersection, return, test time, and errors, considering demographic variables such as gender and educational level (HE: 8 to 16 years; VHE: 17 to 25 years).

Results

Participants with VHE had a better performance in both categories, with a greater total number of words evoked for “animals” and a greater number of switches (p<0.05) for “fruits”. In relation to gender, men obtained a better performance for “animals” with the highest total number of evoked words and in the different time blocks (p<0.05) and greater number of clusters and intersections (p<0.001).

Conclusion

The performance of healthy older adults with a high educational level is influenced by demographic variables such as years of formal education and gender, suggesting that the application of the SVF should consider the impact of these variables, with a cautious interpretation of the results.

Keywords:
Aging; Cognition; Language; Education; Neuropsychological test

RESUMO

Objetivo

comparar o desempenho de idosos saudáveis com alta escolaridade nas análises quantitativas e qualitativas do Teste de Fluência Verbal Semântica, nas categorias ”animais” e ”frutas”, comparar o desempenho de subgrupos de participantes (com escolaridade alta e muito alta) em ambas as categorias, assim como o desempenho entre homens e mulheres.

Métodos

participaram do estudo 31 idosos, (>60 anos), com oito anos ou mais de escolaridade, procedentes do Distrito Federal (DF-Brasil). Todos realizaram o teste de 60 segundos, nas duas categorias. Foram analisados: total de palavras, clustering, switching, intersecção, retorno, tempo de teste e erros, atendendo às variáveis demográficas, como gênero e escolaridade (escolaridade alta: oito a 16 anos de estudo; escolaridade muito alta: 17 a 25 anos de estudo).

Resultados

participantes com escolaridade muito alta obtiveram melhor desempenho em ambas as categorias, com maior número total de palavras evocadas na categoria “animais” e maior quantidade de switches (p<0,05) em “frutas”. Quanto ao gênero, os homens obtiveram melhor desempenho na categoria “animais”, com maior número total de palavras evocadas e nos diferentes blocos de tempo (p<0,05), e maior quantidade de clusters e interseções (p<0,001).

Conclusão

o desempenho de idosos saudáveis com alta escolaridade foi influenciado por variáveis demográficas como anos de estudo e gênero, sugerindo que a aplicação do Teste de Fluência Verbal Semântica deve considerar o impacto dessas variáveis e realizar uma interpretação cautelosa dos resultados.

Palavras-chave:
Envelhecimento; Cognição; Linguagem; Educação; Teste neuropsicológico

INTRODUCTION

The Verbal Fluency Test is an instrument widely used in the neuropsychological assessment of aging(11 Balthazar MLF, Cendes F, Damasceno BP. Category verbal fluency performance may be impaired in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dement Neuropsychol. 2007;1(2):161-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1980-57642008dn10200008. PMid:29213383.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1980-57642008...
) and in the screening of cognitive changes(22 Henry JD, Crawford JR. A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance following focal cortical lesions. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(2):284-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.2.284. PMID: 15099151.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.2...
). According to data from the 2018 Demographic Census of the Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatistica(33 IBGE: Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística. Idosos indicam caminhos para uma melhor idade [Internet]. Rio de Janeiro: IBGE; 2018 [citado em 2020 Set 9]. Disponível em: https://censo2020.ibge.gov.br/2012-agencia-de-noticias/noticias/24036-idosos-indicam-caminhos-para-uma-melhor-idade.html
https://censo2020.ibge.gov.br/2012-agenc...
) (IBGE, Brazilian Institute of Geography and Statistics), the elderly population is expected to increase in the coming decades. It is estimated that 1/4 of Brazilian citizens will be elderly by 2043. Therefore, it is necessary to study these individuals and the cognitive changes resulting from the passage of time. Charchat-Fichman et al.(44 Charchat-Fichman H, Caramelli P, Sameshima K, Nitrini R. Declínio da capacidade cognitiva durante o envelhecimento. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2005;27(21):79-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462005000100017. PMid:15867989.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462005...
), when studying the cognitive decline in the aging process, state that the cognitive trajectory is divided into two groups: stable and benign and in an incipient/transition stage to dementia. Some researchers state that clinical characterization is fundamental and highlight that demographic, cultural, environmental, genetic, and lifestyle variables, influence neuropsychological performance and contribute to the cognitive heterogeneity of the aging process(55 Harada CN, Natelson Love MC, Triebel KL. Normal cognitive aging. Clin Geriatr Med. 2013;29(4):737-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cger.2013.07...
). To assess cognitive changes in aging, several neuropsychological instruments have been developed. Martins et al.(66 Martins NIM, Caldas PR, Cabral ED, Lins CCSA, Coriolano MGWS. Instrumentos de avaliação cognitiva utilizados nos últimos cinco anos em idosos brasileiros. Cien Saude Colet. 2019;24(7):2513-30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1413-81232018247.20862017. PMid:31340270.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1413-812320182...
), in a review study, describe that the Verbal Fluency Test has been the second most used instrument over the last five years with older adults in Brazil. Thus, in addition to its simplicity and speed, it assesses the spontaneous production of words under restricted conditions, involving linguistic, memory (semantic memory), and cognitive processes specific to executive functions (sustained attention, working memory, search strategies, initiation, inhibition of inappropriate responses, planning, and speed of information processing)(77 Lezak M, Howieson D, Bigler E, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment. 5th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2012), as well as vocabulary size and general knowledge(22 Henry JD, Crawford JR. A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance following focal cortical lesions. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(2):284-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.2.284. PMID: 15099151.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.2...
).

According to the literature, among its different versions, the Semantic Verbal Fluency Test (SVF) requires the production of words of predetermined categories, such as, for example, “animals” or “fruits”, in an established time of 60 seconds(77 Lezak M, Howieson D, Bigler E, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment. 5th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2012), and losses in performance would be related mainly to brain changes, specifically lesions in the left hemisphere and requiring greater activation of the temporal lobe regions(77 Lezak M, Howieson D, Bigler E, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment. 5th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2012). In addition to being an ecological instrument and of rapid application, it presents a high sensitivity in the discrimination of the normal aging process, early stages of cognitive decline, and dementia(11 Balthazar MLF, Cendes F, Damasceno BP. Category verbal fluency performance may be impaired in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dement Neuropsychol. 2007;1(2):161-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1980-57642008dn10200008. PMid:29213383.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1980-57642008...
). However, as with most neuropsychological tests, studies have shown that SVF performance can be influenced by sociodemographic variables, such as age, academic level, gender, among others(88 Brucki SMD, Rocha MSG. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(12):1771-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002. PMid:15558183.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004...
,99 Acevedo A, Loewenstein DA, Barker WW, Harwood DG, Luis C, Bravo M, et al. Category Fluency Test: normative data for English- and Spanish-speaking elderly. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):760-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677032. PMid:11105466.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677...
). However, in the SVF performance analysis, in samples of healthy older adults with a high educational level, studies have found different results. In the study by Brucki and Rocha(88 Brucki SMD, Rocha MSG. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(12):1771-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002. PMid:15558183.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004...
), using quantitative and qualitative analyses, there were no statistically significant differences between two groups of participants with high educational levels (nine to 11 years of formal education and 11 years or more), as well as the study by Acevedo et al.(99 Acevedo A, Loewenstein DA, Barker WW, Harwood DG, Luis C, Bravo M, et al. Category Fluency Test: normative data for English- and Spanish-speaking elderly. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):760-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677032. PMid:11105466.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677...
) who reported no impact regarding the years of formal education on the performance of the groups of participants (eight to 12 years; 13 to 16 years; and over 17 years). However, Fichman et al.(1010 Fichman HC, Fernandes CS, Nitrini R, Lourenço RA, Paradela EMP, Carthery-Goulart MT, et al. Age and educational level effects on the performance of normal elderly on category verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2009;3(1):49-54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30100010. PMid:29213610.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009...
) found differences in the quantitative performance of older adults with 12 years or more of formal education, compared to older adults with lower educational levels. For their part, Souza et al.(1111 Souza BCS, Teixeira TOB, Silva LD, Satler C, Cera ML. Fluência verbal de adultos e idosos do Distrito Federal: proposta normativa piloto. Audiol Commun Res. 2020;25:e2315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2020-2315.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2020...
) highlighted the relevance of the task in detecting differences between samples with a higher educational level.

Regarding the relationship between gender and test performance, most studies reported the absence of statistically significant differences between groups of older adults in the “animals”(88 Brucki SMD, Rocha MSG. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(12):1771-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002. PMid:15558183.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004...
,99 Acevedo A, Loewenstein DA, Barker WW, Harwood DG, Luis C, Bravo M, et al. Category Fluency Test: normative data for English- and Spanish-speaking elderly. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):760-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677032. PMid:11105466.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677...
,1212 Cavaco S, Gonçalves A, Pinto C, Almeida E, Gomes F, Moreira I, et al. Semantic fluency and phonemic fluency: regression-based norms for the Portuguese population. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2013;28(3):262-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/act001. PMid:23341434.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/act001...

13 Esteves CS, Oliveira CR, Moret-Tatay C, Navarro-Pardo E, Carli GAD, Silva IG, et al. Phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks: normative data for elderly Brazilians. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2015;28(2):350-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678-7153.201528215.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678-7153.2015...

14 Mathuranath PS, George A, Cherian PJ, Alexander A, Sarma SG, Sarma PS. Effects of age, education and gender on verbal fluency. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2003;25(8):1057-64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/jcen.25.8.1057.16736. PMid:14566579.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/jcen.25.8.1057...
-1515 Santos Nogueira D, Azevedo Reis E, Vieira A. Verbal fluency tasks: effects of age, gender, and education. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2016;68(3):124-33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000450640. PMid:27915338.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000450640...
) SVF category, except in the studies developed by Castro-Costa et al.(1616 Castro-Costa E, Lima-Costa MF, Andrade FB, Souza PRB Jr, Ferri CP. Cognitive function among older adults: ELSI-Brazil results. Rev Saude Publica. 2018 Out 52(Suppl 2):4s. http://dx.doi.org/10.11606/s1518-8787.2018052000629. PMid:30379286.
http://dx.doi.org/10.11606/s1518-8787.20...
) and Silva et al.(1717 Silva TBL, Yassuda MS, Guimaraes VV, Florindo AA. Fluência verbal e variáveis sociodemográficas no processo de envelhecimento: um estudo epidemiológico. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2011;24(4):739-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722011000400014.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722011...
), in which men had a better performance. In turn, some studies have highlighted that women have a better performance in the “fruits” and “vegetables” categories(99 Acevedo A, Loewenstein DA, Barker WW, Harwood DG, Luis C, Bravo M, et al. Category Fluency Test: normative data for English- and Spanish-speaking elderly. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):760-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677032. PMid:11105466.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677...
,1818 Bolla KI, Gray S, Resnick SM, Galante R, Kawas C. Category and letter fluency in highly educated older adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;12(3):330-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/clin.12.3.330.1986.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/clin.12.3.330....
).

It is worth pointing out that few studies(88 Brucki SMD, Rocha MSG. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(12):1771-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002. PMid:15558183.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004...
,1919 Chiossi JSC, Soares AD, Chiari BM. Clustering e switching em deficientes auditivos usuários do português brasileiro: fluência verbal semântica e fonológica. Rev CEFAC. 2016;18(2):369-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201618211815.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620161...

20 Pereira AH, Gonçalves AB, Holz M, Gonçalves HA, Kochhann R, Joanette Y, et al. Influence of age and education on the processing of clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2018;12(4):360-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004. PMid:30546845.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018d...
-2121 Wajman JR, Cecchini MA, Bertolucci PHF, Mansur LL. Quanti-qualitative components of the semantic verbal fluency test in cognitively healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia subtypes. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(6):533-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426. PMid:30375889.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018....
) included qualitative analyses, such as clusters, categories, and switches of the words generated in the SVF in healthy populations of older adults, being highly relevant in understanding the participants' cognitive performance, considering the possible influence of sociodemographic variables on the normal aging process. In a complementary way, it is important to know the cognitive profile of older adults with a high educational level, especially in rapid and sensitive tests to identify neuropsychological changes, considering the greater access to Education, which provides an increase in the educational level, and the lack of qualitative data, in Brazil, regarding the performance of older adults and samples with high educational levels in SVF tasks.

Finally, regarding the type of category, studies carried out in other countries have shown that, in the “animals” category, the SVF presented a better validity criterion than in the “fruits” category, with less influence from cultural and educational level(2222 Farghaly M, Hussein M, Hassan A, Hegazy M, Sabbah A. Testing of verbal fluency in egyptians: cultural and educational challenges. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2018;31(3):133-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000160. PMid:30239463.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000...
), while the performance in this test was influenced by sociodemographic variables in a different way in relation to the “animals” and “fruits” categories in several Spanish-speaking countries(2323 Olabarrieta-Landa L, Rivera D, Lara L, Rute-Pérez S, Rodríguez-Lorenzana A, Galarza-Del-Angel J, et al. Verbal fluency tests: normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation. 2017;41(3):673-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/NRE-172240. PMid:28946591.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/NRE-172240...
). Thus, the objectives of the present study were: to compare the performance of a group of healthy older adults with high educational level in the quantitative and qualitative SVF analyses in the “animal” and “fruit” categories; comparing the performance of two groups of older adults with different educational levels (high and very high) in the SVF, in the “animal” and “fruit” categories; to compare the performance in the SVF between men and women. It was expected that the participants with higher educational levels would have a better performance in the “animals” category, aiming at contributing to a better analysis of SVF performance for Brazilian older adults with high educational levels, especially regarding the strategies used in each category.

METHODS

Ethical aspects

This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Health Sciences of the Universidade de Brasília (UnB, University of Brasília), CAAE: 36747614.5.0000.0030. All participants signed the Informed Consent Term, which was also approved by the Committee.

Participants

This study involved 31 older adults (22 women and 9 men), aged 60 or over, with a mean age of 69.25 years (standard deviation of 4.64), and with eight or more years of formal education, with a mean of 16.75 years (standard deviation of 4.39), recruited from the community and selected for convenience.

The 31 older adults were selected from the 45 participants of a project involving a cognitive training program. Older adults aged 60 or over were included; with more than eight years of formal education; native speakers of Brazilian Portuguese; who reported having good health conditions, no history of drug or alcohol abuse, or psychiatric or neurological disorders, having normal or corrected hearing and vision, not using psychotropic medication; who did not present cognitive impairment (with a score equal to or greater than 24 points in the Montreal Cognitive Assessment [MoCA](2424 Cesar KG, Yassuda MS, Porto FHG, Brucki SMD, Nitrini R. MoCA Test: normative and diagnostic accuracy data for seniors with heterogeneous educational levels in Brazil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2019;77(11):775-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0004-282x20190130. PMid:31826133.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0004-282x20190...
)); individuals who did not have symptomatology suggestive of anxiety (scores below 7 points on the Beck’s Anxiety Inventory - BAI)(2525 Cunha J. Manual da versão em português das Escalas Beck. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2001.) or depression (score below 13 points on the Beck’s Depression Inventory - BDI-II)(2626 Gorenstein C, Pang WY, Argimon IL, Werlang BSG. Inventário Beck de Depressão-II. Manual. São Paulo, SP: Casa do Psicólogo; 2011.).

Instruments

For screening the participants, the following were applied: a general assessment questionnaire, a self-administered instrument that contains the participant's identification, sociodemographic, and clinical data; the MoCA (Brazilian version) proposed by Cesar et al.(2424 Cesar KG, Yassuda MS, Porto FHG, Brucki SMD, Nitrini R. MoCA Test: normative and diagnostic accuracy data for seniors with heterogeneous educational levels in Brazil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2019;77(11):775-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0004-282x20190130. PMid:31826133.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0004-282x20190...
); the BAI (Brazilian version) proposed by Cunha(2525 Cunha J. Manual da versão em português das Escalas Beck. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2001.); and the BDI-II (Brazilian version) proposed by Gorenstein et al.(2626 Gorenstein C, Pang WY, Argimon IL, Werlang BSG. Inventário Beck de Depressão-II. Manual. São Paulo, SP: Casa do Psicólogo; 2011.).

For the analysis of the SVF, the categories chosen for the present study were “animals” and “fruits”. The participants were instructed on the invalidity of words belonging to the error groups, so that they would not emit words that fit those groups. They received the following guidelines: (1) “Now, I would like you to tell me as many names of animals of any species as possible (land or sea animals, four-legged, birds, fish, insects). You have a minute, the more animals you say, the better”. (2) “Now, I would like you to tell me as many fruit names as you can remember. They can be from fruit trees you have in the backyard, wild fruits, any fruit. You have a minute, the more you say, the better ”. The total number of words evoked and the number of words in the time intervals (in seconds) were analyzed: from 0 to 15, 16 to 30, 31 to 45, and 46 to 60. Error analysis and qualitative analysis were performed, such as the order of the words emitted. The inclusion of qualitative analyses suggests relevant information regarding the strategies used, formation of clusters, and change of subcategories when the previous one is over(2727 Rodrigues AB, Yamashita ET, Chiappetta ALML. Teste de fluência verbal no adulto e no idoso: verificação da aprendizagem verbal. Rev CEFAC. 2008;10(4):443-51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462008000400004.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462008...
). The errors considered were: intrusion (words that are not of the requested category), repetition (words spoken more than once), continuous repetition/perseveration (repetition of the same item continuously, persistence in the same word, uttered twice or more), paraphasia (phonological, semantic, or other changes) and derivation (words that vary in number, size, gender, and verbal conjugations), based on the authors Machado et al.(2828 Machado TH, Fichman HC, Santos EL, Carvalho VA, Fialho PP, Koenig AM, et al. Normative data for healthy elderly on the phonemic verbal fluency task - FAS. Dement Neuropsychol. 2009;3(1):55-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30100011. PMid:29213611.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009...
) and Fichman et al.(1010 Fichman HC, Fernandes CS, Nitrini R, Lourenço RA, Paradela EMP, Carthery-Goulart MT, et al. Age and educational level effects on the performance of normal elderly on category verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2009;3(1):49-54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30100010. PMid:29213610.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009...
).

For the study of clustering, switching, intersection, and return, the following groupings were considered for each semantic category: - Animals: first letter, environment (aquatic, farm, Brazilian cerrado, domestic, rainforest, savannah, jungle, and zoo animals) and class (bird, mammal, fish, amphibian, insect, crustacean, and reptile); - Fruits: first letter, flavor (acid, bitter, bittersweet, sweet, and neutral) and climate (tropical, temperate, subtropical, equatorial). Clustering (refers to the subcategories generated by the participants) was calculated based on the study by Wajman(2121 Wajman JR, Cecchini MA, Bertolucci PHF, Mansur LL. Quanti-qualitative components of the semantic verbal fluency test in cognitively healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia subtypes. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(6):533-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426. PMid:30375889.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018....
), from the second word of the cluster, therefore, a single word had a size of 0, a group of two words had a size of 1, a group of three words had a size of 2, and so on. For switching (changes in subcategories and groupings), the number of changes between the subcategories was calculated. Regarding intersection (using the word of a stimulus group to start the next one), the calculation was made by the number of strategies performed. Return is a reversion to the previous subcategory, calculating the switching and the intersection, as described by Pereira et al.(2020 Pereira AH, Gonçalves AB, Holz M, Gonçalves HA, Kochhann R, Joanette Y, et al. Influence of age and education on the processing of clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2018;12(4):360-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004. PMid:30546845.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018d...
). The performance of the participants in this study was also analyzed by the time used in the test, with divisions in blocks of 15 seconds.

Procedure

The instruments were applied individually by researchers, who were trained for the task at the UnB and took place in a well-lit room, with noise control. As part of a broader study, at first, the older adults were asked about their identification, sociodemographic data, and information regarding their current and past health condition, presence of emotional and cognitive symptoms, list of medications in use, and complementary studies performed. Those who met the inclusion criteria were invited to participate in the study. In a second meeting, half of the group performed the “animals” SVF category and the other half, the “fruits” category, with the subsequent application of other neuropsychological tests from the broader study. After two months, the participants returned to reapply the SVF, using a different category than the one previously performed. Their tests were recorded and later digitized for analysis.

Statistical analysis

To characterize the sample regarding the demographic variables of interest, descriptive analyses were used, using mean and standard deviation. The t-test of independent samples was used to compare the demographic and mental status of the participants in the subgroups by educational level: high (HE) and very high (VHE). To analyze the performance of the participants in the SVF, the GLM test (General Linear Model) was used, for unrelated samples, using the Bonferroni post-hoc test and partial eta squared (ηp2) as a measure of the effect size. The dependent variables were: total words, total words in time intervals in seconds (0 to 15, 15 to 30, 30 to 45 and 45 to 60), total clustering, switching, intersections, returns to the first letter, environment and class, total intrusions, repetitions/perseveration, continuous repetition, paraphasias, and derivations. The independent variables were educational level and gender. The statistical program IBM SPSS Statistics, version 25, was used and the level of significance considered was p≤0.05.

RESULTS

The sample involved 31 older adults, with a mean age of 69.25 years (standard deviation of 4.64) and with 16.75 years of formal education (standard deviation of 4.39), 22 of which were female. The comparison of the subgroups of participants, considering the educational levels - HE (from 8 to 16 years of study) and VHE (from 17 years to 25 years of study) - did not show any statistically significant differences by age (p=0.963), global cognition (MoCA, p=0.370), and presence of symptoms of anxiety (BAI, p=0.466) and depression (BDI, p=0.442) (Table 1).

Table 1
Demographic and clinical characterization of the sample

As for the words evoked in the SVF, it was observed that the total mean was significantly higher (p<0.001) in the “animals” semantic category, with the same occurring for all time blocks, however, with utterance values decreasing every 15 seconds (0-15s> 15-30s> 30-45s> 45-60s). It was noted that there was a statistically significant difference in the total first letter (p<0.001) and class (p=0.013) switches, between categories, obtaining higher means in the “animals” category. There were significant differences in the total first letter returns (p<0.001) and environment and class clusters (p<0.001), as well as in the total environment and class intersections (p<0.001). There were also significant differences in the total paraphasia errors (p=0.003) and total derivations (p=0.012), between the two categories studied, presenting a higher occurrence in the mean of the “animals” category, however, with values ​​close to 1 word uttered. The analysis of the comparison of the different time intervals showed significant differences between the categories, with those being first letter switches and class and environment and class clusters (p<0.05). Table 2 compares the SVF performance of the participants in the “animal” and “fruit” categories and presents the mean (standard deviation) and the significance value of the comparison between them (p value).

Table 2
Comparison of the performance of participants in the Semantic Verbal Fluency test

Regarding the comparison of the performance of the HE and VHE groups in the “animal” and “fruit” categories, the results of the analysis of the “animal” category indicated a significant difference in the total number of words (p=0.044), with a greater number of words in the VHE group (24.85 ± 6.02) in comparison to the HE group (20.75 ± 4.42). There was a statistically significant difference in the total words during the first 15 seconds of the test (p=0.023), with a greater number of words in the VHE group (10.92 ± 2.46) than in the HE group (8.56 ± 2.75). The results also indicated that the VHE group performed a greater number of switches, in relation to the first letter of the word (p=0.039) and in the time interval between 0 and 15 seconds (p=0.024). Significant differences were observed between groups for the environment cluster (30-45s) (p=0.007) and class cluster (0-15s) variables (p=0.025). For the “fruit” category, there was no difference in relation to the total number of words, errors, and fruit climate, between both groups. However, there was a statistically significant difference in the total number of words in the (45-60s) interval (p=0.019) and in the total first letter returns (p=0.036), as well as greater production of fruit flavor and climate switches at different intervals of time, especially in the final 15 seconds (flavor p=0.013 and climate p=0.024). Table 3 shows the comparison of the performance of the HE and VHE groups in the “animal” and “fruit” categories.

Table 3
Comparison of the participants' performance in the two categories of the Semantic Verbal Fluency Test considering their educational level

Regarding the performance comparison between genders for the two SVF categories, it was observed that, in the “animals” category, there was a significant difference in the total of words (p<0.001), with better performances being linked to men (27.75 ± 6.47), when compared to women (20.60 ± 3.61). There was a significant difference in the time blocks from 0 to 15 seconds (p=0.038), 30 to 45 seconds (p=0.012), and 45 to 60 seconds (p=0.012). Regarding animal environment, the male group showed a higher occurrence of clustering (p<0.001) in the time intervals from 0 to 15 seconds (p=0.033) and 30 to 45 seconds (p=0.041), intersection (p< 0.001), and switching, from 30 to 45 seconds (p=0.048). They performed a greater number of first letter clusters in the interval between 30 and 45 seconds (p=0.048) and switching in the last 30 seconds of the test (p=0.040). There was also a greater number of clusters of the animal class in the time interval from 0 to 15 seconds (p<0.001), with the male group always obtaining higher scores. For the “fruit” category, the results indicated the absence of statistically significant differences between the groups. Table 4 shows the total SVF performance in the two semantic categories between men and women.

Table 4
Comparison of performance in the two categories of the Semantic Verbal Fluency Test considering the gender of the participants

DISCUSSION

The objectives of the present study were to compare the performance of a group of healthy older adults with a high educational level in the quantitative and qualitative analyses of the SVF in the “animals” and “fruits” categories, to compare the performance of older adults with different educational levels, high and very high, in the SVF, in the “animal” and “fruit” categories, and to compare the performance between men and women in the SVF, based on the analysis of quantitative variables (performance by test run time) and qualitative variables, such as clustering, switching, intersection, and return.

In the “animals” category, the performance of older adults with a high educational level was better than in the “fruits” category, both in quantitative and qualitative analyses. A better performance in the “animals” category, compared to other categories, has already been described in previous studies(99 Acevedo A, Loewenstein DA, Barker WW, Harwood DG, Luis C, Bravo M, et al. Category Fluency Test: normative data for English- and Spanish-speaking elderly. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):760-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677032. PMid:11105466.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677...
,1818 Bolla KI, Gray S, Resnick SM, Galante R, Kawas C. Category and letter fluency in highly educated older adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;12(3):330-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/clin.12.3.330.1986.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/clin.12.3.330....
,2929 Nikolai T, Bezdicek O, Markova H, Stepankova H, Michalec J, Kopecek M, et al. Semantic verbal fluency impairment is detectable in patients with subjective cognitive decline. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2018;25(5):448-57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2017.1326047. PMid:28548549.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2017....
). The smaller number of responses in the “fruit” category may justify the lower occurrence of errors such as paraphasias and derivations. The significant differences in the occurrence of paraphasia and derivation, with higher values ​​in the “animals” category, can also be justified by the possibility of conjugations regarding gender, in this category (for example, lion/lioness), which does not occur in “fruits”. However, as expected, the values ​​were low, as it was a group of healthy participants. It is also worth considering that no studies comparing errors between the “animals” and “fruits” categories were found.

The performance analysis of the SVF in the “animals” category, in the different educational level groups, revealed that the group of older adults with VHE obtained higher scores than those of the HE group, in the total of words (including 0 to 15 seconds), clustering (class and 0 to 15 seconds), switching (first letter, first letter [0 to 15 seconds], and class), and intersection (class). For the “fruit” category, this variable had significant effects on the production of first letter switching, first letter in the last 30 seconds, flavor and climate from 45 to 60 seconds, and return to the first letter of the word.

In both categories, it was noted that the VHE group made more switches, compared to individuals in the HE group. Thus, the results suggest the influence of the years of study on performance, which is in line with the data obtained in the study by Pereira et al.(2020 Pereira AH, Gonçalves AB, Holz M, Gonçalves HA, Kochhann R, Joanette Y, et al. Influence of age and education on the processing of clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2018;12(4):360-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004. PMid:30546845.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018d...
), in which the group with the highest educational level obtained more numbers of switches, compared to the lower educational level group. It is worth noting, in the analyses of this study, that the switching strategy was better for the VHE group and Table 3 shows several comparisons that showed greater use of this strategy for the “fruits” category, which seems to require greater complexity of cognitive activation, according to the comparison of the results between the two categories. The evocation of words within a group (clustering) and change of groups (switching) are abilities dependent on cognitive flexibility, cognitive function, and lexical organization, which are directly related to educational level. Thus, it is expected that individuals with more years of education will obtain a greater number of words, making greater use of this type of strategies(1919 Chiossi JSC, Soares AD, Chiari BM. Clustering e switching em deficientes auditivos usuários do português brasileiro: fluência verbal semântica e fonológica. Rev CEFAC. 2016;18(2):369-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201618211815.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620161...
). According to Pereira et al.(2020 Pereira AH, Gonçalves AB, Holz M, Gonçalves HA, Kochhann R, Joanette Y, et al. Influence of age and education on the processing of clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2018;12(4):360-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004. PMid:30546845.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018d...
), the return strategy requires mnemonic skills, such as episodic and working memory, to remember the previously mentioned subcategory, as well as mental flexibility to search for other words within the same category.

The gender variable had an influence on the SVF in a particular way for each category studied. Men achieved better results in the “animals” category, compared to women, like in previous studies(1616 Castro-Costa E, Lima-Costa MF, Andrade FB, Souza PRB Jr, Ferri CP. Cognitive function among older adults: ELSI-Brazil results. Rev Saude Publica. 2018 Out 52(Suppl 2):4s. http://dx.doi.org/10.11606/s1518-8787.2018052000629. PMid:30379286.
http://dx.doi.org/10.11606/s1518-8787.20...
,1717 Silva TBL, Yassuda MS, Guimaraes VV, Florindo AA. Fluência verbal e variáveis sociodemográficas no processo de envelhecimento: um estudo epidemiológico. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2011;24(4):739-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722011000400014.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722011...
). It is noteworthy that the study developed by Castro-Costa et al.(1616 Castro-Costa E, Lima-Costa MF, Andrade FB, Souza PRB Jr, Ferri CP. Cognitive function among older adults: ELSI-Brazil results. Rev Saude Publica. 2018 Out 52(Suppl 2):4s. http://dx.doi.org/10.11606/s1518-8787.2018052000629. PMid:30379286.
http://dx.doi.org/10.11606/s1518-8787.20...
) also assessed a sample of participants from the Midwestern region of Brazil. In the “fruit” category, women had a slightly higher mean in the total of words, however, the difference had no statistical significance, whereas in the study by Acevedo(99 Acevedo A, Loewenstein DA, Barker WW, Harwood DG, Luis C, Bravo M, et al. Category Fluency Test: normative data for English- and Spanish-speaking elderly. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):760-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677032. PMid:11105466.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677...
), a better performance was found in the total number of words within the same category.

According to the literature, gender-related differences in SVF performance are mainly derived from the test category and would be related mainly to social roles(3030 Rivera D, Olabarrieta-Landa L, Van der Elst W, Gonzalez I, Rodríguez-Agudelo Y, Aguayo Arelis A, et al. Normative data for verbal fluency in healthy Latin American adults: letter M, and fruits and occupations categories. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(3):287-300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000518. PMid:30667250.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000518...
), culturally inherited, and associated with masculinity and femininity archetypes, in which men are seen as providers, working outside home, and women are seen as a caregivers, working at home. Additionally, it is important to emphasize the importance of early childhood in the development of cognitive skills, since men are more stimulated early on the names of animals, an activity that is historically considered more masculine, and women are stimulated with activities that involve motherhood and activities domestic. About the names of fruits, in early childhood, children of both genders are encouraged to eat them, which provides greater equity in the comparison between genders. Studies report that a better performance on the test would be associated with a more stimulating environment, a broader vocabulary, as well as with the greater development of reading and writing skills, among other factors(3030 Rivera D, Olabarrieta-Landa L, Van der Elst W, Gonzalez I, Rodríguez-Agudelo Y, Aguayo Arelis A, et al. Normative data for verbal fluency in healthy Latin American adults: letter M, and fruits and occupations categories. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(3):287-300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000518. PMid:30667250.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000518...
).

Regarding the analysis by time blocks, several studies propose that, during the execution of the test, the number of emissions gradually decreases(1919 Chiossi JSC, Soares AD, Chiari BM. Clustering e switching em deficientes auditivos usuários do português brasileiro: fluência verbal semântica e fonológica. Rev CEFAC. 2016;18(2):369-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201618211815.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620161...
,2929 Nikolai T, Bezdicek O, Markova H, Stepankova H, Michalec J, Kopecek M, et al. Semantic verbal fluency impairment is detectable in patients with subjective cognitive decline. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2018;25(5):448-57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2017.1326047. PMid:28548549.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2017....
). As observed in the present research, the values ​​referring to the total of words decrease every 15 seconds of testing, especially in the second time block, in both categories.

Regarding the identification of the use of strategies, the older adults in this study did not rely on the first letter of a word as a method to produce the greatest possible number of utterances within a category. No studies were found that analyzed the first letter strategy in the semantic type of the test and offered a unique contribution to this research. A plausible assumption is the number of letters in the alphabet, which allows for a greater variety of responses.

The present study had some limitations. First, the sample size was small. Secondly, it was observed that there was a prevalence of female participants and, thirdly, complementary analyses of vocabulary, as well as performance in reading and writing tasks were not included.

CONCLUSION

There were quantitative and qualitative differences in the performance of healthy older adults with a high educational level in the “animal” and “fruit” categories. When comparing the HE and VHE groups, there was a better performance of the VHE group in both categories, however, there were no differences between them for the “fruit” category. Regarding gender, men performed better in the “animals” category. Thus, the application of the SVF, whether in the clinical or research context, must consider the impact of demographic variables on the quantitative and qualitative performance and value a cautious qualitative analysis of the results.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

To the volunteers from the Federal District (DF-Brazil) who agreed to participate in this study.

  • Study carried out at Curso de Fonoaudiologia, Faculdade de Ceilândia, Universidade de Brasília – UNB – Brasília (DF), Brasil.
  • Funding: None.

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Balthazar MLF, Cendes F, Damasceno BP. Category verbal fluency performance may be impaired in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dement Neuropsychol. 2007;1(2):161-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1980-57642008dn10200008 PMid:29213383.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1980-57642008dn10200008
  • 2
    Henry JD, Crawford JR. A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance following focal cortical lesions. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(2):284-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.2.284 PMID: 15099151.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.2.284
  • 3
    IBGE: Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística. Idosos indicam caminhos para uma melhor idade [Internet]. Rio de Janeiro: IBGE; 2018 [citado em 2020 Set 9]. Disponível em: https://censo2020.ibge.gov.br/2012-agencia-de-noticias/noticias/24036-idosos-indicam-caminhos-para-uma-melhor-idade.html
    » https://censo2020.ibge.gov.br/2012-agencia-de-noticias/noticias/24036-idosos-indicam-caminhos-para-uma-melhor-idade.html
  • 4
    Charchat-Fichman H, Caramelli P, Sameshima K, Nitrini R. Declínio da capacidade cognitiva durante o envelhecimento. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2005;27(21):79-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462005000100017 PMid:15867989.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462005000100017
  • 5
    Harada CN, Natelson Love MC, Triebel KL. Normal cognitive aging. Clin Geriatr Med. 2013;29(4):737-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002
  • 6
    Martins NIM, Caldas PR, Cabral ED, Lins CCSA, Coriolano MGWS. Instrumentos de avaliação cognitiva utilizados nos últimos cinco anos em idosos brasileiros. Cien Saude Colet. 2019;24(7):2513-30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1413-81232018247.20862017 PMid:31340270.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1413-81232018247.20862017
  • 7
    Lezak M, Howieson D, Bigler E, Tranel D. Neuropsychological assessment. 5th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2012
  • 8
    Brucki SMD, Rocha MSG. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(12):1771-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002 PMid:15558183.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002
  • 9
    Acevedo A, Loewenstein DA, Barker WW, Harwood DG, Luis C, Bravo M, et al. Category Fluency Test: normative data for English- and Spanish-speaking elderly. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):760-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677032 PMid:11105466.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617700677032
  • 10
    Fichman HC, Fernandes CS, Nitrini R, Lourenço RA, Paradela EMP, Carthery-Goulart MT, et al. Age and educational level effects on the performance of normal elderly on category verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2009;3(1):49-54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30100010 PMid:29213610.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30100010
  • 11
    Souza BCS, Teixeira TOB, Silva LD, Satler C, Cera ML. Fluência verbal de adultos e idosos do Distrito Federal: proposta normativa piloto. Audiol Commun Res. 2020;25:e2315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2020-2315
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-6431-2020-2315
  • 12
    Cavaco S, Gonçalves A, Pinto C, Almeida E, Gomes F, Moreira I, et al. Semantic fluency and phonemic fluency: regression-based norms for the Portuguese population. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2013;28(3):262-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/act001 PMid:23341434.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/act001
  • 13
    Esteves CS, Oliveira CR, Moret-Tatay C, Navarro-Pardo E, Carli GAD, Silva IG, et al. Phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks: normative data for elderly Brazilians. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2015;28(2):350-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678-7153.201528215
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678-7153.201528215
  • 14
    Mathuranath PS, George A, Cherian PJ, Alexander A, Sarma SG, Sarma PS. Effects of age, education and gender on verbal fluency. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2003;25(8):1057-64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/jcen.25.8.1057.16736 PMid:14566579.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/jcen.25.8.1057.16736
  • 15
    Santos Nogueira D, Azevedo Reis E, Vieira A. Verbal fluency tasks: effects of age, gender, and education. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2016;68(3):124-33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000450640 PMid:27915338.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000450640
  • 16
    Castro-Costa E, Lima-Costa MF, Andrade FB, Souza PRB Jr, Ferri CP. Cognitive function among older adults: ELSI-Brazil results. Rev Saude Publica. 2018 Out 52(Suppl 2):4s. http://dx.doi.org/10.11606/s1518-8787.2018052000629 PMid:30379286.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.11606/s1518-8787.2018052000629
  • 17
    Silva TBL, Yassuda MS, Guimaraes VV, Florindo AA. Fluência verbal e variáveis sociodemográficas no processo de envelhecimento: um estudo epidemiológico. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2011;24(4):739-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722011000400014
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722011000400014
  • 18
    Bolla KI, Gray S, Resnick SM, Galante R, Kawas C. Category and letter fluency in highly educated older adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 1998;12(3):330-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/clin.12.3.330.1986
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/clin.12.3.330.1986
  • 19
    Chiossi JSC, Soares AD, Chiari BM. Clustering e switching em deficientes auditivos usuários do português brasileiro: fluência verbal semântica e fonológica. Rev CEFAC. 2016;18(2):369-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201618211815
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216201618211815
  • 20
    Pereira AH, Gonçalves AB, Holz M, Gonçalves HA, Kochhann R, Joanette Y, et al. Influence of age and education on the processing of clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2018;12(4):360-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004 PMid:30546845.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004
  • 21
    Wajman JR, Cecchini MA, Bertolucci PHF, Mansur LL. Quanti-qualitative components of the semantic verbal fluency test in cognitively healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia subtypes. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(6):533-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426 PMid:30375889.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426
  • 22
    Farghaly M, Hussein M, Hassan A, Hegazy M, Sabbah A. Testing of verbal fluency in egyptians: cultural and educational challenges. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2018;31(3):133-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000160 PMid:30239463.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/WNN.0000000000000160
  • 23
    Olabarrieta-Landa L, Rivera D, Lara L, Rute-Pérez S, Rodríguez-Lorenzana A, Galarza-Del-Angel J, et al. Verbal fluency tests: normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation. 2017;41(3):673-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/NRE-172240 PMid:28946591.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/NRE-172240
  • 24
    Cesar KG, Yassuda MS, Porto FHG, Brucki SMD, Nitrini R. MoCA Test: normative and diagnostic accuracy data for seniors with heterogeneous educational levels in Brazil. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2019;77(11):775-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0004-282x20190130 PMid:31826133.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0004-282x20190130
  • 25
    Cunha J. Manual da versão em português das Escalas Beck. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2001.
  • 26
    Gorenstein C, Pang WY, Argimon IL, Werlang BSG. Inventário Beck de Depressão-II. Manual. São Paulo, SP: Casa do Psicólogo; 2011.
  • 27
    Rodrigues AB, Yamashita ET, Chiappetta ALML. Teste de fluência verbal no adulto e no idoso: verificação da aprendizagem verbal. Rev CEFAC. 2008;10(4):443-51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462008000400004
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462008000400004
  • 28
    Machado TH, Fichman HC, Santos EL, Carvalho VA, Fialho PP, Koenig AM, et al. Normative data for healthy elderly on the phonemic verbal fluency task - FAS. Dement Neuropsychol. 2009;3(1):55-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30100011 PMid:29213611.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN30100011
  • 29
    Nikolai T, Bezdicek O, Markova H, Stepankova H, Michalec J, Kopecek M, et al. Semantic verbal fluency impairment is detectable in patients with subjective cognitive decline. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2018;25(5):448-57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2017.1326047 PMid:28548549.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2017.1326047
  • 30
    Rivera D, Olabarrieta-Landa L, Van der Elst W, Gonzalez I, Rodríguez-Agudelo Y, Aguayo Arelis A, et al. Normative data for verbal fluency in healthy Latin American adults: letter M, and fruits and occupations categories. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(3):287-300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000518 PMid:30667250.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000518

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    05 July 2021
  • Date of issue
    2021

History

  • Received
    01 Feb 2021
  • Accepted
    05 Mar 2021
Academia Brasileira de Audiologia Rua Itapeva, 202, conjunto 61, CEP 01332-000, Tel.: (11) 3253-8711, Fax: (11) 3253-8473 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@audiologiabrasil.org.br