Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Performance of Brazilian Portuguese speakers in the Test of Narrative Language (TNL)

ABSTRACT

Purpose

Investigate the performance of native speakers of Brazilian Portuguese in the Test of Narrative Language (TNL) and correlate their performance in its oral narration and narrative comprehension subtests.

Methods

Participants were 140 individuals with typical language development aged 5-0 to 11-11 (years-months) divided according to age group.

Results

Statistically significant difference was observed between the 5-year-old group and all the other age groups and between the 6-year-old group and the 8-, 9-, 10- and 11-year-old groups. The 7-year-old group did not differ from the 6- and 8-year-old groups. Half of the 7-year-old group was in 1st grade of Elementary School and the other half was in 2nd grade, which may have contributed to the non-differentiation between this group and its border-age groups. No statistically significant difference was found between groups as of the age of eight years. Positive correlation was observed between performance in the narrative comprehension and oral narration subtests of the TNL in the 5-, 6- and 7-year-old groups, which did not occur for the other age groups.

Conclusion

The Test of Narrative Language differentiated the 5-, 6- and 7-year-old groups, but did not differentiate groups as of the age of eight years. No correlation was found between performance in the oral narration and narrative comprehension subtests of the TNL as of the age of eight years, which suggests mastery of both skills from this age.

Keywords
Narration; Child Language; Evaluation; Language Development; Language Tests

RESUMO

Objetivo

Investigar o desempenho de falantes do português brasileiro no Test of Narrative Language e correlacionar o desempenho nas tarefas de produção e compreensão da narrativa.

Método

Participaram 140 indivíduos, entre 5 e 11 anos e 11 meses de idade com desenvolvimento típico de linguagem, divididos de acordo com a faixa etária.

Resultados

Diferença significativa foi encontrada entre o grupo de 5 anos e todos os demais grupos etários, bem como para o grupo de 6 anos quando comparado ao grupo de 8, 9, 10 e 11 anos. O grupo de 7 anos não diferiu estatisticamente dos grupos de 6 e 8 anos. Metade do grupo de 7 anos cursava o primeiro ano e a outra metade, o segundo ano, o que pode ter contribuído para a não diferenciação do grupo com as faixas etárias fronteiriças. Não foi encontrada diferença estatisticamente significante entre os grupos a partir dos 8 anos. Observou-se correlação positiva entre o desempenho de compreensão e produção nos grupos etários de 5, 6 e 7 anos, o que não ocorreu para os demais grupos.

Conclusão

O teste diferenciou as faixas etárias de 5, 6 e 7 anos, o que não ocorreu nas faixas etárias a partir dos 8 anos. Também, a partir dos 8 anos, não foi encontrada correlação entre a produção e a compreensão narrativa, sinalizando o domínio de ambas as habilidades a partir desta faixa etária.

Descritores
Narração; Linguagem Infantil; Avaliação; Desenvolvimento da Linguagem; Testes de Linguagem

INTRODUCTION

Oral narration of stories is a complex language task that requires linguistic knowledge (syntactic, semantic, and phonological), knowledge about the world (pragmatics), and mastery of cognitive functions such as working memory, monitoring capacity, organization and integration of information, and ability to make inferences(11 Brandão ACP, Spinillo AG. Produção e compreensão de textos em uma perspectiva de desenvolvimento. Estud Psicol. 2001;6(1):51-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2001000100006.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2001...
,22 Spinillo AG. O efeito da representação pictográfica na produção de narrativas. Psic. Teor e Pesq. 2012;7(3):311-26.); it is, therefore, an important task to investigate typical and deviant developmental trajectories(33 Norbury CF, Gemmell T, Paul R. Pragmatics abilities in narrative production: a cross-disorder comparison. J Child Lang. 2014;41(3):485-510. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S030500091300007X. PMid:23632039.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000913000...
).

Narrative ability begins at the age of three years, and by the age of six children are often able to produce well-structured, complete stories(11 Brandão ACP, Spinillo AG. Produção e compreensão de textos em uma perspectiva de desenvolvimento. Estud Psicol. 2001;6(1):51-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2001000100006.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2001...
). Approximately at the age of ten, development of narrative capacity reaches a peak of complexity(44 Mckeough A. Building on the oral tradition: how story composition and comprehension develop. In: Astington J, editor. Minds in the making. Cambridge: Blackwell; 2000. p. 98-114.), and around the age of twelve it is expected that children master the narrative story scheme and that, together with their linguistic and cognitive skills, be able to produce stories with multiple episodes and complex grammatical structures interconnected by cohesive elements, and use verbs to refer to the mental state of characters(55 Eisenberg SL, Ukrainetz TA, Hsu JR, Kaderavek JN, Justice LM, Gillam RB. Noun phrase elaboration in children’s spoken stories. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2008;39(2):145-57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/014). PMid:18420517.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008...
).

In addition to age and schooling, it is known that children’s contact with the narrative story model in the family and school contexts is also a relevant factor in the development of narrative skills(22 Spinillo AG. O efeito da representação pictográfica na produção de narrativas. Psic. Teor e Pesq. 2012;7(3):311-26.,66 Bento ACP, Befi-lopes DM. Organização e narração de histórias por escolares em desenvolvimento típico de linguagem. Pró-Fono Rev Atual Cient. 2010;22(4):503-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010000400024.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010...

7 Khan KS, Gugiu MR, Justice LM, Bowles RP, Skibbe LE, Piasta SB. Age-related progressions in story structure in young children’s narratives. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2016;59(6):1395-408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0275. PMid:27930767.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-1...
-88 Gillam SL, Gillam RB, Fargo JD, Olszewski A, Segura H. Monitoring indicators of scholarly language: a progress-monitoring instrument for measuring narrative discourse skills. Comm Disord Q. 2017;38(2):96-106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1525740116651442.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/15257401166514...
).

Because it is a task that enables investigation of children's ability to address schemes, organization, constructions, and conventions, even of illiterate children(22 Spinillo AG. O efeito da representação pictográfica na produção de narrativas. Psic. Teor e Pesq. 2012;7(3):311-26.), and because narrative performance is considered a predictor of academic achievement(99 Wellman RL, Lewis BA, Freebairn LA, Avrich AA, Hansen AJ, Stein CM. Narrative ability of children with speech sound disorders and the prediction of later literacy skills. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2011;42(4):561-79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0038). PMid:21969531.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2011...
,1010 Botting N. Narrative as a tool for the assessment of linguistic and pragmatic impairments. Child Lang Teach Ther. 2002;18(1):1-21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1191/0265659002ct224oa.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1191/0265659002ct22...
), oral narration of stories has been widely used in studies conducted with children in typical and atypical circumstances of development(33 Norbury CF, Gemmell T, Paul R. Pragmatics abilities in narrative production: a cross-disorder comparison. J Child Lang. 2014;41(3):485-510. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S030500091300007X. PMid:23632039.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000913000...
,1111 Colozzo P, Gillam RB, Wood M, Schnell RD, Johnston JR. Content and form in the narratives of children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2011;54(6):1609-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0247). PMid:21930617.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011...

12 Crosbie S, Holm A, Wandschneider S, Hemsley G. Narrative skills of children born preterm. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2011;46(1):83-94. PMid:20380599.
-1313 Demir ÖE, Rowe ML, Heller G, Goldin-Meadow S, Levine SC. Vocabulary, syntax, and narrative development in typically developing children and children with early unilateral brain injury: early parental talk about the “there-and-then” matters. Dev Psychol. 2015;51(2):161-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038476. PMid:25621756.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038476...
).

Narrative performance has been investigated by means of different excerpts and parameters of analysis. Analysis based on macrostructure components considers the presence and organization of story elements, such as maintenance of the character and theme, and the relation between events, outcome, and coherence of the narrative(1414 Spinillo AG, Martins ERA. Uma análise da produção de histórias coerentes por crianças. Psicol Reflex Crit. 1997;10(2):219-48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79721997000200004.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79721997...

15 Miles S, Chapman RS. Narrative content as described by individuals with Down syndrome and typically developing children. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(1):175-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/013). PMid:14748647.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002...
-1616 Heilmann JJ, Miller JF, Nockerts A. Using language sample databases. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2010;41(1):84-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2009/08-0075). PMid:20051580.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2009...
). In turn, analysis based on microstructure components considers the sequentiality and meaning of the narrative, in addition to the elements of cohesion that govern the structural sequence, based on the syntactic and semantic principles and measures of vocabulary and syntactic complexity(1616 Heilmann JJ, Miller JF, Nockerts A. Using language sample databases. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2010;41(1):84-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2009/08-0075). PMid:20051580.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2009...
,1717 Justice LM, Bowles RP, Kaderavek JN, Ukrainetz TA, Eisenberg SL, Gillam RB. The index of narrative microstructure: a clinical tool for analyzing school-age children’s narrative performances. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2006;15(2):177-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2006/017). PMid:16782689.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2006...
).

Among the few formal instruments available to investigate the skills of narrative comprehension and oral narration covering both the macro- and micro-structural aspects of the oral narrative, the Test of Narrative Language (TNL)(1818 Gillam RB, Pearson NA. TNL: Test of Narrative Language. Austin: Pro-ed; 2004.) is an instrument widely used in the international literature to assess the performance of children. This instrument has been used in typical and atypical developmental circumstances with the purpose of investigating the narrative performance of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder(1919 Luo F, Timler GR. Narrative organization skills in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and language impairment: Application of the causal network model. Clin Linguist Phon. 2008;22(1):25-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699200701627430. PMid:18092218.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699200701627...
); investigating the effects of noise on narrative skills in children with typical development(2020 Anthony S, Kleinow J, Bobiak J. Narrative ability under noisy conditions in children with low-normal auditory processing skills. Contemp Issues Commun Sci Disord. 2009;36:5-13.); deepening knowledge about the narrative difficulties of children with language disorders(1212 Crosbie S, Holm A, Wandschneider S, Hemsley G. Narrative skills of children born preterm. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2011;46(1):83-94. PMid:20380599.,2121 Domsch C, Richels C, Saldana M, Coleman C, Wimberly C, Maxwell L. Narrative skill and syntactic complexity in school-age children with and without late language emergence. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2012;47(2):197-207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00095.x. PMid:22369060.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.20...
); associating the influence of updating working memory with narrative performance in typical developmental circumstances(2222 Whitely C, Colozzo P. Who’s who? Memory updating and character reference in children’s narratives. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2013;56(5):1625-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0176). PMid:23785191.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013...
), investigating the narrative ability of bilingual children with or without language impairment(2323 Gillam RB, Peña ED, Bedore LM, Bohman TM, Mendez-Perez A. Identification of specific language impairment in bilingual children: I. Assessment in English. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2013;56(6):1813-23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0056). PMid:23882008.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013...
,2424 Squires KE, Lugo-Neris MJ, Peña ED, Bedore MB, Bohman TM, Gillam RB. Story retelling by bilingual children with language impairments and typically-developing controls. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2014;49(1):60-74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12044. PMid:24372886.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.1204...
); monitoring intervention directed to the narrative skills of children with language impairment(2525 Gillam SL, Gillam RB. Narrative discourse intervention for school-aged children with language impairment: supporting knowledge in language and literacy. Top Lang Disord. 2016;36(1):20-34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000081.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000...
).

In Brazil, the TNL has been translated and culturally adapted by Rossi et al.(2626 Rossi NF, Lindau TDA, Gillam RB, Giacheti CM. Adaptação cultural do Test of Narrative Language (TNL) para o Português Brasileiro. CoDAS. 2016;28(5):507-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162016018. PMid:27683830.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/2016...
), and no studies on the performance of children using this tool have been conducted with native speakers of Brazilian Portuguese to date. In view of this, the present study was proposed based on two hypotheses: (a) Will there be difference between age groups with superior performance in narrative comprehension and oral narration for children in older-age groups using the TNL version adapted to Brazilian Portuguese? (b) Will the performance of children in narrative comprehension be superior to that in oral narration in younger-age groups and will this performance be similar with increasing age?

The purpose of this study was to investigate the performance of native speakers of Brazilian Portuguese in the Test of Narrative Language (TNL) and correlate their performance in its oral narration and narrative comprehension subtests.

METHODS

Ethical aspects

This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the aforementioned Institution under protocol no. 1015/2014. Prior to study commencement, all participating children signed an Assent Term and their parents and/or legal guardians signed an Informed Consent Form (ICF) prepared for the specific purposes of this research according to resolution no. 466/12 of the National Health Council (CNS) on the Guidelines and Standards of Research Involving Human Beings.

Study sample

Initially, 146 individuals aged 5-0 to 11-11 (years-months) were selected for Speech-language Pathology (SLP) screening. Children who agreed to participate in the study and had an ICF signed by their parents and/or legal guardians underwent assessment to confirm the absence of language disorders prior to being submitted to the Test of Narrative Language (TNL). SLP screening was conducted according to the Descriptive Mapping of Child Speech-language Pathology Assessment(2727 Giacheti CM. Avaliação da fala e da linguagem: perspectivas interdisciplinares [Internet]. São Paulo: Cultura Acadêmica; 2016. p. 245-61 [citado em 2017 Abr 15]. Disponível em: www.marilia.unesp.br/#!/ebooks). This descriptive guide is organized in eight sections, namely(11 Brandão ACP, Spinillo AG. Produção e compreensão de textos em uma perspectiva de desenvolvimento. Estud Psicol. 2001;6(1):51-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2001000100006.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2001...
), identification data and information about the child(22 Spinillo AG. O efeito da representação pictográfica na produção de narrativas. Psic. Teor e Pesq. 2012;7(3):311-26.); perceptual-sensory functions(33 Norbury CF, Gemmell T, Paul R. Pragmatics abilities in narrative production: a cross-disorder comparison. J Child Lang. 2014;41(3):485-510. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S030500091300007X. PMid:23632039.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000913000...
); cognitive functions(44 Mckeough A. Building on the oral tradition: how story composition and comprehension develop. In: Astington J, editor. Minds in the making. Cambridge: Blackwell; 2000. p. 98-114.); speech(55 Eisenberg SL, Ukrainetz TA, Hsu JR, Kaderavek JN, Justice LM, Gillam RB. Noun phrase elaboration in children’s spoken stories. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2008;39(2):145-57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/014). PMid:18420517.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008...
); speech-related dimensions (voice, articulation, fluency, and oral and verbal praxis)(66 Bento ACP, Befi-lopes DM. Organização e narração de histórias por escolares em desenvolvimento típico de linguagem. Pró-Fono Rev Atual Cient. 2010;22(4):503-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010000400024.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010...
); functions associated with manipulation, ingestion and deglutition of food(77 Khan KS, Gugiu MR, Justice LM, Bowles RP, Skibbe LE, Piasta SB. Age-related progressions in story structure in young children’s narratives. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2016;59(6):1395-408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0275. PMid:27930767.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-1...
); written language(88 Gillam SL, Gillam RB, Fargo JD, Olszewski A, Segura H. Monitoring indicators of scholarly language: a progress-monitoring instrument for measuring narrative discourse skills. Comm Disord Q. 2017;38(2):96-106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1525740116651442.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/15257401166514...
); mathematical skills(2727 Giacheti CM. Avaliação da fala e da linguagem: perspectivas interdisciplinares [Internet]. São Paulo: Cultura Acadêmica; 2016. p. 245-61 [citado em 2017 Abr 15]. Disponível em: www.marilia.unesp.br/#!/ebooks). Of the 146 children selected, six (one girl and five boys) were excluded from the survey because of their low language skill performance. Inclusion criteria were as follows: history of visual, auditory, sensory and/or neuropsychomotor impairments and absence of language and/or learning impairments.

The final study sample was composed by 140 native speakers of Brazilian Portuguese with typical language development, of both genders, with chronological age between 5-0 and 11-11 (years-months). Study participants were divided into seven age groups according to the normative data of the original version of the TNL (Table 1).

Table 1
Distribution of the study sample according to age and gender

Table 2 shows the distribution of the participants according to schooling and age group.

Table 2
Distribution of the study sample according to schooling

Procedures

In order to investigated narrative performance, the translated and culturally adapted version(2626 Rossi NF, Lindau TDA, Gillam RB, Giacheti CM. Adaptação cultural do Test of Narrative Language (TNL) para o Português Brasileiro. CoDAS. 2016;28(5):507-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162016018. PMid:27683830.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/2016...
) of the Test of Narrative Language (TNL)(1818 Gillam RB, Pearson NA. TNL: Test of Narrative Language. Austin: Pro-ed; 2004.) was applied to the study participants. The test is divided into two subtests: narrative comprehension and oral narration, which are measured by six tasks comprising three different formats: without picture support, with support of sequenced pictures, and with support of a single picture. The measures provided by the TNL enable establishment of values that represent, separately, the performance in the subtests of narrative comprehension and oral narration. These measures are represented by a raw score, age equivalent, and standard percentile score. The test also predicts a global measure, represented by the Narrative Language Ability Index (NLAI), percentile rank, and descriptive rank of children’s performance (very superior, superior, above-average, average, below-average, poor, and very poor)(2626 Rossi NF, Lindau TDA, Gillam RB, Giacheti CM. Adaptação cultural do Test of Narrative Language (TNL) para o Português Brasileiro. CoDAS. 2016;28(5):507-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162016018. PMid:27683830.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/2016...
).

Participants’ narration accounts were recorded and transcribed for analysis according to the norms contained in the manual for the original instrument. For this study, among the measures provided by the TNL, the following were used: narrative comprehension subtest raw score, oral narration subtest raw score, and raw total score (sum of the scores of both subtests). The raw total score was used in the present study because it represents the sum of the correct responses in each subtest.

Statistical analysis of the data

Data were organized in Microsoft-Excel 2013® spreadsheet and processed using Statistical Package for Social Sciences - IBM-SPSS 23.0® software. The following statistical tests were applied: Jonckheere-Terpstra, Mann-Whitney adjusted with Bonferroni correction, and Spearman’s correlation. A significance level of 5% (p<0.05) was adopted for all statistical analyses.

RESULTS

Table 3 presents the descriptive and comparative analyses of the raw scores for the TNL narrative comprehension and oral narration subtests, as well as the raw total score (sum of the scores in both subtests) of the Test of Narrative Language (TNL) according to age group. Statistically significant difference was observed between the age groups regarding raw score in the three measures analyzed.

Table 3
Performance comparison between age groups regarding the raw total score of the Test of Narrative Language (TNL) and the raw scores in its narrative comprehension and oral narration subtests

The Mann-Whitney test adjusted with the Bonferroni correction was used to investigate the differences in the raw scores (narrative comprehension, oral narration, and total) between the age groups aiming to identify differing age groups in the pairwise comparison (Table 4).

Table 4
Pairwise comparison between age groups regarding the total raw score of the Test of Narrative Language (TNL) and the raw scores in its narrative comprehension and oral narration subtests

Statistically significant difference was found between the 5-year-old group and all other age groups for all variables analyzed. The 6-year-old group did not present statistically significant difference compared with the 7-year-old group for all variables investigated, which did not occur in the comparison with the other age groups (8-, 9-, 10-, and 11-year-olds). The 7-year-old group did not show statistically significant difference compared with the 8-year-old group, but difference was observed in the comparison between the first and the other age groups (9-, 10-, and 11-year-olds). As of the age of eight years, the groups did not present statistically significant difference between them for all variables assessed.

The Spearman’s correlation coefficient was used to verify the degree of correlation between the total scores of oral narration and narrative comprehension subtest in each age group (Table 5). Correlation was found between the variables (oral narration and narrative comprehension) in the 5-, 6-, and 7-year-old groups. No correlation was observed as of the age of eight years.

Table 5
Correlation between the total scores in the oral narration and narrative comprehension subtests of the Test of Narrative Language (TNL)

DISCUSSION

The objective of the present study was to investigate the performance of native speakers of Brazilian Portuguese in the Test of Narrative Language (TNL) and correlate their performance in its oral narration and narrative comprehension subtests.

Study participants’ performances, analyzed by means of the raw total score of the TNL and the raw scores of its narrative comprehension and oral narration subtests, are shown in Table 3. As demonstrated in the comparison between the age groups, statistically significant differences were found regarding the variables analyzed (raw oral narration, narrative comprehension and total scores). However, in the pairwise comparison (Table 4), only the 5-year-old group presented statistically significant difference compared with all other age groups investigated.

No statistically significant differences were observed between the 6- and 7-year-old groups in the narrative comprehension and oral narration subtests of the TNL. The same occurred with respect to differences between the 7- and 8-year-old groups for all variables (Table 4). Nevertheless, the 7-year-old group differed from its border-age groups (6- and 8-year-olds); this may have occurred because the group was heterogeneous in terms of school grade attendance, although all children were in the 7-year-old group, half of them were in 1st grade whereas the other half were in 2nd grade when the test was applied. (Table 2).

This finding may be attributed to the schooling of the individuals, considering that children in the 7-year-old group who were attending 1st grade (50%) had the same time of exposure to school as those in the 6-year-old group. Children with longer time of exposure to school may have developed the skills needed to perform well in oral narration, such as literacy, which begins in 1st grade and consolidates in the following year. Some studies have highlighted literacy as a determining factor in the development of oral narration, because writing requires more than just learning a code, but mastery of resources such as cohesion, logical sequence, and linguistic constructions and conventions(22 Spinillo AG. O efeito da representação pictográfica na produção de narrativas. Psic. Teor e Pesq. 2012;7(3):311-26.,88 Gillam SL, Gillam RB, Fargo JD, Olszewski A, Segura H. Monitoring indicators of scholarly language: a progress-monitoring instrument for measuring narrative discourse skills. Comm Disord Q. 2017;38(2):96-106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1525740116651442.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/15257401166514...
,99 Wellman RL, Lewis BA, Freebairn LA, Avrich AA, Hansen AJ, Stein CM. Narrative ability of children with speech sound disorders and the prediction of later literacy skills. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2011;42(4):561-79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0038). PMid:21969531.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2011...
). It is worth mentioning that this is still a speculative explanation, because in this study a specific analysis was not performed to enable proper discussion of the influence of literacy on oral narration performance, and it is suggested that it be conducted in further studies.

A for the other age groups, no statistically significant differences were observed in the pairwise comparison between the 8-, 9-, 10-, and 11-year-old groups for any of the variables analyzed (Table 4). This last finding indicated that performance in oral narration, as measured by the TNL, does not discriminate the 8- to 11-year-old groups.

In the original version of the TNL(1818 Gillam RB, Pearson NA. TNL: Test of Narrative Language. Austin: Pro-ed; 2004.), the effect of age on test performance was investigated, and positive correlation was found between age and the raw scores in the oral narration and narrative comprehension subtests, although the means of the raw scores, mainly that of narrative comprehension, were also similar between the age groups as of the age of nine years.

It is worth mentioning that the TNL currently has a new version(2828 Gillam RB, Pearson NA. TNL: Test of Narrative Language. 2nd ed. Austin: Pro-ed; 2017.), with expanded analysis data in groups for children aged 4-0 to 15-11 (years-months), suggesting continuity of studies with this instrument in Brazil including its new version. Increased sample size, including a representative sample from private education and other regions of the country, should promote a more robust discussion on the data found so far.

In addition, what could justify the results observed more specifically for the groups as of 9 years of age is the fact that, according to some previous studies, the most representative changes in narrative performance would occur until the age of eight years, when most of the narrative scheme has been acquired by children(2929 Melo LE. Produção de narrativa oral infantil e memória construtiva a partir da leitura de imagens. Calidoscopio. 2008;6(1):20-7.), although narrative capacity continues to develop after this age(11 Brandão ACP, Spinillo AG. Produção e compreensão de textos em uma perspectiva de desenvolvimento. Estud Psicol. 2001;6(1):51-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2001000100006.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2001...

2 Spinillo AG. O efeito da representação pictográfica na produção de narrativas. Psic. Teor e Pesq. 2012;7(3):311-26.
-33 Norbury CF, Gemmell T, Paul R. Pragmatics abilities in narrative production: a cross-disorder comparison. J Child Lang. 2014;41(3):485-510. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S030500091300007X. PMid:23632039.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0305000913000...
,88 Gillam SL, Gillam RB, Fargo JD, Olszewski A, Segura H. Monitoring indicators of scholarly language: a progress-monitoring instrument for measuring narrative discourse skills. Comm Disord Q. 2017;38(2):96-106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1525740116651442.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/15257401166514...
).

Regarding the correlation between performance in oral narration and narrative comprehension, positive correlation was observed between the variables (oral narration and narrative comprehension) in the 5-, 6- and 7-year-old groups, indicating that the higher the oral narration score, the higher the narrative comprehension score (Table 5). The absence of correlation as of the age of eight years could be explained by the fact that, from this age, there would be no significant differences between the skills of oral narration and narrative comprehension.

Finally, it is important to consider that the results found should not be generalized for all regions of the country, because collection was performed in the southeast region, and it is known that Brazil is a country with wide cultural diversity. It is suggested that studies be continued so that the sample is expanded, including rural areas and inhabitants from different regions of the country, as well as extension of the sample for collection in private schools.

Differences in the performance of speech and written narrative of children from public vs. private schools have been observed(3030 Lins ME, Spinillo AG. Uma análise comparativa da escrita de histórias pelos alunos de escolas públicas e particulares. Rev Bras Estud Pedagogicos. 2007;79(193):5-16.), and should also be explored in the context of oral narrative.

CONCLUSION

The Test of Narrative Language (TNL) statistically differentiated the 5-, 6- and 7-year-old groups, but did not differentiate the groups as of the age of eight years.

Correlation was observed between performance in the narrative comprehension and oral narration subtests of the TNL, with positive correlation between the 5-, 6- and 7-year-old groups, that is, the better the performance in the narrative comprehension tasks, the better the performance in the oral narration tasks, which did not occur for the older-age groups. Analysis of narrative performance should consider both narrative comprehension and oral narration. These skills are correlated early in the development of narrative capacity and will only become independent when children acquire large repertoire of narrative skills.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors are grateful to Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP) for funding the research (proc. no. 2014/08040-4) and to Ronald B. Gillam, Utah State University, USA, for the clarifications provided throughout the course of this study.

  • Study conducted at Laboratório de Estudos, Avaliação e Diagnóstico Fonoaudiológico – LEAD, Faculdade de Filosofia e Ciências, Universidade Estadual Paulista “Júlio de Mesquita Filho” – UNESP - Marília (SP), Brazil.
  • Financial support: Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq, proc. no. 465686/2014-1) and Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP, proc. no. 2014/50909-8).

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Brandão ACP, Spinillo AG. Produção e compreensão de textos em uma perspectiva de desenvolvimento. Estud Psicol. 2001;6(1):51-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2001000100006
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1413-294X2001000100006
  • 2
    Spinillo AG. O efeito da representação pictográfica na produção de narrativas. Psic. Teor e Pesq. 2012;7(3):311-26.
  • 3
    Norbury CF, Gemmell T, Paul R. Pragmatics abilities in narrative production: a cross-disorder comparison. J Child Lang. 2014;41(3):485-510. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S030500091300007X PMid:23632039.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S030500091300007X
  • 4
    Mckeough A. Building on the oral tradition: how story composition and comprehension develop. In: Astington J, editor. Minds in the making. Cambridge: Blackwell; 2000. p. 98-114.
  • 5
    Eisenberg SL, Ukrainetz TA, Hsu JR, Kaderavek JN, Justice LM, Gillam RB. Noun phrase elaboration in children’s spoken stories. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2008;39(2):145-57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/014) PMid:18420517.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/014)
  • 6
    Bento ACP, Befi-lopes DM. Organização e narração de histórias por escolares em desenvolvimento típico de linguagem. Pró-Fono Rev Atual Cient. 2010;22(4):503-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010000400024
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010000400024
  • 7
    Khan KS, Gugiu MR, Justice LM, Bowles RP, Skibbe LE, Piasta SB. Age-related progressions in story structure in young children’s narratives. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2016;59(6):1395-408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0275 PMid:27930767.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0275
  • 8
    Gillam SL, Gillam RB, Fargo JD, Olszewski A, Segura H. Monitoring indicators of scholarly language: a progress-monitoring instrument for measuring narrative discourse skills. Comm Disord Q. 2017;38(2):96-106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1525740116651442
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1525740116651442
  • 9
    Wellman RL, Lewis BA, Freebairn LA, Avrich AA, Hansen AJ, Stein CM. Narrative ability of children with speech sound disorders and the prediction of later literacy skills. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2011;42(4):561-79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0038) PMid:21969531.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0038)
  • 10
    Botting N. Narrative as a tool for the assessment of linguistic and pragmatic impairments. Child Lang Teach Ther. 2002;18(1):1-21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1191/0265659002ct224oa
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1191/0265659002ct224oa
  • 11
    Colozzo P, Gillam RB, Wood M, Schnell RD, Johnston JR. Content and form in the narratives of children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2011;54(6):1609-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0247) PMid:21930617.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0247)
  • 12
    Crosbie S, Holm A, Wandschneider S, Hemsley G. Narrative skills of children born preterm. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2011;46(1):83-94. PMid:20380599.
  • 13
    Demir ÖE, Rowe ML, Heller G, Goldin-Meadow S, Levine SC. Vocabulary, syntax, and narrative development in typically developing children and children with early unilateral brain injury: early parental talk about the “there-and-then” matters. Dev Psychol. 2015;51(2):161-75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038476 PMid:25621756.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038476
  • 14
    Spinillo AG, Martins ERA. Uma análise da produção de histórias coerentes por crianças. Psicol Reflex Crit. 1997;10(2):219-48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79721997000200004
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79721997000200004
  • 15
    Miles S, Chapman RS. Narrative content as described by individuals with Down syndrome and typically developing children. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45(1):175-89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/013) PMid:14748647.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/013)
  • 16
    Heilmann JJ, Miller JF, Nockerts A. Using language sample databases. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2010;41(1):84-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2009/08-0075) PMid:20051580.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2009/08-0075)
  • 17
    Justice LM, Bowles RP, Kaderavek JN, Ukrainetz TA, Eisenberg SL, Gillam RB. The index of narrative microstructure: a clinical tool for analyzing school-age children’s narrative performances. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2006;15(2):177-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2006/017) PMid:16782689.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2006/017)
  • 18
    Gillam RB, Pearson NA. TNL: Test of Narrative Language. Austin: Pro-ed; 2004.
  • 19
    Luo F, Timler GR. Narrative organization skills in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and language impairment: Application of the causal network model. Clin Linguist Phon. 2008;22(1):25-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699200701627430 PMid:18092218.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699200701627430
  • 20
    Anthony S, Kleinow J, Bobiak J. Narrative ability under noisy conditions in children with low-normal auditory processing skills. Contemp Issues Commun Sci Disord. 2009;36:5-13.
  • 21
    Domsch C, Richels C, Saldana M, Coleman C, Wimberly C, Maxwell L. Narrative skill and syntactic complexity in school-age children with and without late language emergence. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2012;47(2):197-207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00095.x PMid:22369060.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00095.x
  • 22
    Whitely C, Colozzo P. Who’s who? Memory updating and character reference in children’s narratives. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2013;56(5):1625-36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0176) PMid:23785191.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0176)
  • 23
    Gillam RB, Peña ED, Bedore LM, Bohman TM, Mendez-Perez A. Identification of specific language impairment in bilingual children: I. Assessment in English. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2013;56(6):1813-23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0056) PMid:23882008.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0056)
  • 24
    Squires KE, Lugo-Neris MJ, Peña ED, Bedore MB, Bohman TM, Gillam RB. Story retelling by bilingual children with language impairments and typically-developing controls. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2014;49(1):60-74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12044 PMid:24372886.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12044
  • 25
    Gillam SL, Gillam RB. Narrative discourse intervention for school-aged children with language impairment: supporting knowledge in language and literacy. Top Lang Disord. 2016;36(1):20-34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000081
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000081
  • 26
    Rossi NF, Lindau TDA, Gillam RB, Giacheti CM. Adaptação cultural do Test of Narrative Language (TNL) para o Português Brasileiro. CoDAS. 2016;28(5):507-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162016018 PMid:27683830.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20162016018
  • 27
    Giacheti CM. Avaliação da fala e da linguagem: perspectivas interdisciplinares [Internet]. São Paulo: Cultura Acadêmica; 2016. p. 245-61 [citado em 2017 Abr 15]. Disponível em: www.marilia.unesp.br/#!/ebooks
  • 28
    Gillam RB, Pearson NA. TNL: Test of Narrative Language. 2nd ed. Austin: Pro-ed; 2017.
  • 29
    Melo LE. Produção de narrativa oral infantil e memória construtiva a partir da leitura de imagens. Calidoscopio. 2008;6(1):20-7.
  • 30
    Lins ME, Spinillo AG. Uma análise comparativa da escrita de histórias pelos alunos de escolas públicas e particulares. Rev Bras Estud Pedagogicos. 2007;79(193):5-16.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    19 July 2018
  • Date of issue
    2018

History

  • Received
    21 July 2017
  • Accepted
    17 Dec 2017
Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia Al. Jaú, 684, 7º andar, 01420-002 São Paulo - SP Brasil, Tel./Fax 55 11 - 3873-4211 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@codas.org.br