Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Dichotic Sentences Test Performance of Adults with communication complaints

ABSTRACT

Purpose

To analyze the performance of normal-hearing adults with communication complaints in the Dichotic Sentences Test.

Methods

We selected from the database 15 normal-hearing participants with normal results in the Digits Dichotic Test, aged between 19 and 44 years, right-handed, who reported communicative complaints. The Dichotic Sentences Test was applied using two protocols consisting of four different combinations of lists called sequences 1 and 2, in the following order: training, divided attention step, right and left directed attention steps.

Results

In the first application sequence, the average performance in the divided attention step was 84.7% for the right ear and 60.67% for the left, with statistical difference between ears. The asymmetry between ears varied from -50% to 60%. In the directed attention step, the average performance was 99.33% for the right ear and 98% for the left, with no statistical difference. In the second application sequence, there was a tendency for better results, more pronounced for the left ear, with no statistical significance, with the performance variation and asymmetry between ears remaining high. In the comparison between the sequences, in the divided attention step, it was found that, for the right ear, 40% of the individuals did not vary, 33% performed worse, and 26.7% performed better; for the left ear, 6.6% did not vary, 20% performed worse, and 73.33% performed better. There was result stability in the directed attention step.

Conclusion

The normal-hearing adults with communication complaints presented a heterogeneous profile, especially in the divided attention step, with a marked difference between ears and response variability.

Keywords:
Hearing; Auditory Perception; Hearing Tests; Adult; Medical History Taking

RESUMO

Objetivo

Analisar o desempenho de adultos normo-ouvintes com queixa comunicativa no Teste Dicótico de Sentenças.

Método

Selecionou-se em banco de dados, 15 participantes normo-ouvintes com resultados normais no Teste Dicótico de Dígitos, idades entre 19 a 44 anos, destros, que referiram queixa comunicativa. O Teste de Dicótico de Sentenças foi aplicado por meio de dois protocolos compostos por quatro diferentes combinações de listas denominados de sequências 1 e 2, de acordo com a seguinte ordem: treino, etapa de atenção dividida, etapas de atenção direcionada à direita e à esquerda.

Resultados

Na sequência de aplicação 1, a média de desempenho na etapa de atenção dividida foi 84,67% na orelha direita e 60,67% na esquerda, com diferença estatística entre orelhas, com assimetria entre orelhas variando de -50% a 60%. Na etapa de atenção direcionada, a média de desempenho foi 99,33% na orelha direita e 98% na esquerda, sem diferença estatística. Na sequência de aplicação 2, houve tendência de melhores resultados, mais acentuada na orelha esquerda, sem significância estatística, mantendo elevadas a variação de desempenho e a assimetria entre orelhas. Verificou-se na comparação entre as sequências, na etapa de atenção dividida, que na orelha direita 40% dos indivíduos não variaram, 33% pioraram, 26,7% melhoraram; na orelha esquerda, 6,6% não variaram, 20% pioraram e 73,33% melhoraram. Na etapa de atenção direcionada houve estabilidade nos resultados.

Conclusão

Os adultos normo-ouvintes, com queixas comunicativas, apresentaram um perfil heterogêneo, especialmente na etapa de atenção dividida, com acentuada diferença entre as orelhas e variabilidade de respostas.

Descritores:
Audição; Percepção Auditiva; Testes Auditivos; Adulto; Anamnese

INTRODUCTION

The sense of hearing is what makes us able to apprehend the physical world of sound vibrations and, from that, form the mental images of the lived experiences(11 Pereira LD, Schochat E. Testes auditivos comportamentais para avaliação do processamento auditivo central. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2011.). In our everyday lives, we are exposed to countless simultaneous auditory stimuli, requiring hearing integrity not only at the peripheral level but also at the central level for effective communication to be established(22 Mendes-Civitella M, Comerlatto A Jr, Ferreira MIC, Guedes MC, Balen AS, Frota S. Guia de orientação. Avaliação e intervenção no processamento auditivo central. Brasília: Conselho Federal de Fonoaudiologia; 2020.).

In the clinical routine, there is an increase in adult individuals who seek audiological assessments for presenting low academic or professional performance, reporting complaints such as difficulties hearing, paying attention, and memorizing, but especially understanding speech, primarily in unfavorable situations ​(with competing verbal messages) However, a large share of such individuals presents auditory thresholds within normality standards, which, in turn, renders the performance of the Central Auditory Processing (CAP) assessment fundamental to investigate the origin of such complaints(33 Turcatto LG, Scharlach RC, Braga J Jr, Pinheiro MMC. Time-compressed speech test in adults with and without central auditory processing disorders. Rev CEFAC. 2020;22(4):e2520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/20202242520.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/2020...
).

CAP is a set of auditory skills responsible for the perception and processing of acoustic information(44 ASHA: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association [Internet]. ASHA Practice Policy. Rockville: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association; c2005 [citado em 2020 Nov]. Disponível em: https://www.asha.org/policy
https://www.asha.org/policy...
). For its assessment, there is a wide range of behavioral tests that evaluate the auditory function and its relationship with communication(55 Andrade AN, Gil D, Iório MCM. Benchmarks for the dichotic sentence identification test in Brazilian Portuguese for ear and age. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2015;81(5):459-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2015.07.003. PMid:26277591.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2015.0...
). Among them are the dichotic listening tests, with the use of at least one of such tests in CAP assessment being proposed(44 ASHA: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association [Internet]. ASHA Practice Policy. Rockville: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association; c2005 [citado em 2020 Nov]. Disponível em: https://www.asha.org/policy
https://www.asha.org/policy...
,66 AAA: American Academy of Audiology [Internet]. Diagnosis, treatment and management of children and adults with central auditory processing disorder. Clinical practice guidelines. Reston: American Academy of Audiology; 2010 [citado em 2021 Jun]. Disponível em: https://audiology-web.s3.amazonaws.com/migrated/CAPD%20Guidelines%208-2010.pdf_539952af956c79.73897613.pdf
https://audiology-web.s3.amazonaws.com/m...
).

This type of task may have, as a verbal stimulus, syllables, digits, words, and sentences, and corresponds to the concomitant presentation of two competing stimuli in both ears, enabling the performance of binaural integration and separation tasks with the purpose of investigating the figure-ground skill(77 Roup CM, Leigh ED. Individual differences in behavioral and electrophysiological measures of binaural processing across the adult life span. Am J Audiol. 2015;24(2):204-15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJA-14-0017. PMid:25651479.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJA-14-00...
), in addition to providing information on the attention, auditory pathway maturation, executive function, and hemispheric and inter-hemispheric function via the corpus callosum(88 Kimura D. From ear to brain. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(2):214-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.009. PMid:21236541.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.1...

9 D’Anselmo A, Marzoli D, Brancucci A. The influence of memory and attention on the ear advantage in dichotic listening. Hear Res. 2016;342:144-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016.10.012. PMid:27770621.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016....
-1010 Westerhausen R. A primer on dichotic listening as a paradigm for the assessment of hemispheric asymmetry. Laterality. 2019;24(6):740-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2019.1598426. PMid:30922169.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2019....
).

Recently, the Dichotic Sentences Test (DST) was developed(1111 Costa MJ, Santos SN. Development of the Brazilian Portuguese dichotic sentence lists test. Audiol Commun Res. 2016;21(1):1-8.), aiming to investigate the performance of an individual in competing listening situations through stimuli composed of simple sentences that represent everyday situations.

For being a new test, with characteristics differing from the other dichotic listening tests found in the Brazilian and international literature, measures are necessary to establish parameters in different populations that guide the evaluator in the interpretation of the results, which, thus, justifies the conduction of this research, focused on investigating the test sensitivity. Considering the aspects mentioned above, this study aimed to analyze and compare the performance of normal-hearing adults with communication complaints in the Dichotic Sentences Test in application sequences 1 and 2.

METHODS

This is an applied, experimental, quantitative-analytical study of retrospective nature conducted by analyzing the database connected to a research project previously approved by the Research Ethics Committee under protocol No. 2.764.720. To compose the study group, the assessments performed from October 2016 to February 2017 of normal-hearing adult patients aged 19 to 44 years were analyzed.

The study group was selected from a database consisting of 101 individuals submitted to a specific anamnesis, basic audiological assessment, to obtain the tone thresholds and middle ear conditions. As for the assessment of the auditory skills of binaural integration and separation, the Dichotic Digit Test was performed in the integration stage, and the dichotic test of sentences, sequences 1 and 2, in the stages of divided and directed attention to the right and to the left, whose stimuli were presented at a level of 50 dB SL in relation to the tritone average (frequency of 500, 1000 and 2000 Hz).

To select the sample, we used the following eligibility criteria: having normal-hearing, considering audibility thresholds less than or equal to 25 dB HL(1212 Lloyd LL, Kaplan H. Audiometric interpretation: a manual of basic audiometry. Baltimore: University Park Press; 1978.) with Dichotic Digits Test (DDT) performance greater than or equal to 95%(11 Pereira LD, Schochat E. Testes auditivos comportamentais para avaliação do processamento auditivo central. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2011.) for both ears in the binaural integration step; being right-handed; having at least a complete high school education; having referred in the anamnesis one or more complaints related to the difficulty of understanding speech in unfavorable listening situations, such as in reverberant environments or in the presence of competitive noise, as well as presenting a complaint of tinnitus and aspects related to attention and memory, emphasizing that these were pointed out by them as factors that could be associated with the main complaint mentioned.

We excluded individuals who presented earwax excess, possible conductive aspects, and evident or self-reported neurological and/or verbal fluency alterations. Hence, the sample group was composed of fifteen individuals, with six male and nine female and an average age of 28.7 years.

The measurements with the Sentences Dichotic Test were obtained by applying two protocols composed of four different combinations of lists called Sequences 1 and 2, according to the following order: training, divided attention step, right and left directed attention step(1111 Costa MJ, Santos SN. Development of the Brazilian Portuguese dichotic sentence lists test. Audiol Commun Res. 2016;21(1):1-8.). All participants were instructed on how to present the stimuli and the response (verbal repetition) required for each step of the assessments.

The second application sequence was performed with the purpose of investigating the repeatability of the results obtained in the first application, given that it is a new instrument with verbal stimulus and application characteristics different from all dichotic tests that exist in Portuguese and, for this reason, there is no test that may be considered the gold standard for analyzing comparison and sensitivity.

Regarding the DST performance analysis, the score determined was 10% for each sentence, considering that each application list of both sequences is formed by a set of ten pairs of sentences, thus totalizing 100% per ear for each step. To assess the responses obtained in the different test steps, the complete repetition of the entire presented sentences was considered correct(1111 Costa MJ, Santos SN. Development of the Brazilian Portuguese dichotic sentence lists test. Audiol Commun Res. 2016;21(1):1-8.).

To analyze the differences in the scores obtained between the ears, it was considered that a negative score meant that it was better for the left ear, a zero score, that there was no difference between the ears, and a positive score, that it was better for the right ear.

In turn, when the differences in the scores obtained in the first and second application sequences were analyzed, they were considered according to the ear side using the following criteria: obtaining a negative score meant that the performance of the respective ear was better in the second sequence than in the first; a score of zero meant that the same score was obtained in both sequences; a positive score meant that the first sequence had a better score than the second.

The assessments were carried out by a single evaluator in an acoustically treated room with a Grason-Stadler GSI-61 audiometer and TDH 50 in-ear headphones. The DDTs and DSTs were presented digitally recorded in WAV format through a tablet coupled to the audiometer.

The data were analyzed statistically using the SPSS program. The Shapiro Wilk test was applied using the 5% level as a significance determination criterion to verify the normality of the variables. The non-parametric Wilcoxon test was used to investigate the performance of the individuals in the DST and the different applications of the instrument since the normality hypothesis was rejected. The analyses that presented p ≤ 0.05 were deemed statistically significant.

RESULTS

Table 1 shows the absolute performance values in the divided and directed attention steps of the DST of adults with communication complaints in Table 1.

Table 1
Analysis of the performance of normal-hearing adults with communication complaints in the Dichotic Sentences Test in the divided and directed attention steps in the first application sequence

In Table 2, one may observe the differences in the performance obtained between the right and left ears in the divided attention step of the first application sequence.

Table 2
Differences between the scores obtained between the right and left ears in the first application sequence in the divided attention step of the Dichotic Sentences Test (n = 15)

The comparison of the performance of the adults with communicative complaints in the divided and directed attention steps between DST Application Sequences 1 and 2 may be observed in Table 3.

Table 3
Comparison between the performances obtained by adults with communication complaints in the Dichotic Sentences Test in the divided and directed attention steps in the first and second application sequences

Table 4 presents the performance variations between the right and left ears obtained with the second DST application sequence.

Table 4
Differences between the scores obtained between the right and left ears in the second application sequence in the divided attention step of the Dichotic Sentences Test (n = 15)

In the comparison of the performance of adults with communicative complaints between Application Sequences 1 and 2 according to the ear side, one may observe the difference between the scores in the divided attention step in Figure 1 and the difference between the scores in the directed attention step in Figure 2.

Figure 1
Difference between the scores obtained in the first and second application sequences in the divided attention step according to the ear side
Figure 2
Difference between the scores obtained in the first and second application sequences in the directed attention step according to the ear side

DISCUSSION

In the present study, based on the analysis of the performance of adults with communicative complaints evaluated with the SDT, in application sequence 1, we found that the mean performance in the divided attention stage was 84.67% in the right ear and 60.67% in the left ear (Table 1), which is lower than expected for this age group(1313 Costa MJ, Santos SN, Schochat E. Dichotic sentence identification test in Portuguese: a study in young adults. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;87(4):478-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.11.018. PMid:33461909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.1...
). The mean value of correct answers found in a study carried out with the same test material(1313 Costa MJ, Santos SN, Schochat E. Dichotic sentence identification test in Portuguese: a study in young adults. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;87(4):478-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.11.018. PMid:33461909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.1...
), which assessed adults without complaints, were 93.59% in the right ear and 86.06% in the left ear, and it was suggested, as normality by these authors in the RE, values greater or equal to 90% of correct answers and, in the LE, greater or equal to 80% of correct answers.

Results similar to these were found in a study carried out with the Dichotic Sentences Identification Test (DSI) in Brazilian Portuguese(55 Andrade AN, Gil D, Iório MCM. Benchmarks for the dichotic sentence identification test in Brazilian Portuguese for ear and age. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2015;81(5):459-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2015.07.003. PMid:26277591.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2015.0...
), with an average performance of 93.70% of hits for the right ear and 88.60% for the left; despite having characteristics different from those of the test used in this study, it also showed similar results in adults with no complaints.

From these findings, it was possible to verify the influence of the communication complaints on the performance of normal-hearing adults assessed through the DST compared to other studies(1313 Costa MJ, Santos SN, Schochat E. Dichotic sentence identification test in Portuguese: a study in young adults. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;87(4):478-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.11.018. PMid:33461909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.1...
,1414 Ferreira GC, Costa MJ. Variability of the dichotic sentence test in the normal hearing adults test and retest. CoDAS. 2020;32(3):e20190040. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20202019040. PMid:32609223.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/2020...
) developed with normal-hearing adults with no complaints, considering that the complaints were the only diverging aspects between the analyzed groups.

The common behavioral manifestations reported during the initial interview, such as difficulty understanding speech in conflict situations (noise or speech) or reverberating acoustic environments, difficulty following fast speech, and frequent requests for repetition, among others, are possible indicators of risk of CAP alteration. Hence, these issues must be raised, considering that the basic audiological assessment employed may not indicate an alteration because it often ends up not representing the real functional impact since the CAP involves many processes measured at different levels of the central auditory system(66 AAA: American Academy of Audiology [Internet]. Diagnosis, treatment and management of children and adults with central auditory processing disorder. Clinical practice guidelines. Reston: American Academy of Audiology; 2010 [citado em 2021 Jun]. Disponível em: https://audiology-web.s3.amazonaws.com/migrated/CAPD%20Guidelines%208-2010.pdf_539952af956c79.73897613.pdf
https://audiology-web.s3.amazonaws.com/m...
).

Besides the inferior results relative to individuals with no complaints, this sample group presented a more considerable performance variation in the divided attention step, with results ranging from -50% (advantage of the left ear) to 60% (advantage of the right ear), whereas the differences varied from -20% to 30%for the group within normality(1313 Costa MJ, Santos SN, Schochat E. Dichotic sentence identification test in Portuguese: a study in young adults. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;87(4):478-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.11.018. PMid:33461909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.1...
). During the literary search to compose this work, no studies were found with sample groups constituted of adults in the same age range as this study that could justify these findings, given that the heterogeneous profile and results so inferior to normality draw attention.

It was also found that the asymmetry between ears was significant, with a marked advantage of the right ear. The literature points to the existence of an advantage of the right ear in dichotic listening tests with right-handed individuals(55 Andrade AN, Gil D, Iório MCM. Benchmarks for the dichotic sentence identification test in Brazilian Portuguese for ear and age. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2015;81(5):459-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2015.07.003. PMid:26277591.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2015.0...

6 AAA: American Academy of Audiology [Internet]. Diagnosis, treatment and management of children and adults with central auditory processing disorder. Clinical practice guidelines. Reston: American Academy of Audiology; 2010 [citado em 2021 Jun]. Disponível em: https://audiology-web.s3.amazonaws.com/migrated/CAPD%20Guidelines%208-2010.pdf_539952af956c79.73897613.pdf
https://audiology-web.s3.amazonaws.com/m...

7 Roup CM, Leigh ED. Individual differences in behavioral and electrophysiological measures of binaural processing across the adult life span. Am J Audiol. 2015;24(2):204-15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJA-14-0017. PMid:25651479.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJA-14-00...

8 Kimura D. From ear to brain. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(2):214-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.009. PMid:21236541.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.1...
-99 D’Anselmo A, Marzoli D, Brancucci A. The influence of memory and attention on the ear advantage in dichotic listening. Hear Res. 2016;342:144-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016.10.012. PMid:27770621.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016....
) resulting from the left hemispheric dominance for linguistic stimuli(1010 Westerhausen R. A primer on dichotic listening as a paradigm for the assessment of hemispheric asymmetry. Laterality. 2019;24(6):740-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2019.1598426. PMid:30922169.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2019....
,1515 Bless JJ, Westerhausen R, von Koss Torkildsen J, Gudmundsen M, Kompus K, Hugdahl K. Laterality across languages: results from a global dichotic listening study using a smartphone application. Laterality. 2015;20(4):434-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2014.997245. PMid:25588000.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2014....
,1616 Musiek FE, Weihing J. Perspectives on dichotic listening and the corpus callosum. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(2):225-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.011. PMid:21531063.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.0...
). Another aspect that justifies this advantage is related to the physiological structure - the dominant contralateral path, which presents more considerable complexity for the processing since the information received by the left ear is initially processed by the right hemisphere and transmitted via callous corpus to the left hemisphere, which in turn increases the acoustic processing time of the left ear, with there being a more significant demand for cognitive resources (attention, memory, executive function)(88 Kimura D. From ear to brain. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(2):214-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.009. PMid:21236541.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.1...
,1717 Westerhausen R, Kompus K. How to get a left-advantage: a technical review of assessing brain asymmetry with dichotic listening. Scand J Psychol. 2018;59(1):66-73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12408. PMid:29356005.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12408...
), so it is thus frequent and expected for there to occur a difference between the ears with the advantage of the right ear.

However, although the asymmetry is expected even for individuals deemed within normality, we highlight that this studied group presented an interaural asymmetry well above the expected for the age range(1313 Costa MJ, Santos SN, Schochat E. Dichotic sentence identification test in Portuguese: a study in young adults. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;87(4):478-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.11.018. PMid:33461909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.1...
), with it being found that 80% of the individuals showed superior performance for the right ear, with score differences ranging from 10% to 60% between ears, and 20% presented a superior performance for the left ear, varying from -10% to -50% (Table 2), while another study referred as expected, within normality, a difference between ears up to 20%, with the advantage of the right ear(1212 Lloyd LL, Kaplan H. Audiometric interpretation: a manual of basic audiometry. Baltimore: University Park Press; 1978.). The difference between ears in dichotic listening was also analyzed by researchers who assessed adult individuals with tone thresholds up to 40 dB HL and suggested that this difference cannot exceed 16%(1818 Fifer RC, Jerger JF, Berlin CI, Tobey EA, Campbell JC. Development of a dichotic sentence identification test for hearing-impaired adults. Ear Hear. 1983;4(6):300-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00003446-198311000-00007. PMid:6653934.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00003446-19831...
).

The inferior performance and more considerable asymmetry between ears found here may be considered indicators of alterations to the auditory processing that may be related to the figure-ground skill, temporal processing, and auditory memory(99 D’Anselmo A, Marzoli D, Brancucci A. The influence of memory and attention on the ear advantage in dichotic listening. Hear Res. 2016;342:144-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016.10.012. PMid:27770621.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016....
,1616 Musiek FE, Weihing J. Perspectives on dichotic listening and the corpus callosum. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(2):225-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.011. PMid:21531063.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.0...
,1919 Babkoff H, Fostick L. Age-related changes in auditory processing and speech perception: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Eur J Ageing. 2017;14(3):269-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-017-0410-y. PMid:28936137.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-017-041...
).

Besides the relationship of the presented communication complaints with the auditory skills mentioned before, we must consider that attention and cognitive control are fundamental for the execution of dichotic listening, also influenced by aspects such as memory, language, and planning ability(2020 Spajdel M. Brain asymmetry for verbal stimuli in relation to dichotic listening performance, verbal and nonverbal intellectual abilities. Act Nerv Super Rediviva. 2017;59(1):33-7.

21 Andrade AN, Rocha JGM, Iório MCM, Gil D. Influence of the age, level of schooling and gender on the occurrence of the DSI test pauses. CoDAS. 2018;30(5):e20170286. PMid:30304129.

22 Hugdahl K, Westerhausen R. Speech processing asymmetry revealed by dichotic listening and functional brain imaging. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93(Pt B):466-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.011. PMid:26706774.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychol...
-2323 Cameron S, Glyde H, Dillon H, Whitfield J, Seymour J. The Dichotic Digits difference Test (DDdT): development, normative data, and test-retest reliability studies part 1. J Am Acad Audiol. 2016;27(6):458-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.15084. PMid:27310404.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.15084...
). Factors such as memory and attention may act significantly on auditory perceptual asymmetry(99 D’Anselmo A, Marzoli D, Brancucci A. The influence of memory and attention on the ear advantage in dichotic listening. Hear Res. 2016;342:144-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016.10.012. PMid:27770621.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016....
).

In turn, in the directed attention step of the first application sequence, a performance equivalence was found, with hit scores varying from 90% to 100% in both ears (Table 1), which agrees with the findings of other studies carried out with the DST that presented slightly inferior results for the left ear relative to the right ear(1111 Costa MJ, Santos SN. Development of the Brazilian Portuguese dichotic sentence lists test. Audiol Commun Res. 2016;21(1):1-8.,1313 Costa MJ, Santos SN, Schochat E. Dichotic sentence identification test in Portuguese: a study in young adults. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;87(4):478-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.11.018. PMid:33461909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.1...
,1414 Ferreira GC, Costa MJ. Variability of the dichotic sentence test in the normal hearing adults test and retest. CoDAS. 2020;32(3):e20190040. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20202019040. PMid:32609223.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/2020...
).

Considering the different results of this sample group relative to the reference values for adults with no complaints present in the literature(1313 Costa MJ, Santos SN, Schochat E. Dichotic sentence identification test in Portuguese: a study in young adults. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;87(4):478-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.11.018. PMid:33461909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.1...
), the analysis of the second application sequence was carried out to verify the constancy of the results, which allowed observing that, in the divided attention step, there was a tendency for better results for both ears, more pronounced for the left ear yet without statistical significance (Table 3). It should be noted that, despite the better performance presented with the second test protocol, it was observed that the performance variation and asymmetry between ears remained high relative to the performance shown in studies carried out with individuals in the same age range without communication complaints(1313 Costa MJ, Santos SN, Schochat E. Dichotic sentence identification test in Portuguese: a study in young adults. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;87(4):478-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.11.018. PMid:33461909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.1...
,1414 Ferreira GC, Costa MJ. Variability of the dichotic sentence test in the normal hearing adults test and retest. CoDAS. 2020;32(3):e20190040. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20202019040. PMid:32609223.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/2020...
). One may verify in the analysis of the difference between ears in the second application sequence that 13.3% of the individuals presented the same performance scores, 66.7% presented differences between ears ranging from 20% to 50% with better performance for the right ear, and 20% of the individuals showed a better performance for the left ear, with a difference between ears ranging from -30% to -20% (Table 4).

Another relevant aspect is the performance between the application sequences according to the ear side; it was found that, in the divided attention step, for the right ear, 40% of the individuals did not vary, 33% performed worse, and 26.7% performed better in the second application of the DST. In turn, there were more considerable variations between the results obtained in the different application sequences for the left ear, with only 6.6% of the individuals not varying, 20% performing worse, and 73.3% performing better (Figure 1).

As verified in the literature, the occurrence of improvements in the second application of an assessment is expected because it is related to the familiarization process(2323 Cameron S, Glyde H, Dillon H, Whitfield J, Seymour J. The Dichotic Digits difference Test (DDdT): development, normative data, and test-retest reliability studies part 1. J Am Acad Audiol. 2016;27(6):458-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.15084. PMid:27310404.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.15084...

24 Frascá MFSS, Lobo IFN, Schochat E. Processamento auditivo em teste e reteste: confiabilidade da avaliação. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;16(1):42-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342011000100009.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342011...
-2525 Alexandre NMC, Gallasch CH, Lima MHM, Rodrigues RCM. A confiabilidade no desenvolvimento e avaliação de instrumentos de medida na área da saúde. Rev Eletr Enf. 2013;15(3):802-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.5216/ree.v15i3.20776.
http://dx.doi.org/10.5216/ree.v15i3.2077...
). However, although Application Sequences 1 and 2 were carried out the same day, it was possible to observe an improvement trend, yet the results obtained were remarkably heterogeneous, with considerable variability when compared to studies carried out with and without an interval between applications(1414 Ferreira GC, Costa MJ. Variability of the dichotic sentence test in the normal hearing adults test and retest. CoDAS. 2020;32(3):e20190040. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20202019040. PMid:32609223.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/2020...
,2626 Bhat M, Palaniswamy HP, Varsha U, Krishna Y. Development and validation of an automated dichotic double word test in Indian English using MATLAB. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;144:110697. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2021.110697. PMid:33799101.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2021....
). It was observed that, in many cases, when there was better performance for the worst ear (left), there was a decline for the best ear (right) in the divided attention step, which indicates that there was a change to the response strategy, thus demonstrating the difficulty that exists in this group for the more complex task.

In the directed attention task, there was no performance variation for the right ear, but the variation occurred for 33.33% of the individuals for the left ear, ranging from 20% to -10% between assessment protocols (Figure 2). A more homogeneous behavior was observed in this task, corroborating other research(55 Andrade AN, Gil D, Iório MCM. Benchmarks for the dichotic sentence identification test in Brazilian Portuguese for ear and age. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2015;81(5):459-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2015.07.003. PMid:26277591.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2015.0...
,1313 Costa MJ, Santos SN, Schochat E. Dichotic sentence identification test in Portuguese: a study in young adults. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;87(4):478-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.11.018. PMid:33461909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.1...
).

Hence, based on the analysis performed and discussed here, the relevance of the communication complaints presented by the patient becomes evident, even if the basic audiological assessment and DDT results are within normality. With the difficulty level being higher for the DST due to the degree of cognitive aspect requirements, especially short-term memory and the high linguistic load level(2727 Hiscock M, Kinsbourne M. Attention and the right-ear advantage: what is the connection? Brain Cogn. 2011;76(2):263-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.016. PMid:21507543.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.0...
), the difference in stimuli and complexity level between the DDT and DST may be a sign for us to direct our attention also to individuals who present communication complaints, even if the results of routine tests are within normality, given that such individuals perceive some difficulties in everyday communication, which may compromise their life quality and professional performance since most sound environments occur in unfavorable listening situations; therefore, the complaints must not be disregarded.

Moreover, it may be determined that the DST proved sensitive to indicate the possibility of auditory processing alterations, but its results must be analyzed together with other CAP tests and, when possible, with complementary assessments that help detect or exclude language or cognition alterations or other factors that may be influencing performance, such as the general health state of the patient, possible tiredness, concern, interest in the performance of the test, in addition to ensuring that the test instructions were understood correctly.

Thus, based on the joint analysis of these data, when an auditory processing alteration is confirmed, it is suggested that the individuals be referred to hearing rehabilitation.

CONCLUSION

The performance of normal-hearing adults with communication complaints was inferior to that expected for the age range, with a striking difference between the ears, especially in the divided attention step, showing a heterogeneous profile due to the considerable response variability observed among the tested individuals and for the same individual when using two test protocols.

  • Study conducted at Programa de Pós-graduação em Distúrbios da Comunicação Humana, Universidade Federal de Santa Maria - UFSM - Santa Maria (RS), Brasil.
  • Financial support: Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior - CAPES. Case number: 42002010017P9.

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Pereira LD, Schochat E. Testes auditivos comportamentais para avaliação do processamento auditivo central. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2011.
  • 2
    Mendes-Civitella M, Comerlatto A Jr, Ferreira MIC, Guedes MC, Balen AS, Frota S. Guia de orientação. Avaliação e intervenção no processamento auditivo central. Brasília: Conselho Federal de Fonoaudiologia; 2020.
  • 3
    Turcatto LG, Scharlach RC, Braga J Jr, Pinheiro MMC. Time-compressed speech test in adults with and without central auditory processing disorders. Rev CEFAC. 2020;22(4):e2520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/20202242520
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/20202242520
  • 4
    ASHA: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association [Internet]. ASHA Practice Policy. Rockville: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association; c2005 [citado em 2020 Nov]. Disponível em: https://www.asha.org/policy
    » https://www.asha.org/policy
  • 5
    Andrade AN, Gil D, Iório MCM. Benchmarks for the dichotic sentence identification test in Brazilian Portuguese for ear and age. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2015;81(5):459-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2015.07.003 PMid:26277591.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2015.07.003
  • 6
    AAA: American Academy of Audiology [Internet]. Diagnosis, treatment and management of children and adults with central auditory processing disorder. Clinical practice guidelines. Reston: American Academy of Audiology; 2010 [citado em 2021 Jun]. Disponível em: https://audiology-web.s3.amazonaws.com/migrated/CAPD%20Guidelines%208-2010.pdf_539952af956c79.73897613.pdf
    » https://audiology-web.s3.amazonaws.com/migrated/CAPD%20Guidelines%208-2010.pdf_539952af956c79.73897613.pdf
  • 7
    Roup CM, Leigh ED. Individual differences in behavioral and electrophysiological measures of binaural processing across the adult life span. Am J Audiol. 2015;24(2):204-15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJA-14-0017 PMid:25651479.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJA-14-0017
  • 8
    Kimura D. From ear to brain. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(2):214-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.009 PMid:21236541.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.11.009
  • 9
    D’Anselmo A, Marzoli D, Brancucci A. The influence of memory and attention on the ear advantage in dichotic listening. Hear Res. 2016;342:144-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016.10.012 PMid:27770621.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2016.10.012
  • 10
    Westerhausen R. A primer on dichotic listening as a paradigm for the assessment of hemispheric asymmetry. Laterality. 2019;24(6):740-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2019.1598426 PMid:30922169.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2019.1598426
  • 11
    Costa MJ, Santos SN. Development of the Brazilian Portuguese dichotic sentence lists test. Audiol Commun Res. 2016;21(1):1-8.
  • 12
    Lloyd LL, Kaplan H. Audiometric interpretation: a manual of basic audiometry. Baltimore: University Park Press; 1978.
  • 13
    Costa MJ, Santos SN, Schochat E. Dichotic sentence identification test in Portuguese: a study in young adults. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;87(4):478-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.11.018 PMid:33461909.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bjorl.2020.11.018
  • 14
    Ferreira GC, Costa MJ. Variability of the dichotic sentence test in the normal hearing adults test and retest. CoDAS. 2020;32(3):e20190040. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20202019040 PMid:32609223.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20202019040
  • 15
    Bless JJ, Westerhausen R, von Koss Torkildsen J, Gudmundsen M, Kompus K, Hugdahl K. Laterality across languages: results from a global dichotic listening study using a smartphone application. Laterality. 2015;20(4):434-52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2014.997245 PMid:25588000.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2014.997245
  • 16
    Musiek FE, Weihing J. Perspectives on dichotic listening and the corpus callosum. Brain Cogn. 2011;76(2):225-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.011 PMid:21531063.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.011
  • 17
    Westerhausen R, Kompus K. How to get a left-advantage: a technical review of assessing brain asymmetry with dichotic listening. Scand J Psychol. 2018;59(1):66-73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12408 PMid:29356005.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12408
  • 18
    Fifer RC, Jerger JF, Berlin CI, Tobey EA, Campbell JC. Development of a dichotic sentence identification test for hearing-impaired adults. Ear Hear. 1983;4(6):300-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00003446-198311000-00007 PMid:6653934.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00003446-198311000-00007
  • 19
    Babkoff H, Fostick L. Age-related changes in auditory processing and speech perception: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Eur J Ageing. 2017;14(3):269-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-017-0410-y PMid:28936137.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-017-0410-y
  • 20
    Spajdel M. Brain asymmetry for verbal stimuli in relation to dichotic listening performance, verbal and nonverbal intellectual abilities. Act Nerv Super Rediviva. 2017;59(1):33-7.
  • 21
    Andrade AN, Rocha JGM, Iório MCM, Gil D. Influence of the age, level of schooling and gender on the occurrence of the DSI test pauses. CoDAS. 2018;30(5):e20170286. PMid:30304129.
  • 22
    Hugdahl K, Westerhausen R. Speech processing asymmetry revealed by dichotic listening and functional brain imaging. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93(Pt B):466-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.011 PMid:26706774.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.12.011
  • 23
    Cameron S, Glyde H, Dillon H, Whitfield J, Seymour J. The Dichotic Digits difference Test (DDdT): development, normative data, and test-retest reliability studies part 1. J Am Acad Audiol. 2016;27(6):458-69. http://dx.doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.15084 PMid:27310404.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.15084
  • 24
    Frascá MFSS, Lobo IFN, Schochat E. Processamento auditivo em teste e reteste: confiabilidade da avaliação. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;16(1):42-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342011000100009
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342011000100009
  • 25
    Alexandre NMC, Gallasch CH, Lima MHM, Rodrigues RCM. A confiabilidade no desenvolvimento e avaliação de instrumentos de medida na área da saúde. Rev Eletr Enf. 2013;15(3):802-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.5216/ree.v15i3.20776
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.5216/ree.v15i3.20776
  • 26
    Bhat M, Palaniswamy HP, Varsha U, Krishna Y. Development and validation of an automated dichotic double word test in Indian English using MATLAB. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2021;144:110697. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2021.110697 PMid:33799101.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2021.110697
  • 27
    Hiscock M, Kinsbourne M. Attention and the right-ear advantage: what is the connection? Brain Cogn. 2011;76(2):263-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.016 PMid:21507543.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.03.016

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    07 Aug 2023
  • Date of issue
    2023

History

  • Received
    18 Nov 2021
  • Accepted
    17 Aug 2022
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br