Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The effect of matinal active walking on cognitive, fine motor coordination task performances and perceived difficulty in 12-13 young school boys

Abstract

Aim:

The current study examined the relationship between cognitive performances (executive function, selective attention and reaction time), fine motor coordination skills and perceived difficulty after active transport to school.

Method:

Fifteen right-handed children’s underwent session, 15-min walking session at 30% (WS1) and 15-min walking session (WS2) at 50% of maximal aerobic speed. Subjects performed tests to evaluate executive function, reaction time and selective attention. After each trial, a questionnaire of perceived difficulty (PD) was completed.

Results:

Average time in TMT part A (F(2,22) = 4.44; p = 0.024; η2= 0.288) and TMT part B (F(2,22) = 4.54; p = 0.022; η2= 0.292), and committed errors (F(2,22) = 7.78; p = 0.003; η2= 0.414) was improved after walking sessions in comparison by CS. The mean scores were significantly higher after walking sessions for both long and short-distance throws (p < 0.05). Moreover, a significant negative correlation was found between committed errors (TMT part B) and both dart throwing consistency and accuracy (r = - 0.6; r = - 0.64; p < 0.05) (respectively). Post-hoc analysis showed that PD was better after walking sessions with low intensity for both short and long throwing distance. However, it seems that walking session with sustained intensity allows speed and accuracy improvement of cognitive processing.

Conclusion:

Thus, active walking to school with low intensity was sufficient to produce positives changes in psychomotor performance and decrease in perceived difficulty scores. By including individual differences in gross motor coordination as well as physical activity level, the exact nature of the link between psychomotor skills and cognitive performance could be more addressed.

Keywords:
active transport; boys; fine motor coordination; cognitive performances; perceived difficulty

Introduction

Physical activity (PA) is important for optimal childhood growth and development; the majority of children in developed countries are insufficiently active11. Pabayo R, Maximova K, Spence JC, Vander Ploeg K, Wu B, Veugelers PJ. The importance of Active Transportation to and from school for daily physical activity among children. Prev Med. 2012; 55(3): 196-200.. However, recent findings of systematic review confirm that PA is associated with numerous health benefits in school-aged children and youth. Journey between home and school become an important, potential source of PA for children22. Tudor-Locke C, Ainsworth BE, Popkin BM. Active commuting to school: an overlooked source of childrens' physical activity? Sports Med. 2001; 31(5):309-313.), (33. Heelan KA, Donnelly JE, Jacobsen DJ, Mayo MS, Washburn R, Greene L. Active commuting to and from school and BMI in elementary school children-preliminary data. Child Care Health Dev. 2005; 31(3): 341-349.), (44. Dessing D, de Vries S, Graham JM, Pierik HF. Active transport between home and school assessed with GPS: a cross-sectional study among Dutch elementary school children. BMC Public Health. 2014; 14(1): 1-8.. Active Transportation (AT) to and/or from school (i.e., walking and biking) is one potential opportunity to be physically active, given that many children can incorporate this behavior into their daily routine11. Pabayo R, Maximova K, Spence JC, Vander Ploeg K, Wu B, Veugelers PJ. The importance of Active Transportation to and from school for daily physical activity among children. Prev Med. 2012; 55(3): 196-200.), (22. Tudor-Locke C, Ainsworth BE, Popkin BM. Active commuting to school: an overlooked source of childrens' physical activity? Sports Med. 2001; 31(5):309-313.), (55. Sirard JR, Riner Jr, McIver KL, Pate RR. Physical activity and active commuting to elementary school. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2005; 37(12): 2062-2069.. Recently, Cooper et al. (66. Cooper AR, Page AS, Wheeler BW, Griew P, Davis L, Hillsdon M, et al. Mapping the walk to school using accelerometry combined with a global positioning system. Am J Prev Med. 2010; 38( 2):178-183.) showed that the journey to school of children who used AT, could contribute towards reaching daily PA requirements. Although recently research support thesis that AT is associated with a healthier body composition and higher levels of cardio-respiratory fitness among youth77. Lubans DR, Boreham CA, Kelly P, Foster CE. The relationship between active travel to school and health-related fitness in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Ac. 2011; 26: 8-5..

Generally, the literature shows that executive functions including attention, working memory, problem solving, cognitive flexibility, verbal fluency, decision making, and inhibitory control receive the most benefit from acute exercise88. Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, Etnier JL. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012; 1453: 87-101.. In addition, a previous study concluded that increasing task difficulty or complexity may help to augment the effect of acute exercise on accuracy and speed of processing99. McMorris T, Hale BJ. Differential effects of differing intensities of acute exercise on speed and accuracy of cognition: A meta-analytical investigation. Brain Cogn. 2012; 80(3): 338-51.. Moreover, moderate intensity exercise may be more beneficial to executive function (EF); whereas high-intensity exercise may be beneficial to information processing88. Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, Etnier JL. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012; 1453: 87-101.. In primary school-aged children, author’s found positive relation between total volume of light intensity physical activity and executive function (namely: planning performance) (1010. Van der Niet AG, Smith J, Sherder EJ, Oosterlaan J, Hartman E, Visscher C. Associations between daily physical activity and executive functioning in primary school-aged children. J Sci Med Sport. 2014; 18(6): 673-677..

Recent studies suggest specific relationships between aspects of motor coordination and EF1111. Rigoli D, Piek JP, Kane R, Oosterlaan J. An examination of the relationship between motor coordination and executive functions in adolescents. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2012; 54(11): 1025-1031.. In addition, fine motor skills and EF can be considered as powerful predictors of school readiness and of subsequent academic achievement1212. Grissmer D, Grimm KJ, Aiyer SM, Murrah WM, Steele JS. Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators. Dev Psychol. 2010; 46(5): 1008-1017.), (1313. Cameron CE, Brock LL, Murrah WM, Bell LH, Worzalla SL, Grissmer D, et al. Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to Kindergarten achievement. Child Development. 2012; 83(4): 1229-1244.. Thus, a recent hypothesis highlighted that physical activity targeted to increase both physical fitness and motor skills may have the potential to positively affect executive functions and academic performance1414. Aadland KN, Moe VF, Aadland E, Anderssen SA, Resaland GK, Ommundsen Y. Relationships between physical activity, sedentary time, aerobic fitness, motor skills and executive function and academic performance in children. Ment Health Phys Act. 2017; 12: 10-18.. Furthermore, the results show a significant correlation between perceived difficulty (PD) and EF in situations which higher levels of difficulty. Higher EF performance approves the ability to manage better the level of task difficulty1515. Elghoul Y, Frikha M, Masmoudi L, Chtourou H, Chaouachi A, Chamari K, et al. Diurnal variation of cognitive performance and perceived difficulty in dart-throwing performance in 9-10 year old boys. Biol Rhythm Res. 2014; 45(5): 789-801.. Li and Belkin1616. Li Y, Belkin NJ. A faceted approach to conceptualizing tasks in information seeking. NFORM PROCESS MANAG. 2008; 44(6): 1822-1837. define task difficulty as a subjective perception assessed by task doers. It could be formed in both pre and post-task1717. Kim J. Task difficulty as a predictor and indicator of web searching interaction. Proceedings of CHI '06. 2006; April: 959-964.. Recent research shows that performance is limited by task difficulty, often in the form of a trade-off between speed and accuracy. Learning consists of breaking through this limit1818. Reis J, Schambra HM, Cohen LG, Buch ER, Fritsch B, Zarahn E, et al. Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc Nat Acad Sci. 2009; 106(5): 1590-1595.. This new information about how to manipulate task difficulty is important when adapting tasks for use with children of different ages1919. Lindqvist S, Thorell LB. Brief report: manipulation of task difficulty in inhibitory control tasks. Child Neuropsychol. 2009; 15(1): 1-7..

Regarding active transport study, only one reported that active commuting to school and its duration (more than 15 min) may positively influence cognitive performance (verbal, numeric, and reasoning abilities and an overall score) in adolescent girls but not in boys2020. Martinez-Gomez D, Ruiz JR, Gomez-Martinez S, Chillon P, Rey-Lopez JP, Diaz LE, et al. Active commuting to school and cognitive performance in adolescents: the AVENA study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011; 165(4): 300-305.. Furthermore, this study uses subjective data participants; do not consider their extracurricular physical activity and walking intensity to school. The majority of walking studies to school were based on parents’ reports2121. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Barriers to children walking and biking to school-United States, 1999. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2002; 51(32): 701-704..

The current study examined the relationship between (i) ognitive performances (executive function, selective attention and reaction time) (ii) and perceived difficulty in fine motor coordination tasks after walking to school among 12-13 young boys.

Participants

Fifteen right-handed children (age = 12.33 ± 0.9 years, body height = 157 ± 8.4, body mass = 45.4 ± 8.1 kg and MAS = 13.8 ± 1; mean ± SD) volunteered to participate in this study. They had no previous experience of the tasks they were asked to perform. All subjects participated in their physical activity classes 1-2 times per week and they did not have any outside-school activities. The protocol was explained in full and any questions were answered before a written informed consent was obtained from the children’s parents and the children themselves. The study design received clearance from the Institutional Research Board before the commencement of this study procedure. The University Scientific Board approved this study with the number H2017/86. Participants visited the laboratory on four occasions in order to complete a familiarization session and three experimental trials.

Procedure

During the familiarization session, participants threw a bloc of ten darts in each condition and then performed the Yo-Yo intermittent recovery test (Yo-Yo) in order to determine the maximal aerobic velocity (MAV) 2222. Dupont G, Defontaine M, Bosquet L, Blondel N, Moalla W, Berthoin, S. Yo-Yo intermittent recovery test versus the Université de Montréal Track Test: Relation with a high-intensity intermittent exercise. J Sci Med Sport. 2010; 13(1): 146-150.. The Yo-Yo protocol was performed on a nonslip wooden floor of an indoor gym. Participants then participated in three experimental trials in a randomized manner at the same time of day (07:00 h), separated at least by five days. The three trials consisted of a no-walking session (no-WS: Control session (CS)) (Subjects performed a dart-throwing test and cognitive tasks) (1515. Elghoul Y, Frikha M, Masmoudi L, Chtourou H, Chaouachi A, Chamari K, et al. Diurnal variation of cognitive performance and perceived difficulty in dart-throwing performance in 9-10 year old boys. Biol Rhythm Res. 2014; 45(5): 789-801. and two 15-min walking sessions (WS), with each WS trial performed at a different intensity as reported by44. Dessing D, de Vries S, Graham JM, Pierik HF. Active transport between home and school assessed with GPS: a cross-sectional study among Dutch elementary school children. BMC Public Health. 2014; 14(1): 1-8.. After each WS subjects performed a dart-throwing test in two experimental sessions and cognitive tasks.

All WS trials were performed on the same motorized treadmill (COSMED T170, Italy). The intensity of each WS was determined as follows:

  • WS 1 = intensity at 30% of maximal aerobic speed

  • WS 2 = intensity at 50% of maximal aerobic speed

After each dart throw, the score for the dart was recorded and then the dart was collected for the next throw2323. Edwards B, Waterhouse J. Effects of one night of partial sleep deprivation upon diurnal rhythms of accuracy and consistency in throwing darts. Chronobiol Int, 2009; 26(4): 756-768.. An official dartboard was placed on a wall so that its center was at eye level for each subject. Participants threw ten darts from two distances in randomized order. The first distance was 2 m (SD); the second was at the regular distance of 2.37 m (LD). The two distances were marked by a line on the floor1515. Elghoul Y, Frikha M, Masmoudi L, Chtourou H, Chaouachi A, Chamari K, et al. Diurnal variation of cognitive performance and perceived difficulty in dart-throwing performance in 9-10 year old boys. Biol Rhythm Res. 2014; 45(5): 789-801.. After each session, a questionnaire to asses PD (DP-15) was completed by the subjects. This scale is composed of 15 points numbered 1-15 and is anchored at the two extremities by verbal labels - “Extremely easy” and “Extremely difficult” 2424. Delignières D, Famose JP, Thépaut-Mathieu C, Fleurance P. A psychophysical study on difficulty ratings in rock climbing. Int J Sport Psychol. 1993; 24: 404-416..

Yo-Yo intermittent recovery test

The Yo-Yo IR1 consisted of repeated 2 × 20-m of running at a progressively increased speed controlled by audio beeps from a tape recorder2525. Krustrup P, Mohr M, Amstrup T, Rysgaard T, Johansen J, Steensberg A, et al. The Yo-Yo Intermittent Recovery Test: Physiological response, reliability and validity. Med Sci Sport Exer. 2003; 35(4): 697-705.. Between each running bout the participants had a 10-s rest period. When the participant failed to reach the finishing line in time twice, the distance covered was recorded and represented the test result. The test was performed indoor (on a 2-m-wide and 20-m-long running lane marked by cones). Six participants performed the test simultaneously with strong verbal encouragement provided to the subjects throughout the test. Total distance was reported as the performance criterion in the Yo-Yo IR12626. Bangsbo J, Laia FM, Krustrup P. The Yo-Yo Intermittent Recovery Test. A Useful Tool for Evaluation of Physical Performance in Intermittent Sports. Sports Med. 2008; 38(1): 37-51..

Score calculations

Each throw was scored according to its position on the board (0-10). A dart that missed the board or that bounced off was given a score of “0”. The target consisted of a series of 10 concentric rings. Participants’ dart throwing accuracy and consistency were evaluated using three scores. The first was the mean score of the ten throws. This score could range from 0 (all misses) to 10 (all bulls-eyes); it can be considered a measure of accuracy, a high score indicating high accuracy. The second measurement was the numbers of zeros scored (number of times the target was missed). This score could range from 0 to 10, a low number of zeros indicating high accuracy. The third measure of performance was the coefficients of variation of the score: [SD scores] ⁄ [mean score], a lower coefficient indicating a higher consistency1515. Elghoul Y, Frikha M, Masmoudi L, Chtourou H, Chaouachi A, Chamari K, et al. Diurnal variation of cognitive performance and perceived difficulty in dart-throwing performance in 9-10 year old boys. Biol Rhythm Res. 2014; 45(5): 789-801..

The barrage test (i.e. a paper-pencil test)

The barrage test is a psychometric task, which measure visual-spatial ability, attention, and recognition. It involves visual discrimination; the subject must circle all the targets (bells) that are encountered. A total of 35 targets (bells), were distributed equally in seven columns. In each column, there was the same number of targets (N = 5) and of distracters (N = 40). All drawings were black - like Chinese shadows. Subject performance was evaluated quantitatively (number of bells crossed and omitted). The duration of the test was 3 min, a higher number of correct responses reflecting better performance2727. Gauthier L, Dehaut F, Joanette Y. The bells test: a quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. Int J Clin Neuropsychol. 1989; 11(2): 49-54..

The choice RT (Using React’s software)

A colored geometric form (used as a “target”) was presented to the subject. When the test was started, there was a succession of different colored geometric forms. When the target appeared, the subject was required to press a button as soon as possible, and the computer calculated the RT. The time between presenting each form was 300 ms and each subject had 20 targets presented1515. Elghoul Y, Frikha M, Masmoudi L, Chtourou H, Chaouachi A, Chamari K, et al. Diurnal variation of cognitive performance and perceived difficulty in dart-throwing performance in 9-10 year old boys. Biol Rhythm Res. 2014; 45(5): 789-801.. Scores were expressed in seconds; higher scores reflect poorer performance.

Trail making test

This is a test exploring mental flexibility EF (aptitude to move quickly from one task to another) 2828. Corrigan JD, Hinkeldey MS. Relationships between parts A and B of the trail making test. J Clin Psychol. 1987; 43(4): 402-409.. In Part A, circles were numbered 1-25 and presented randomly on a sheet of paper; subjects were required to draw lines to connect the numbers in ascending order. In Part B, the circle included both numbers (1-15) and letters (A-L); as in Part A, the child was required to draw lines to connect the circles in an ascending pattern, but with the added task of alternating between the numbers and letters (i.e. 1-A-2-B-3-C, etc.); the child was also instructed to connect the circles as quickly as possible without lifting the pen or pencil from the paper. The duration of the test was 3 min for each part. The trail-making test (TMT) measures visual conceptual and visuo-motor tracking. TMT part A purportedly measures attention, visual search, and motor function, whereas TMT part B is seen as a measure of EF, speed of attention, visual search, and motor function2929. Strauss E, Sherman EM, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. 3rd Ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2006.. Outcome measures for both tasks included time to completion and number of errors2929. Strauss E, Sherman EM, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. 3rd Ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2006.. Results for both TMT A and B are reported as the number of seconds required to complete the task3030. Dobbs BM, Shergill SS. How effective is the trail making test (Parts A and B) in identifying cognitively impaired drivers? Age Ageing. 2013; 42(5): 577-581. errors committed and corrected. Higher scores reveal greater impairment.

Statistical analysis

All statistical tests were processed using STATISTICA Software (StatSoft, Paris, France). Data were reported as mean ± SD. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test of normality revealed the data which were normally distributed; therefore, parametric tests were performed. To examine the associations between cognitive performance and other variables, Pearson correlation analyzes were used.

Performance measures and difficulty of perception were analyzed using a two-way ANOVA with repeated measures [2(Distance) × 3(walking session)]. ANOVA with repeated measures used to analyze walking sessions and cognitive performance. When appropriate, significant differences between means were assessed using post hoc tests. Effect sizes were calculated as partial eta-squared η2 to estimate the meaningfulness of significant findings. Partial eta squared values of 0.01, 0.06 and 0.13 represent small, moderate, and large effect sizes, respectively. The level of statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

Results

Analysis of the performance throws (mean scores: measure of accuracy) resulted in no significant interaction effect walking session × distance of throw (F(2,22) = 0.41; p =0.66; η2 = 0.036). There was a significant main effect for walking session (WS) (F(2,22) = 4.68; p = 0.02; η2= 0.298) and distance of throw (F(2,22) = 8.21; p = 0.01; η2 = 0.427). Post-hoc analyses demonstrated a significant difference between mean scores after the two walking sessions (WS1 and WS2). The mean scores were significantly higher after WS1 and WS2 for both long- and short-distance (LD and SD) throws (p < 0.05). Furthermore, there were significant differences between mean scores after WS 2 between SD and LD (p = 0.03). Additionally, the ANOVA revealed no significant effect of walking session (F(2,22) = 1.27; p = 0.29; η2 = 0.103) and distance of throw (F(2,22)= 0.37; p = 0.55; η2 = 0.033) for errors (numbers of zeros). Finally, ANOVA also revealed a significant walking session effect (F(2,22) = 5.75; p = 0.009; η2 = 0.343) for the consistency which the darts were thrown. The pos-hoc analysis showed that the coefficient of variation (CV) of the mean of the scores was significantly different only in the CS between SD and LD. CV was better after WS1 than WS2 and CS (respectively).

Analysis revealed a significant effect of walking session (F(2,22) = 4.44; p = 0.024; η2 = 0.287) for EF (average time in TMT part A). The post-hoc analysis showed that completion time (TMT part A) was significantly different after both WS1 and WS2 in comparison by CS (Table1). No significant difference was found for committed and corrected errors in TMT part A.

Table 1
Mean ± SD values for cognitive performances measures: RT; barrage test; and EF (TMT parts A and B) at CS, WS1 and WS2.

Analysis revealed significant effect of walking session (F(2,22) = 4.54; p = 0.022; η2= 0.292) for EF (average time in TMT part B). Post-hoc analyses demonstrated a significant difference between time in TMT part B after the two walking sessions (WS1 and WS2) in comparison by CS (Figure 1). Moreover, significant difference was found in committed errors in TMT part B (F(2,22) = 7.78; p = 0.002; η2= 0.414) (Table 1). Post-hoc analysis shows that better performance was recorded after WS1 in comparison by WS2 and CS (respectively). Results showed significant positive correlations after CS between committed errors in TMT part A and corrected errors in TMT part B (r = 0.63; p < 0.05). Results showed also a significant positive correlation after CS in EF (TMT part B) between average time and corrected errors (r = 0.67; p < 0.05). The links between fine motor coordination skills and EF were investigated. Data’s showed no significant difference between measured variables in fine coordination task (throwing task) and TMT (part A and B) after CS and WS1. There was a significant positive correlation after WS2 between mean score in LD and committed errors (TMT part B) (r = 0.76; p < 0.01). Moreover, a significant negative correlation was found between committed errors (TMT part B) and both CV and number of zeros (r = - 0.6; p < 0.05; r = - 0.64; p < 0.05) (respectively).

Figure 1
Average time completion and committed error in Trail Making Test (Part B) (second and number of error, respectively) at CS, WS1 and WS2. CS indicates Control session; WS30%: Walking session at 30% of the maximal aerobic velocity (MAV); WS50%: Walking session at 50% of the MAV.

Analysis of reaction time data’s, results showed a no significant main effect of walking session (F (2,22) = 1.97; p = 0.163; η2 = 0.152) on average time in reaction time (Table 1). There was a significant positive correlation between RT and average time after WS2 (r = 0.58; p < 0.05). No significant correlation was found between RT and measured variables in fine coordination task after all sessions.

The ANOVA of selective attention revealed a no significant walking session effect (F(2,22) = 1.801; p = 0.189; η2 = 0.141) for SA (Table 1). There was no significant correlation between all variables of our study in CS and WS1. In WS2, there was a significant positive correlation between SA (Crossed bells) and EF (committed errors in TMT part B) (r = 0.76; p < 0.01). Results also showed significant negative correlations only after WS2 between SA (Unbarred bells) and FE (committed errors in TMT part B) (r = - 0.76; p < 0.01). In addition, significant correlation was found only after WS2 between SA and accuracy measures (errors and mean scores). Results showed significant negative correlation between SA (Crossed bells) and errors in LD (r = - 0.57; p < 0.05), in contrary a positive significant correlation between SA (Unbarred bells) and errors in LD (r = 0.57; p < 0.05). Finally, there was a significant positive correlation between SA (Crossed bells) and means scores in SD (r = 0.66; p < 0.05). A significant negative correlation was found between SA (Unbarred bells) and means scores in SD (r = -0.66; p < 0.05).

There was a significant main effect of walking session for perceived difficulty (PD) (F(2,22)= 11.54; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.512). Additionally, the ANOVA revealed significant effect of distance of throw (F(2,22) = 50.72; p < 0.001;η2 = 0.821) (Figure 2). However, the interaction walking session × distance of throw was not significant (F(2,22) = 0.412; p > 0.05;η2 = 0.036). The pos-hoc analysis showed that PD was better after WS1 for both short and long distance compared to WS1 and CS (respectively). Moreover, in CS, PD was correlated significantly with both average time in TMT part A (r = 0.58; p < 0.05) in SD and negatively with committed errors in TMT part B in SD and LD of throw (r = -0.75; p < 0.01; r = - 0.71; p < 0.01). No significant correlation was found between PD and variables of study in WS1. In WS2, there was a significant positive correlation between PD and EF (average time in TMT part B) in both SD (r = 0.66; p < 0.05) and LD of throw (r = 0.56; p < 0.05).

Figure 2
Perceived difficulty score on SD and LD at CS, WS1 and WS2. CS indicates Control session; WS30%, Walking session at 30% of the maximal aerobic velocity (MAV); WS50%, Walking session at 50% of the MAV; SD, Short distance of throw; LD, Long distance of throw.

Table 2
Correlations between cognitive performances measures: reaction time (RT); barrage test; and executive function (TMT parts A and B), dart throwing scores: CV; Errors; and Mean scores) and perceived difficulty score (PD) at CS.

Table 3
Correlations between cognitive performances measures: reaction time (RT); barrage test; and executive function (TMT parts A and B), dart throwing scores: CV; Errors; and Mean scores) and perceived difficulty score (PD) at WS1.

Table 4
Correlations between cognitive performances measures: reaction time (RT); barrage test; and executive function (TMT parts A and B), dart throwing scores: CV; Errors; and Mean scores) and perceived difficulty score (PD) at WS2.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to explore the relationship of the cognitive performances (executive function, selective attention and reaction time) and perceived difficulty in fine motor coordination tasks after the portion of active transport to school. The data supported the hypothesis that achieving walking session to school with appropriate intensity enables differential improvement on the measured cognitive variables (executive function, selective attention and reaction time). Findings confirm that only EF (cognitive flexibility) can be improved by gains in speed process and in errors committed numbers. Positives changes in psychomotor performance were demonstrated by accessing throwing tasks and a diminution PD scores in the same fine motor coordination tasks was observed.

Improvement in EF was demonstrated by the decrease in time completion for trail-making task (part A and B) and committed errors only in trail-making task (part B). To our knowledge, this is the first study that shows that the improvement in cognitive function differs between the processes and it was related to intensities of walking session to school. Previous findings suggest that active commuting to school was associated with better in boys was positively associated with objectively PA assessment3131. Chillon P, Ortega FB, Ruiz JR, Veidebaum T, Oja L, Mäestu J, et al. Active commuting to school in children and adolescents: an opportunity to increase physical activity and fitness. Scand J Public Health. 2010; 38(8): 873-879. and cognitive performance (namely: verbal ability, numeric ability, reasoning ability, overall cognitive performance) in adolescent girls who spent more than 15 minutes than those who spent less time2020. Martinez-Gomez D, Ruiz JR, Gomez-Martinez S, Chillon P, Rey-Lopez JP, Diaz LE, et al. Active commuting to school and cognitive performance in adolescents: the AVENA study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011; 165(4): 300-305.. Other researchers have suggested that in older adults performing an executive processing task, response times decreased and response errors increased when walking at faster speed compared with walking at slower speed3232. Tomporowski PD, Audiffren M. Dual-task performance in young and older adults: speed-accuracy radeoffs in choice responding while treadmill walking. Journal of aging And physical activity. 2014; 22(4): 557-63.. On the other hand, the young healthy subjects can increase the ability to walk at higher speeds and a difficult cognitive task with no drop in performance3333. Kline JE, Poggensee K, Ferris DP. Your brain on speed: cognitive performance of a spatial working memory task is not affected by walking speed. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014; 8(8): 1-16.. In addition, the majority of studies observed positive associations between PA and number of tests measuring EF3434. Kamijo K, Takeda Y. Regular physical activity improves executive function duringtask switching in young adults. Int J Psychophysiol. 2010; 75(3): 304-311.), (3535. Padilla C, Perez L, Andres P, Parmentier FBR. Exercise improves cognitive control: evidence from the stop signal task. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2013; 27(4): 505-511.), (3636. Pérez L, Padilla C, Parmentier FBR, Andrés P. The effects of chronic exercise on attentional networks. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9(7): e101478. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101478
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.010...
. Furthermore, three studies have identified no significant positive associations3737. Woo E, Sharps MJ. Cognitive aging and physical exercise. Educ Gerontol. 2003; 29(4): 327-337.), (3838. Keeley TJ, Fox KR. The impact of physical activity and fitness on academic achievement and cognitive performance in children. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2009; 2(2): 198-214.), (3939. Winneke AH, Godde B, Reuter EM, Vieluf S, Voelcker-Rehage C. The association between physical activity and attentional control in younger and older middle-aged adults: an ERP study. GeroPsych. 2012; 25(4): 207-221.. Despite the fact that it is not yet clear the typical physical activity patterns of children involving performance at the executive level1010. Van der Niet AG, Smith J, Sherder EJ, Oosterlaan J, Hartman E, Visscher C. Associations between daily physical activity and executive functioning in primary school-aged children. J Sci Med Sport. 2014; 18(6): 673-677.. Studies stipulate that children are engaged in low-intensity physical that can place a demand on their executive functioning1010. Van der Niet AG, Smith J, Sherder EJ, Oosterlaan J, Hartman E, Visscher C. Associations between daily physical activity and executive functioning in primary school-aged children. J Sci Med Sport. 2014; 18(6): 673-677.. The variation in the level of cognitive commitment of activity and age4040. Best JR. Effects of physical activity on children's executive function: contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Dev Rev. 2010; 30(4): 331-351. may influence the development of the executive function. Recently, EF was included as a predictor of early academic achievement4141. Roebers CM, Röthlisberger M, Neuenschwander R, Cimeli P, Michel E, Jäger K. The relation between cognitive and motor performance and their relevance for children's transition to school: a latent variable approach. Hum Mov Sci. 2014; 33: 212-219..

In line with our main finding, the associations between physical activity and cognition, several mechanisms have been suggested to explain a beneficial effect of physical activity on cognition4242. Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008; 9(1): 58-65.. The effect of exercise on the brain could be the result of several factors including increased flow of blood and oxygen to the brain4343. Vicente-Campos D, Mora J, Castro-Pinero J, Gonzalez-Montesinos JL, Conde-Caveda J, Chicharro JL. Impact of a physical activity program on cerebral vasoreactivity in sedentary elderly people. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. 2012; 52(5): 537-544.. Other explanation supports the hypothesis of levels of chemicals and increased activity-dependent synaptic plasticity4444. Vivar C, Potter MC, van Praag H. All about running: synaptic plasticity, growth factors and adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2013; 15: 189-210.. Moreover, previous studies conducted with children have indicated increased efficiency in neural processing and changes in brain structures4545. Khan NA, Hillman CH. The Relation of Childhood Physical Activity and Aerobic Fitness to Brain Function and Cognition: A Review. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2014; 26(2): 138-146.), (4646. Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Castelli D, Etnier JL, Lee S, Tomporowski P, et al. Physical Activity, Fitness, Cognitive Function, and Academic Achievement in Children: A Systematic Review. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2016; 48(6): 1197-1222.. In addition, the moderate arousal level has been associated with the increased performance on the Trail-making test and these findings are discussed in light of the inverted-U hypothesis4747. Murray NP, Russoniell C. Acute Physical Activity on Cognitive Function: A Heart Rate Variability Examination. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2012 ; 37(4): 219-27..

Concerning the performance in the throwing task registered after the two experimental sessions, secondary findings of the present study showed that the mean scores and the standard deviations of the mean score (CV) were better after WS1 compared to WS2 and CS (respectively). After WS1 sessions, our results showed that means score were not significantly different in comparison with WS2. In addition, no significant difference was recorded between WS1 and WS2 in errors (number of zeros). However, the standard deviations of the mean score were significantly different only in LD after SW1 in comparison with WS2 conditions. It is difficult to compare our results with those in the scientific literature due the lack of previous study. In addition, the result of the present study could be attributed to the consistency improvement. The enhancement of the subject performance is linked to its ability to reduce disparities between the thrown. Strong positive correlations between the number of zeros scored and the standard deviation for only LD after WS2 support these findings.

Further explanation for the results considers that the improvement of psychomotor performance can be explained by the increase of the activity from the same groups of muscles essential to the test. In this context, the current results support the view that there is a trade-off between force and accuracy4848. Edwards B, Waterhouse J, Atkinson G, Reilly T. Effects of time of day and distance upon accuracy and consistency of throwing darts. J Sports Sci. 2007; 25(13): 1531-1538.), (4949. Roebers CM, Kauer M. Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year-olds. DEVELOPMENTAL SCI. 2009; 12(1): 175-181.. This compromise was found in other studies of darts and tennis and badminton serves5050. Atkinson G, Reilly T. Circadian variation in sports performance. Sports Med. 1996; 21(4): 292-312.), (5151. Etnyre BR. Accuracy characteristics of throwing as a result of maximum force effort. Percept Mot Skills. 1998; 86: 1211-1217.), (5252. Edwards B, Lindsay K, Waterhouse J. Effect of time of day on the accuracy and consistency of the badminton serve. Ergonomics. 2005; 48(11-14): 1488-98. but not in all studies of darts throwing task5353. Cauraugh J, Gabert T, White J. Tennis serving velocity and accuracy. Percept Mot Skills. 1990; 70(3): 719-722.. That is, these results appear to be in dispute with those of Elghoul et al. 1515. Elghoul Y, Frikha M, Masmoudi L, Chtourou H, Chaouachi A, Chamari K, et al. Diurnal variation of cognitive performance and perceived difficulty in dart-throwing performance in 9-10 year old boys. Biol Rhythm Res. 2014; 45(5): 789-801., and the improved psychomotor performance is correlated with executive function for the throws long distances.

In the morning, our results showed that PD values were significantly higher after the no-walking session in comparison with WS2 and WS1 conditions (p<0.001) respectively. However, the PD scores were significantly higher after no-WS both for SD and LD (p<0.001). In addition, no significant correlations between PD and performances measures were found. One of the main findings in the current study was that PD of a throwing darts task at a dartboard was better after WS1 in both SD and LD. It seems that the same task was perceived less difficult after SW1 than SW2 and no-WS conditions (respectively). Previous studies have shown that the concept of task difficulty is commonly introduced into psychological models dealing with motivation and emotion5454. Frömer R, Hafner V, Sommer W. Aiming for the bull's eye: Preparing for throwing investigated with event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology. 2012; 49(3): 335-344.. PD reflects mainly the amount of resources, or effort, that subjects have invested in the task in order to reach a given level of performance5555. Delignières D, Brisswalter J, Legros P. Influence of physical exercise on choice reaction time in sport experts: the mediating role of resource allocation. J Hum Movement Stud. 1994; 27(4): 173-188.. PD could be considered as an indicator of the amount of effort that a subject intends to invest and/or has invested on the task. This finding may be supported by mean scores result which better after WS1. Therefore, subjective goal-difficulty could have a positive effect on performance; in addition effects intensity walking, time of day, time awake, and the distance the darts need to be thrown5656. Elghoul Y, Frikha M, Abdelmlak S, Chtourou H, Dammak K, Chamari K, et al. Time of-day effect on dart-throwing performance and the perception of the difficulty of the task in 9-10 year-old boys. Biol Rhythm Res. 2014; 45(4): 523-532..

Beyond the contradiction regarding the effect of an active transport to school, gender and age of the participants3131. Chillon P, Ortega FB, Ruiz JR, Veidebaum T, Oja L, Mäestu J, et al. Active commuting to school in children and adolescents: an opportunity to increase physical activity and fitness. Scand J Public Health. 2010; 38(8): 873-879., our results induce new findings. It seems that 15 min duration of walking session at 50% of MAS causes fatigue to our subjects but allows better EF improvement. In addition, the active walk to school with low intensity (30% of MAS) was sufficient to produce positives changes in psychomotor performance and diminution in PD scores in comparison with passive transport to the school. The results of our study show that perceived difficulty supports this finding.

The strength of this study come from the use of objective measures to assess walking sessions with different intensities, which gives an accurate result and possible reflection in order to better understand what beneficial for children. Also, this study used several measures for cognitive processes, examining the relationships between cognitive and psychomotor performances. It is worth noting that effect sizes of this study were consistent with medium to large effect. However, this study focuses on fine motor coordination only. By including individual differences in gross motor coordination and physical activity level, the exact nature of the link between cognitive performance and psychomotor skills could be more resolved.

Conclusions

In conclusion, our results show evidence of a positive relationship of active walk to school with both cognitive and fine motor coordination performance. In addition, findings support decrease in the value of PD for the same psychomotor tasks. Additional explanations are needed to adjust the intensity and the duration of walking sessions to the demands of students or educators. However, more intervention studies are warranted in order to clarify possible mechanisms that made optimal relationship. In addition, it would be interesting to investigate whether using objective measures of commuting to school with different level of physical activity and tasks may provide more information on the relationships that we have found. The mode of transport to school can be retained as a source of differentiation between students.

References:

  • 1
    Pabayo R, Maximova K, Spence JC, Vander Ploeg K, Wu B, Veugelers PJ. The importance of Active Transportation to and from school for daily physical activity among children. Prev Med. 2012; 55(3): 196-200.
  • 2
    Tudor-Locke C, Ainsworth BE, Popkin BM. Active commuting to school: an overlooked source of childrens' physical activity? Sports Med. 2001; 31(5):309-313.
  • 3
    Heelan KA, Donnelly JE, Jacobsen DJ, Mayo MS, Washburn R, Greene L. Active commuting to and from school and BMI in elementary school children-preliminary data. Child Care Health Dev. 2005; 31(3): 341-349.
  • 4
    Dessing D, de Vries S, Graham JM, Pierik HF. Active transport between home and school assessed with GPS: a cross-sectional study among Dutch elementary school children. BMC Public Health. 2014; 14(1): 1-8.
  • 5
    Sirard JR, Riner Jr, McIver KL, Pate RR. Physical activity and active commuting to elementary school. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2005; 37(12): 2062-2069.
  • 6
    Cooper AR, Page AS, Wheeler BW, Griew P, Davis L, Hillsdon M, et al. Mapping the walk to school using accelerometry combined with a global positioning system. Am J Prev Med. 2010; 38( 2):178-183.
  • 7
    Lubans DR, Boreham CA, Kelly P, Foster CE. The relationship between active travel to school and health-related fitness in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Ac. 2011; 26: 8-5.
  • 8
    Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, Etnier JL. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012; 1453: 87-101.
  • 9
    McMorris T, Hale BJ. Differential effects of differing intensities of acute exercise on speed and accuracy of cognition: A meta-analytical investigation. Brain Cogn. 2012; 80(3): 338-51.
  • 10
    Van der Niet AG, Smith J, Sherder EJ, Oosterlaan J, Hartman E, Visscher C. Associations between daily physical activity and executive functioning in primary school-aged children. J Sci Med Sport. 2014; 18(6): 673-677.
  • 11
    Rigoli D, Piek JP, Kane R, Oosterlaan J. An examination of the relationship between motor coordination and executive functions in adolescents. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2012; 54(11): 1025-1031.
  • 12
    Grissmer D, Grimm KJ, Aiyer SM, Murrah WM, Steele JS. Fine motor skills and early comprehension of the world: Two new school readiness indicators. Dev Psychol. 2010; 46(5): 1008-1017.
  • 13
    Cameron CE, Brock LL, Murrah WM, Bell LH, Worzalla SL, Grissmer D, et al. Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to Kindergarten achievement. Child Development. 2012; 83(4): 1229-1244.
  • 14
    Aadland KN, Moe VF, Aadland E, Anderssen SA, Resaland GK, Ommundsen Y. Relationships between physical activity, sedentary time, aerobic fitness, motor skills and executive function and academic performance in children. Ment Health Phys Act. 2017; 12: 10-18.
  • 15
    Elghoul Y, Frikha M, Masmoudi L, Chtourou H, Chaouachi A, Chamari K, et al. Diurnal variation of cognitive performance and perceived difficulty in dart-throwing performance in 9-10 year old boys. Biol Rhythm Res. 2014; 45(5): 789-801.
  • 16
    Li Y, Belkin NJ. A faceted approach to conceptualizing tasks in information seeking. NFORM PROCESS MANAG. 2008; 44(6): 1822-1837.
  • 17
    Kim J. Task difficulty as a predictor and indicator of web searching interaction. Proceedings of CHI '06. 2006; April: 959-964.
  • 18
    Reis J, Schambra HM, Cohen LG, Buch ER, Fritsch B, Zarahn E, et al. Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc Nat Acad Sci. 2009; 106(5): 1590-1595.
  • 19
    Lindqvist S, Thorell LB. Brief report: manipulation of task difficulty in inhibitory control tasks. Child Neuropsychol. 2009; 15(1): 1-7.
  • 20
    Martinez-Gomez D, Ruiz JR, Gomez-Martinez S, Chillon P, Rey-Lopez JP, Diaz LE, et al. Active commuting to school and cognitive performance in adolescents: the AVENA study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011; 165(4): 300-305.
  • 21
    Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Barriers to children walking and biking to school-United States, 1999. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2002; 51(32): 701-704.
  • 22
    Dupont G, Defontaine M, Bosquet L, Blondel N, Moalla W, Berthoin, S. Yo-Yo intermittent recovery test versus the Université de Montréal Track Test: Relation with a high-intensity intermittent exercise. J Sci Med Sport. 2010; 13(1): 146-150.
  • 23
    Edwards B, Waterhouse J. Effects of one night of partial sleep deprivation upon diurnal rhythms of accuracy and consistency in throwing darts. Chronobiol Int, 2009; 26(4): 756-768.
  • 24
    Delignières D, Famose JP, Thépaut-Mathieu C, Fleurance P. A psychophysical study on difficulty ratings in rock climbing. Int J Sport Psychol. 1993; 24: 404-416.
  • 25
    Krustrup P, Mohr M, Amstrup T, Rysgaard T, Johansen J, Steensberg A, et al. The Yo-Yo Intermittent Recovery Test: Physiological response, reliability and validity. Med Sci Sport Exer. 2003; 35(4): 697-705.
  • 26
    Bangsbo J, Laia FM, Krustrup P. The Yo-Yo Intermittent Recovery Test. A Useful Tool for Evaluation of Physical Performance in Intermittent Sports. Sports Med. 2008; 38(1): 37-51.
  • 27
    Gauthier L, Dehaut F, Joanette Y. The bells test: a quantitative and qualitative test for visual neglect. Int J Clin Neuropsychol. 1989; 11(2): 49-54.
  • 28
    Corrigan JD, Hinkeldey MS. Relationships between parts A and B of the trail making test. J Clin Psychol. 1987; 43(4): 402-409.
  • 29
    Strauss E, Sherman EM, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. 3rd Ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2006.
  • 30
    Dobbs BM, Shergill SS. How effective is the trail making test (Parts A and B) in identifying cognitively impaired drivers? Age Ageing. 2013; 42(5): 577-581.
  • 31
    Chillon P, Ortega FB, Ruiz JR, Veidebaum T, Oja L, Mäestu J, et al. Active commuting to school in children and adolescents: an opportunity to increase physical activity and fitness. Scand J Public Health. 2010; 38(8): 873-879.
  • 32
    Tomporowski PD, Audiffren M. Dual-task performance in young and older adults: speed-accuracy radeoffs in choice responding while treadmill walking. Journal of aging And physical activity. 2014; 22(4): 557-63.
  • 33
    Kline JE, Poggensee K, Ferris DP. Your brain on speed: cognitive performance of a spatial working memory task is not affected by walking speed. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014; 8(8): 1-16.
  • 34
    Kamijo K, Takeda Y. Regular physical activity improves executive function duringtask switching in young adults. Int J Psychophysiol. 2010; 75(3): 304-311.
  • 35
    Padilla C, Perez L, Andres P, Parmentier FBR. Exercise improves cognitive control: evidence from the stop signal task. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2013; 27(4): 505-511.
  • 36
    Pérez L, Padilla C, Parmentier FBR, Andrés P. The effects of chronic exercise on attentional networks. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9(7): e101478. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101478
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101478
  • 37
    Woo E, Sharps MJ. Cognitive aging and physical exercise. Educ Gerontol. 2003; 29(4): 327-337.
  • 38
    Keeley TJ, Fox KR. The impact of physical activity and fitness on academic achievement and cognitive performance in children. Int Rev Sport Exerc Psychol. 2009; 2(2): 198-214.
  • 39
    Winneke AH, Godde B, Reuter EM, Vieluf S, Voelcker-Rehage C. The association between physical activity and attentional control in younger and older middle-aged adults: an ERP study. GeroPsych. 2012; 25(4): 207-221.
  • 40
    Best JR. Effects of physical activity on children's executive function: contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Dev Rev. 2010; 30(4): 331-351.
  • 41
    Roebers CM, Röthlisberger M, Neuenschwander R, Cimeli P, Michel E, Jäger K. The relation between cognitive and motor performance and their relevance for children's transition to school: a latent variable approach. Hum Mov Sci. 2014; 33: 212-219.
  • 42
    Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008; 9(1): 58-65.
  • 43
    Vicente-Campos D, Mora J, Castro-Pinero J, Gonzalez-Montesinos JL, Conde-Caveda J, Chicharro JL. Impact of a physical activity program on cerebral vasoreactivity in sedentary elderly people. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. 2012; 52(5): 537-544.
  • 44
    Vivar C, Potter MC, van Praag H. All about running: synaptic plasticity, growth factors and adult hippocampal neurogenesis. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2013; 15: 189-210.
  • 45
    Khan NA, Hillman CH. The Relation of Childhood Physical Activity and Aerobic Fitness to Brain Function and Cognition: A Review. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2014; 26(2): 138-146.
  • 46
    Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Castelli D, Etnier JL, Lee S, Tomporowski P, et al. Physical Activity, Fitness, Cognitive Function, and Academic Achievement in Children: A Systematic Review. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2016; 48(6): 1197-1222.
  • 47
    Murray NP, Russoniell C. Acute Physical Activity on Cognitive Function: A Heart Rate Variability Examination. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2012 ; 37(4): 219-27.
  • 48
    Edwards B, Waterhouse J, Atkinson G, Reilly T. Effects of time of day and distance upon accuracy and consistency of throwing darts. J Sports Sci. 2007; 25(13): 1531-1538.
  • 49
    Roebers CM, Kauer M. Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year-olds. DEVELOPMENTAL SCI. 2009; 12(1): 175-181.
  • 50
    Atkinson G, Reilly T. Circadian variation in sports performance. Sports Med. 1996; 21(4): 292-312.
  • 51
    Etnyre BR. Accuracy characteristics of throwing as a result of maximum force effort. Percept Mot Skills. 1998; 86: 1211-1217.
  • 52
    Edwards B, Lindsay K, Waterhouse J. Effect of time of day on the accuracy and consistency of the badminton serve. Ergonomics. 2005; 48(11-14): 1488-98.
  • 53
    Cauraugh J, Gabert T, White J. Tennis serving velocity and accuracy. Percept Mot Skills. 1990; 70(3): 719-722.
  • 54
    Frömer R, Hafner V, Sommer W. Aiming for the bull's eye: Preparing for throwing investigated with event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology. 2012; 49(3): 335-344.
  • 55
    Delignières D, Brisswalter J, Legros P. Influence of physical exercise on choice reaction time in sport experts: the mediating role of resource allocation. J Hum Movement Stud. 1994; 27(4): 173-188.
  • 56
    Elghoul Y, Frikha M, Abdelmlak S, Chtourou H, Dammak K, Chamari K, et al. Time of-day effect on dart-throwing performance and the perception of the difficulty of the task in 9-10 year-old boys. Biol Rhythm Res. 2014; 45(4): 523-532.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    23 Nov 2018
  • Date of issue
    2018

History

  • Received
    19 Apr 2018
  • Accepted
    16 July 2018
Universidade Estadual Paulista Universidade Estadual Paulista, Av. 24-A, 1515, 13506-900 Rio Claro, SP/Brasil, Tel.: (55 19) 3526-4330 - Rio Claro - SP - Brazil
E-mail: motriz.rc@unesp.br