Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Early detection of learning difficulties using the BADyG-E2r Battery during primary education

Abstract

The aim of the present study was to assess the predictive capacity of several of the most relevant cognitive skills in the academic field that were evaluated using Differential and General Skills Battery(BADyG-E2r). Particular attention was focused on the variables that need to be overcome regarding the curricular objectives related to pass/fail grading as evaluated by the teachers in the instrumental disciplines of Mathematics and Language. The psychometric battery was applied to the 3rd year students in primary education (a total of 512 students) at 4 public schools that were randomly selected in the province of Alicante (Spain). A follow-up of their academic evolution was under taken until the end of primary education. The obtained results show that high scores in Verbal Reasoning, Numerical Reasoning, and Verbal Syllogisms positively and significantly predict academic success at the end of primary education in the subjects of Language and Mathematics.

Cognitive skills; Mathematics; Language; Learning difficulties; Primary education

Introduction

Throughout primary education, some students have difficulty achieving the curricular objectives which are expected during their academic year. If difficulties which are initially mild are left unattended, then in many cases they can lead to bigger problems, such as low academic performance and early abandonment of the education system ( Duncan et al., 2007 Duncan , G ., Dowsett , C ., Claessens , A ., Magnuson , K ., Huston , A ., Klebanov , P ., … Japel , C . ( 2007 ). School readiness and later achievement . Developmental Psychology , 43 ( 6 ), 1428 – 1446 . https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428 .
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1...
; Kern & Friedman, 2009 Kern , M. L. , Friedman , H. S. ( 2009 ). Early educational milestones as predictors of lifelong academic achievement, midlife adjustment and longevity . Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology , 30 , 419 – 430 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.025 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12...
). To a large extent, these latter examples would lead to school failure. Academic difficulties are preceded by weakness in aptitude concerning cognitive processes because such processes allow the student to learn at a pace which is assumed to be reasonable for their age (Grañeras, & Díaz-Caneja,, & Gil, N., 2011; Navarro-Soria, 2016 Navarro-Soria , I . ( 2016 ). Cognitive, school and socio-demographic variables predictive of academic achievement in Primary Education ( Doctoral Thesis ). Universidad de Alicante , Alicante, España . doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.21077.32482
https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.21077.32...
; Nisbett et al., 2012) Nisbett , R. , Aronson , J. , Blair , C. , Dickens , W. , Flynn , J. , Halpern , D. , Turkheimer , E. ( 2012 ). Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments . American Psychologist , 67 ( 2 ), 130 – 159 . https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026699 .
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026699...
. Therefore, a special focus is required concerning classroom activities with respect to the acquisition of formal knowledge and the development and maturity of cognitive skills, which will allow an adequate access to the curriculum objectives for students.

According to research in the peer-reviewed literature on this subject, it is clear that intelligence and the differential abilities that compose intelligence are the most influential variables when predicting academic success (Fergusson, Horwood, & Ridder, 2005; Gagné & Père, 2001 Gagné , F. , Père , F . ( 2001 ). When IQ is controlled, does motivation still predict achievement? Intelligence , 30 , 71 – 100 . https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00068-X .
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00...
; Kuncel, Hezlett, & Ones, 2004; Kytala & Lehto, 2008 Kytala , M. , Lehto , J. E. ( 2008 ). Some factors underlying mathematical performance: the role of visuospatial working memory and non-verbal intelligence . EuropeanJournalofPsychology of Education , 23 , 77 – 94 . ; Sternberg & Kaufman, 1998 Sternberg , R. , Kaufman , J. ( 1998 ). Human abilities . Annual Review of Psychology , 49 , 479 – 502 . https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.479 .
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.49...
; Strenze, 2007 Strenze , T . ( 2007 ). Intelligence and socioeconomic success: a meta-analytic review of longitudinal research . Intelligence , 35 , 401 – 426 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.004 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.09...
; Taub, Keith, Floyd, & Mcgrew, 2008). Further, it has been verified by research that an optimal cognitive development in certain school aptitudes can be an adequate predictor of academic success for instrumental disciplines such as Mathematics and Language. Thus, verbal skills are considered the best predictor of general academic performance, followed by numerical skills, numerical reasoning, and spatial skills ( Burnet & Lane, 1980 Burnet , S. , Lane , C. M. ( 1980 ). Effects of academic instruction of spational visualization . Intelligence , 4 , 233 – 247 . https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(80)90021-5 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(80)900...
; Cassidy, Roche, Colbert, Stewart, & Grey, 2016; Cooper & Reagan, 1982 Cooper , L . & Reagan , D . ( 1982 ). Attention, perception and intelligence. Handbook of human Intelligence . In Sternberg (Ed.), CambridgeUniversity Press , 123 - 170 . ; Hawes, Moss, Caswell, & Poliszczuk, 2015; Prior, Bavin, & Ong, 2010; Smith, 1964) Smith , S . ( 1964 ) The Psychology of Illness . Nursing Forum 3 ( 1 ), 34 – 47 . . This skill hierarchy is modified in the event of predicting performance for specific academic areas, such as Mathematics. In this case, numerical skills occupy position one on the ranking, displacing verbal aptitudes to position two. Similarly, if the academic sphere is Language, then numerical aptitude is displaced to position three, yielding verbal aptitudes and logical reasoning in positions one and two, respectively ( Cerda et al., 2015 Cerda , G. , Pérez , C. , Navarro , J. I. , Aguilar , M. , Casas , J. A. , Aragón , E . ( 2015 ). Explanatory model of emotinal-cognitive variables in school mathematics performance: a longitudinal study in primary school . Frontiers in Psychology , 6 , 1363 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01363 .
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01363...
; Marrero & Espino, 1988 Marrero , H. , Espino , O. ( 1988 ). Evaluación comparativa del poder predictor de las aptitudes sobre notas escolares y pruebas objetivas . Revista de Educación , 287 , 97 – 112 . ; Spinath, Spinath, Harlaar, & Plomin, 2006; Toll & Van Luit, 2014) Toll , S ., & Van Luit , J . ( 2014 ). Explaining numeracy development in weak performing kindergartners . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 124 , 97 – 111 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.001 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.0...
. In general, such investigations conclude that an important part of learning difficulties in the academic context find their etiology in the cognitive aptitudes which are required for the completion of traditional school activities. Hence, these aptitude deficiencies are the prelude to poor school performance, being detected later on and once the student already presents difficulties in achieving the curricular objectives of his/her academic year (Bennett, Seashore, & Wesman, 1997; Navarro-Soria, 2016 Navarro-Soria , I . ( 2016 ). Cognitive, school and socio-demographic variables predictive of academic achievement in Primary Education ( Doctoral Thesis ). Universidad de Alicante , Alicante, España . doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.21077.32482
https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.21077.32...
; Pérez, Cupani, & Ayllón, 2005; Robles & Vázquez, 2014) Robles , C. , Vázquez , E. ( 2014 ). The influence of verbal skills on school success . International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology , 6 ( 1 ), 351 – 362 . . As the student gets promoted to higher classes, the curriculum demands increase, and the late detection and initiation of resources for school reinforcement aggravates the difficulties and increases the differences in levels of development in relation to the other students in the class ( Navarro-Soria, 2016) Navarro-Soria , I . ( 2016 ). Cognitive, school and socio-demographic variables predictive of academic achievement in Primary Education ( Doctoral Thesis ). Universidad de Alicante , Alicante, España . doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.21077.32482
https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.21077.32...
.

The emphasis of recent research in the literature has been on finding causal relationships between academic performance and the different cognitive skills that allow students to access knowledge in school ( Alloway & Passolunghi, 2011 Alloway , T. , Passolunghi , M. ( 2011 ). The relationship between working memory, IQ, and mathematical skills in children . Learning and Individual Differences , 21 ( 1 ), 133 – 137 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.09.013 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.09...
; Cornu, Schiltz, Pazouki, & Martin, 2017; Geary & VanMarie, 2016 Geary , D. C. , VanMarie , K. ( 2016 ). Young children’s core symbolic and nonsymbolic quantitative knowledge in the prediction of later mathematics achievement . Developmental Psychology , 52 ( 12 ), 2130 – 2144 . https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000214 .
https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000214...
; Harvey & Miller, 2016 Harvey , H. , Miller , G. ( 2016 ). Executive function skills, early mathematics, and vocabulary in head start preschool children . Early Education and Development , 28 ( 3 ), 290 – 307 . https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2016.1218728 .
https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2016.12...
; Matthews, Lewis, & Hubbard, 2015; Navarro-Soria & González-Gómez, 2010; Pitchford, Papini, Outhwaite, & Gulliford, 2016; Ye et al., 2016) Ye , A. , Resnick , I. , Hansen , N. , Rodrigues , J. , Rinne , L. , Jordan , N. ( 2016 ). Pathways to fraction learning: numerical abilities mediate the relation between early cognitive competencies and later fraction knowledge . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 152 , 242 – 263 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.001 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.0...
. Based on these causal relationships, programs have been developed to control, manipulate, and modify the maturation and capacity of these cognitive skills, in order to improve the preparation of students so that they can confront the different academic challenges they face during their training ( Cassidy et al., 2016 Cassidy , S. , Roche , B. , Colbert , D. , Stewart , I. , Grey , I. M. ( 2016 ). A realional frame skills training intervention to increase general intelligence and scholastic aptitude . Learning and Individual Differences , 47 , 222 – 235 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.03.001 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.03...
; Cheng & Mix, 2014 Cheng , Y. L. , Mix , K. S. ( 2014 ). Spatial training improves children’s mathematics ability . Journal of Cognition and Development , 15 , 2 – 11 . https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.725186 .
https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.72...
; Di Lieto et al., 2017 Di Lieto , M ., Inguaggiato , E ., Castro , E ., Cecchi , F ., Cioni , G ., Dell’Omo , M ., … Pecini , C . ( 2017 ). Educational robotics intervention on executive functions in preschool children: a pilot study . Computers in Human Behavior , 71 , 16 – 23 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.018 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.01...
; Hill, Serpell, & Faison, 2016; Park, Bermudez, Roberts, & Brannon, 2016; Peng & Fuchs, 2016) Peng , P. , Fuchs , D. ( 2016 ). A meta-analysis of working memory deficits in children with learning difficulties: is there a difference between verbal domain and numerical domain? Journal of Learning Disabilities , 49 ( 1 ), 3 – 20 . https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219414521667 .
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219414521667...
. At the same time, knowing which cognitive skills have a greater influence on academic success and which tools measure these cognitive skills effectively can facilitate an individualized intervention and the implementation of the psychopedagogical resources that currently act on the problem from a scholarly assistance perspective (Rogowsky, Papamichalis, Villa, Heim, & Tallal, 2013), rather than from a preventive perspective ( Bergman et al., 2011 Bergman , S. , Söderqvist , S. , Bryde , S. , Thorell , L. B. , Humphreys , K. , Klingberg , T. ( 2011 ). Gains in fluid intelligence after training non-verbal reasoning in 4-year-old children: a controlled, randomized study . Developmental Science , 14 ( 3 ), 591 – 601 . https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01022.x .
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010...
; Blair, McKinnon, & the Family Life Project Investigators, 2016; Blair & Raver, 2014 Blair , C. , Raver , C. ( 2014 ). Closing the achievement gap through modification of neurocognitive and neuroendocrine function: results from a cluster randomized controlled trial of an innovative approach to the Education of Children in Kindergarten . PLoS One , 9 ( 11 ). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112393 .
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.011...
; Di Lieto et al., 2017 Di Lieto , M ., Inguaggiato , E ., Castro , E ., Cecchi , F ., Cioni , G ., Dell’Omo , M ., … Pecini , C . ( 2017 ). Educational robotics intervention on executive functions in preschool children: a pilot study . Computers in Human Behavior , 71 , 16 – 23 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.018 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.01...
; Raver et al., 2011) Raver , C. C. , Jones , S. M. , Li-Grining , C. , Zhai , F. , Bub , K. , Pressler , E. ( 2011 ). CSRP’s impact on low-income preschoolers’ preacademic skills: self-regulation as a mediating mechanism . Child Development , 82 , 362 – 378 . https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01561.x .
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010...
.

As it has been shown, the tests used to measure different cognitive variables are different. An example of this is the Differential Aptitude Test (DAT) (Bennett et l., 1997), the Multiple Intelligences Self-efficacy Inventory Revised (MISEI-R) ( Pérez et al., 2005 Pérez , E. , Cupani , M. , Ayllón , S . ( 2005 ). Predictors of academic performance in middle school: skills, self-efficacy and personality traits . Avaliaço Psicológica , 4 ( 1 ), 1 – 11 . ), or the AC-MT test ( Alloway & Passolunghi, 2011 Alloway , T. , Passolunghi , M. ( 2011 ). The relationship between working memory, IQ, and mathematical skills in children . Learning and Individual Differences , 21 ( 1 ), 133 – 137 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.09.013 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.09...
). The BADyG test, in addition to demonstrating being a more complete measurement covering different fields, has also proved to be a good predictor of academic performance. Authors such as Miñano and Castejón (Miñano, Cantero, & Castejón, 2008; Miñano & Castejón, 2008) Miñano , P. , Castejón , J . ( 2008 ). Predictive ability of cognitive-motivational variables on academic performance . Revista Electrónica de Motivación y Emoción , 28 ( 11 ). found that the variable of Verbal Aptitudes and General Intelligence have a high predictive power for academic performance with its effectiveness being proven not only in schools but also at university level ( Vélez-van Meerbeke & Roa-González, 2005) Vélez-van Meerbeke , A ., & Roa-González , C. N . ( 2005 ). Factores asociados al rendimiento académico en estudiantes de medicina . Educación médica , 8 ( 2 ), 74 – 82 . .

Taking into account the previously described research results, the focus of the above studies was on explaining what skills justify the difficulties related to mastering the content at middle and upper academic levels. Such learning difficulties are evident, and the first symptoms of potential failure in school have already appeared. We consider it more interesting and novel to focus on the preventive purpose. We can find in scientific literature investigations in this scope in the early educational stages. However, none of these studies uses a longitudinal approach, which would allow to measure students’ cognitive aptitudes at early stages of schooling and therefore predict future learning difficulties, with students who have lower scores in cognitive aptitudes presenting more severe learning difficulties at later stages of education.

The aim of the present study was to assess the predictive capacity of several of the most relevant cognitive skills in the academic field that were evaluated using the Differential and General Skills Battery (BADyG-E2r). The specific objectives of the present study and our research hypotheses are shown in Table 1 .

Table 1
Objectives and hypotheses of the study

Method

Participants

For the present study, a random or probability sampling using the sampling unit known as the conglomerate was carried out. The target areas consisted of four public schools in the province of Alicante (Spain), with one school from each cardinal point (north, south, east, and west) of the province with a total of 24 groups/classrooms, and the total number of students evaluated was 607. Information was collected during three school years and from all of the students in the sample who were enrolled in the 3rd year of primary education (8–9 years old).

Hence, the first inclusion criterion was that the subject was enrolled at that moment of time, in the 3rd year of primary education, and that they remained in the center until the end of the said educational stage.

A total number of 22 students (3.6%) were excluded at the end of the follow-up, being those students who did not show continuity in their schooling in the referenced educational centers and for whom their academic evolution could not be followed up. Another inclusion criterion meant that in presenting a General Intelligence higher than 80 measured by WISC-IV since, in the case that it was inferior, the results would be affected by the cognitive capacity of the subject.

The number of excluded students increased to 33 (5.4%). Finally, a total of 40 (6.6%) students were excluded from the sample, being those students who had stopped attending Language or Mathematics classes in the 2nd year of primary education. After the follow-up, the total sample had been reduced to 512 students, and of the students that made up the final sample, 232 were males (45.4%) and 280 (54.6%) were females.

The socioeconomic level of the families was distributed by income between upper-middle 92 (18%), middle 220 (43%), middle-low 122 (24%), and low 78 (15%). With respect to the academic training of the families of the students that made up the sample, 113 (22%) had university studies, 144 (28%) had chosen a vocational training, 209 (41%) had finished secondary school, and 46 (9%) presented studies or qualified training. Regarding the ethnic and cultural origin of the families, 342 (67%) of the sample had families in Spain, 72 (14%) were from Latin America, 41 (8%) were from the Maghreb in North Africa, 31(6%) were from sub-Saharan Africa, and 26(5%) were from other European countries.

Before initiating student evaluation and monitoring in this research, the procedure was presented before the District Education Board which is the highest decision-making body of an educational center in Spain. The District Education Board related to the four target schools from where the sample was to be drawn approved the research procedure for those four schools. In addition, the psychometric evaluation and the corresponding analysis of the results of the evaluation were reported to all families by way of traditional mailing. In the letter that the families received, there was an opportunity for the children to not participate in the investigation. None of the families in the total sample exempted their children from participating in the evaluation and subsequent follow-up. This research was conducted in accordance with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. Finally, approval was requested from the Ethics Committee of the University of Alicante, which provides and approves the methodology used, and the approval was assigned the file number UA-2018-03-08.

Procedure and materials

In order to verify which cognitive skills are the most influential in academic development in primary education, an aptitude assessment was carried out at the beginning of the 3rd year of primary education (8 years of age), through psychometric testing using the collective application of the Battery of Differential and General Abilities BADyG-E2r ( Yuste-Hernanz, 2012 Yuste-Hernanz , C . ( 2012 ). Battery of differential and general skills. Technical manual . Madird : CEPE . ). Furthermore, at the end of this educational stage, information was collected concerning the academic performance in the subjects of Mathematics and Language and evaluated/measured using the grades that were issued by the center.

As aforementioned, the BADyG-E2r battery measures cognitive development in skills related to verbal, numerical, spatial, and logical reasoning, as well as the ability to solve Verbal Syllogisms, Numerical Syllogisms, and Spatial Syllogisms, and the speed and efficiency with which students solve the academic problems they encounter. The aim was to provide basic information to the teachers concerning the skill levels of the class which permits the adaptation of teaching and learning rhythms to the abilities and real needs of the students ( Navarro-Soria & Gonzalez-Gómez, 2010 Navarro-Soria , I . y Gonzalez-Gómez , C . ( 2010 ). Systematic detection of aptitude deficits as a strategy to prevent academic difficulties .IN: R. Roig Vila , M. Fiorucci , Keys to research in educational quality and innovation .(págs. 297 - 309 ). Alcoi : Marfil . ) Table 2 .

Table 2
BADyG-E2r scales

The General Intelligence Index (GI) is obtained through the sum of the scores of all the tests. The Intellectual Coefficient (IC) is measured from the direct score in GI and taking into account the chronological age of the student (according to the corresponding age scale). In the exploratory factor analysis, a one-dimensional structure is obtained, which confirms the existence of the said General Intelligence factor.

The scales in Verbal Reasoning, Numerical Reasoning, Spatial Reasoning, and Logical Reasoning each consist of 24 elements that are ordered according to the difficulty index (the first items being solved more than the last items) with five response alternatives. Through verbal analogies, numerical problems, puzzles, relationships between figures, and logical problems, the level of development in the different types of reasoning is measured. However, the Verbal Syllogisms, Numerical Syllogisms, Spatial Syllogisms, and Attention Syllogisms are composed of cloze-type exercises with a range of meanings (multiple-choice items) whereby numerical calculations are made, and there are figures in which the student must discriminate between shape rotations, sizes, orientation, or form, and the speed of discrimination of visual differences is documented. Each of the dimensions that are measured by the test is evaluated with a specific response scale, with a time limit for completion of each scale.

This test has been subjected to multiple psychometric controls ( Yuste-Hernanz, 2002 Yuste-Hernanz , C . ( 2002 ). Battery of differential and general skills. BADYG E-2 . Madrid : TEA Ediciones . ; Yuste-Hernanz, 2012 Yuste-Hernanz , C . ( 2012 ). Battery of differential and general skills. Technical manual . Madird : CEPE . ). In each of these controls, the exercises have been restructured according to the analysis of the performed elements, obtaining information for the sample from which the scale of the test is developed, with the final results pertaining to the Cronbach’s coefficient alpha value of .83 for Verbal Reasoning, .91 for Numerical Reasoning, .79 for Spatial Reasoning, .93 for Logical Reasoning, .77 for Verbal Syllogisms, .85 for Numerical Syllogisms, .83 for Spatial Syllogisms, and .95 for General Intelligence. In turn, in relation to the sample of this study, the Cronbach’s coefficient alpha values have ranged between .76 and .94 for the separate elements and the total had a Cronbach’s coefficient alpha value of .82.

Also, the construct validity of this test was based on its structure in the factorialist and hierarchical psychometric theories that, starting from authors such as Spearman, Cattell, Horn, Eysenk, or Sternberg, nowadays form a solid theoretical line of reflection. Hierarchical models provide a good conceptualization of the aptitudes measured by test and are what best explain the empirical data obtained when solving test elements.

In addition, the academic performance of the students was measured using the grading by the teachers in the subjects of Mathematics and Language during the final year of primary education. Taking into account the cognitive abilities (high, medium, or low) of the students and, based on their school results (pass or fail), the influence of the differences in aptitude on the academic performance has been determined.

Statistical analysis

Simple logistic regression—the logit model—was used to analyze the influence of the linear combination of the predictor variables, following the forward step-wise selection procedure based on the Wald statistic (García, Alvarado, & Jiménez, 2000). Logistic modeling allows the estimation of the probability of an occurrence, event, or outcome, as opposed to a non-occurrence, in the presence of one or more predictors. This probability is estimated using the odds ratio (OR) statistic. If OR > 1, for each time the event occurs in the absence of the independent variable, it will be given twice if that independent variable is present. However, if OR < 1, then the probability of the event occurring in the absence of the independent variable will be greater than if that independent variable would be present ( De Maris, 2003) De Maris , A . ( 2003 ). Logistic regression . En J.A. Schinka y W. F. Velicer ( Eds .) , Research Methods in Psychology (pp. 509 - 532 ). New Jersey : John Wiley & Sons . . To analyze the adjustment of the proposed models, two indicators were taken into account: (a) Nagelkerke’s R 2 , which indicates the percentage of variance explained by the model ( Nagelkerke, 1991) Nagelkerke , N. J. ( 1991 ). A note on a general definition of the coefficient of determination . Biometrika , 78 ( 3 ), 691 – 692 . https://doi.org/10.2307/2337038 .
https://doi.org/10.2307/2337038...
, and (b) the percentage of correctly classified cases, which allows to determine to what extent the predictor variable is useful for estimating the criterion variable in the proposed model.

Results

The data permitted the creation of logistic regression models that make it possible to carry out correct estimations regarding the probability of not repeating a course and to overcome the two instrumental subjects (Language and Mathematics), based on the scores in cognitive maturity in the different cognitive skills that were evaluated.

Table 3 shows the steps followed by the model in the introduction of explanatory variables that have been significant regarding the probability of not repeating a course. For the evaluation in the 3rd year of primary education using the BADYG-E2r, the proposed model allows a correct estimation of 88.8% of cases in Verbal Reasoning, 90.2% in Numerical Reasoning, 88.0% in Spatial Reasoning, 88.4% in Verbal Syllogisms, 87.8% in Numerical Syllogisms, 87.4% in Spatial Syllogisms, 88.0% in Discrimination of Differences, 90.8% in Logical Reasoning, 91.2% in General Intelligence, and 90.6% in Intelligence Quotient (IQ) for the total sample.

Table 3
Logistic regression for the predictive probability of each one of the cognitive aptitudes of the BADYG-E2 regarding the potential for no repeating the course

Nagelkerke’s R 2 (adjusted version of Cox-Snell R 2 , adjusting scale of the statistic to cover whole range 0 to 1) oscillated in the estimation of the adjustment value between .14 for Spatial Syllogisms and .52 for Logical Reasoning and General Intelligence. The model created for Verbal Reasoning explained 38% of the variance, Numerical Reasoning explained 44% of the variance, Spatial Reasoning explained 24% of the variance, Verbal Syllogisms explained 32% of the variance, Numerical Syllogisms explained 22% of the variance, Spatial Syllogisms explained 14% of the variance, Attention explained 16% of the variance, Logical Reasoning explained 52% of the variance, General Intelligence explained 52%, and finally, Intelligence Quotient explained 51% of the variance.

The odds ratio (OR) values were obtained for the elaborated models of the sample oscillate between 1.18 for Intellectual Coefficient and 1.57 for Verbal Syllogisms ( Table 3 ). Thus, the probability that students do not repeat a course during the final stage of primary education is greater for each incremental point increase in the results obtained in the following indices: Verbal Reasoning 54%, Numerical Reasoning 47%, Spatial Reasoning 35%, Verbal Syllogisms 57%, Numerical Syllogisms 25%, Spatial Syllogisms 21%, Discrimination of Differences 29%, Logical Reasoning 23%, General Intelligence 13%, and in an Intelligence Quotient of 18%, for the entire sample and introducing the variables one by one.

Table 4 shows the steps followed by the model in the introduction of explanatory variables that have been significant in the probability of passing the instrumental subject of Language. For the evaluation with BADYG-E2 in the 4th year of primary education, the proposed model allows a correct estimation of 79.2% of the cases in Verbal Reasoning, 78.8% in Numerical Reasoning, 71.8% in Spatial Reasoning, 73.6% in Verbal Syllogisms, 72.4% in Numerical Syllogisms, 67.2% in Spatial Syllogisms, 67.8% in Discrimination of Differences, 83.2% in Logical Reasoning, 84% in General Intelligence, and 83.6% in Intelligence Quotient.

Table 4
Logistic regression for the predictive probability of each one of the cognitive aptitudes of BADYG-E2 and the student not dropping the instrumental subject of Language

Nagelkerke’s R 2 has oscillated in the estimation of the adjustment value between .17 for Spatial Syllogisms and .64 for Logical Reasoning. The model created for Verbal Reasoning accounted for 53% of the variance, for Numerical Reasoning explained 48% of the variance, for Spatial Reasoning explained 31% of the variance, for Verbal Syllogisms explained 36% of the variance, for Numerical Syllogisms explained 29% of the variance, for Spatial Syllogisms explained 17% of the variance, for Attention it accounted for 19% of the variance, for Logical Reasoning it explained 64% of the variance, for General Intelligence it explained 63%, and for Intelligence Quotient it explained 63% of the variance.

The odds ratio (OR) was obtained for the elaborated models of the sample oscillate between 1.14 for General Intelligence and 1.70 for Verbal Reasoning ( Table 4 ). Thus, the probability that students do not drop the Language course during the final years of primary education is greater for each incremental point increase in the result obtained in the following indices: Verbal Reasoning 70%, Numerical Reasoning 34%, Spatial Reasoning 34%, Verbal Syllogisms 44%, Numerical Syllogisms 26%, Space Syllogisms 19%, Discrimination of Differences 28%, Logical Reasoning 26%, General Intelligence 14%, and Intellectual Coefficient 20%, for the whole sample and introducing the variables one by one.

In Table 5 , the introduction of explanatory variables that have been significant for the probability of not dropping the instrumental subject of Mathematics can be observed. For the evaluation with BADYG-E2 in the 4th year of primary education, the proposed model allows a correct estimate for the total sample in Verbal Reasoning 76.2%, Numerical Reasoning 84.4%, Spatial Reasoning 71.6%, Verbal Syllogisms 73%, Numerical Syllogisms 73.2%, Spatial Syllogisms 71.4%, Discrimination of Differences 66.8%, Logical Reasoning 84.8%, General Intelligence 85.2%, and Intelligence Quotient 86%.

Table 5
Logistic regression for the predictive probability of each one of the cognitive aptitudes of the BADYG-E2 of not dropping the instrumental subject of Mathematics

Nagelkerke’s R 2 has oscillated in the estimation of the adjustment value between .19 for Discrimination of Differences and .70 for General Intelligence and Intelligence Quotient. The model created for Verbal Reasoning accounted for 42% of the variance, for Numerical Reasoning explained 62% of the variance, for Spatial Reasoning explained 34% of the variance, for Verbal Syllogisms explained 34% of the variance, for Numerical Syllogisms explained 33% of the variance, for Spatial Syllogisms explained 24% of the variance, for Attention explained 19% of the variance, for Logical Reasoning explained 69% of the variance, for General Intelligence explained 70%, and for Intelligence Quotient explained 70% of the variance.

The odds ratio (OR) obtained for the elaborated models of the sample oscillate between 1.18 for General Intelligence and 1.50 for Verbal and Numerical Reasoning ( Table 5 ). Thus, the probability that students do not drop the instrumental subject of Mathematics during the latter years of primary education is greater for each incremental point increase in the result obtained for the whole sample and introducing the variables one by one in the following indices: Verbal Reasoning 50%, Numerical Reasoning 50%, Spatial Reasoning by 37%, Verbal Syllogisms 41%, Numerical Syllogisms 30%, Spatial Syllogisms 25%, Discrimination of Differences 28%, Logical Reasoning 32%, General Intelligence 18%, and Intelligence Quotient 18%.

Discussion and conclusions

Following the analysis of the obtained results and regarding the relationship between maintaining the same mean level inaptitude development in the different measured competences and the probability of repeating the course in the latter stage(s) of primary education, we can conclude that the research hypothesis (H a ) is fulfilled, because the skills in Vocabulary and Verbal Reasoning are the variables that were measured with greater predictive capacity. Therefore, these results confirm that regarding the BADyG-E2r test which is taken at the end of primary education, it has a good capacity for predicting the appearance of difficulties in students who, in initial courses of primary education, did not present notable difficulties in overcoming the curricular objectives of the instrumental subjects. However, those students did have low scores in the aforementioned aptitudes during those initial courses. When evaluating a student’s aptitude development midway through primary education using the BADyG-E2r battery, it was detected that with each incremental point increase in the results in the skills related to Language (Vocabulary and Verbal Reasoning), the probability that the student does not repeat a subjects oscillates between 57 and 54% respectively. These results denote that, according to a student’s promotion to the next grade in their educational training, an adequate understanding and a correct usage of Language is fundamental to achieving the different curricular objectives. These data reinforce the results of previous research that also concluded that verbal aptitudes are strong predictors of learning difficulties ( Bennett et al., 1997 Bennett , G. , Seashore , H. y Wesman , A. ( 1997 ). Test de Aptitudes Diferenciales DAT Forma T, Manual . Buenos Aires : Paidós . ; Burnet & Lane, 1980 Burnet , S. , Lane , C. M. ( 1980 ). Effects of academic instruction of spational visualization . Intelligence , 4 , 233 – 247 . https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(80)90021-5 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(80)900...
; Cooper & Reagan, 1982 Cooper , L . & Reagan , D . ( 1982 ). Attention, perception and intelligence. Handbook of human Intelligence . In Sternberg (Ed.), CambridgeUniversity Press , 123 - 170 . ; Pérez et al., 2005 Pérez , E. , Cupani , M. , Ayllón , S . ( 2005 ). Predictors of academic performance in middle school: skills, self-efficacy and personality traits . Avaliaço Psicológica , 4 ( 1 ), 1 – 11 . ; Robles & Vázquez, 2014) Robles , C. , Vázquez , E. ( 2014 ). The influence of verbal skills on school success . International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology , 6 ( 1 ), 351 – 362 . . This conclusion reinforces the importance of these skills being specifically used in student training in order to enhance student development, and therefore, such training is considered an adequate academic response, which favors a student’s acquisition of the proposed curricular objectives with respect to the different disciplines in order to correctly complete primary education.

In the same line of thought, when analyzing the results of which aptitudes have a greater predictive capacity of potentially dropping subjects at the end of primary education, we observed that in the BADyG-E2r test and for the subject of Language, the Verbal Reasoning Index has a greater predictive capacity; insofar as for each incremental point increase in the result for the Verbal Reasoning ability, the probability that the student does not drop the instrumental subject of Language increases by 70%, which in turn does not completely fulfill the research hypothesis (H b ). Justification that knowledge of vocabulary (Verbal Syllogisms) loses importance at the predictive level of learning difficulties at the end of primary education may be due to the fact that in the latter years of primary education, a greater employment of logic that is more reflective of Language is required. Similarly, for the instrumental subject of Mathematics, we obtained a result which showed that the skills with greater predictive ability are Verbal Reasoning and Numerical Reasoning, to the extent that for each incremental point increase for the abilities in Verbal Reasoning or Numeric Reasoning, the probability that the student does not drop the instrumental subject of Mathematics increases by 52 and 50%, respectively. Therefore, our research hypothesis (H c ) is not completely fulfilled. It may seem unusual that from the analysis of the results for the aptitudes measured in the instrumental subject of Mathematics using the BADyG-E2r test, the Verbal Reasoning Index stands out as the best predictor, over the rest of the aptitudes, of potentially dropping a course. This fact can be justified because if a student in primary education does not understand the concepts that are employed to explain the numerical operations or problems, then that student will be unable to correctly solve Mathematical exercises, due to the great level of abstraction required by cognitive processes associated with this type of activity at this educational level. In the latter years of primary education, the discipline of Mathematics requires the resolution of numerical problems to a greater extent than numerical analogies. These results are in agreement with those obtained by Harvey and Miller (2016) Harvey , H. , Miller , G. ( 2016 ). Executive function skills, early mathematics, and vocabulary in head start preschool children . Early Education and Development , 28 ( 3 ), 290 – 307 . https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2016.1218728 .
https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2016.12...
, whose conclusions highlight the importance of the development of Mathematical Language in the early stages of education for the appropriate acquisition of Mathematics in later stages.

On the other hand, indices such as Spatial Reasoning or Spatial Syllogisms do not stand out as predictors of academic success in the instrumental subject of Language, which fulfills our hypothesis (H d ). This contrasts with the results obtained in other studies in which these skills are understood to be determinants in the correct acquisition of reading and writing, and reading comprehension skills ( Bender, 1977 Bender , L . ( 1977 ). The Visomotor Guestaltico Test: Uses and clinical applications . México : Paidós . ; Koppitz, 1980 Koppitz , E . ( 1980 ). The Visomotor Gestalt Test for children . Buenos Aires : Guadalupe . ; Mlodnosky, 1968 Mlodnosky , L. ( 1968 ). The Frostig and the Bender gestalt as predictors of reading achievement . Technical Report , 3 ( 2 ), 129 – 139 . ; Spitz, 2009 Spitz , H . ( 2009 ). The raising of intelligence: a selected history of attempts to raise retarded intelligence . New Jersey : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates . ; Valett, 1989 Valett , R . ( 1989 ). Dyslexia . Barcelona : CEAC . ). Perhaps, these data could present a more significant difference in relation to the other indices, if instead of making the estimate by taking into account the final stage of primary education, the results would be isolated at the academic level for the subject of Language, during the first cycle of primary education, which is a period in which the acquisition of reading and writing is apriority objective and an academic competence that requires adequate maturation at the levels of Spatial Reasoning and Spatial Syllogisms.

Another noteworthy result, which confirms our hypothesis (H d ) and coincides with different investigations, is that General Intelligence is not the most relevant variable in terms of predicting school success at a general level or in specific academic areas such as Mathematics or Language (Deary, Strand, Smith, & Fernandes, 2007; Edel, 2003 Edel , R . ( 2003 ). Factors associated with academic performance . REICE. Revista Electrónica Iberoamericana sobre Calidad, Eficacia y Cambio en la Educación , 20 ( 1 ), 112 – 121 . ; Laidra, & Pullman, H.,& Allik, J., 2006; Miñano & Castejón, 2008 Miñano , P. , Castejón , J . ( 2008 ). Predictive ability of cognitive-motivational variables on academic performance . Revista Electrónica de Motivación y Emoción , 28 ( 11 ). ; Watkins, Lei, & Canivez, 2007). On the other hand, one must account for the fact that the above statement is controversial since in like manner, it is not difficult to compare it with other investigations with General Intelligence among their results, and General Intelligence is presented as a cognitive aptitude with a greater correlation with respect to academic success ( Deary et al., 2007 Deary , I. J. , Strand , S. , Smith , P. , Fernandes , C. ( 2007 ). Intelligence and educational achievement . Intelligence , 35 , 13 – 21 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.02...
; Gygi, Schweizer, & Grob, 2017; Kaufman, Reynolds, Liu, Kaufman, & McGrew, 2012; Roth et al., 2015 Roth , B. , Becker , N. , Romeyke , S. , Schäfer , S. , Domnick , F. , Spinath , F. M. ( 2015 ). Intelligence and school grades: a meta-analysis . Intelligence , 53 , 118 – 137 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.002 .
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.09...
; Schult & Sparfeldt, 2016) Schult , J. , Sparfeldt , J. R. ( 2016 ). Do non-g factors of cognitive ability test align with specific academic achievements? A combined bifactor modeling approach . Intelligence , 59 , 96 – 102 . https://doi.org/10.1006/jintell.2016.08.004 .
https://doi.org/10.1006/jintell.2016.08....
. Notwithstanding, for the sample in the present study, the highest score in this index, as measured by the BADyG E2r psychometric test, has not been associated with a greater probability of repeating a course. Therefore, although General Intelligence has some influence on student success with respect to achieving the curricular objectives of the reference course, it is not the most determining factor among the measured aptitudes.

In terms of study limitations, it is noteworthy that for future investigations, the sample of both subjects and provinces in which information was collected should be expanded, and this is among the most important limitations of this study. In addition, it would be advisable to not limit the study to a single evaluation instrument, but to include different psychometric tests that allow the collection of data from other variables beyond cognitive aptitudes, in order to be able to assert in a more substantial manner what academic successes and failures can be attributed to.

Based on the obtained results and as an implication for educational practice, we can affirm that the evaluation tool used in this study, the Battery of Differential and General Skills (BADyG-E2r), can be considered effective in anticipating learning difficulties within a program of early detection that aims to implement pedagogical strategies which favor an appropriate maturity in the aptitude levels of students, and anticipating any potential academic failure. The above points should be linked to the development of models of psychopedagogical intervention ( Raver et al., 2011 Raver , C. C. , Jones , S. M. , Li-Grining , C. , Zhai , F. , Bub , K. , Pressler , E. ( 2011 ). CSRP’s impact on low-income preschoolers’ preacademic skills: self-regulation as a mediating mechanism . Child Development , 82 , 362 – 378 . https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01561.x .
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010...
), which aim to reduce learning difficulties in the first cycles of primary education. This is the stage in which student difficulties that go unnoticed or are not given the necessary importance will present their first signs of deficiencies in student learning, which in many cases will accumulate on each course undertaken by the student. In fact, recent research ( Serpell & Esposito, 2016 Serpell , Z. , Esposito , A. ( 2016 ). Development of executive functions. Implications for educational policy and practice . Policy Insights From the Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 3 ( 2 ), 203 – 201 . https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732216654718 .
https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732216654718...
) underlines the importance of these types of strategies being transferred from government institutions through relevant legislation and legislation, to educational programs that are aimed at the early prevention of learning difficulties, in order to reduce the notable current rates of school failure.

As a final conclusion and in agreement with other research that collected their data from populations in Primary and Secondary Education ( Duncan et al., 2007 Duncan , G ., Dowsett , C ., Claessens , A ., Magnuson , K ., Huston , A ., Klebanov , P ., … Japel , C . ( 2007 ). School readiness and later achievement . Developmental Psychology , 43 ( 6 ), 1428 – 1446 . https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428 .
https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1...
; Harvey & Miller, 2016 Harvey , H. , Miller , G. ( 2016 ). Executive function skills, early mathematics, and vocabulary in head start preschool children . Early Education and Development , 28 ( 3 ), 290 – 307 . https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2016.1218728 .
https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2016.12...
; Serpell & Esposito, 2016) Serpell , Z. , Esposito , A. ( 2016 ). Development of executive functions. Implications for educational policy and practice . Policy Insights From the Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 3 ( 2 ), 203 – 201 . https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732216654718 .
https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732216654718...
, we wish to emphasize that early academic capacity is a strong predictor of later academic ability/achievement. Children with greater knowledge and understanding of letters and numerical concepts at the beginning of compulsory schooling achieve higher academic levels at later levels than their peers who are less prepared. The relevance of this research and the obtained data pertain to the Battery of Differential and General Abilities E-2r (BADyG-E2r), which is an adequate prediction tool at an early age, providing technical arguments and objectives regarding reinforcement measures at the initial schooling level, with no evident learning difficulties being present at that time.

References

  • Alloway , T. , Passolunghi , M. ( 2011 ). The relationship between working memory, IQ, and mathematical skills in children . Learning and Individual Differences , 21 ( 1 ), 133 – 137 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.09.013 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.09.013
  • Bender , L . ( 1977 ). The Visomotor Guestaltico Test: Uses and clinical applications . México : Paidós .
  • Bennett , G. , Seashore , H. y Wesman , A. ( 1997 ). Test de Aptitudes Diferenciales DAT Forma T, Manual . Buenos Aires : Paidós .
  • Bergman , S. , Söderqvist , S. , Bryde , S. , Thorell , L. B. , Humphreys , K. , Klingberg , T. ( 2011 ). Gains in fluid intelligence after training non-verbal reasoning in 4-year-old children: a controlled, randomized study . Developmental Science , 14 ( 3 ), 591 – 601 . https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01022.x .
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01022.x
  • Blair , C. , McKinnon , R. D. , the Family Life Project Investigators ( 2016 ). Moderating effects of executive functions and the teacher–child relationship on the development of mathematics ability in kindergarten . Learning and Instruction , 41 , 85 – 93 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.10.001 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.10.001
  • Blair , C. , Raver , C. ( 2014 ). Closing the achievement gap through modification of neurocognitive and neuroendocrine function: results from a cluster randomized controlled trial of an innovative approach to the Education of Children in Kindergarten . PLoS One , 9 ( 11 ). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112393 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112393
  • Burnet , S. , Lane , C. M. ( 1980 ). Effects of academic instruction of spational visualization . Intelligence , 4 , 233 – 247 . https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(80)90021-5 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(80)90021-5
  • Cassidy , S. , Roche , B. , Colbert , D. , Stewart , I. , Grey , I. M. ( 2016 ). A realional frame skills training intervention to increase general intelligence and scholastic aptitude . Learning and Individual Differences , 47 , 222 – 235 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.03.001 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.03.001
  • Cerda , G. , Pérez , C. , Navarro , J. I. , Aguilar , M. , Casas , J. A. , Aragón , E . ( 2015 ). Explanatory model of emotinal-cognitive variables in school mathematics performance: a longitudinal study in primary school . Frontiers in Psychology , 6 , 1363 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01363 .
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01363
  • Cheng , Y. L. , Mix , K. S. ( 2014 ). Spatial training improves children’s mathematics ability . Journal of Cognition and Development , 15 , 2 – 11 . https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.725186 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.725186
  • Cooper , L . & Reagan , D . ( 1982 ). Attention, perception and intelligence. Handbook of human Intelligence . In Sternberg (Ed.), CambridgeUniversity Press , 123 - 170 .
  • Cornu , V. , Schiltz , C. , Pazouki , T. , Martin , R. ( 2017 ). Training early visuo-spatial abilities: a controlled classroom-based intervention study . Applied Developmental Science . Advance online publication . doi. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2016.1276835 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2016.1276835
  • De Maris , A . ( 2003 ). Logistic regression . En J.A. Schinka y W. F. Velicer ( Eds .) , Research Methods in Psychology (pp. 509 - 532 ). New Jersey : John Wiley & Sons .
  • Deary , I. J. , Strand , S. , Smith , P. , Fernandes , C. ( 2007 ). Intelligence and educational achievement . Intelligence , 35 , 13 – 21 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.02.001
  • Di Lieto , M ., Inguaggiato , E ., Castro , E ., Cecchi , F ., Cioni , G ., Dell’Omo , M ., … Pecini , C . ( 2017 ). Educational robotics intervention on executive functions in preschool children: a pilot study . Computers in Human Behavior , 71 , 16 – 23 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.018 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.01.018
  • Duncan , G ., Dowsett , C ., Claessens , A ., Magnuson , K ., Huston , A ., Klebanov , P ., … Japel , C . ( 2007 ). School readiness and later achievement . Developmental Psychology , 43 ( 6 ), 1428 – 1446 . https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428
  • Edel , R . ( 2003 ). Factors associated with academic performance . REICE. Revista Electrónica Iberoamericana sobre Calidad, Eficacia y Cambio en la Educación , 20 ( 1 ), 112 – 121 .
  • Fergusson , D. , Horwood , J. , Ridder , E. ( 2005 ). Show me the child at seven II: childhood intelligence and later outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood . The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 46 ( 8 ), 850 – 858 . https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01472.x .
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01472.x
  • Gagné , F. , Père , F . ( 2001 ). When IQ is controlled, does motivation still predict achievement? Intelligence , 30 , 71 – 100 . https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00068-X .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00068-X
  • García , M. V. , Alvarado , J. M. , Jiménez , A . ( 2000 ). The prediction of academic performance: linear regression versus logistic regression . Psicothema , 12 ( 2 ), 248 – 252 .
  • Geary , D. C. , VanMarie , K. ( 2016 ). Young children’s core symbolic and nonsymbolic quantitative knowledge in the prediction of later mathematics achievement . Developmental Psychology , 52 ( 12 ), 2130 – 2144 . https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000214 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000214
  • Grañeras , M ., Díaz-Caneja , & Gil , N . ( 2011 ). Success actions in European schools . Madrid : Secretaría General Técnica del Ministerio de Educación .
  • Gygi , J.T ., Hagmann-von,P ., Schweizer , F ., & Grob , A . ( 2017 ). The predictive validity of four intelligence tests for school grades: a small sample longitudinal study . Frontiers in Psychology , 8 : 375 . doi: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00375
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00375
  • Harvey , H. , Miller , G. ( 2016 ). Executive function skills, early mathematics, and vocabulary in head start preschool children . Early Education and Development , 28 ( 3 ), 290 – 307 . https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2016.1218728 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2016.1218728
  • Hawes , Z. , Moss , J. , Caswell , B. , Poliszczuk , D. ( 2015 ). Effects of mental rotation training on children’s spatial and mathematics performance: a randomized controlled study . Trends in Neuroscience and Education , 4 ( 3 ), 60 – 68 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2015.05.001 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2015.05.001
  • Hill , O. , Serpell , Z. , Faison , M. ( 2016 ). The efficacy of the LearningRx cognitive training program: modality and transfer effects . The Journal of Experimental Education , 84 ( 3 ), 600 – 620 . https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb01421.x .
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb01421.x
  • Kaufman , K. A. , Reynolds , M. R. , Liu , X. , Kaufman , A. S. , McGrew , K. S. ( 2012 ). Are cognitive g and academic achievement g on and the same g? Intelligence , 40 , 123 – 138 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.009 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.009
  • Kern , M. L. , Friedman , H. S. ( 2009 ). Early educational milestones as predictors of lifelong academic achievement, midlife adjustment and longevity . Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology , 30 , 419 – 430 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.025 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.025
  • Koppitz , E . ( 1980 ). The Visomotor Gestalt Test for children . Buenos Aires : Guadalupe .
  • Kuncel , N. R. , Hezlett , S. A. , Ones , D. S. ( 2004 ). Academic performance, career potential, creativity, and job performance: can one construct predict them all? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 86 , 148 – 161 .
  • Kytala , M. , Lehto , J. E. ( 2008 ). Some factors underlying mathematical performance: the role of visuospatial working memory and non-verbal intelligence . EuropeanJournalofPsychology of Education , 23 , 77 – 94 .
  • Laidra , K. , Pullman , H. , Allik , J. ( 2006 ). Personality and intelligence as predictors of academic achievement: a cross-sectional study from elementary to secondary school . Personality and Individual Differences , 42 ( 3 ), 441 – 451 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.001 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.001
  • Marrero , H. , Espino , O. ( 1988 ). Evaluación comparativa del poder predictor de las aptitudes sobre notas escolares y pruebas objetivas . Revista de Educación , 287 , 97 – 112 .
  • Matthews , P. G. , Lewis , M. R. , Hubbard , E. M. ( 2015 ). Individual differences in nonsymbolic ratio processing predict symbolic math performance . Psychological Science , 27 , 191 – 202 . https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615617799 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615617799
  • Miñano , P. , Cantero , M. P. , Castejón , J. L . ( 2008 ). Predicción del rendimiento escolar de los alumnos a partir de las aptitudes, el autoconcepto académico y las atribuciones causales . Revista Horizontes educacionales , 13 ( 2 ), 11 – 23 .
  • Miñano , P. , Castejón , J . ( 2008 ). Predictive ability of cognitive-motivational variables on academic performance . Revista Electrónica de Motivación y Emoción , 28 ( 11 ).
  • Mlodnosky , L. ( 1968 ). The Frostig and the Bender gestalt as predictors of reading achievement . Technical Report , 3 ( 2 ), 129 – 139 .
  • Nagelkerke , N. J. ( 1991 ). A note on a general definition of the coefficient of determination . Biometrika , 78 ( 3 ), 691 – 692 . https://doi.org/10.2307/2337038 .
    » https://doi.org/10.2307/2337038
  • Navarro-Soria , I . ( 2016 ). Cognitive, school and socio-demographic variables predictive of academic achievement in Primary Education ( Doctoral Thesis ). Universidad de Alicante , Alicante, España . doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.21077.32482
    » https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.21077.32482
  • Navarro-Soria , I . y Gonzalez-Gómez , C . ( 2010 ). Systematic detection of aptitude deficits as a strategy to prevent academic difficulties .IN: R. Roig Vila , M. Fiorucci , Keys to research in educational quality and innovation .(págs. 297 - 309 ). Alcoi : Marfil .
  • Nisbett , R. , Aronson , J. , Blair , C. , Dickens , W. , Flynn , J. , Halpern , D. , Turkheimer , E. ( 2012 ). Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments . American Psychologist , 67 ( 2 ), 130 – 159 . https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026699 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026699
  • Park , J. , Bermudez , V. , Roberts , R. , Brannon , E. ( 2016 ). Non-symbolic approximate arithmetic training improves math performance in preschoolers . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 152 , 278 – 293 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.07.011 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.07.011
  • Peng , P. , Fuchs , D. ( 2016 ). A meta-analysis of working memory deficits in children with learning difficulties: is there a difference between verbal domain and numerical domain? Journal of Learning Disabilities , 49 ( 1 ), 3 – 20 . https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219414521667 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219414521667
  • Pérez , E. , Cupani , M. , Ayllón , S . ( 2005 ). Predictors of academic performance in middle school: skills, self-efficacy and personality traits . Avaliaço Psicológica , 4 ( 1 ), 1 – 11 .
  • Pitchford , N. , Papini , C. , Outhwaite , L. , Gulliford , A. ( 2016 ). Fine motor skills predict maths ability better than they predict reading ability in the early primary school years . Frontiers in Psychology , 7 , 783 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00783 .
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00783
  • Prior , M. , Bavin , E. , Ong , B. ( 2010 ). Predictors of school readiness in five- to six- year-old children from an Australian longitudinal community sample . Educatinal Psycholgy , 31 ( 1 ), 3 – 16 . https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2010.541048 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2010.541048
  • Raver , C. C. , Jones , S. M. , Li-Grining , C. , Zhai , F. , Bub , K. , Pressler , E. ( 2011 ). CSRP’s impact on low-income preschoolers’ preacademic skills: self-regulation as a mediating mechanism . Child Development , 82 , 362 – 378 . https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01561.x .
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01561.x
  • Robles , C. , Vázquez , E. ( 2014 ). The influence of verbal skills on school success . International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology , 6 ( 1 ), 351 – 362 .
  • Rogowsky , B. A. , Papamichalis , P. , Villa , L. , Heim , S. , Tallal , P. ( 2013 ). Neuroplasticity-based cognitive and linguistic skills training improves reading and writing skills in college students . Frontiers in Psychology , 4 , 137 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00137 .
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00137
  • Roth , B. , Becker , N. , Romeyke , S. , Schäfer , S. , Domnick , F. , Spinath , F. M. ( 2015 ). Intelligence and school grades: a meta-analysis . Intelligence , 53 , 118 – 137 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.002 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.002
  • Schult , J. , Sparfeldt , J. R. ( 2016 ). Do non-g factors of cognitive ability test align with specific academic achievements? A combined bifactor modeling approach . Intelligence , 59 , 96 – 102 . https://doi.org/10.1006/jintell.2016.08.004 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1006/jintell.2016.08.004
  • Serpell , Z. , Esposito , A. ( 2016 ). Development of executive functions. Implications for educational policy and practice . Policy Insights From the Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 3 ( 2 ), 203 – 201 . https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732216654718 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732216654718
  • Smith , S . ( 1964 ) The Psychology of Illness . Nursing Forum 3 ( 1 ), 34 – 47 .
  • Spinath , B. , Spinath , F. M. , Harlaar , N. , Plomin , R. ( 2006 ). Predicting school achievement from general cognitive ability, self-perceived ability, and intrinsic value . Intelligence , 34 ( 4 ), 363 – 374 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.11.004 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.11.004
  • Spitz , H . ( 2009 ). The raising of intelligence: a selected history of attempts to raise retarded intelligence . New Jersey : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates .
  • Sternberg , R. , Kaufman , J. ( 1998 ). Human abilities . Annual Review of Psychology , 49 , 479 – 502 . https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.479 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.479
  • Strenze , T . ( 2007 ). Intelligence and socioeconomic success: a meta-analytic review of longitudinal research . Intelligence , 35 , 401 – 426 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.004 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.09.004
  • Taub , G. , Keith , T. , Floyd , R. , Mcgrew , K. ( 2008 ). Effects of general and broad cognitive abilities on mathematics achievement . School Psychology Quarterly , 23 , 187 – 198 . https://doi.org/10.1037/1045-3830.23.2.187 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/1045-3830.23.2.187
  • Toll , S ., & Van Luit , J . ( 2014 ). Explaining numeracy development in weak performing kindergartners . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 124 , 97 – 111 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.001 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.001
  • Valett , R . ( 1989 ). Dyslexia . Barcelona : CEAC .
  • Vélez-van Meerbeke , A ., & Roa-González , C. N . ( 2005 ). Factores asociados al rendimiento académico en estudiantes de medicina . Educación médica , 8 ( 2 ), 74 – 82 .
  • Watkins , M. , Lei , P. , Canivez , G. ( 2007 ). Psychometric intelligence and achievement: a cross-lagged panel analysis . Intelligence , 35 ( 1 ), 59 – 68 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.04.005 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2006.04.005
  • Ye , A. , Resnick , I. , Hansen , N. , Rodrigues , J. , Rinne , L. , Jordan , N. ( 2016 ). Pathways to fraction learning: numerical abilities mediate the relation between early cognitive competencies and later fraction knowledge . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 152 , 242 – 263 . https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.001 .
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.08.001
  • Yuste-Hernanz , C . ( 2002 ). Battery of differential and general skills. BADYG E-2 . Madrid : TEA Ediciones .
  • Yuste-Hernanz , C . ( 2012 ). Battery of differential and general skills. Technical manual . Madird : CEPE .
  • Abbreviations
    BADYG: Differential and General Skills Battery χ: Chi square R 2 : R 2 Nagelkerke B: Coefficient p: Probability OR: Odds ratio CI: Confidence interval
  • Availability of data and materials
    Data are available from the first author.
  • Ethics approval and consent to participate
    The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Alicante, which provides and approves the methodology used, and the approval was assigned the file number UA-2018-03-08.
  • Consent for publication
    Not applicable.
  • Publisher’s Note
    Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
  • Funding
    This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    19 June 2020
  • Date of issue
    2020

History

  • Received
    15 Apr 2019
  • Accepted
    8 Apr 2020
  • Published
    07 May 2020
Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia da Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul Rua Ramiro Barcelos, 2600 - sala 110, 90035-003 Porto Alegre RS - Brazil, Tel.: +55 51 3308-5691 - Porto Alegre - RS - Brazil
E-mail: prc@springeropen.com