Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

A comparison between affiliative and agonistic behaviours in wild and captive Sapajus libidinosus (Spix, 1823) (Mammalia, Primates, Cebidae)

Abstract

Organisms modulate the expression of their behaviours through environmental contexts. Several studies have suggested that the frequencies of social behaviours may differ between captive and free-living primates. In the present study, we compared the social behaviours displayed by captive and free-living groups of the bearded capuchin monkey (Sapajus libidinosus), describing and analysing their social behaviours. We observed through focal animal sampling 59 animals distributed in 10 social groups, analysing 191:45 h of videos of their behaviours. Captivity reduced the frequency of agonistic, but not of affiliative behaviours. Furthermore, neither group size nor sex could explain the overall variability in affiliative behaviour. We conclude that captivity has indeed an important impact only on some aspects of social behaviour, namely, on agonistic behaviours.

Keywords.
Capuchin monkeys; Free-living; Neotropical primates; Sociality

INTRODUCTION

Primate social groups are hierarchically structured, so that the behaviour displayed by each individual reflects its social position, and hence its role within the group (Carvalho, 2008Carvalho, J.I.S. 2008. Comportamento social em Chimpanzés (Pan troglodytes): Manutenção de estatuto social e hierarquia social de dominância. Master thesis. Centro de Biologia Ambiental da Faculdade de Ciências da Universidade de Lisboa. 67p.). Given their dominance-based hierarchy, primate social groups are frequently dominated by few individuals with preferential access to resources (such as reproductive opportunities, access to food, protection from other individuals), and with greater chances of reproductive success (Fedigan, 1993Fedigan, L.M. 1993. Sex differences and intersexual relations in adult white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). International Journal of Primatology, 14(6): 853-877.; Jurmain & Nelson, 1994Jurmain, R. & Nelson, H. 1994. Introduction to physical anthropology. St. Paul, West Publishing Company. 592p.). Notwithstanding the social constraints derived from a hierarchical organisation, the variety of social interactions (agonistic, affiliative) and contexts (feeding, parental care, resting, mating) connected to social life provide opportunities for a broad variety of interactions among group members (Jolly, 1985Jolly, A. 1985. The evolution of primate behaviour. New York, Macmillan. 416p.; Casanova, 2006Casanova, C. 2006. Introdução à antropologia biológica: princípios evolutivos, genética e primatologia. Lisboa, ISCSP. 362p.; Boyd & Silk, 2009Boyd, R. & Silk, J. 2009. How humans evolved. Los Angeles, University of California. 480p.).

In general, social relations in primates are established in two ways, through agonistic (conflicts and/or threats) and affiliative behaviour (Fragaszy et al., 2004Fragaszy, D.M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fedigan, L. 2004. The complete capuchin: The biology of the genus Cebus. Athens, University of Geogia. 399p.). Aggression and affiliation are evident features of life in most primate social groups (Carpenter, 1942Carpenter, C.R. 1942. Societies of monkeys and apes. Biological Symposia, 8: 177-204.). Affiliative behaviours include positive associations, social proximity, social gathering, social play, reconciliations and sexual behaviours (Fragaszy et al., 2004Fragaszy, D.M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fedigan, L. 2004. The complete capuchin: The biology of the genus Cebus. Athens, University of Geogia. 399p.), while agonism refers to behaviours that range from threat and submission to attack and struggle (Huntingford & Chellappa, 2011Huntingford, F.A. & Chellappa, S. 2011. Agressão. In: Yamamoto, M.E. & Volpato, G.L. (Eds.). In: Comportamento animal. Natal, EDUFRN. p. 191-207.).

Affiliative behaviours represent more than 80% of the time budget for social activities in primates (Sussman et al., 2005Sussman, R.W.; Garber, P.A. & Cheverud, J.M. 2005. Importance of cooperation and affiliation in the evolution of primate sociality. American Journal Physical Anthropology, 128: 84-97.). As such, affiliative behaviours play an important role in forming social alliances between individuals and are important for maintaining complex primate social systems, reducing competition within groups, increasing the likelihood of accessing resources, relieving social tensions after aggression (Mitani & Watts, 2010Mitani, J.C. & Watts, D.P. 2001. Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Animal Behaviour, 61(5): 915-924.) and maintaining or improving social status within a group (Surbeck & Hohmann, 2011Surbeck, M. & Hohmann, G. 2011. Mothers matter! Maternal support, dominance status and mating success in male bonobos Pan paniscus. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1705): 590-598.), eventually leading to reproductive gains (Pope, 2000Pope, T.R. 2000. Reproductive success increases with degree of kinship in cooperative coalitions of female red howler monkeys Alouatta seniculus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 48(4): 253-267.; Surbeck & Hohmann, 2011Surbeck, M. & Hohmann, G. 2011. Mothers matter! Maternal support, dominance status and mating success in male bonobos Pan paniscus. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1705): 590-598.).

In turn, agonistic behaviours are relatively rare in primates, composing usually less than 1% of the time budget for social activities (Sussman et al., 2005Sussman, R.W.; Garber, P.A. & Cheverud, J.M. 2005. Importance of cooperation and affiliation in the evolution of primate sociality. American Journal Physical Anthropology, 128: 84-97.). Aggressions are generally related to group hierarchy (Fragaszy et al., 2004Fragaszy, D.M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fedigan, L. 2004. The complete capuchin: The biology of the genus Cebus. Athens, University of Geogia. 399p.; Ferreira et al., 2006Ferreira, R.G.; Izar, P. & Lee, P.C. 2006. Exchange, affiliation and protective interventions in semifree-ranging brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). American Journal of Primatology, 68(8): 765-776.), with dominant individuals frequently involved in these interactions, often as aggressors of subordinate individuals (Sapolsky, 2005Sapolsky, R.M. 2005. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science, 308(5722): 648-652. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1106477.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1106477...
). Agonism is fundamental for the formation and maintenance of groups (De Waal, 2005De Waal, F.B.M. 2005. A century of getting to know the chimpanzee. Nature, 437(7055): 56-59. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03999.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03999...
), and food competition is the main ecological factor for the increase of agonistic behaviours (Crook & Gartland, 1966Crook, J.H. & Gartland, J.S. 1966. On the evolution of primate societies. Nature, 210(5042): 1200-1203.; Eisenberg et al., 1972Eisenberg, J.F.; Muckenhirn, N.A.; Rudran, R. 1972. The relation between ecology and social structure in primates. Science, 176(4037): 863-74.; Dittus, 1977Dittus, W.P.J. 1977. The social regulation of population density and age-sex distribution in the toque monkey. Behaviour, 63(3-4): 281-322.; Janson, 1988Janson, C.H. 1988. Food competition in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): quantitative effects of group size and tree productivity. Behaviour, 105: 53-76.). Adaptively, the hierarchy would be a “regulator for a system of privileges, usually measured by observing agonistic acts between the dyads, in determining leadership, to resolve conflicts or even to avoid excessive aggression” (Jorge & Collaço, 2008Jorge, B. & Collaço, R.; 2008. Hierarquia de dominância durante a obtenção de alimento em Cebus apella e Cebus libidinosus em cativeiro. Master’s thesis. Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte (UFRN). 57p.).

On top of the hierarchical constraints on social behaviour, it has been suggested that relevant contextual factors, such as captivity versus free-living conditions, would modulate social interactions. For example, it has been shown that living in high densities (a condition usually associated to captivity) tends to increase the frequency and duration of affiliative and prosocial behaviours, a result associated with increased agonistic interactions, mainly between females (De Waal, 1989De Waal, F.B.M. 1989. The myth of a simple relation between space and aggression in captive primates. Zoo Biology, 8: 141-148.; Judge & De Waal, 1997Judge, P.G. & De Waal, F.B.M. 1997. Rhesus monkey behaviour under diverse population densities: coping with long-term crowding. Animal Behaviour, 54: 643-662.). Subsequent studies found nevertheless that crowding (higher confinement) had no effect on aggression, and a marginal effect on affiliative behaviour (only male-female grooming increased) (Judge et al., 2006Judge, P.G.; Griffaton, N.S. & Fincke, A.M. 2006. Conflict management by Hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas) during crowding: A tensionreduction strategy. American Journal of Primatology, 68(10): 993-1006. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp...
). Captive males show high standards of tolerance, considering that they can live in captivity for years without a single physical agonistic event, a situation at odds with observations in natural settings; in addition, captive alpha males can maintain their status in the group for much longer than wild alpha males, a result interpreted as a consequence of reduced agonism and increased time devoted to play and grooming interactions, possibly as a consequence of the favourable captivity conditions on the quantity and distribution of food resources (Fragaszy et al., 2004Fragaszy, D.M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fedigan, L. 2004. The complete capuchin: The biology of the genus Cebus. Athens, University of Geogia. 399p.).

These putative social behaviour differences between wild and captive primates could be related to a variety of contextual factors. The documentation of social behaviours in primates appears mostly either in studies focussing on a group in captivity or, alternatively, in studies that focus on a free-living group. More rarely we find empirical studies focussing simultaneously on multiple groups (or populations, see for example Izar et al., 2012Izar, P.; Verderane, M.P.; Peternelli-dos-Santos, L.; Mendonça-Furtado, O.; Presotto, A.; Tokuda, M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. 2012. Flexible and conservative features of social systems in tufted capuchin monkeys: Comparing the socioecology of Sapajus libidinosus and Sapajus nigritus. American Journal of Primatology, 74(4): 315-331. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20968.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20968...
). However, up to now we found no study aiming to test empirically the effect of captivity on primate social behaviour by measuring simultaneously distinct wild and distinct captive groups. While the comparison between results from (available studies focussing on) wild groups and results from (available studies focussing on) captive groups is certainly feasible, any conclusion regarding the effect of captivity on social behaviour spanning from such a systematic review of multiple studies should necessarily rely on the control of a variety of inter-studies differences (Hedges & Olkin, 2014Hedges, L.V. & Olkin, I. 2014. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Academic Press.). Some differences are difficult to handle, such as studies that use ethograms beyond comparison, while others are more amenable to statistic controls, such as studies that focus on distinct species (Adams & Collyer, 2018Adams, D.C. & Collyer, M.L. 2018. Multivariate Phylogenetic Comparative Methods: Evaluations, Comparisons, and Recommendations. Systematic Biology, 67(1): 14-31. https://doi.org/10.1093/sysbio/syx055.
https://doi.org/10.1093/sysbio/syx055...
), or that use different ecological or behavioural sampling protocols. A simpler solution, one that we follow here, is the direct and simultaneous measurement of behaviour in various social groups, under both wild and captive conditions. Directly comparing animal activity budgets across distinct environmental conditions may provide a reliable assessment on the context-specificity of primate behaviour (i.e., behavioural repertoire breadth) and on the frequencies at which different behaviours are performed (Howell & Cheyne, 2019Howell, C.P. & Cheyne, S.M. 2019. Complexities of using wild versus captive activity budget comparisons for assessing captive primate welfare. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 22(1): 78-96.).

Here we compare the expression of social behaviours in captive and free-living groups of bearded capuchin monkeys, Sapajus libidinosus (Primates, Cebidae). They are considered the most intelligent Neotropical primates, showing behavioural traditions and tool use (Ottoni, 2009Ottoni, E. 2009. Uso de ferramentas e tradições comportamentais em macacos-prego (Cebus spp.). Tese de livre docência. Departamento de Psicologia Experimental da Universidade de São Paulo. 201p.; Falótico & Ottoni, 2013Falótico, T. & Ottoni, E.B. 2013. Stone throwing as a sexual display in wild female bearded capuchin monkeys, Sapajus libidinosus. PLoS ONE, 8(11): 1-7.; Falótico et al., 2017Falótico, T.; Siqueira, J.O. & Ottoni, E.B. 2017. Digging up food: excavation stone tool use by wild capuchin monkeys. Scientific Reports, 7: 6278.; Visalberghi et al., 2017Visalberghi, E.; Di Bernardi, C.M.; Luca, A.; Fragaszy, D. & Izar, P. 2017. Female bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) use objects to solicit the sexual partner. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 131(3): 207-213.; Falótico et al., 2018Falótico, T.; Coutinho, P.H.M.; Bueno, C.Q.; Rufo, H.P. & Ottoni, E.B. 2018. Stone tool use by wild capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) at Serra das Confusões National Park, Brazil. Primates, 59(4): 385-394.). Bearded Capuchin monkeys (S. libidinosus), as well as other species of the genus, exhibit multimale/multifemale social organisation with a dominant male controlling food access while being responsible for group defence and mating (Fragaszy et al., 2004Fragaszy, D.M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fedigan, L. 2004. The complete capuchin: The biology of the genus Cebus. Athens, University of Geogia. 399p.; Izar et al., 2012Izar, P.; Verderane, M.P.; Peternelli-dos-Santos, L.; Mendonça-Furtado, O.; Presotto, A.; Tokuda, M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. 2012. Flexible and conservative features of social systems in tufted capuchin monkeys: Comparing the socioecology of Sapajus libidinosus and Sapajus nigritus. American Journal of Primatology, 74(4): 315-331. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20968.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20968...
; Verderane et al., 2013Verderane, M.P.; Izar, P.; Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. 2013. Socioecology of wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus): an analysis of social relationships among female primates that use tools in feeding. Behaviour, 150: 659-689. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003076.
https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-0000307...
). In addition, capuchin monkeys organise themselves into matrilineal systems, with the possibility of fission-fusion in periods of food shortages (Izar et al., 2012Izar, P.; Verderane, M.P.; Peternelli-dos-Santos, L.; Mendonça-Furtado, O.; Presotto, A.; Tokuda, M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. 2012. Flexible and conservative features of social systems in tufted capuchin monkeys: Comparing the socioecology of Sapajus libidinosus and Sapajus nigritus. American Journal of Primatology, 74(4): 315-331. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20968.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20968...
). They have a polygynous mating system in which the females even request copulation from dominant males, making facial expressions, specific vocalisations and postural displays (Di Bitetti & Janson, 2001Di Bitetti, M.S. & Janson, C.H. 2001. Reproductive socioecology of tufted capuchins (Cebus apella nigritus) in northeastern Argentina. International Journal of Primatology, 22: 127-142.; Visalberghi & Fragaszy, 2002Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. 2002. Do monkeys ape? Ten years later. In: Dautenhahn, K. & Nehaniv, C. (Eds.). Imitation in animals and artifacts. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press. p. 471-479.; Carosi et al., 2005Carosi, M.; Linn, G.S. & Visalberghi, E. 2005. The sexual behaviour and breeding system of tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 35: 105-149.; Falótico & Ottoni, 2013Falótico, T. & Ottoni, E.B. 2013. Stone throwing as a sexual display in wild female bearded capuchin monkeys, Sapajus libidinosus. PLoS ONE, 8(11): 1-7.). As is common among primates, grooming is a frequent affiliative behaviour (Izar, 1994Izar, P. 1994. Análise da estrutura social de um grupo de macacos-prego (Cebus apella) em condições de semi-cativeiro. Master thesis, Instituto de psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 128p.). The social structure is based on separate dominance hierarchies among males and females, and involves a series of affiliative, cooperative and agonistic behaviours (Tiddi et al., 2012Tiddi, B.; Aureli, F. & Schino, G. 2012. Grooming up the hierarchy: the exchange of grooming and rank-related benefits in a new world primate. PLoS ONE, 7: 3-8.; Scarry, 2013Scarry, C.J. 2013. Between-group contest competition among tufted capuchin monkeys, Sapajus nigritus, and the role of male resource defense. Animal Behaviour, 85: 1-9.). Their groups are behaviourally plastic in relation to the variety of environments they occupy (Ottoni & Mannu, 2001Ottoni, E. & Mannu, M. 2001. Semi-free ranging tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts. International Journal of Primatology, 22(3): 347-358.; Fragaszy et al., 2004Fragaszy, D.M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fedigan, L. 2004. The complete capuchin: The biology of the genus Cebus. Athens, University of Geogia. 399p.) and to their social structure (Izar & Ferreira, 2007Izar, P. & Ferreira, R.G. 2007. Socioecologia de macacos-prego (Cebus spp.) selvagens e provisionados: uma análise comparativa. In: Congresso Brasileiro de Primatologia, 11. Anais. Porto Alegre, Sociedade Brasileira de Primatologia. p. 323-338. (A Primatologia no Brasil, 10).).

In the present study, we tested for differences in affiliative and agonistic behaviours between captive and wild individuals. We also analysed whether group size and gender influence these putative differences between wild and captive animals. Following the rationale above, we hypothesised that captive animals should have a higher proportion of affiliative interactions, while wild animals should show a higher proportion of agonistic behaviours.

MATERIAL AND METHODS

Study sites

We compared S. libidinosus social behaviours under two main conditions: free-living and captivity. Five free-living groups were studied at Parque Nacional Serra da Capivara (Piauí State), three captive groups were studied at the Parque Zoobotânico de Teresina (Piauí State), one group in the Parque Estadual Dois Irmãos (Pernambuco State), and one group in the Fundação Jardim Zoológico de Brasilia (Distrito Federal). Given the timescale of the study, each group can be considered independent from the others, since there was no record of any type of contact between members of the different groups during the study.

The Parque Nacional Serra da Capivara is located in the municipality of São Raimundo Nonato, Piauí State, north-eastern Brazil (Appendix 1, Fig. 1). The park is a Brazilian integral protection area, with an area of 100,764.19 hectares. It is located in the south-east of Piauí within the Caatinga biome. The mean annual temperature is 28℃, with June presented coldest month, the mean temperature ranging 25℃-35℃. In the beginning of the rainy season, in October and November, the park presents warmest periods of the year, with a mean temperature varying from 31℃ to 47℃. The mean annual precipitation is 689 mm (https://www.wikiparques.org/wiki/Parque_Nacional_da_Serra_da_Capivara). All groups of monkeys, as well as other animals in the park, are provisioned with corn and bananas, three times a week, during the driest months of the year (June to October), using an artificial feeder made of concrete. Only the Pedra Furada group has contact with tourists. The other free-living groups only have contact with researchers.

Figure 1
Sampling areas for the ten groups of Sapajus libidinosus included in the study: five groups of free-living animals (Baixa Grande (BG), Jurubeba (JB), Pedra Furada (PF), Oitenta (OT) and Gato (GT)), and five groups living in captivity (Fundação Jardim Zoológico de Brasilia (FJZB), Island 1, 2 and 3 of Parque Zoobotânico de Teresina (PZT1, PZT2 and PZT3, respectively) and Parque Estadual Dois Irmãos (PEDI)).

The three groups of capuchin monkeys (Appendix 1, Fig. 1) living in the Parque Zoobotânico de Teresina (PZT; Piauí State, Brazil) are kept in distinct islands (each with approximately 120 m²), surrounded by water, but which allows the visualization of individuals from other islands. These primates were exposed to the public and are fed once a day with fruits, vegetables and leaves. Each island has a wooden house (1,5 × 1,5 meters) and perches composed of wooden trunks. There is no record of environmental enrichment activities on these islands. At Parque Estadual Dois Irmãos (PEDI; Recife, Pernambuco State, Brazil) the primates are kept in a rectangular screened enclosure, present in the primate wing, approximately 30 m² (6 m long, 4.5 m wide and 5 m high) (Appendix 1, Fig. 1), surrounded on one side by trees and on the other by enclosures for other primate species. This enclosure had a brick house (shelter made of masonry of 2 m²) to protect the animals against the weather, a shelter made of braided ropes, which allowed greater contact with the external environment, hanging ropes to allow movement throughout the enclosure and dry trunks. The recint is provisioned with fruits, vegetables, ration and leaves twice a day. The food is made available in four hanging feeders located on the sides of the enclosure. PEDI visitors can observe the park fauna four days a week, the other three days of the week, the keepers are destined to carry out environmental enrichment activities with the primates in the enclosure. The Fundação Jardim Zoológico de Brasilia (FJZB; Distrito Federal, Brazil) hold the capuchin monkeys on an island of approximately 250 m² (Appendix 1, Fig. 1) located in an artificial lake, and exposed to the public six days per week. The primates are fed twice a day with fruits, vegetables and leaves placed in a suspended metal feeder. The centre of the island is equipped with a two-level wooden platform connected to a small shack by perches composed of wooden trunks and rubber bands. A stone den is also present on the island. There is no record of environmental enrichment activities on this island. None of the zoos had detailed information about their origin, time in the zoo, and age at which they arrived at the zoo, of each individual. The only information we have is that all adult individuals were brought from CETAS, coming from trafficking or capture in the urban environment (probably raised as a domestic animal). Juveniles and sub-adults were born at the Zoo. All adult animals were more than three years old at the Zoo.

Study animals

To test the hypothesis that social behaviour changes in captivity it was not necessary to obtain a complete description of the social organisation at each group. Of course, this complete description could help to interpret the differences between groups in one same condition, but this was not the objective of the present study. Considering that our focus is on social behaviour differences across conditions, we need to have estimates of the social organisation in each group (each unit of analysis) and, accordingly, we sampled 10 groups across the two conditions (free-living vs captivity). To render social behaviour samples comparable across groups, interactions were recorded always in one same, provisioned foraging context. Also, to enhance comparability, all groups were regularly provisioned, and the researcher was always unfamiliar to the animals at the beginning of the experiment. To have an estimative of the social organisation, we selected six individuals at each group, the first seen in each site, with the exception of one group (FJZB, see below) with only five individuals, because one animal was quarantined during the sampling period. We thus observed a total of 59 individuals, adults and subadults of S. libidinosus (Appendix 1).

Infants and juveniles were excluded from the sampling in order to avoid age biases. However, when the behaviours were directed from adult/sub-adult to infant/juvenile, they were considered in the analysis. We estimated the age-group of each individual following criteria available in the literature. An individual is regarded as “infant” from birth to independence, when he stops being breastfed and carried out by the mother or other individuals in the group. An individual is regarded as “juvenile” when he is already independent, and as “subadult” when he is totally independent but does not show the characteristic body and behavioural development of adults. Finally, an individual is regarded as “adult” when he shows the characteristic body and behavioural development in the case of males and in females after they have their first preceptive period. Females reach sexual maturity when aged three, while males reach sexual maturity at four years old (Izawa, 1980Izawa, K. 1980. Social behaviour of the wild black-capped capuchin (Cebus apella). Primates, 21(4): 443-467.; Torres de Assumpção, 1983Torres de Assumpção, C. 1983. An ecological study of primates of Southeastern Brazil with a reappraisal on Cebus apella races. Doctoral thesis. Edinburgh University. 402p.; Fragaszy & Adams-Curtis, 1991Fragaszy, D.M. & Adams-Curtis, L.E. 1991. Generative aspects of manipulation in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105(4): 387-397.; Izar, 1994Izar, P. 1994. Análise da estrutura social de um grupo de macacos-prego (Cebus apella) em condições de semi-cativeiro. Master thesis, Instituto de psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 128p.).

Data collection

The free-living groups were previously habituated to human presence before the onset of the present study. During the habituation process each individual received a name. To identify each individual, we used natural marks, such as tuft size and shape, hair colour, face shape, and scars.

Sampling was conducted with an ethogram adapted from previous studies (e.g.,Falótico, 2011Falótico, T. 2011. Uso de ferramentas por macacos-prego (Sapajus libidinosus) do Parque Nacional Serra da Capivara - PI. PhD thesis. Instituto de Psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 170p.; Camargo, 2012Camargo, M.R. 2012. O efeito do uso de ferramentas no comportamento e no bem-estar de macacos-prego (Sapajus libidinosus) cativos. Master thesis. Departamento de Processos Psicológicos Básicos, Instituto de Psicologia da Universidade de Brasília. 50p.), and we performed 19:30 h of observations per group (Altmann, 1974Altmann, J. 1974. Observational study of behaviour: Sampling methods. Behaviour, 49(3-4): 227-267.). For the construction of the ethogram, observations made in all the ten groups were taken into account. The resulting ethogram consisted of two behavioural categories, agonistic and affiliative, further subdivided into 10 behavioural elements (Appendix 2). We considered agonistic behaviours those involving threats, attacks, persecutions, bites, as well as those shown in situations of competitive conflicts (seeJanson, 1985Janson, C.H. 1985. Aggressive and individual food consumption in wild brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behaviour al Ecology and Sociobiology, 18(2): 125-138.; Bernstein, 1976Bernstein, I.S. 1976. Dominance, aggression and reproduction in primate societies. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 60(2): 459-472.). Also, we considered affiliative the behaviours promoting the development and maintenance of social ties between the members of the group, for example, allogrooming and playing (seeStoesz et al., 2013Stoesz, B.M.; Hare, J.F. & Snow, W.M. 2013. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying affiliative social behaviour: insights from comparative research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 37: 123-132.).

We video-recorded the selected individuals from December/2015 to August/2016. Each group was followed for a period of 7 days, so as to sample each social organisation for one same amount of time, enhancing comparability of the estimative of the level of social interactions across groups. The three groups of Parque Zoobotânico de Teresina were observed between December/2015 and January/2016; the five groups of Parque Nacional Serra da Capivara, between May/2016 and June/2016, in the dry season; the group from Parque Estadual Dois Irmãos, in July/2016; and the group from Fundação Jardim Zoológico de Brasilia in August/2016. We filmed focal animals, between 08:00 h to 17:00 h, for 03:15 hours each individual (continuously), until fulfilling 19:30 h per group, thus comprising a total sampling effort of 191:45 h across the 10 groups.

Data analysis

Initially one of us (DSSL) decoded the video with the aid of the Software of Analysis of Animal Behaviour - PACCA (Santana et al. in development, available under request) and the ethogram of social behaviours (Appendix 2). Considering that most elements in the ethogram are events with negligible duration, the frequency of these elements summarise most of the available information, and accordingly we registered the frequency of the social behaviours (affiliative and agonistic).

The absolute frequency of behaviours corresponding to each activity (grooming, playing, threatening) was transformed into relative frequency, through Ci=100ni/N, in order to allow comparisons of the group activity budgets. In the formula, ‘n’ is the number of records of a given activity ‘I’ during the period of analysis, and ‘N’ is the total number of focal activities recorded during the same period (Cullen-Jr. & Valladares-Pádua, 1997Cullen-Jr., L. & Valladares-Pádua, C. 1997. Métodos para estudos de ecologia, manejo e conservação de primatas na natureza. In: Valladares-Pádua, C.; Bodmer, R.E. & Cullen-Jr., L. (Eds.). Manejo e conservação de vida silvestre no Brasil. Brasília, DF, CNPq-Sociedade Civil Mamirauá. p. 239-269.).

Group size and the sex of the individuals seemed to correlate with the conditions and, therefore, to ascertain whether these variables were responsible for the observed results, we assessed their possible effects by means of an ANOVA using group size and sex as a factor.

We tested for captive/free-living group differences with a linear mixed model (LMM; Henderson, 1984Henderson, C.R. 1984. Applications of linear models in animal breeding. Guelphi, University of Guelphi. 462p.). The condition (free-living versus captive) was the independent, fixed factor, and the frequency of behaviour (either affiliative or aggressive) the dependent variable. Study sites (PZT1, PZT2, PZT3, PEDI, FJZB, OT, BG, JB, GT, PF) were nested within the fixed effect ‘condition’, while individuals were nested within their respective groups. We estimated model parameters and associated statistics through Maximum Likelihood (Fisher, 1922Fisher, R.A. 1922. On the mathematical foundations of theorical statistics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 222: 309-368.), with a maximum of 1,000 iterations, using the absolute values of log-likelihood (0.00001), parameters (0.000001), and Hessian (0.00001) convergence. The estimative of confidence limits (95%) was performed with 10,000 bootstrap iterations. All analyses were run using IBM® SPSS Statistics v.20 (IBM, 2011IBM Corp. Released. 2011. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows , Version 20.0. Armonk, NY, IBM Corp.) and the software R® v.3.6.0 (R Core Team, 2019R Core Team. 2019. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org.
http://www.R-project.org...
), using the nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2019Pinheiro, J.; Bates, D.; DebRoy, S.; Sarkar, D. & R Core Team. 2019. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models . R package Version 3.1-143. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme.
https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme...
) and lme4 packages (Bates et al., 2015Bates, D.; Maechler, M.; Bolker, B. & Walker, S. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1): 1-48.).

RESULTS

We recorded a total of 696 behaviours belonging to 10 different social behaviour categories. Together, these behaviours were part of agonistic and affiliative interactions (Fig. 2). In captivity we observed a total of 367 social behaviours, 300 of which were affiliative (Females=101; Males=199) and 67 agonistic (Females=43; Males=24). In free-living animals we observed a total of 329 social behaviours, of which 173 were affiliative (Females=100; Males=73) and 156 were agonistic (Females=46; Males=110). Nine types of social behaviours were observed in captivity: five affiliative and four agonistic. Ten types of social behaviours were observed in free-living: six affiliative and four agonistic. Allogrooming was the most frequent affiliative behaviour both in captive (45%, N=165, mean=5.6 by individual, SD=9.04) and in free-living animals (43%, N=139, mean=4.6 by individual, SD=4.67) (Fig. 2). Threatening was the most frequent agonistic behaviour both in captive (10.61%, N=38, mean=1.3 by individual, SD=2.87) and in free-living animals (28.8%, N=93, mean=3.1 by individual; SD=4.07) (Fig. 2).

Figure 2
Relative frequencies of social activities presented by free-living (Grey) and captive individuals (Black): (A) Affiliative behaviours and (B) Agonistic behaviours. We observed 10 different types of social behaviour, which occurred under both captivity and free-living conditions.

Free-living groups showed a larger proportion of agonistic behaviours in their social repertoire, when compared to captive groups (Fig. 3). There is large within-condition variation between groups. For example, agonistic behaviours in the wild corresponded to 55% (N=52) of all social activities in the OT group, while in the GT group they comprised 74% (N=34) of the total. There was a strong monopoly of food resources in the GT group, resulting from the behaviour of a single adult male (Itamar) who presented agonistic behaviours whenever a subordinate approached the feeder. In captivity, the animals of PZT1 showed the highest proportion of agonistic behaviours (72%, N=41) (Fig. 3). This result may be attributed to the behaviour exhibited by a pair of individuals (Juluca and Esaú), which repeatedly threatened each other. Moreover, the alpha male (Hilton) also exhibited threatening behaviours whenever food was provisioned.

Figure 3
Relative frequency of social behaviours for each of the ten studied groups (BG=Baixa Grande; JB=Jurubeba; PF=Pedra Furada; OT=Oitenta; GT=Gato; FJZB=Fundação Jardim Zoológico de Brasília; PEDI=Parque Estadual Dois Irmãos; PZT2=Parque Zoobotânico de Teresina (ilha 2); PZT3=Parque Zoobotânico de Teresina (ilha 3); PZT1=Parque Zoobotânico de Teresina (ilha 1). Under free-living conditions, agonistic behaviours were proportionally more frequent than were affiliative behaviours.

There was no difference in affiliative behaviours between free-living and captive animals (N=59, F=2.735, p=0.103, Fig. 4A). Neither group size (N=59, F=1.223, p=0.302) nor sex (N=59, F<0.0001, p=0.989) could explain the overall variability in affiliative behaviour across conditions. Variability within conditions was not explained by group size (free-living: N=30, F=0.713, p=0.589; captivity: N=29, F=1.150, p=0.345), but free-living females exhibited more affiliative behaviour than males (N=30, F=7.710, p=0.009), a finding that did not extend to captive animals (N=29, F=2.376, p=0.134).

Figure 4
Captivity reduces agonistic interactions and does not change affiliative interactions. (A) Median and quartiles of the frequency of affiliative (A) and agonistic (B) behaviours presented by captive and free-living groups of S. libidinosus, separated by sex.

Agonistic behaviours were more frequent in free-living than captive groups (N=59, F=4.233, p=0.044, Fig. 4B). Neither group size (N=59, F=1.650, p=0.130), nor sex (N=59, F=0.067, p=0.796) could explain the overall variability in agonistic behaviour. Variability within conditions was not explained by group size (free-living: N=30, F=1.338, p=0.279; captivity: N=29, F=0.409) or by sex (free-living: N=30, F=0.179, p=0.676; captivity: N=29, F=0.351, p=0.558).

DISCUSSION

As suggested by the literature (Janson, 1985Janson, C.H. 1985. Aggressive and individual food consumption in wild brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behaviour al Ecology and Sociobiology, 18(2): 125-138.; Izar, 1994Izar, P. 1994. Análise da estrutura social de um grupo de macacos-prego (Cebus apella) em condições de semi-cativeiro. Master thesis, Instituto de psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 128p.; Pinha, 2007Pinha, P.S. 2007. Interações sociais em grupos de macacos-prego (Cebus libidinosus) no Parque Nacional de Brasília. Master thesis. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia da Universidade de Brasília. 67p.; Vogel & Janson, 2007Vogel, E.R. & Janson, C. 2007. Predicting the frequency of food-related agonism in white faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus) using a novel focal-tree method. American Journal of Primatology, 69(5): 533-550.), agonistic behaviours were indeed more frequent in free-living than in captive groups of capuchin monkeys (Fig. 4B). The finding that agonistic behaviours decreased in captivity is compatible with a survey from the literature. For example, while in captivity the individuals of Cebus apella, Sapajus nigritus and Sapajus apella spent very small percentages of their activity budget in agonistic activities, usually associated with the onset of food supply (Moro, 2007Moro, D.D. 2007. Comportamento social e hierarquia em um grupo de macacos-prego (Cebus apella) no centro de reabilitação de animais silvestres, Campo Grande, Mato Grosso do Sul. Master thesis. Departamento de Psicologia da Universidade Católica Dom Bosco, Campo Grande, MS. 76p.; Santos & Reis, 2009Santos, L.B. & Reis, N.R. 2009. Estudo comportamental de Cebus nigritus (Goldfuss, 1809) (Primates, Cebidae) em cativeiro. Semina: Ciências Biológicas e da Saúde, 30: 175-184.; Simões, 2013Simões, A.S. 2013. Interações e relações sociais de macaco-prego (Sapajus apella) em cativeiro com isolamento do indivíduo durante a alimentação: um estudo descritivo. Master’s thesis. Núcleo de teoria e pesquisa do comportamento, Universidade Federal do Pará. p. 61.), in natural conditions agonistic behaviours seemed to compose a larger bit of the activity budget throughout the day, also in connection to competition for food (Freese & Oppenheimer, 1981Freese, C.H. & Oppenheimer, J.R. 1981. The Capuchin monkeys, genus Cebus. In: Coimbra-Filho, A.F. & Mittermeier, R.A. (Eds.). Ecology and behaviour of neotropical primates. Rio de Janeiro, Academia Brasileira de Ciências. p. 331-390.; Isbell & Young, 2002Isbell, L.A. & Young, T.P. 2002. Ecological models of female social relationships in primates: similarities, disparities, and some directions for future clarity. Behaviour, 139: 177-202.; Izar, 2004Izar, P. 2004. Female social relationships of Cebus apella nigritus in southeastern Atlantic Forest: an analysis through ecological models of primate social evolution. Behaviour, 141: 71-99.; Ferreira et al., 2006Ferreira, R.G.; Izar, P. & Lee, P.C. 2006. Exchange, affiliation and protective interventions in semifree-ranging brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). American Journal of Primatology, 68(8): 765-776.; Ferreira et al., 2008Ferreira, R.G.; Lee, P.C. & Izar, P. 2008. Food competition in a semi-free-ranging Cebus apella group. Folia Primatologica, 79(6): 463-475. https://doi.org/10.1159/000158525.
https://doi.org/10.1159/000158525...
; Jaman & Huffman, 2013Jaman, M.F. & Huffman, M.A. 2013. The effect of urban and rural habitats and resource type on activity budgets of commensal rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) in Bangladesh. Primates, 54: 49-59. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-012-0330-6.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-012-0330-...
; Orihuela et al., 2014Orihuela, G.; Terborgh, J.; Ceballos, N. & Glander, K. 2014. When Top-Down Becomes Bottom Up: Behaviour of Hyperdense Howler Monkeys (Alouatta seniculus) Trapped on a 0.6 Ha Island. PLoS ONE, 9(4): e82197. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0082197.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.008...
). The most frequent agonistic behaviour was aggressive threatening. Primates are proficient in resolving conflicts with threats in place of physical fights (van Staaden et al., 2011van Staaden, M.J.; Searcy, W.A. & Hanlon, R.T. 2011. Signaling aggression. In: Aggression. Huber, R.; Bannasch, D.L.; Brennan, P. & Frishman, K. (Eds.). San Diego, CA, Elsevier/Academic Press. p. 23-49.), as aggressions are costlier and bring the risk of injuries (Strier, 2011Strier, K.B. 2011. Primate behavioural ecology. New Jersey: Prentice Hall. 432p.).

Contrary to the sugested idea in the literature (Beisner & Isbell, 2008Beisner, B.A. & Isbell, L. 2008. Ground substrate affects activity budgets and hair loss in outdoor captive groups of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of Primatology, 70(12): 1160-1168.; Lessa, 2009Lessa, M.A.M. 2009. Bem-estar em cativeiro: Análise e planejamento da ocupação do tempo em macacos-prego (Cebus apella). Master thesis. Núcleo de teoria e pesquisa do comportamento da Universidade Federal do Pará. 75p.; Jaman & Huffman, 2013Jaman, M.F. & Huffman, M.A. 2013. The effect of urban and rural habitats and resource type on activity budgets of commensal rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) in Bangladesh. Primates, 54: 49-59. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-012-0330-6.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-012-0330-...
; Simões, 2013Simões, A.S. 2013. Interações e relações sociais de macaco-prego (Sapajus apella) em cativeiro com isolamento do indivíduo durante a alimentação: um estudo descritivo. Master’s thesis. Núcleo de teoria e pesquisa do comportamento, Universidade Federal do Pará. p. 61.), we found no effect of captivity on affiliative behaviours in primate groups (Fig. 4A). This discrepancy may have arisen because previous reviews did not conduct systematic comparisons between these environmental conditions, and because no previous study tested empirically the hypothesis that captivity affects social behaviour using the same behavioural metrics across conditions in a paired design, so as to directly collect social data from all groups in one single study. Rather, usually each independent study investigated a single environmental condition (e.g., free-living but not captive groups). For example, while Sussman et al. (2005Sussman, R.W.; Garber, P.A. & Cheverud, J.M. 2005. Importance of cooperation and affiliation in the evolution of primate sociality. American Journal Physical Anthropology, 128: 84-97.), in their study of 28 genus and 60 primate species, showed that primates living under natural conditions spent less than ten percent of their daily time in affiliative behaviours, others studies showed that captive animals spent most of their daily time in affiliative behaviours (Moro, 2007Moro, D.D. 2007. Comportamento social e hierarquia em um grupo de macacos-prego (Cebus apella) no centro de reabilitação de animais silvestres, Campo Grande, Mato Grosso do Sul. Master thesis. Departamento de Psicologia da Universidade Católica Dom Bosco, Campo Grande, MS. 76p.; Santos & Reis, 2009Santos, L.B. & Reis, N.R. 2009. Estudo comportamental de Cebus nigritus (Goldfuss, 1809) (Primates, Cebidae) em cativeiro. Semina: Ciências Biológicas e da Saúde, 30: 175-184.; Simões, 2013Simões, A.S. 2013. Interações e relações sociais de macaco-prego (Sapajus apella) em cativeiro com isolamento do indivíduo durante a alimentação: um estudo descritivo. Master’s thesis. Núcleo de teoria e pesquisa do comportamento, Universidade Federal do Pará. p. 61.), and the comparison of these independent studies could prompt one to the conclusion that affiliative behaviour increases in captivity. Nevertheless, strictly, this conclusion does not follow from this superficial comparison of results. Different studies use different ethograms, different observers, different sampling methods, different species, at different seasons, years or decades, and a true systematic comparison would have to statistically control at least the major differences across these studies to provide a valid conclusion.

Grooming was the most frequent affiliative behaviour across conditions (Fig. 2), corroborating previous reports (seeSussman et al., 2005Sussman, R.W.; Garber, P.A. & Cheverud, J.M. 2005. Importance of cooperation and affiliation in the evolution of primate sociality. American Journal Physical Anthropology, 128: 84-97.). Grooming seems to serve many functions. Monkeys use grooming to form coalitions (Rose, 1998Rose, L.M. 1998. Behavioral ecology of white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) in Costa Rica. PhD. Thesis from Department of Anthropology, Washington University, St. Louis. MO USA. 357p.; Tiddi et al., 2011Tiddi, B.; Aureli, F.; Polizzi di Sorrentino, E.; Janson, C.H. & Schino, G. 2011. Grooming for tolerance? Two mechanisms of exchange in wild tufted capuchin monkeys. Behavioral Ecology, 22(3): 663-669. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr028.
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr028...
), in addition to serving as an affiliate strategy for the purpose of relieving agonistic events (Tiddi et al., 2011Tiddi, B.; Aureli, F.; Polizzi di Sorrentino, E.; Janson, C.H. & Schino, G. 2011. Grooming for tolerance? Two mechanisms of exchange in wild tufted capuchin monkeys. Behavioral Ecology, 22(3): 663-669. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr028.
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr028...
), it also serves to establish and strengthen affective bonds (Sparks, 1967Sparks, J. 1967. Allogrooming in primates: A review. In: Primate Ethology. Morris, D. (Ed.). Weinfeld and Nicholson, London.; Seyfarth, 1977Seyfarth, R.M. 1977. A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. Journal of Theorical Biology, 65(4): 671-698.; Gillam, 2011Gillam, E. 2011. An introduction to animal communication. Nature Education Knowledge, 3(10): 70.), in addition to promoting social cohesion (Yamamoto, 1991Yamamoto, M.E. 1991. Comportamento social do gênero Callithrix em cativeiro. In: Congresso Brasileiro de Primatologia, 3º. Anais. Belo Horizonte, MG, Fundação Biodiversitas. p. 63-72. (Primatologia no Brasil, 3).). Such functional diversity in a single behavioural modality could partially explain why grooming was carried out at comparable levels under both captive and free-living conditions: some functions could be relevant in captivity, while others could be relevant for free-living groups. Considering that aggressiveness was more frequent in the wild, it could be the case that grooming would mostly play the function of reconciliation in the wild, while being used mostly for cleaning or for coping with chronic stress in captivity. (Lima, 2011Lima, C.C.A. 2011. Grooming, estereotipia e dinâmica social de um grupo cativo de macacos-prego-do-peito-amarelo, Cebus xanthosternos (Weid-Neuwied 1826) (Primates: Cebidae) do Parque Zoobotânico de Salvador. Master thesis. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia, Universidade Federal da Bahia. 79p.; Sonnweber et al., 2015Sonnweber, R.S.; Ravignani, A.; Stobbe, N.; Schiestl, G.; Wallner, B. & Fitch, W.T. 2015. Rank dependent grooming patterns and cortisol alleviation in Barbary macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 77(6): 688-700. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22391.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22391...
; Wooddell et al., 2017Wooddell, L.J.; Hamel, A.F.; Murphy, A.M.; Byers, K.L.; Kaburu, S.S.K.; Meyer, J.S.; Suomi, S.J. & Dettmer, A.M. 2017. Relationships between affiliative social behavior and hair cortisol concentrations in semi‐free ranging rhesus monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 84: 109-115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.06.018.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017....
; Yamanashi et al., 2018Yamanashi, Y.; Teramoto, M.; Morimura, N.; Nogami, E. & Hirata, S. 2018. Social relationship and hair cortisol level in captive male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Primates, 59(2): 145-152. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-017-0641-8.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-017-0641-...
).

Sex did not explain the overall variability in affiliative behaviour. Sex is considered a factor with a known influence on social behaviour in primates (Irwin & Ehardt, 1985Irwin, S. & Ehardt, C.L. 1985. Agonistic Aiding: Kinship, Rank, Age, and Sex Influences. American Journal of Primatology, 8(1): 37-52.; De Waal & Luttrell, 1986De Waal, F.B.M. & Luttrell, L.M. 1986. The similarity principle underlying social bonding among female rhesus monkeys. Folia Primatologica, 46(4): 215-234. https://doi.org/10.1159/000156255.
https://doi.org/10.1159/000156255...
). Despite this, free-living females were more affiliative than free-living males (Fig. 4). In relation to sex, females tend to remain more in proximity (Perry, 1996Perry, S. 1996. Female-female social relationships in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus. American Journal of Primatology, 40(2): 167-182. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1996)40:2<167::AID-AJP4>3.0.CO;2-W.
https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(...
) and do more picking among themselves (O’Brien, 1991O’Brien, T.G. 1991. Female-male social interactions in wedge-capped capuchin monkeys: benefits and costs of group living. Animal Behaviour, 41(4): 555-567. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80896-6.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80...
; Perry, 1996Perry, S. 1996. Female-female social relationships in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus. American Journal of Primatology, 40(2): 167-182. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1996)40:2<167::AID-AJP4>3.0.CO;2-W.
https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(...
, Di Bitetti, 1997Di Bitetti, M.S. 1997. Evidence for an important social role of allogrooming in a platyrrhine primate. Animal Behaviour, 54(1): 199-211. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0416.
https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0416...
). In relationships between males and females, in affiliation behaviours males are more responsible for maintaining dyadic proximity (Perry, 1997Perry, S. 1997. Male-female social relationships in wild white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Behaviour, 134: 477-510.), while females generally do more picking, especially in the alpha male (Tiddi et al., 2012Tiddi, B.; Aureli, F. & Schino, G. 2012. Grooming up the hierarchy: the exchange of grooming and rank-related benefits in a new world primate. PLoS ONE, 7: 3-8.).

Although we cannot point to specific differences across free-living and captive conditions that could be causally connected to these results, we could statistically rule out group size and sex as potential factors. Still, a variety of other factors could potentially explain these results. Primates modulate their behaviour in response to changes in food resources (Southwick, 1967Southwick, C.H. 1967. An experimental study of intragroup agonistic behavior in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behaviour, 28: 181-209.; Janson, 1985Janson, C.H. 1985. Aggressive and individual food consumption in wild brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behaviour al Ecology and Sociobiology, 18(2): 125-138.; Izar, 1994Izar, P. 1994. Análise da estrutura social de um grupo de macacos-prego (Cebus apella) em condições de semi-cativeiro. Master thesis, Instituto de psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 128p.; Phillips, 1995Phillips, K.A. 1995. Foraging-related agonism in capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Folia Primatologica, 65(3): 159-162.; Casanova, 2006Casanova, C. 2006. Introdução à antropologia biológica: princípios evolutivos, genética e primatologia. Lisboa, ISCSP. 362p.; Pinha, 2007Pinha, P.S. 2007. Interações sociais em grupos de macacos-prego (Cebus libidinosus) no Parque Nacional de Brasília. Master thesis. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia da Universidade de Brasília. 67p.; Vogel & Janson, 2007Vogel, E.R. & Janson, C. 2007. Predicting the frequency of food-related agonism in white faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus) using a novel focal-tree method. American Journal of Primatology, 69(5): 533-550.; Ferreira et al., 2008Ferreira, R.G.; Lee, P.C. & Izar, P. 2008. Food competition in a semi-free-ranging Cebus apella group. Folia Primatologica, 79(6): 463-475. https://doi.org/10.1159/000158525.
https://doi.org/10.1159/000158525...
; Beisner & Isbell, 2008Beisner, B.A. & Isbell, L. 2008. Ground substrate affects activity budgets and hair loss in outdoor captive groups of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of Primatology, 70(12): 1160-1168.; Lessa, 2009Lessa, M.A.M. 2009. Bem-estar em cativeiro: Análise e planejamento da ocupação do tempo em macacos-prego (Cebus apella). Master thesis. Núcleo de teoria e pesquisa do comportamento da Universidade Federal do Pará. 75p.), space availability, structural complexity and stimulus diversity (Morgan & Tromborg, 2007Morgan, K.N. & Tromborg, C.T. 2007. Sources of stress in captivity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 102(3-4): 262-302.), the presence of visitors (Hosey, 2005Hosey, G.R. 2005. How does the zoo environment affect the behaviour of captive primates? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 90(2): 107-129.), or even to changes in the actual possibility of control over daily routines (e.g., patrolling large distances may be sometimes impossible), or in the actual possibility of choosing social partners (Machairas et al., 2003Machairas, I.; Camperio, C.A. & Sgardelis, S. 2003. Interpopulation differences in activity patterns of Macaca sylvanus in Maroccan Middle Atlas. Human Evolution, 18(3-4): 185-202.; Morgan & Tromborg, 2007Morgan, K.N. & Tromborg, C.T. 2007. Sources of stress in captivity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 102(3-4): 262-302.; Scott & Lockard, 2007Scott, J. & Lockard, J.S. 2007. Competition coalitions and conflict interventions among captive female gorillas. International Journal Primatology, 28(4): 761-781.). Overall, these factors could make captivity a potentially less stimulating context for primates (Simões, 2013Simões, A.S. 2013. Interações e relações sociais de macaco-prego (Sapajus apella) em cativeiro com isolamento do indivíduo durante a alimentação: um estudo descritivo. Master’s thesis. Núcleo de teoria e pesquisa do comportamento, Universidade Federal do Pará. p. 61.).

From the many factors that could explain the relatively higher proportion of agonistic interactions in the wild (Fig. 3), relative food scarcity during the dry season in tropical dry forests (Caatinga), associated with an insufficient, partial food supply provided for free-living groups, could possibly be the most important. Food supply strongly affects primate social behaviour (Carpenter & MacMillen, 1976Carpenter, F.L. & MacMillen, R.E. 1976. Threshold model of feeding territoriality and test with a Hawaiian Honeycreeper. Science, Washington, 194(4265): 639-642.; Wolf, 1978Wolf, L.L. 1978. Aggressive social organization in nectarivorous birds. American Zoologist, 18(4): 765-778.; Janson, 1985Janson, C.H. 1985. Aggressive and individual food consumption in wild brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behaviour al Ecology and Sociobiology, 18(2): 125-138.; Izar, 1994Izar, P. 1994. Análise da estrutura social de um grupo de macacos-prego (Cebus apella) em condições de semi-cativeiro. Master thesis, Instituto de psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 128p.; Phillips, 1995Phillips, K.A. 1995. Foraging-related agonism in capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Folia Primatologica, 65(3): 159-162.; Pinha, 2007Pinha, P.S. 2007. Interações sociais em grupos de macacos-prego (Cebus libidinosus) no Parque Nacional de Brasília. Master thesis. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia da Universidade de Brasília. 67p.; Vogel & Janson, 2007Vogel, E.R. & Janson, C. 2007. Predicting the frequency of food-related agonism in white faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus) using a novel focal-tree method. American Journal of Primatology, 69(5): 533-550.; Ferreira et al., 2008Ferreira, R.G.; Lee, P.C. & Izar, P. 2008. Food competition in a semi-free-ranging Cebus apella group. Folia Primatologica, 79(6): 463-475. https://doi.org/10.1159/000158525.
https://doi.org/10.1159/000158525...
; Izar et al., 2012Izar, P.; Verderane, M.P.; Peternelli-dos-Santos, L.; Mendonça-Furtado, O.; Presotto, A.; Tokuda, M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. 2012. Flexible and conservative features of social systems in tufted capuchin monkeys: Comparing the socioecology of Sapajus libidinosus and Sapajus nigritus. American Journal of Primatology, 74(4): 315-331. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20968.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20968...
), and considering that food monopolisation was facilitated by provisioning in the feeders, the monopolisation of a scarce food resource could potentially be at the root of increase agonistic interactions (Janson, 1985Janson, C.H. 1985. Aggressive and individual food consumption in wild brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behaviour al Ecology and Sociobiology, 18(2): 125-138.; Boccia et al., 1988Boccia, M.L.; Laudenslager, M. & Reite, M. 1988. Food distribution, dominance, and aggressive behaviors in Bonnet macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 16(2): 123-130.; Isbell, 1991Isbell, L.A. 1991. Contest and scramble competition: patterns of female aggression and ranging behavior among primates. Behavioral Ecology, 2(2): 143-155. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/2.2.143.
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/2.2.143...
; Sterck & Steenbeek, 1997Sterck, E.H.M. & Steenbeek, R. 1997. Female dominance relationships and food competition in the sympatric Thomas langur and long-tailed macaque. Behaviour, 134: 749-774.; Pellegrini et al., 2007Pellegrini, A.D.; Roseth, C.J.; Mliner, S.; Bohn, C.M.; Van Ryzin, M.; Vance, N.; Cheatham, C.L. & Tarullo, A. 2007. Social dominance in preschool classrooms. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121: 54-64. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.121.1.54.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.121.1....
; Pellegrini, 2008Pellegrini, A.D. 2008. The roles of aggressive and affiliative behaviors in resource control: a behavioral ecological perspective. Developmental Review, 28(4): 461-487. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2008.03.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2008.03.001...
; Huntingford & Chellappa, 2011Huntingford, F.A. & Chellappa, S. 2011. Agressão. In: Yamamoto, M.E. & Volpato, G.L. (Eds.). In: Comportamento animal. Natal, EDUFRN. p. 191-207.; Back et al., 2019Back, J.P.; Suzin, A. & Aguiar, L.M. 2019. Activity budget and social behaviour of urban capuchin monkeys, Sapajus sp. (Primates: Cebidae). Zoologia, 36: 1-10. https://doi.org/10.3897/zoologia.36.e30845.
https://doi.org/10.3897/zoologia.36.e308...
; Overduin-de-Vries et al., 2020Overduin-de-Vries, A.M.; de Vries, H.; Vermande, M.M.; Reijntjes, A.H.A. & Sterck, E.H.M. 2020. Both aggressive and affiliative behaviour facilitate resource access in high-ranking female long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behaviour, 157(3-4): 267-287. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-bja10001.
https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-bja1000...
).

On the other hand, it is worth considering that, in captivity, due to the limited space (including the height of the enclosure in the PEDI), individuals tend to “avoid conflict”, and thus captive individuals tend to become more tolerant. Primates living in captive conditions increase the rate at which they perform conflict-reducing responses in order to minimize the amount of aggression, increasing, for example, grooming to calm others and increasing friendly affiliate behavior to reduce social tension (De Waal, 1989De Waal, F.B.M. 1989. The myth of a simple relation between space and aggression in captive primates. Zoo Biology, 8: 141-148.; Judge & De Waal, 1997Judge, P.G. & De Waal, F.B.M. 1997. Rhesus monkey behaviour under diverse population densities: coping with long-term crowding. Animal Behaviour, 54: 643-662.; Mitani & Waats, 2001Mitani, J.C. & Watts, D.P. 2001. Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Animal Behaviour, 61(5): 915-924.; Judge et al., 2006Judge, P.G.; Griffaton, N.S. & Fincke, A.M. 2006. Conflict management by Hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas) during crowding: A tensionreduction strategy. American Journal of Primatology, 68(10): 993-1006. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp...
). Fragaszy et al. (2004Fragaszy, D.M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fedigan, L. 2004. The complete capuchin: The biology of the genus Cebus. Athens, University of Geogia. 399p.) found differences between captive males, indicating high standards of tolerance and suggesting that, when in a group, they can live in captivity for years and not even have a single physical agonistic event.

Considering that S. libidinosus has an ample geographic distribution (Rylands et al., 2013Rylands, A.B.; Mittermeier, R.A.; Bezerra, B.M.; Paim, F.P.; Helder, L. & Queiroz, H.L. 2013. Family Cebidae (Squirrel Monkeys and Capuchins). In: Mittermeier, R.; Rylands, A.B. & Wilson, D.E. Handbook of the Mammals of the World. Barcelona, Lynx. vol. 3, 952p.), the present results could be extended in generality with the addition of wild groups from other geographic areas, particularly populations from other biomes, such as Atlantic forest and Cerrado (Savannah). Also, a more through sampling of groups, and of social behaviour within each group, would help to tease apart the multiple factors that could explain the resulting pattern, while also allowing to pinpoint factors that are actually driving the variability within each condition.

CONCLUSION

Captivity had indeed an impact on social behaviours. As expected, it entailed a reduction in agonistic interactions in S. libidinosus. Nevertheless, and contrarily to prevailing interpretations based on previous studies, captivity did not affect affiliative interactions. We thus found no evidence for the hypothesis that captivity leads to an increase in affiliative behaviours. Future studies should focus on the specific mechanisms underlying the findings reported here. Although group size and sex did not explain social behaviour variability across conditions, we found mixed evidence concerning the interaction between sex and condition, and so experiments designed specifically to disentangle and test these and other putative factors would clarify the details of these interactions.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We also thank FAPESB - Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado da Bahia (for granting a master’s scholarship, grant n. 8171/2015), and the Parque Nacional Serra da Capivara, Parque Zoobotânico de Teresina, Parque Estadual Dois Irmãos and Fundação Jardim Zoológico de Brasília for the partnership and support given.

REFERENCES

  • Adams, D.C. & Collyer, M.L. 2018. Multivariate Phylogenetic Comparative Methods: Evaluations, Comparisons, and Recommendations. Systematic Biology, 67(1): 14-31. https://doi.org/10.1093/sysbio/syx055
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/sysbio/syx055
  • Almeida, M.F.R. 2012. Comportamento social em Gorilas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla): o padrão de actividades diárias e as preferências espaciais Master thesis. Instituto de Ciências Sociais e Politicas da Universidade técnica de Lisboa. 100p.
  • Altmann, J. 1974. Observational study of behaviour: Sampling methods. Behaviour, 49(3-4): 227-267.
  • Back, J.P.; Suzin, A. & Aguiar, L.M. 2019. Activity budget and social behaviour of urban capuchin monkeys, Sapajus sp. (Primates: Cebidae). Zoologia, 36: 1-10. https://doi.org/10.3897/zoologia.36.e30845
    » https://doi.org/10.3897/zoologia.36.e30845
  • Bates, D.; Maechler, M.; Bolker, B. & Walker, S. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1): 1-48.
  • Beisner, B.A. & Isbell, L. 2008. Ground substrate affects activity budgets and hair loss in outdoor captive groups of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). American Journal of Primatology, 70(12): 1160-1168.
  • Bernstein, I.S. 1976. Dominance, aggression and reproduction in primate societies. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 60(2): 459-472.
  • Boccia, M.L.; Laudenslager, M. & Reite, M. 1988. Food distribution, dominance, and aggressive behaviors in Bonnet macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 16(2): 123-130.
  • Boyd, R. & Silk, J. 2009. How humans evolved Los Angeles, University of California. 480p.
  • Camargo, M.R. 2012. O efeito do uso de ferramentas no comportamento e no bem-estar de macacos-prego (Sapajus libidinosus) cativos Master thesis. Departamento de Processos Psicológicos Básicos, Instituto de Psicologia da Universidade de Brasília. 50p.
  • Carosi, M.; Linn, G.S. & Visalberghi, E. 2005. The sexual behaviour and breeding system of tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 35: 105-149.
  • Carpenter, C.R. 1942. Societies of monkeys and apes. Biological Symposia, 8: 177-204.
  • Carpenter, F.L. & MacMillen, R.E. 1976. Threshold model of feeding territoriality and test with a Hawaiian Honeycreeper. Science, Washington, 194(4265): 639-642.
  • Carvalho, J.I.S. 2008. Comportamento social em Chimpanzés (Pan troglodytes): Manutenção de estatuto social e hierarquia social de dominância Master thesis. Centro de Biologia Ambiental da Faculdade de Ciências da Universidade de Lisboa. 67p.
  • Casanova, C. 2006. Introdução à antropologia biológica: princípios evolutivos, genética e primatologia. Lisboa, ISCSP. 362p.
  • Crook, J.H. & Gartland, J.S. 1966. On the evolution of primate societies. Nature, 210(5042): 1200-1203.
  • Cullen-Jr., L. & Valladares-Pádua, C. 1997. Métodos para estudos de ecologia, manejo e conservação de primatas na natureza. In: Valladares-Pádua, C.; Bodmer, R.E. & Cullen-Jr., L. (Eds.). Manejo e conservação de vida silvestre no Brasil Brasília, DF, CNPq-Sociedade Civil Mamirauá. p. 239-269.
  • Cutrim, F.H.R. 2013. Padrão comportamental e uso de ferramentas em macacos-prego (Sapajus libidinosus) residentes em manguezal. PhD thesis. Instituto de Psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 116p.
  • De Waal, F.B.M. 1989. The myth of a simple relation between space and aggression in captive primates. Zoo Biology, 8: 141-148.
  • De Waal, F.B.M. 2005. A century of getting to know the chimpanzee. Nature, 437(7055): 56-59. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03999
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03999
  • De Waal, F.B.M. & Luttrell, L.M. 1986. The similarity principle underlying social bonding among female rhesus monkeys. Folia Primatologica, 46(4): 215-234. https://doi.org/10.1159/000156255
    » https://doi.org/10.1159/000156255
  • Di Bitetti, M.S. 1997. Evidence for an important social role of allogrooming in a platyrrhine primate. Animal Behaviour, 54(1): 199-211. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0416
    » https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0416
  • Di Bitetti, M.S. & Janson, C.H. 2001. Reproductive socioecology of tufted capuchins (Cebus apella nigritus) in northeastern Argentina. International Journal of Primatology, 22: 127-142.
  • Dittus, W.P.J. 1977. The social regulation of population density and age-sex distribution in the toque monkey. Behaviour, 63(3-4): 281-322.
  • Eisenberg, J.F.; Muckenhirn, N.A.; Rudran, R. 1972. The relation between ecology and social structure in primates. Science, 176(4037): 863-74.
  • Falótico, T. 2011. Uso de ferramentas por macacos-prego (Sapajus libidinosus) do Parque Nacional Serra da Capivara - PI. PhD thesis. Instituto de Psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 170p.
  • Falótico, T. & Ottoni, E.B. 2013. Stone throwing as a sexual display in wild female bearded capuchin monkeys, Sapajus libidinosus PLoS ONE, 8(11): 1-7.
  • Falótico, T.; Coutinho, P.H.M.; Bueno, C.Q.; Rufo, H.P. & Ottoni, E.B. 2018. Stone tool use by wild capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) at Serra das Confusões National Park, Brazil. Primates, 59(4): 385-394.
  • Falótico, T.; Siqueira, J.O. & Ottoni, E.B. 2017. Digging up food: excavation stone tool use by wild capuchin monkeys. Scientific Reports, 7: 6278.
  • Fedigan, L.M. 1993. Sex differences and intersexual relations in adult white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). International Journal of Primatology, 14(6): 853-877.
  • Ferreira, R.G.; Izar, P. & Lee, P.C. 2006. Exchange, affiliation and protective interventions in semifree-ranging brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). American Journal of Primatology, 68(8): 765-776.
  • Ferreira, R.G.; Lee, P.C. & Izar, P. 2008. Food competition in a semi-free-ranging Cebus apella group. Folia Primatologica, 79(6): 463-475. https://doi.org/10.1159/000158525
    » https://doi.org/10.1159/000158525
  • Fisher, R.A. 1922. On the mathematical foundations of theorical statistics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 222: 309-368.
  • Fragaszy, D.M. & Adams-Curtis, L.E. 1991. Generative aspects of manipulation in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105(4): 387-397.
  • Fragaszy, D.M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fedigan, L. 2004. The complete capuchin: The biology of the genus Cebus Athens, University of Geogia. 399p.
  • Freese, C.H. & Oppenheimer, J.R. 1981. The Capuchin monkeys, genus Cebus. In: Coimbra-Filho, A.F. & Mittermeier, R.A. (Eds.). Ecology and behaviour of neotropical primates Rio de Janeiro, Academia Brasileira de Ciências. p. 331-390.
  • Gillam, E. 2011. An introduction to animal communication. Nature Education Knowledge, 3(10): 70.
  • Gouveia, P.S. 2009. Padrão de atividades, dieta e uso do espaço de um grupo de Cebus xanthosternos (Wied-Neuwied, 1820) (Primates , Cebidae), na Reserva Biológica de Una, Bahia, Brasil. Dissertação (Mestrado em Zoologia). Ilhéus, Universidade Estadual de Santa Cruz. 138p.
  • Hedges, L.V. & Olkin, I. 2014. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Academic Press.
  • Henderson, C.R. 1984. Applications of linear models in animal breeding Guelphi, University of Guelphi. 462p.
  • Hosey, G.R. 2005. How does the zoo environment affect the behaviour of captive primates? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 90(2): 107-129.
  • Howell, C.P. & Cheyne, S.M. 2019. Complexities of using wild versus captive activity budget comparisons for assessing captive primate welfare. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 22(1): 78-96.
  • Huntingford, F.A. & Chellappa, S. 2011. Agressão. In: Yamamoto, M.E. & Volpato, G.L. (Eds.). In: Comportamento animal Natal, EDUFRN. p. 191-207.
  • IBM Corp. Released. 2011. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows , Version 20.0 Armonk, NY, IBM Corp.
  • Irwin, S. & Ehardt, C.L. 1985. Agonistic Aiding: Kinship, Rank, Age, and Sex Influences. American Journal of Primatology, 8(1): 37-52.
  • Isbell, L.A. 1991. Contest and scramble competition: patterns of female aggression and ranging behavior among primates. Behavioral Ecology, 2(2): 143-155. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/2.2.143
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/2.2.143
  • Isbell, L.A. & Young, T.P. 2002. Ecological models of female social relationships in primates: similarities, disparities, and some directions for future clarity. Behaviour, 139: 177-202.
  • Izar, P. 1994. Análise da estrutura social de um grupo de macacos-prego (Cebus apella) em condições de semi-cativeiro. Master thesis, Instituto de psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 128p.
  • Izar, P. 2004. Female social relationships of Cebus apella nigritus in southeastern Atlantic Forest: an analysis through ecological models of primate social evolution. Behaviour, 141: 71-99.
  • Izar, P. & Ferreira, R.G. 2007. Socioecologia de macacos-prego (Cebus spp.) selvagens e provisionados: uma análise comparativa. In: Congresso Brasileiro de Primatologia, 11. Anais Porto Alegre, Sociedade Brasileira de Primatologia. p. 323-338. (A Primatologia no Brasil, 10).
  • Izar, P.; Verderane, M.P.; Peternelli-dos-Santos, L.; Mendonça-Furtado, O.; Presotto, A.; Tokuda, M.; Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. 2012. Flexible and conservative features of social systems in tufted capuchin monkeys: Comparing the socioecology of Sapajus libidinosus and Sapajus nigritus American Journal of Primatology, 74(4): 315-331. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20968
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20968
  • Izawa, K. 1980. Social behaviour of the wild black-capped capuchin (Cebus apella). Primates, 21(4): 443-467.
  • Jaman, M.F. & Huffman, M.A. 2013. The effect of urban and rural habitats and resource type on activity budgets of commensal rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) in Bangladesh. Primates, 54: 49-59. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-012-0330-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-012-0330-6
  • Janson, C.H. 1985. Aggressive and individual food consumption in wild brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behaviour al Ecology and Sociobiology, 18(2): 125-138.
  • Janson, C.H. 1988. Food competition in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): quantitative effects of group size and tree productivity. Behaviour, 105: 53-76.
  • Jolly, A. 1985. The evolution of primate behaviour. New York, Macmillan. 416p.
  • Jorge, B. & Collaço, R.; 2008. Hierarquia de dominância durante a obtenção de alimento em Cebus apella e Cebus libidinosus em cativeiro Master’s thesis. Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte (UFRN). 57p.
  • Judge, P.G. & De Waal, F.B.M. 1997. Rhesus monkey behaviour under diverse population densities: coping with long-term crowding. Animal Behaviour, 54: 643-662.
  • Judge, P.G.; Griffaton, N.S. & Fincke, A.M. 2006. Conflict management by Hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas hamadryas) during crowding: A tensionreduction strategy. American Journal of Primatology, 68(10): 993-1006. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp
  • Jurmain, R. & Nelson, H. 1994. Introduction to physical anthropology St. Paul, West Publishing Company. 592p.
  • Lessa, M.A.M. 2009. Bem-estar em cativeiro: Análise e planejamento da ocupação do tempo em macacos-prego (Cebus apella). Master thesis. Núcleo de teoria e pesquisa do comportamento da Universidade Federal do Pará. 75p.
  • Lima, C.C.A. 2011. Grooming, estereotipia e dinâmica social de um grupo cativo de macacos-prego-do-peito-amarelo, Cebus xanthosternos (Weid-Neuwied 1826) (Primates: Cebidae) do Parque Zoobotânico de Salvador. Master thesis. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia, Universidade Federal da Bahia. 79p.
  • Machairas, I.; Camperio, C.A. & Sgardelis, S. 2003. Interpopulation differences in activity patterns of Macaca sylvanus in Maroccan Middle Atlas. Human Evolution, 18(3-4): 185-202.
  • Martins, W.P. 2010. Densidade Populacional e Ecologia de um grupo macaco-prego-de-crista (Cebus robustus; Kuhl, 1820) na Reserva Natural Vale. Tese de doutorado. Instituto de Ciências Biológicas da Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais. 118p.
  • Mitani, J.C. & Watts, D.P. 2001. Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Animal Behaviour, 61(5): 915-924.
  • Morgan, K.N. & Tromborg, C.T. 2007. Sources of stress in captivity. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 102(3-4): 262-302.
  • Moro, D.D. 2007. Comportamento social e hierarquia em um grupo de macacos-prego (Cebus apella) no centro de reabilitação de animais silvestres, Campo Grande, Mato Grosso do Sul. Master thesis. Departamento de Psicologia da Universidade Católica Dom Bosco, Campo Grande, MS. 76p.
  • O’Brien, T.G. 1991. Female-male social interactions in wedge-capped capuchin monkeys: benefits and costs of group living. Animal Behaviour, 41(4): 555-567. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80896-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80896-6
  • Oliveira, C.G.L. de, 2014. Estresse e complexidade comportamental em macacos-prego-de-peito-amarelo (Sapajus xantosthernos). Dissertação de mestrado. Universidade Federal da Bahia. 70p.
  • Orihuela, G.; Terborgh, J.; Ceballos, N. & Glander, K. 2014. When Top-Down Becomes Bottom Up: Behaviour of Hyperdense Howler Monkeys (Alouatta seniculus) Trapped on a 0.6 Ha Island. PLoS ONE, 9(4): e82197. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0082197
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0082197
  • Ottoni, E. 2009. Uso de ferramentas e tradições comportamentais em macacos-prego (Cebus spp.) Tese de livre docência. Departamento de Psicologia Experimental da Universidade de São Paulo. 201p.
  • Ottoni, E. & Mannu, M. 2001. Semi-free ranging tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts. International Journal of Primatology, 22(3): 347-358.
  • Overduin-de-Vries, A.M.; de Vries, H.; Vermande, M.M.; Reijntjes, A.H.A. & Sterck, E.H.M. 2020. Both aggressive and affiliative behaviour facilitate resource access in high-ranking female long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behaviour, 157(3-4): 267-287. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-bja10001
    » https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-bja10001
  • Pellegrini, A.D. 2008. The roles of aggressive and affiliative behaviors in resource control: a behavioral ecological perspective. Developmental Review, 28(4): 461-487. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2008.03.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2008.03.001
  • Pellegrini, A.D.; Roseth, C.J.; Mliner, S.; Bohn, C.M.; Van Ryzin, M.; Vance, N.; Cheatham, C.L. & Tarullo, A. 2007. Social dominance in preschool classrooms. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121: 54-64. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.121.1.54
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.121.1.54
  • Perry, S. 1996. Female-female social relationships in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus American Journal of Primatology, 40(2): 167-182. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1996)40:2<167::AID-AJP4>3.0.CO;2-W
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1996)40:2<167::AID-AJP4>3.0.CO;2-W
  • Perry, S. 1997. Male-female social relationships in wild white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Behaviour, 134: 477-510.
  • Phillips, K.A. 1995. Foraging-related agonism in capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Folia Primatologica, 65(3): 159-162.
  • Pinha, P.S. 2007. Interações sociais em grupos de macacos-prego (Cebus libidinosus) no Parque Nacional de Brasília Master thesis. Programa de Pós-Graduação em Ecologia da Universidade de Brasília. 67p.
  • Pinheiro, J.; Bates, D.; DebRoy, S.; Sarkar, D. & R Core Team. 2019. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models . R package Version 3.1-143 https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme
    » https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme
  • Pope, T.R. 2000. Reproductive success increases with degree of kinship in cooperative coalitions of female red howler monkeys Alouatta seniculus Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 48(4): 253-267.
  • R Core Team. 2019. R: A language and environment for statistical computing Vienna, R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.R-project.org
    » http://www.R-project.org
  • Rodrigues. B. 2014. Etologia aplicada ao enriquecimento ambiental para macacos-prego (Sapajus spp.) semi-cativos Dissertação de mestrado. Instituto de Piscicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 108p.
  • Rose, L.M. 1998. Behavioral ecology of white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) in Costa Rica PhD. Thesis from Department of Anthropology, Washington University, St. Louis. MO USA. 357p.
  • Rylands, A.B.; Mittermeier, R.A.; Bezerra, B.M.; Paim, F.P.; Helder, L. & Queiroz, H.L. 2013. Family Cebidae (Squirrel Monkeys and Capuchins). In: Mittermeier, R.; Rylands, A.B. & Wilson, D.E. Handbook of the Mammals of the World Barcelona, Lynx. vol. 3, 952p.
  • Santos, L.B. & Reis, N.R. 2009. Estudo comportamental de Cebus nigritus (Goldfuss, 1809) (Primates, Cebidae) em cativeiro. Semina: Ciências Biológicas e da Saúde, 30: 175-184.
  • Sapolsky, R.M. 2005. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science, 308(5722): 648-652. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1106477
    » https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1106477
  • Scarry, C.J. 2013. Between-group contest competition among tufted capuchin monkeys, Sapajus nigritus, and the role of male resource defense. Animal Behaviour, 85: 1-9.
  • Scott, J. & Lockard, J.S. 2007. Competition coalitions and conflict interventions among captive female gorillas. International Journal Primatology, 28(4): 761-781.
  • Seyfarth, R.M. 1977. A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. Journal of Theorical Biology, 65(4): 671-698.
  • Simões, A.S. 2013. Interações e relações sociais de macaco-prego (Sapajus apella) em cativeiro com isolamento do indivíduo durante a alimentação: um estudo descritivo. Master’s thesis. Núcleo de teoria e pesquisa do comportamento, Universidade Federal do Pará. p. 61.
  • Sonnweber, R.S.; Ravignani, A.; Stobbe, N.; Schiestl, G.; Wallner, B. & Fitch, W.T. 2015. Rank dependent grooming patterns and cortisol alleviation in Barbary macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 77(6): 688-700. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22391
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22391
  • Southwick, C.H. 1967. An experimental study of intragroup agonistic behavior in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behaviour, 28: 181-209.
  • Sparks, J. 1967. Allogrooming in primates: A review. In: Primate Ethology. Morris, D. (Ed.). Weinfeld and Nicholson, London.
  • Sterck, E.H.M. & Steenbeek, R. 1997. Female dominance relationships and food competition in the sympatric Thomas langur and long-tailed macaque. Behaviour, 134: 749-774.
  • Stoesz, B.M.; Hare, J.F. & Snow, W.M. 2013. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying affiliative social behaviour: insights from comparative research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 37: 123-132.
  • Strier, K.B. 2011. Primate behavioural ecology New Jersey: Prentice Hall. 432p.
  • Surbeck, M. & Hohmann, G. 2011. Mothers matter! Maternal support, dominance status and mating success in male bonobos Pan paniscus. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1705): 590-598.
  • Sussman, R.W.; Garber, P.A. & Cheverud, J.M. 2005. Importance of cooperation and affiliation in the evolution of primate sociality. American Journal Physical Anthropology, 128: 84-97.
  • Tiddi, B.; Aureli, F. & Schino, G. 2012. Grooming up the hierarchy: the exchange of grooming and rank-related benefits in a new world primate. PLoS ONE, 7: 3-8.
  • Tiddi, B.; Aureli, F.; Polizzi di Sorrentino, E.; Janson, C.H. & Schino, G. 2011. Grooming for tolerance? Two mechanisms of exchange in wild tufted capuchin monkeys. Behavioral Ecology, 22(3): 663-669. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr028
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr028
  • Torres de Assumpção, C. 1983. An ecological study of primates of Southeastern Brazil with a reappraisal on Cebus apella races. Doctoral thesis. Edinburgh University. 402p.
  • van Staaden, M.J.; Searcy, W.A. & Hanlon, R.T. 2011. Signaling aggression. In: Aggression. Huber, R.; Bannasch, D.L.; Brennan, P. & Frishman, K. (Eds.). San Diego, CA, Elsevier/Academic Press. p. 23-49.
  • Verderane, M.P. 2010. Socieoecologia de macacos-prego (Cebus libidinosus) em área de ecótono cerrado/caatinga Tese Doutorado apresentada ao Inst. Psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 235p.
  • Verderane, M.P.; Izar, P.; Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. 2013. Socioecology of wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus): an analysis of social relationships among female primates that use tools in feeding. Behaviour, 150: 659-689. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003076
    » https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003076
  • Visalberghi, E. & Fragaszy, D. 2002. Do monkeys ape? Ten years later. In: Dautenhahn, K. & Nehaniv, C. (Eds.). Imitation in animals and artifacts Cambridge, MA, MIT Press. p. 471-479.
  • Visalberghi, E.; Di Bernardi, C.M.; Luca, A.; Fragaszy, D. & Izar, P. 2017. Female bearded capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus) use objects to solicit the sexual partner. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 131(3): 207-213.
  • Vogel, E.R. & Janson, C. 2007. Predicting the frequency of food-related agonism in white faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus) using a novel focal-tree method. American Journal of Primatology, 69(5): 533-550.
  • Wolf, L.L. 1978. Aggressive social organization in nectarivorous birds. American Zoologist, 18(4): 765-778.
  • Wooddell, L.J.; Hamel, A.F.; Murphy, A.M.; Byers, K.L.; Kaburu, S.S.K.; Meyer, J.S.; Suomi, S.J. & Dettmer, A.M. 2017. Relationships between affiliative social behavior and hair cortisol concentrations in semi‐free ranging rhesus monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 84: 109-115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.06.018
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.06.018
  • Yamamoto, M.E. 1991. Comportamento social do gênero Callithrix em cativeiro. In: Congresso Brasileiro de Primatologia, 3º. Anais Belo Horizonte, MG, Fundação Biodiversitas. p. 63-72. (Primatologia no Brasil, 3).
  • Yamanashi, Y.; Teramoto, M.; Morimura, N.; Nogami, E. & Hirata, S. 2018. Social relationship and hair cortisol level in captive male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Primates, 59(2): 145-152. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-017-0641-8
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-017-0641-8
  • FUNDING ACQUISITION: Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico and CAPES - Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior for funding the National Institute of Science and Technology in Interdisciplinary and Transdisciplinary Studies in Ecology and Evolution - INCT IN-TREE (grants n. 465767/2014-1 and 23038.000776/2017-54, respectively). C.N.E. received a research fellowship from CNPq (grant n. 303011/2017-3).
  • Published with the financial support of the "Programa de Apoio às Publicações Científicas Periódicas da USP".

APPENDIX 1


Number of individuals, name, sex, age and origin of capuchin monkeys observed for this study in captive and wild conditions. (* Alpha male).

APPENDIX 2


Ethogram for behavioural data collection of Sapajus libidinosus, adapted from Santos & Reis (2009Santos, L.B. & Reis, N.R. 2009. Estudo comportamental de Cebus nigritus (Goldfuss, 1809) (Primates, Cebidae) em cativeiro. Semina: Ciências Biológicas e da Saúde, 30: 175-184.), Gouveia (2009Gouveia, P.S. 2009. Padrão de atividades, dieta e uso do espaço de um grupo de Cebus xanthosternos (Wied-Neuwied, 1820) (Primates , Cebidae), na Reserva Biológica de Una, Bahia, Brasil. Dissertação (Mestrado em Zoologia). Ilhéus, Universidade Estadual de Santa Cruz. 138p.), Verderane (2010Verderane, M.P. 2010. Socieoecologia de macacos-prego (Cebus libidinosus) em área de ecótono cerrado/caatinga. Tese Doutorado apresentada ao Inst. Psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 235p.), Martins (2010Martins, W.P. 2010. Densidade Populacional e Ecologia de um grupo macaco-prego-de-crista (Cebus robustus; Kuhl, 1820) na Reserva Natural Vale. Tese de doutorado. Instituto de Ciências Biológicas da Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais. 118p.), Cutrim (2013Cutrim, F.H.R. 2013. Padrão comportamental e uso de ferramentas em macacos-prego (Sapajus libidinosus) residentes em manguezal. PhD thesis. Instituto de Psicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 116p.), Rodrigues (2014Rodrigues. B. 2014. Etologia aplicada ao enriquecimento ambiental para macacos-prego (Sapajus spp.) semi-cativos. Dissertação de mestrado. Instituto de Piscicologia da Universidade de São Paulo. 108p.), and Oliveira (2014Oliveira, C.G.L. de, 2014. Estresse e complexidade comportamental em macacos-prego-de-peito-amarelo (Sapajus xantosthernos). Dissertação de mestrado. Universidade Federal da Bahia. 70p.).

Edited by

Edited by: Luís Fábio Silveira.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    03 Oct 2022
  • Date of issue
    2022

History

  • Received
    15 Sept 2021
  • Accepted
    21 Apr 2022
  • Published
    27 May 2022
Museu de Zoologia da Universidade de São Paulo Av. Nazaré, 481, Ipiranga, 04263-000 São Paulo SP Brasil, Tel.: (55 11) 2065-8133 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: einicker@usp.br