Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Mapping the clockworks: what does the Clock Drawing Test assess in normal and pathological aging?

Mapeando as engrenagens: o que o Teste do Desenho do Relógio avalia no envelhecimento normal e patológico?

Abstracts

The Clock Drawing Test (CDT) is a cognitive screening tool used in clinical and research settings. Despite its role on the assessment of global cognitive functioning, the specific cognitive components required for test performance are still unclear. We aim to assess the role of executive functioning, global cognitive status, visuospatial abilities, and semantic knowledge on Shulman’s CDT performance. Fifty-three mild cognitive impairment, 60 Alzheimer’s dementia, and 57 normal elderly controls performed the CDT, the Frontal Assessment Battery, the Mini-Mental State Examination, the Stick Design Test, and a naming test (TN-LIN). An ordinal regression assessed specific neuropsychological influences on CDT performance. All the cognitive variables were related to the CDT, accounting for 53% of variance. The strongest association was between the CDT and executive functions, followed by global cognitive status, visuospatial processing, and semantic knowledge. Our result confirms the multidimensional nature of the test and the major role of executive functions on performance.

clock drawing test; executive functions; visuospatial abilities; semantic knowledge; dementia


O Teste do Desenho do Relógio (TDR) é um instrumento de rastreio cognitivo amplamente usado em contextos clínicos e de pesquisa. Embora seu papel na avaliação do funcionamento cognitivo global seja reconhecido, os componentes específicos necessários à sua realização não são claros. Objetivo: avaliar a influência das funções executivas, estado cognitivo global, processamento visioespacial e conhecimento semântico no TDR. Realizaram o TDR, a Bateria de Avaliação Frontal, o Mini-Exame do Estado Mental, a Construção com Palitos, e um teste de nomeação (TN-LIN) 53 pacientes com comprometimento cognitivo leve, 60 com Alzheimer e 57 idosos normais.. A influência de variáveis neuropsicológicas específicas no TDR foi avaliada através de regressão ordinal. Todas as variáveis se relacionaram com o teste, respondendo por aproximadamente 53% da variância. A associação mais forte foi entre o TDR e as funções executivas, seguidas pela cognição geral, processamento visioespacial e conhecimento semântico. Nossos resultados confirmam a natureza multidimensional da tarefa e o papel das funções executivas em sua realização.

teste do desenho do relógio; funções executivas; habilidades visioespaciais; conhecimento semântico; demência


Brief cognitive tests are widely used in neuropsychology and related areas, in order to assess and diagnose developmental, neurological, and psychiatric conditions. The Clock Drawing Test (CDT) is also adopted as a screening tool for cognitive impairment in several conditions such as dementia, stroke, schizophrenia, mild cognitive impairment and mood disorders 11. Freedman M, Kaplan E, Delis D, Morris R. Clock Drawing: a neuropsychological analysis. New York: Oxford University Press, 1994. . The CDT basically consists of the drawing of different elements of a clock, including the clock face, numbers, and arrow-hands. The instructions and scoring methods vary according to each version. Influences of aging and education are also related to test performance, where children and the elderly perform worse than adults and lower education associates with worse performance 22. Marcopulos BA, McLain CA. Are our norms “Normal”? A 4-year follow-up study of a biracial sample of rural elders with low education. Clin Neuropsychol 2003;17:19-33. .

Different versions of the CDT were developed. Although very similar and highly correlated³, they differ in diagnosis accuracy 44. Tuokko H, Hadjistavropoulos T, Rae S, O’Rourke N. A comparison of alternative approaches to the scoring of clock drawing. 2000. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2000;15:137-148. and probably on the cognitive processes involved in test performance. Some versions 11. Freedman M, Kaplan E, Delis D, Morris R. Clock Drawing: a neuropsychological analysis. New York: Oxford University Press, 1994. , 55. Rouleau I, Salmon DP, Butters N. Longitudinal analysis of clock drawing in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Brain Cognition 1996;18:79-87. require the patient to freely draw the clock face, demanding more pronounced visuoconstructional abilities, while other versions 66. Shulman KI. Clock-drawing: is the ideal cognitive screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:548-561. , 77. Sunderland T, Hill JL, Mellow AM, et al. Clock drawing in Alzheimer’s disease: a novel measure of dementia severity. J Am Geriatr Soc 1989;37:725-729. offer a pre-drawn circle for the clock face, focusing on the abilities to organize the clock elements on the empty space.

Different versions of the CDT are used in Brazil for the neuropsychological assessment of elderly patients, including the one adopted in this research, which seems a validity test for the distinction of patients with normal or impaired cognitive status 88. de Paula JJ, Schlottfeldt CG, Moreira L, et al. Psychometric properties of a brief neuropsychological protocol for use in geriatric populations. Rev Psiq Clin 2010;37:246-250. . A study compared different versions of the CDT in terms of criterion validity and did not find significant differences between them 99. Lourenço RV, Ribeiro-Filho ST, Moreira IFH, Paradela EMP, Miranda AS. Clock Drawing Test: performance among elderly with low educational level. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 2008;30:309-315. . In addition, it showed good evidence of reliability, but reduced criterion-related validity for this population 99. Lourenço RV, Ribeiro-Filho ST, Moreira IFH, Paradela EMP, Miranda AS. Clock Drawing Test: performance among elderly with low educational level. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 2008;30:309-315. . Other studies 1010. Aprahamian I, Martinelli JE, Cecato J, Yassuda MS. Screening for Alzheimer’s disease among illiterate elderly: accuracy analysis for multiple instruments. J Alzheimers Dis 2011;26:221-229. , 1111. Aprahamian I, Martinelli JE, Neri AL, Yassuda MS. The accuracy of the Clock Drawing Test compared to that of standard screening tests for Alzheimer’s disease: results from a study of Brazilian elderly with heterogeneous educational backgrounds. Int Psychogeriatr 2010;22:64-71. , however, assessed CDT validity for cognitive assessment among individuals with heterogeneous educational backgrounds, including illiteracy, showing good results, but emphasizing the role of formal education on test performance.

Despite these factors, the CDT usually shows significant correlations with measures of visuospatial abilities 1212. Rothenberg KG, Piądło R, Nagaraj UD, Friedland RP. The complex picture test in dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra 2012;2:411-417. , executive functions 33. Royall DR, Mulroy AR, Chiodo LK, Polk MJ. Clock drawing is sensitive to executive control: a comparison of six methods. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1999;54:328-333. , semantic processing 1313. Libon DJ, Malamut BL, Swenson R, Sands LP, Cloud BS. Further analyses of clock drawings among demented and non-demented older subjects. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1996;11:193-205. , or more global and diffuse cognitive abilities 88. de Paula JJ, Schlottfeldt CG, Moreira L, et al. Psychometric properties of a brief neuropsychological protocol for use in geriatric populations. Rev Psiq Clin 2010;37:246-250. . In Brazil, it is primarily used for the assessment of executive functions 1414. Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bottino CMC, Damasceno BP, Brucki SMD, Anghinah R. Diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in Brazil: cognitive and functional evaluation. Recommendations of the Scientific Department of Cognitive Neurology and Aging of the Brazilian Academy of Neurology. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2005;63:720-727. . In this sense, since the score is related to several cognitive abilities impaired performance is less informative concerning which cognitive domain is more affected. According to a review of different scoring methods 66. Shulman KI. Clock-drawing: is the ideal cognitive screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:548-561. the CDT shows good evidence of psychometric properties, such as criterion validity and reliability. However, concerning construct validity, the studies are heterogeneous, using different neuropsychological measures as the gold standard for specific functions. Therefore, in this study we aim to verify what type of cognitive processes (executive functions, visuospatial processing, and semantics) is more related to the performance in the CDT. Our hypothesis is that the CDT is influenced by several cognitive domains, most importantly executive functions.

METHODS

Participants

We assessed 170 older adults. The groups were composed of patients diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment (MCI, N =53, 68% women), mild Alzheimer’s dementia (AD, N =60, 55% women) and normal controls matched by age, gender, and education (NC, N =57, 67% women). The inclusion criteria were age equal to or greater than 60 years, at least 1 year of formal education, score on the Short Version of the Geriatric Depression Scale 1515. Almeida OP Almeida AS. Short versions of the Geriatric Depression Scale: a study of their validity for the diagnosis of a major depressive episode according to ICD-10 and DSM-IV. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1999;14:858-865. below the cutoff for depression, and lack of current use of typical or atypical antipsychotic drugs. MCI diagnosis was performed based on Petersen’s criteria 1616. Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 2001;58:1985-1992. and AD on the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke (NINCDS) and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (ADRDA) criteria 1717. McKhann G, Crachman D, Folstein M, Katman R, Price D, Stadlan ME. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984;34:939-944. . All participants underwent a clinical and neuropsychological assessment at the Instituto Jenny Faria de Atenção à Saúde do Idoso, a secondary/tertiary heath unit of the city of Belo Horizonte for elderly people. Diagnoses were performed in consensus by at least one geriatrician and one clinical neuropsychologist. The control group underwent neuropsychological assessment for the exclusion of cognitive impairment and answered a semi-structured interview for the exclusion of psychiatric, neurologic, or other disorders. All participants and their caregivers signed the written consent for participation. The study was approved by the Ethical Board of the Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (COEP 334/06).

Neuropsychological assessment

For the present study Shulman’s version of the CDT 66. Shulman KI. Clock-drawing: is the ideal cognitive screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:548-561. was chosen. This is one of the most sensitive versions for cognitive impairment and dementia 44. Tuokko H, Hadjistavropoulos T, Rae S, O’Rourke N. A comparison of alternative approaches to the scoring of clock drawing. 2000. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2000;15:137-148. . On this CDT version the scoring system ranges from 0 (worst) to 5 (best). Based on previous CDT studies, we aim at the assessment of global cognitive functioning, executive functions, visuospatial abilities, and semantic knowledge on CDT performance, choosing four neuropsychological tests to represent these abilities.

Global cognitive functioning

The Brazilian version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) 1818. Bertolucci PHF, Brucki SMD, Campacci SR, Juliano Y. O Mini-Exame do Estado Mental em uma população geriátrica: impacto da escolaridade. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 1994;52:1-7. was chosen as a global measure of cognitive functioning. The test consists of a brief assessment of temporal and spatial orientation, short-term memory, attention/calculation, language, motor, and constructional praxis. Scores range from 0 (worst) to 30 (best).

Executive functions

This complex and multidimensional cognitive domain was assessed by the Brazilian version of the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) 1919. Beato RG, Nitrini R, Formigoni AP, Caramelli P. Brazilian version of the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB). Dem Neuropsychol 2007;1:59-65. , a bedside short screening battery for the assessment of frontal/executive functions. The battery has six subtests that have been developed for mapping specific components of executive functions. Scores range from 0 (worst) to 18 (best).

Visuospatial abilities

We chose a non-timed assembly model task for assessing this cognitive domain. The Stick Design Test (SDT) 2020. Baiyewu O, Unverzagt FW, Lane KA, et al. The Stick Design test: a new measure of visuoconstructional ability. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2005;11:598-605. was developed for the constructional praxis examination in elderly individuals with little formal education, and has shown solid evidence of validity for the Brazilian population 2121. de Paula JJ, Costa MV, Bocardi MB, et al. The Stick Design Test on the Assessment of older adults with low formal education: evidences of construct, criterion-related and ecological validity. Int Psychogeriatr 2013; epub ahead of print. . The scores range from 0 (worst) to 12 (best).

Semantic knowledge

A naming test was chosen for the semantic assessment. Since cultural differences strongly influence naming abilities, a Brazilian Test with regionalized stimuli was chosen: the Teste de Nomeação do Laboratório de Investigações Neuropsicológicas/Naming Test of the Laboratory of Neuropsychological Investigations (TN-LIN) 2222. Malloy-Diniz LF, Bentes RC, Figueiredo PM, et al. Normalización de uma batería de tests para evaluar las habilidades de comprensión del lenguaje, fluidez verbal y denominación em niños brasileños de 7 a 10 años: resultados preliminares. Rev Neurol 2007;44:275-280. .

Statistical procedures

Histogram analysis suggests non-normal distribution for most of the neuropsychological tests. In this sense, comparisons considering sociodemographic variables and neuropsychological assessment were performed by the Kruskal–Wallis test. Mann–Whitney tests (Bonferroni corrected) were used for specific group comparisons. An exploratory analysis of the association of the CDT with other cognitive measures was carried out by the Spearman rank-order correlation. After this preliminary procedure, the association between the different cognitive measures and CDT performance was investigated by ordinal regression models. Six models were tested. The first contains the CDT as dependent variable, and age and education as predictors. Models 2, 3, 4, and 5 contain the previous variables and each of the four neuropsychological tests independently. The final model (6) contains age, education, and all the neuropsychological measures together as predictors. We aim at the analysis of specific contributions of each predictor (Wald statistic) and the effect size of the model (Negelkerke R² statistic).

RESULTS

The participants did not differ in terms of age, formal education, depressive symptoms, and proportion of men and women among groups (all p>0.05). On the neuropsychological assessment, differences were found in all cognitive measures, with NC, MCI, and AD patients performing progressively worse on the cognitive tests except for the SDT ( Table 1 ).

The correlational coefficients between CDT scores and the other neuropsychological measures were all significant at p<0.001. The strongest correlation was found for the executive functions (0.628), followed by general cognitive functioning (0.595), semantic knowledge (0.574), and, lastly, visuospatial abilities (0.518).

All proposed regression models were significant (p<0.001). Alone, the sociodemographic predictors account for 16% of the explained variance, while the models containing these predictors and one neuropsychological measure explain from 29% to 42% of the variance. When all the cognitive measures are taken together, the model reaches the largest effect size, with 53% of explained variance. In the last model, the executive functions, assessed by the FAB, were the best predictor (Wald χ²=13.79, p<0.001) ( Table 2 ).

DISCUSSION

Our result suggests that different cognitive domains are related to CDT performance. The study uses different and well validated measures for each cognitive domain and they were all related to CDT performance. Together, the influence of the cognitive predictors was large, accounting for 53% of CDT variance.

The cognitive domain most related to CDT performance in the present study was the executive functioning, measured by the FAB total score. This cognitive construct has been related to CDT performance before, integrating a general factor of executive function in a participant’s sample similar to the one found in our study 2323. Gibbons LE, Carle AC, Mackin RS, et al and for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. A composite score for executive functioning validated in Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) participants with baseline mild cognitive impairment. Brain Imaging Behav 2012;6:517-527 . The prefrontal cortex is related to CDT performance, especially to the time required for the drawing, possibly due to more efficient planning 2424. Shoyama M, Nishioka T, Okumura M, et al. Brain activity during the Clock-Drawing Test: multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy study. Appl Neuropsychol 2011;18:243-251. . Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies also suggest bilateral frontal lobe activation during the CDT, although other regions are also involved 2525. Ino T, Asada T, Ito J, Kimura T, Fukuyama H. Parieto-frontal networks for clock drawing revealed with fMRI. Neurosci Res 2003;45:71-77. . Studies with patients with focal lesions, circumscribed to the frontal lobes, have reported CDT impairment 11. Freedman M, Kaplan E, Delis D, Morris R. Clock Drawing: a neuropsychological analysis. New York: Oxford University Press, 1994. . Freedman et al. 11. Freedman M, Kaplan E, Delis D, Morris R. Clock Drawing: a neuropsychological analysis. New York: Oxford University Press, 1994. argue that a different pattern of CDT impairment may be seen in right frontal hemisphere lesions (characterized by mild spatial deficits, misorientation of arrow-hands, and number inversion) when compared with left frontal lobe lesions (difficulty in understanding the test instructions, plotting an inverse number pattern, and perseverations), although, as shown by another study 2626. Suhr J, Grace J, Allen J, Nadler J, McKenna M. Quantitative and qualitative performance of stroke versus normal elderly on six clock drawing systems. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1998;13:495-502. , this pattern may not be precisely clear. CDT performance may demand planning skills (for overall task goals), abstract reasoning (for number–space–arrow-hand comprehension), inhibitory control (for correctly sequencing the numbers and arrow-hand placing), and cognitive-shifting for adopting new strategies. All these specific cognitive processes are involved in executive functioning, the best single predictor of CDT performance according to our results.

Global cognitive impairment was the second predictor of CDT performance in our study and may be related to the integration of different cognitive domains necessary for task performance. As reported by previous studies 44. Tuokko H, Hadjistavropoulos T, Rae S, O’Rourke N. A comparison of alternative approaches to the scoring of clock drawing. 2000. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2000;15:137-148. , 66. Shulman KI. Clock-drawing: is the ideal cognitive screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:548-561. , 2727. Royall DR, Cordes JA, Polk M. CLOX: an executive clock drawing task. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;64:588-594. the CDT is a powerful screening test for cognitive impairment, with evidence of sensitivity, specificity, and significant correlations with global cognitive measures, such as the MMSE. The multiple cognitive domains involved in test performance and the necessary integration of these independent aspects for accurate drawing may justify its role in cognitive screening. As a more general cognitive measure, the CDT can be used for detection of cognitive impairment 66. Shulman KI. Clock-drawing: is the ideal cognitive screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:548-561. , 88. de Paula JJ, Schlottfeldt CG, Moreira L, et al. Psychometric properties of a brief neuropsychological protocol for use in geriatric populations. Rev Psiq Clin 2010;37:246-250. , staging of cognitive impairment 44. Tuokko H, Hadjistavropoulos T, Rae S, O’Rourke N. A comparison of alternative approaches to the scoring of clock drawing. 2000. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2000;15:137-148. , 66. Shulman KI. Clock-drawing: is the ideal cognitive screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:548-561. , 2727. Royall DR, Cordes JA, Polk M. CLOX: an executive clock drawing task. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;64:588-594. , differential diagnosis 66. Shulman KI. Clock-drawing: is the ideal cognitive screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:548-561. , 28 , 28. Moretti R, Torre P, Antonello RM, Gazzato G, Bava A. Ten-point clock test: a correlation analysis with other neuropsychological tests in dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2002;17:347-353. and for predicting subsequent cognitive decline 66. Shulman KI. Clock-drawing: is the ideal cognitive screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:548-561. , 2929. O’Rourke N, Tuokko H, Hayden S, Beattie BL. Early identification of dementia: predictive validity of the clock test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1997;12:257-267. . In more discrete conditions, however, the results are inconsistent. Our results suggest worse performance of MCI patients than NC, but a review of similar studies found negative results 3030. Ehreke L, Luppa M, Konig HH, Riedel-Heller SG. Is the Clock Drawing Test a screening tool for the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment? A systematic review. Int Psychogeriatr 2010;22:56-63. . The second predictor of CDT performance in our study may be related to the integration of different cognitive domains necessary for task performance.

The third significant predictor of CDT performance was semantic knowledge, assessed by a naming task. A previous study 99. Lourenço RV, Ribeiro-Filho ST, Moreira IFH, Paradela EMP, Miranda AS. Clock Drawing Test: performance among elderly with low educational level. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 2008;30:309-315. deconstructing the CDT into different components found moderate associations with tasks related to semantic processing, such as naming and category fluency. Drawing involves the search and recall of semantic representations of the clock itself, such as number disposition, hand-arrow meaning, and the spatial logics of its organization. In this sense, the degraded semantic knowledge, assessed by naming tests, may impair CDT performance. Other authors 3131. Blair M, Kertesz A, McMonagle P, Davidson W, Bodi N. Quantitative and qualitative analyses of clock drawing in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006;12:159-165. , working with AD and frontotemporal dementia patients (including semantic dementia), found quantitative and qualitative impairments in CDT performance that may, at least partially, be related to semantic deficits. Functional neuroimaging studies with AD patients identified posterior left temporal lobe (areas related to language and semantic processing) blood flow to be a predictor of CDT performance 3232. Cahn-Weiner DA, Sullivan EV, Shear PK, et al. Brain structural and cognitive correlates of clock drawing performance in Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1999;5:502-509. , 3333. Ueda H, Kitabayashi Y, Narumoto J, et al. Relationship between clock drawing test performance and regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s disease: a single photon emission computed tomography study. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002;56:25-29. .

Finally, visuospatial abilities were related to CDT performance in our sample. Posterior brain regions are essential for drawing tasks, which involve the perception/semantic recall of an image, planning, and the subsequent motor programming for task execution. CDT usually presents moderate correlations with other measures of visuospatial processing, such as block assembly and figure drawing 1212. Rothenberg KG, Piądło R, Nagaraj UD, Friedland RP. The complex picture test in dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra 2012;2:411-417. , 1313. Libon DJ, Malamut BL, Swenson R, Sands LP, Cloud BS. Further analyses of clock drawings among demented and non-demented older subjects. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1996;11:193-205. . Patients with vascular lesions, especially those on the posterior right hemisphere 11. Freedman M, Kaplan E, Delis D, Morris R. Clock Drawing: a neuropsychological analysis. New York: Oxford University Press, 1994. , 3434. de Guise E, LeBlanc J, Gosselin N, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of the clock drawing test in patients with traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 2010;24:1568-1574. , usually show expressive impairment in the CDT. A large study of patients with focal brain injury suggests that CDT scores are associated with the right parietal cortex and the left inferior frontal-parietal opercular cortices 35 .35. Tranel D, Rudrauf D, Vianna E, Damasio H. Does the Clock Drawing Test have focal neuroanatomical correlates? Neuropsychology 2008;22:553-562. Visuospatial abilities may contribute to different aspects of CDT performance, from visual perception of the ongoing copy to spatial planning and visual working memory. Even basic aspects of spatial reasoning, such as magnitude perception, may be related to CDT performance. Only a small fraction of the explained variance of CDT performance was explained by the adopted measure of visuospatial abilities. This may be secondary to the relative small variance of the SDT measure (the only significant differences were between non-demented and AD participants). Future studies should investigate the contributions of this cognitive domain to CDT performance on a more heterogeneous sample in order to maximize test variance.

Certainly, different versions of the CDT may present different cognitive demands. The selected version uses a pre-drawn circle where the patients must organize the clock, which may reduce the visuospatial demands for test performance, focusing on more executive components as planning and online monitoring. The procedures adopted here could be used in future research, examining different CDT versions and comparing the cognitive demands of each one. These studies may guide neuropsychologists and other clinicians in the choice of which CDT version may be more accurate for different hypotheses about cognitive functioning.

The influence of formal education on test performance must also be addressed in this discussion. Although education entered as a covariate on the ordinal linear regression models, the low variance between the groups (not significant) may reduce its influence on test performance. Although some studies reported good criterion validity for the CDT among poorly educated older adults 88. de Paula JJ, Schlottfeldt CG, Moreira L, et al. Psychometric properties of a brief neuropsychological protocol for use in geriatric populations. Rev Psiq Clin 2010;37:246-250. , 1010. Aprahamian I, Martinelli JE, Cecato J, Yassuda MS. Screening for Alzheimer’s disease among illiterate elderly: accuracy analysis for multiple instruments. J Alzheimers Dis 2011;26:221-229. , 1111. Aprahamian I, Martinelli JE, Neri AL, Yassuda MS. The accuracy of the Clock Drawing Test compared to that of standard screening tests for Alzheimer’s disease: results from a study of Brazilian elderly with heterogeneous educational backgrounds. Int Psychogeriatr 2010;22:64-71. , these findings are controversial 33. Royall DR, Mulroy AR, Chiodo LK, Polk MJ. Clock drawing is sensitive to executive control: a comparison of six methods. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1999;54:328-333. , 99. Lourenço RV, Ribeiro-Filho ST, Moreira IFH, Paradela EMP, Miranda AS. Clock Drawing Test: performance among elderly with low educational level. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 2008;30:309-315. . Concerning construct validity, for patients with very low formal education, the drawing processes may demand, besides more cognitive effort than better educated subjects, the recruitment of different cognitive abilities to improve task performance. In this sense, the greater association of executive functions with CDT performance may represent a compensatory process for drawing performance. Future studies should replicate our findings in elderly patients with higher formal education.

Our finding indicated that the CDT must be interpreted with caution in a clinical setting. For screening purposes of global cases of cognitive impairment, the test seems to be an adequate measure, with moderate correlations with other screening tests. However, in a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment, the CDT scores must be interpreted carefully, considering the influences of different specific cognitive domains on test performance.

CONCLUSION

The CDT in normal aging, MCI, and mild AD subjects was influenced by executive functions, semantic knowledge, visuospatial abilities, and general cognitive functioning. Together these components accounted for about half of the explained variance of the CDT. The results suggest that the CDT is a complex multidimensional measure, and should be interpreted along with other cognitive tests in the neuropsychological assessment.

Table 1
. Participants’ description and group comparisons of sociodemographic characteristics and neuropsychological tests.
Table 2
. Ordinal regression models assessing the influence of age, education, executive functioning, general cognitive status, visuospatial abilities and semantic knowledge on Clock Drawing Test performance.

References

  • 1
    Freedman M, Kaplan E, Delis D, Morris R. Clock Drawing: a neuropsychological analysis. New York: Oxford University Press, 1994.
  • 2
    Marcopulos BA, McLain CA. Are our norms “Normal”? A 4-year follow-up study of a biracial sample of rural elders with low education. Clin Neuropsychol 2003;17:19-33.
  • 3
    Royall DR, Mulroy AR, Chiodo LK, Polk MJ. Clock drawing is sensitive to executive control: a comparison of six methods. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1999;54:328-333.
  • 4
    Tuokko H, Hadjistavropoulos T, Rae S, O’Rourke N. A comparison of alternative approaches to the scoring of clock drawing. 2000. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2000;15:137-148.
  • 5
    Rouleau I, Salmon DP, Butters N. Longitudinal analysis of clock drawing in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Brain Cognition 1996;18:79-87.
  • 6
    Shulman KI. Clock-drawing: is the ideal cognitive screening test? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:548-561.
  • 7
    Sunderland T, Hill JL, Mellow AM, et al. Clock drawing in Alzheimer’s disease: a novel measure of dementia severity. J Am Geriatr Soc 1989;37:725-729.
  • 8
    de Paula JJ, Schlottfeldt CG, Moreira L, et al. Psychometric properties of a brief neuropsychological protocol for use in geriatric populations. Rev Psiq Clin 2010;37:246-250.
  • 9
    Lourenço RV, Ribeiro-Filho ST, Moreira IFH, Paradela EMP, Miranda AS. Clock Drawing Test: performance among elderly with low educational level. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 2008;30:309-315.
  • 10
    Aprahamian I, Martinelli JE, Cecato J, Yassuda MS. Screening for Alzheimer’s disease among illiterate elderly: accuracy analysis for multiple instruments. J Alzheimers Dis 2011;26:221-229.
  • 11
    Aprahamian I, Martinelli JE, Neri AL, Yassuda MS. The accuracy of the Clock Drawing Test compared to that of standard screening tests for Alzheimer’s disease: results from a study of Brazilian elderly with heterogeneous educational backgrounds. Int Psychogeriatr 2010;22:64-71.
  • 12
    Rothenberg KG, Piądło R, Nagaraj UD, Friedland RP. The complex picture test in dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra 2012;2:411-417.
  • 13
    Libon DJ, Malamut BL, Swenson R, Sands LP, Cloud BS. Further analyses of clock drawings among demented and non-demented older subjects. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1996;11:193-205.
  • 14
    Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bottino CMC, Damasceno BP, Brucki SMD, Anghinah R. Diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in Brazil: cognitive and functional evaluation. Recommendations of the Scientific Department of Cognitive Neurology and Aging of the Brazilian Academy of Neurology. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 2005;63:720-727.
  • 15
    Almeida OP Almeida AS. Short versions of the Geriatric Depression Scale: a study of their validity for the diagnosis of a major depressive episode according to ICD-10 and DSM-IV. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1999;14:858-865.
  • 16
    Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 2001;58:1985-1992.
  • 17
    McKhann G, Crachman D, Folstein M, Katman R, Price D, Stadlan ME. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984;34:939-944.
  • 18
    Bertolucci PHF, Brucki SMD, Campacci SR, Juliano Y. O Mini-Exame do Estado Mental em uma população geriátrica: impacto da escolaridade. Arq Neuropsiquiatr 1994;52:1-7.
  • 19
    Beato RG, Nitrini R, Formigoni AP, Caramelli P. Brazilian version of the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB). Dem Neuropsychol 2007;1:59-65.
  • 20
    Baiyewu O, Unverzagt FW, Lane KA, et al. The Stick Design test: a new measure of visuoconstructional ability. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2005;11:598-605.
  • 21
    de Paula JJ, Costa MV, Bocardi MB, et al. The Stick Design Test on the Assessment of older adults with low formal education: evidences of construct, criterion-related and ecological validity. Int Psychogeriatr 2013; epub ahead of print.
  • 22
    Malloy-Diniz LF, Bentes RC, Figueiredo PM, et al. Normalización de uma batería de tests para evaluar las habilidades de comprensión del lenguaje, fluidez verbal y denominación em niños brasileños de 7 a 10 años: resultados preliminares. Rev Neurol 2007;44:275-280.
  • 23
    Gibbons LE, Carle AC, Mackin RS, et al and for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. A composite score for executive functioning validated in Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) participants with baseline mild cognitive impairment. Brain Imaging Behav 2012;6:517-527
  • 24
    Shoyama M, Nishioka T, Okumura M, et al. Brain activity during the Clock-Drawing Test: multichannel near-infrared spectroscopy study. Appl Neuropsychol 2011;18:243-251.
  • 25
    Ino T, Asada T, Ito J, Kimura T, Fukuyama H. Parieto-frontal networks for clock drawing revealed with fMRI. Neurosci Res 2003;45:71-77.
  • 26
    Suhr J, Grace J, Allen J, Nadler J, McKenna M. Quantitative and qualitative performance of stroke versus normal elderly on six clock drawing systems. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1998;13:495-502.
  • 27
    Royall DR, Cordes JA, Polk M. CLOX: an executive clock drawing task. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;64:588-594.
  • 28
    Moretti R, Torre P, Antonello RM, Gazzato G, Bava A. Ten-point clock test: a correlation analysis with other neuropsychological tests in dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2002;17:347-353.
  • 29
    O’Rourke N, Tuokko H, Hayden S, Beattie BL. Early identification of dementia: predictive validity of the clock test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1997;12:257-267.
  • 30
    Ehreke L, Luppa M, Konig HH, Riedel-Heller SG. Is the Clock Drawing Test a screening tool for the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment? A systematic review. Int Psychogeriatr 2010;22:56-63.
  • 31
    Blair M, Kertesz A, McMonagle P, Davidson W, Bodi N. Quantitative and qualitative analyses of clock drawing in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006;12:159-165.
  • 32
    Cahn-Weiner DA, Sullivan EV, Shear PK, et al. Brain structural and cognitive correlates of clock drawing performance in Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1999;5:502-509.
  • 33
    Ueda H, Kitabayashi Y, Narumoto J, et al. Relationship between clock drawing test performance and regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s disease: a single photon emission computed tomography study. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002;56:25-29.
  • 34
    de Guise E, LeBlanc J, Gosselin N, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of the clock drawing test in patients with traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 2010;24:1568-1574.
  • 35
    Tranel D, Rudrauf D, Vianna E, Damasio H. Does the Clock Drawing Test have focal neuroanatomical correlates? Neuropsychology 2008;22:553-562.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Oct 2013

History

  • Received
    17 Sept 2012
  • Accepted
    13 May 2013
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia - ABNEURO R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices Torre Norte, 04101-000 São Paulo SP Brazil, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista.arquivos@abneuro.org