Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Performance comparison between right-handers and left-handers in a Brazilian sample of the Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment

Comparação de desempenho entre destros e canhotos em uma amostra brasileira da "A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment"

Abstracts

Studies that investigate handedness are performed in order to relate hemispheric specialization and cognitive performance. The aim of study was compare the neuropsychological performance between right-handed and left-handed children with ages from five to six years who participated in the Brazilian study of standardization of the Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment. For analysis of socio-demographic characteristics, descriptive statistics were used. To compare the mean scores between the groups, normality was tested using Analysis of Variance (one-way Anova) and the Kruskal-Wallis test. When comparing right-handed and left-handed children with typical development, left-handed children presented poorer performance on the subtests involving Attention and Executive Functioning, Language, and Learning and Memory.

Functional laterality; Manual preference; Neuropsychology


Pesquisas sobre preferência manual são realizadas com o intuito de relacionar a especialização hemisférica cerebral e o desempenho cognitivo. O objetivo deste estudo foi comparar o desempenho neuropsicológico entre crianças destras e canhotas de cinco e seis anos de idade, participantes da pesquisa de normatização brasileira da Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment. Para análise das características sociodemográficas utilizou-se estatística descritiva. Para comparação dos escores entre os grupos, foi testada a normalidade, feita a Análise de Variância (Anova one-way) e o teste Kruskal-Wallis. Ao comparar crianças destras e canhotas com desenvolvimento típico, as canhotas apresentaram pior desempenho em subtestes que envolvem Atenção e Funções Executivas, Linguagem e Aprendizado e Memória.

Lateralidade funcional; Preferência manual; Neuropsicologia


For over a century, the relationship between the Left Hemisphere (LH) and language has been associated with the human brain hemispheric asymmetry (Arteaga & Poblano, 2008; Willems, Van der Haegen, Fisher, & Francks, 2014Siengthai, B., Kritz-Silverstein, D., & Barrett-Connor, E. (2008). Handedness and cognitive function in older men and women: A comparison of methods. The Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 12(9), 641-647. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF03008275.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF03008275...
). In right-handed individuals, 97% present linguistic dominance in the LH and 3% in the Right Hemisphere (RH) or bilateral dominance. Among left-handed individuals, 70% present linguistic dominance in LH and 30% in the RH or bilateral dominance (Toga & Thompson, 2003Preslar, J., Kushner, H. I., Marino, L., & Pearce, B. (2014). Autism, lateralisation, and handedness: A review of the literature and meta-analysis. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 19(1), 64-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2013.772621.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2013....
).

Studies on manual preference are carried out to relate hemispheric specialization and cognitive performance (Willems et al., 2014). Hand preference begins early, between the 10th and 12th week of gestation, occurring during the same period as brain asymmetry. Initially, genetic factors predominate, but environmental factors such as greater movement of non-dominant limb can lead to reduced asymmetry (McCartney & Hepper, 1999Johnston, D. W., Shah, M., & Shields, M. A. (2007). Handedness, time use and early childhood development. IZA Discussion Paper, 2752.; Teixeira & Paroli, 2000). Recently, Reissland, Aydin, Francis, and Exley (2014)Nicholls, M. E., Johnston, D. W., & Shields, M. A. (2012). Adverse birth factors predict cognitive ability, but not hand preference. Neuropsychology, 26(5), 578-587. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029151.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029151...
showed that maternal stress between the 24th and 36th week induces increased fetal self-touching with the left limb. Thus, the process of brain lateralization could be influenced by the biochemical imbalance that maternal stress causes in the uterus.

The morphological and functional asymmetry between the hemispheres is not fully understood from the viewpoint of molecular-based genetics. In the meta-analysis conducted by Brandler et al. (2013)Beratis, I. N., Rabavilas, A., Nanou, E. D., Hountala, C., Maganioti, A. E., Capsalis, C. N., ... Papageorgiou, C. (2009). Effect of initiation-inhibition and handedness on the patterns of the P50 event-related potential component: A low resolution electromagnetic tomography study. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5(51), 1-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-5-51.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-5-51...
with 728 individuals, the Proprotein Convertase Subtilisin/Kexin type 6 (PCSK6) gene was associated with the asymmetry of the planum temporale found in dyslexia. However, the locus is not associated with hand prevalence in the general population. The authors believe that the definition of handedness is polygenic, controlled by molecular mechanisms active early in development.

Hormonal factors justify the predominance of left-handed men. The low amount of testosterone in the womb may delay the development of the left hemisphere, which may explain why the prevalence of developmental disorders occur more in men (Johnston, Shah, & Shields, 2007Cunha, J. A. (2000). Psicodiagnóstico. Porto Alegre: Artmed.; Milenkovic, Brkic, & Belojevic, 2013Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY II: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation. ). In a study conducted by Hampson and Sankar (2012), saliva sampling from 180 right-handers and left-handers was used to assay levels of bioavailable testosterone in saliva along with Desoxyribunucleic Acid (DNA) genotyping to quantify the extent of the Androgen Receptor of trinucleotide Cytosine-Adenine-Guanine (AR-CAG) repeat length, a genetic marker of the capacity of the androgen receptor to respond to testosterone. They found that left-handed men presented low levels of free testosterone, while those with ambidexterity presented weak androgen receptor activity. These findings were not found in right-handed individuals. As the AR-CAG gene is located in the X chromosome, they suggested the existence of a bridge between theory and genetics that handedness would be in a locus linked to the X chromosome. These genetic and environmental influences are already expressed earlier and therefore, handedness may be present in early childhood.

The greatest moment of development of handedness occurs early in childhood and the development of pyramidal and striatal system completes at the age of 2 and the cerebellar system at the age of 5. It is important to identify handedness of the child because some tasks can suffer interference when performed and they participate in the organization of cognitive functions (Cunha, 2000Casasanto, D., Willems, R. M., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Body-specific representations of action verbs: Evidence from fMRI in right- and left-handers. Psychological Science, 21, 67-74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/095679760935 4072.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/095679760935 4...
). Observing the development of handedness in children may bring insights to the relationship of handedness with cognition.

Several studies exclude left-handed individuals to reduce variance of sample. However, left-handers are important when investigating the cognitive differences based on the embodied cognition approach (Willems et al., 2014). The theory explains cognitive functioning, using the body as reference. Thus, by studying the ability of lexical decision using verbs to represent manual and non-manual actions, differences in premotor cortex activation between right- and left-handed individuals were observed. The left premotor cortex was activated more than the right premotor cortex in non-manual action verbs in right-handers. The opposite occurred with left-handers (Casasanto, Williems, & Hagoort, 2010Brandler, W. M., Morris, A. P., Evans, D. M., Scerri, T. S., Kemp, J. P., Nicholas, J. T., ... Silvia, P. (2013). Common variants in left/right asymmetry genes and pathways are associated with relative hand skill. PLoS Genetics, 9(9): e1003751. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal. pgen.1003751.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal. pgen....
).

When investigating 50 healthy children between the ages of 8 and 10, Arteaga and Poblano (2008) found that left-handers take longer to recognize monosyllabic pseudowords and discriminate fewer trisyllabic words. The hypothesis for these results would be due to incomplete left hemispheric specialization for language and possible difficulties would include verbal fluency, decoding and language processing speed. Thus, these children, although they have no complaints related to language, belong to a group at high risk for reading and writing disorders.

Agostini and Dellatolas (2001)Agostini, M., & Dellatolas, G. (2001). Lateralities in normal children ages 3 to 8 and their role in cognitive performances. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 429-444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S153269 42DN2001_7.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S153269 42DN20...
measured the skills for movement repetition, vocabulary, visual memory, reading words and pseudowords, and repetition of number series in right-handed and left-handed French children. They observed that right-handers performed better at tasks such as the reading of pseudowords, recognition and visual memory, picture puzzles, prosopagnosia, and phonological fluency. Right-handed boys showed slightly better performance in semantic fluency activities and coherence between hand and foot preferences. The differences in cognitive performance between right-handed and left-handed children were not significant enough to confirm cognitive superiority.

Analyzing a cohort study of 10,000 British children, Nicholls, Johnston, and Shields (2012) noted a dissociation whereby adverse birth factors affect the brain's cognitive ability, but not handedness, and by implication, brain laterality. This study demonstrated a connection between left-handedness and reduced levels of cognitive skills in vocabulary tests at the age of five.

In a meta-analysis conducted by Preslar, Kushner, Marino, and Pearce (2014)Milenkovic, S., Brkic, M., & Belojevic, G. (2013). Left-handedness and neurotic disturbances in adult urban population. Srpski Arhiv Za Celokupno Lekarstvo, 141(11-12), 785-788. http://dx.doi.org/10.2298/SARH1312785M.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2298/SARH1312785M...
, they tested the hypothesis that there would be an association between autism and laterality that would be moderated by handedness, gender, age, brain region studied and level of Autism Spectrum Disorder. They found a moderate but non-significant effect size of group on lateralization. There was no significant effect for sex and handedness.

Tsuang et al. (2011)Preti, A., Sisti, D., Rocchi, M. B., Busca, M., Vellante, M., Camboni, M. V., ... Masala, C. (2014). Male-female differences in left-handedness in Sardinia, Italy. Laterality, 16(6), 737-752. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2010.515991.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2010....
examined the relationship between handedness and schizotypal traits in participants with a high frequency of schizophrenia in the family. The results showed that non-psychotic left-handedness, siblings of psychotic patients, presented more schizotypal traits that indicate that an atypical lateralization process may contribute to the disorder.

Using multiple regression analysis, Nettle (2003)McCartney, G., & Hepper, P. (1999). Development of lateralized behaviour in the human fetus from 12 to 27 weeks' gestation. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 41(2), 83-86. evaluated the relationship between handedness and high skills. Surprisingly, the author found that strength of right- or left-hand preference was associated with average intellectual capacity. In other words, early definition and defined hand preference is associated with improved cognitive capacity. This may explain the higher frequency of undefined hand preference among children with developmental disorders.

Left hand preference is associated with a risk factor for head trauma (Zverev & Adeloye, 2001Toga, A. W., & Thompson, P. M. (2003). Mapping brain asymmetry. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(1), 37-48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1009.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1009...
), intestinal parasites, asthma, suggesting immunological changes (Uslu, Dane, Uyanik, & Ayyildiz, 2010Reissland, N., Aydin, E., Francis, B., & Exley, K. (2014). Laterality of foetal self-touch in relation to maternal stress. Laterality, 20(1), 82-94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2014.920339.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2014....
) and including the controversial association with uncertainty of sexuality in early adolescence (Zucker, Beaulieu, Bradley, Grimshaw, & Wilcox, 2001Teixeira, L. A., & Paroli, R. (2000). Assimetrias laterais em ações motoras: preferência versus desempenho. Motriz, 6(1), 1-8.) and sexual orientation (Valenzuela, 2010Rigal, R. A. (1992). Which handedness: Preference or performance? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75(3), 851-866. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1992.75. 3.851.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1992.75. 3...
).

Based on the current literature, handedness is fertile ground for further research, particularly regarding the neuropsychological aspects. The aim of this study was to compare the performance in neuropsychological tests between right-handers and left-handers in a sample of 5- to 6-year-old children with typical development.

Method

Participants

From the normative data of Brazilian validation study of the A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment (NEPSY-II instrument) (Korkman, Kirk, & Kemp, 2007Johnston, D. W., Nicholls, M., Shah, M., & Shields, M. (2010). Handedness, health and cognitive development: Evidence from children in the NLSY. IZA Discussion Paper, 4774.) translated by the researchers, we selected 203 children from 1,260 individuals with ages between 5 and 6 for school analysis. Hand preference was defined during training of the literacy process in the age group between 5 and 6, reason why we chose this age group (Rigal, 1992).

Instruments and Procedures

The NEPSY-II is an instrument for developmental neuropsychological assessment of children between the ages of 3 and 16. The battery is theoretically divided into six sections or Cognitive Domains: Attention/Executive Functioning, Language, Visuospatial Processing, Sensorimotor, Learning and Memory, and Social Perception (Table 1). Of the 32 subtests of the instrument, 26 are applicable to 5- to 6-year-old children and these were used in the present study (see Table 1) (II NEPSY Manual, Korkman et al., 2007). Briefly, given the complexity of the instrument, the NEPSY-II is a flexible battery that is specifically designed for children and it is not an adaptation of a test developed for adults. As an example, in the Design Copying subtest the child has to copy two-dimensional geometric pictures in increasing complexity to observe perceptual motor and visual aspects. In the Visuomotor Precision subtest, the child has to draw a line inside winding paths without touching the sides in the shortest possible time. It evaluates fine motor speed and precision.

Table 1
- Subtest of NEPSY II for 5- to 6-year-olds

The individual application of the battery occurred between 2010 and 2012 by a trained staff at the child's school or residence. The parents were asked to sign an informed consent form and the child agreed to participate in the study. Handedness was evaluated by an examiner, in accordance with the American NEPSY-II Clinical and Interpretive Manual, as right-handers, left-handers or undefined.

Descriptive statistics were used to analyze socio-demographic characteristics. In this stage of the study, in addition to the general analysis, the sample was divided into two groups: left-handers and right-handers. To compare the scores between the groups, the normality of sample distribution was tested using Shapiro-Wilk test, histogram normal curve, skewness and kurtosis. Analysis of Variance (one-way Anova) was used to analyze possible differences in performance between right-handers and left-handers for each subtest with normal distribution. For the subtests without normal distribution, data was analyzed using Kruskal-Wallis test. The value of p < 0.05 was used to define the subtests that were statistically different between the two groups. To perform the analysis of the data, the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS, version 18.0) was used.

The study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Hospital Santa Izabel, report obtained on August 11, 2006 (Sistema Nacional de Informação sobre Ética em Pesquisas Envolvendo Seres Humanos, process nº 98513).

Results

Table 2 describes the socio-demographic characteristics of the sample. There is a predominance of right-handers, totaling more than 90.0% of the sample. When comparing the groups of hand preference, no statistically significant differences were found for any of the socio-demographic data. No significant percentage differences between sex and type of school were found intra-group. In both groups, a high percentage of mothers (47.5%) had 8 to 11 years of education in the left-handed group and 46.8% in the right-handed. In the right-handed sample, we observed that approximately half of the children were in each age group (5 and 6 years old), but in the left-handed group, 63.2% were 5-year-old.

Table 2
- Socio-demographic characteristics of participants

Of the 32 battery subtests, four showed statistically significant differences between the two groups (right-handers x left-handers) in the domains Attention/Executive Functioning, Language and Memory. In the Attention/Executive Functioning domain, two subtests showed differences: Auditory Attention Total Comission Errors score; and Design Fluency (DF), two scores - Random Series and Total Score. In Language, the Total Score for the Word Generation (WG) subtest and Learning and Memory, the Total Score for Memory for Faces, but not for Memory for Faces Delayed.

The Auditory Attention Subtest (AARS), in the Attention and Executive Functioning domain, provides scores for the number of right answers, omission errors, commission errors and inhibition errors, as well as qualitative observations (distracted behavior and moving around/getting up from the chair), in task of selective auditory attention and auditory sustained attention. The performance of left-handers was significantly lower in one of the scores (p = 0.01), as they committed more action errors than right-handers (Mean - M = 12.21, M = 6.74, respectively).

Design Fluency, in the Attention and Executive Functioning domain, is a subtest that assesses nonverbal fluency through a paper/pencil task generating original pictures, connecting five points, within a time limit. It consists of two steps: Structured Series and Random Series. In the Random Series, where the points do not follow a simple geometric pattern, left-handers presented a significantly lower performance (M = 1.98, p = 0.008). The Total Score for DF consists of the sum of scores of the Structured and Random Series. In this subtest right-handers produced more drawings than left-handers (M = 12.5, M = 9.63, p = 0.016).

In the Language domain, the WG subtest, which assesses verbal fluency within a time limit, consists of two categories (semantic and phonological). The 5- and 6-year-olds only performed the semantic category. A lower performance was found for left-handers than right-handers (M = 13.47, M = 16.91, respectively, p = 0.002).

In the Memory and Learning domain, the Memory for Faces subtest assesses encoding, discrimination and recognition of facial features, in which a number of faces must be recognized by the child. In addition, the later stage assesses long-term memory for faces. The right-handers recalled more faces in the shorter period than the left-handers (M = 8.6, M = 7.11, p = 0.045) (Table 3).

Table 3
- Statistical and clinically significant differences between right-handers and left-handers

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to identify performance differences between right-handers and left-handers with typical development using NEPSY-II, a developmental neuropsychological assessment instrument. With regard to the socio-demographic variables, the frequency of left-handers (9.4%) was equivalent to the percentage described in the literature (Johnston, Nicholls, Shah, & Shields, 2010).

As for hand preference associated with gender, there was no significant difference, which corroborates the studies of Arteaga and Poblano (2008), Beratis et al. (2009)Arteaga, C., & Poblano, A. (2008). Handedness of children determines preferential facial and eye movements related to hemispheric specialization. Arquivos de NeuroPsiquiatria, 66(3-A), 488-493. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2008000400010.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2008...
and Preti et al. (2014)Nettle, D. (2003). Hand laterality and cognitive ability: A multiple regression approach. Brain and Cognition, 52(3), 390-398. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-26 26(03)00187-8.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-26 26(03...
. Other authors (Johnston et al., 2007; Milenkovi et al., 2013) found a prevalence of left-handed men. The association between sex and hand preference is still controversial. Because it is related to multifactorial inheritance, other factors may be important for handedness. Sexuality cannot be predominant in men with normal development; however, it could be so in clinical samples of developmental disorders. These disorders have genetic and/or environmental causes. The study of Pogetti, Souza, Tudella, and Teixeira (2013) exemplifies the strength of environmental factors on handedness. In that study, the authors conducted a unilateral visual occlusion and observed that there was a decrease in the use of the ipsilateral limb during and immediately after occlusion. The authors concluded that visual availability interferes with hand choice. Other factors, such as maternal stress (Reissland et al., 2014), are mentioned. Preslar et al. (2014) also found no relationship between gender and handedness.

Left-handers performed poorly in two subtests of the Attention and Executive Functioning domain, AARS and DF. In AARS, the left-handers committed more errors, showing difficulty in inhibitory control. In DF, they presented fewer drawings, indicating low visuomotor fluency. Why auditory attention, verbal fluency and design fluency is more deficient in left-handers in comparison with right-handers is difficult to explain. However, Beratis et al. (2009) showed that, when dealing with tasks that include executive functions and language, left-handers activate both hemispheres in a more balanced way than right-handers. Thus, the lower hemispheric specialization presented by left-handers could be a disadvantage in these cognitive domains. Would performing both functions cause one of them to be deficient? The deficit-attention disorder/hyperactivity disorder is the prototype of disability in attention/executive function. Are left-handed children with this disorder are more likely to have language disorders? Do left-handed children with specific language disorders have a deficiency in attention and executive function tasks? To answer these questions further studies will be needed.

The hypothesis of Arteaga and Poblano (2008) for hemispheric asymmetry development should also be emphasized. Considering the results of the subtests in the language domain, there were significant differences in verbal fluency subtest, WG. Left-handers presented a poorer performance than right-handers in the total semantic score (sum of animal, food and beverage categories) (p = 0.002). As well as the findings of Arteaga and Poblano (2008), in which left-handers took longer to recognize monosyllabic pseudowords and discriminated fewer trisyllabic words, the hypothesis is that incomplete hemispheric specialization may interfere with skills involving language in left-handers. Siengthai, Kritz-Silverstein, and Barrett-Connor (2008)Pogetti, L. S., Souza, R. M., Tudella, E., & Teixeira, L. A. (2013). Visibilidade dos braços afeta a preferência manual em bebês. Motriz, 19(1), 160-170. , in cross-sectional study with the elderly, found that left-handed women have lower verbal fluency than right-handers. In a longitudinal study conducted by Johnston et al. (2010), one of the tests used with right-handers and left-handers was the Memory for Digit Span Test (operating verbal and short-term memory), in which left-handers presented a poorer performance. The study suggests that other aspects of language, such as verbal memory, may be underdeveloped in left-handers.

Both Design Fluency as Word Generation are subtests that evolve time and productivity, visuomotor and semantics, respectively. It is important to note that both subtests evaluate functions in the Attention/Executive Functioning domain. In the Learning and Memory domain a difference was found for the subtest that assesses Memory for Faces (MF) (p = 0.045), nonverbal short-term test, in which left-handers presented the poorer result. Still referring to the study of Agostini and Dellatolas (2001), left-handers performed more poorly during the task that assessed prosopagnosia, which also involves the ability to recognize faces, as in the subtest used in the present study.

All subtests of cognitive domains that showed differences in the performance between left-handers and right-handers are related to anterior brain structures: prefrontal, premotor and temporal. This is an important finding as these structures tend to develop later.

Through the obtained results, it may be concluded that when comparing right-handers and left-handers with normal development, it was possible to find differences in performance in the subtests involving Attention and Executive Functioning, Language, and Memory and Learning, with no differences between gender, maternal education, socio-economic status and type of school. The predominance of performance differences in subtests of the Attention/Executive functioning, Language and Memory domains suggests involvement of anterior brain structures and development may be delayed. Questions can be raised from this finding: in brain development, may performance differences in these subtests disappear? If so, at what age will development compensate? What influences may these findings have on learning in and out of school? Further studies are needed to answer these questions.

The limitation of the present study was the small sample of left-handers. Since the data were taken from normative data, the frequency of left-handers was the general population. This limitation does not underestimate the value of data, since they derived from a population of individuals with normal development, participating in an extensive neuropsychological research.

References

  • Agostini, M., & Dellatolas, G. (2001). Lateralities in normal children ages 3 to 8 and their role in cognitive performances. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 429-444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S153269 42DN2001_7
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S153269 42DN2001_7
  • Arteaga, C., & Poblano, A. (2008). Handedness of children determines preferential facial and eye movements related to hemispheric specialization. Arquivos de NeuroPsiquiatria, 66(3-A), 488-493. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2008000400010
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2008000400010
  • Beratis, I. N., Rabavilas, A., Nanou, E. D., Hountala, C., Maganioti, A. E., Capsalis, C. N., ... Papageorgiou, C. (2009). Effect of initiation-inhibition and handedness on the patterns of the P50 event-related potential component: A low resolution electromagnetic tomography study. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5(51), 1-10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-5-51
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-5-51
  • Brandler, W. M., Morris, A. P., Evans, D. M., Scerri, T. S., Kemp, J. P., Nicholas, J. T., ... Silvia, P. (2013). Common variants in left/right asymmetry genes and pathways are associated with relative hand skill. PLoS Genetics, 9(9): e1003751. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal. pgen.1003751
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal. pgen.1003751
  • Casasanto, D., Willems, R. M., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Body-specific representations of action verbs: Evidence from fMRI in right- and left-handers. Psychological Science, 21, 67-74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/095679760935 4072
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/095679760935 4072
  • Cunha, J. A. (2000). Psicodiagnóstico. Porto Alegre: Artmed.
  • Hampson, E., & Sankar, J. S. (2012). Hand preference in humans is associated with testosterone levels and androgen receptor gene polymorphism. Neuropsychologia, 50(8), 2018-2025. http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.027
    » http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.027
  • Johnston, D. W., Nicholls, M., Shah, M., & Shields, M. (2010). Handedness, health and cognitive development: Evidence from children in the NLSY. IZA Discussion Paper, 4774.
  • Johnston, D. W., Shah, M., & Shields, M. A. (2007). Handedness, time use and early childhood development. IZA Discussion Paper, 2752.
  • Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY II: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.
  • McCartney, G., & Hepper, P. (1999). Development of lateralized behaviour in the human fetus from 12 to 27 weeks' gestation. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 41(2), 83-86.
  • Milenkovic, S., Brkic, M., & Belojevic, G. (2013). Left-handedness and neurotic disturbances in adult urban population. Srpski Arhiv Za Celokupno Lekarstvo, 141(11-12), 785-788. http://dx.doi.org/10.2298/SARH1312785M
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.2298/SARH1312785M
  • Nettle, D. (2003). Hand laterality and cognitive ability: A multiple regression approach. Brain and Cognition, 52(3), 390-398. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-26 26(03)00187-8
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-26 26(03)00187-8
  • Nicholls, M. E., Johnston, D. W., & Shields, M. A. (2012). Adverse birth factors predict cognitive ability, but not hand preference. Neuropsychology, 26(5), 578-587. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029151
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029151
  • Pogetti, L. S., Souza, R. M., Tudella, E., & Teixeira, L. A. (2013). Visibilidade dos braços afeta a preferência manual em bebês. Motriz, 19(1), 160-170.
  • Preslar, J., Kushner, H. I., Marino, L., & Pearce, B. (2014). Autism, lateralisation, and handedness: A review of the literature and meta-analysis. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 19(1), 64-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2013.772621
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2013.772621
  • Preti, A., Sisti, D., Rocchi, M. B., Busca, M., Vellante, M., Camboni, M. V., ... Masala, C. (2014). Male-female differences in left-handedness in Sardinia, Italy. Laterality, 16(6), 737-752. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2010.515991
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2010.515991
  • Reissland, N., Aydin, E., Francis, B., & Exley, K. (2014). Laterality of foetal self-touch in relation to maternal stress. Laterality, 20(1), 82-94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2014.920339
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2014.920339
  • Rigal, R. A. (1992). Which handedness: Preference or performance? Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75(3), 851-866. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1992.75. 3.851
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1992.75. 3.851
  • Siengthai, B., Kritz-Silverstein, D., & Barrett-Connor, E. (2008). Handedness and cognitive function in older men and women: A comparison of methods. The Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 12(9), 641-647. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF03008275
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF03008275
  • Teixeira, L. A., & Paroli, R. (2000). Assimetrias laterais em ações motoras: preferência versus desempenho. Motriz, 6(1), 1-8.
  • Toga, A. W., & Thompson, P. M. (2003). Mapping brain asymmetry. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(1), 37-48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1009
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1009
  • Tsuang, H. C., Liu, C. M., Hwang, T. J., Hsieh, M. H., Faraone, S. V., Tsuang, M. T., & Chen, W. J. (2011). Handedness and schizotypy in non-psychotic relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 16(6), 690-706. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2010.511646
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2010.511646
  • Uslu, H., Dane, S., Uyanik, M. H., & Ayyildiz, A. (2010). Relationships between intestinal parasitosis and handedness. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 15(4), 465-474. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13576500903049316
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13576500903049316
  • Valenzuela, C. Y. (2010). Sexual orientation, handedness, sex ratio and fetomaternal tolerance-rejection. Biological Research, 43(3), 347-356. http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0716-97602010000300012
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.4067/S0716-97602010000300012
  • Willems, R. M., Van der Haegen, L., Fisher, S. E., & Francks, C. (2014). On the other hand: Including left-handers in cognitive neuroscience and neurogenetics. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15, 193-201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3679
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3679
  • Zucker, K. J., Beaulieu, N., Bradley, S. J., Grimshaw, G. M., & Wilcox, A. (2001). Handedness in boys with gender identity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 767-776. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00773
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00773
  • Zverev, Y., & Adeloye, A. (2001). Left-handedness as a risk factor for head injuries. East African Medical Journal, 78(1), 22-24. http://dx.doi.org/10.4314/eamj.v78i1.9107
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.4314/eamj.v78i1.9107

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Apr-Jun 2016

History

  • Received
    29 July 2014
  • Reviewed
    09 Jan 2015
  • Accepted
    07 Apr 2015
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia, Pontifícia Universidade Católica de Campinas Núcleo de Editoração SBI - Campus II, Av. John Boyd Dunlop, s/n. Prédio de Odontologia, 13060-900 Campinas - São Paulo Brasil, Tel./Fax: +55 19 3343-7223 - Campinas - SP - Brazil
E-mail: psychologicalstudies@puc-campinas.edu.br