Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Mediumship: review of quantitatives studies published in the 21st century

Abstract

Background

Mediumship can be defined as the alleged ability to communicate with deceased persons. The last decade has been particularly productive for this field of research and the study of mediumship can help the understanding of the human mind-brain relationship and provide objective data to the scientific community and to the general population.

Objective

The aim of this review is to summarize and discuss the results found on recent studies investigating mediumship. Furthermore, we aim to discuss the psychophysiology underlying mediumship and future perspectives for this study topic.

Methods

A literature search for articles in English, Portuguese and Spanish published from January 2000 up to June 2015 was conducted using three electronic databases (PubMed, Lilacs and Web of Science). Review articles, qualitative studies and studies investigating altered states of consciousness caused by psychoactive substances were excluded. The original search returned 150 articles, but the application of exclusion criteria resulted in the inclusion of 19 articles for final analysis.

Results

The general findings were: (1) an association of mediumship with good mental health, predominantly in experienced mediums, (2) heterogeneous findings regarding the ability of mediums to provide accurate information what may be due to different study methodologies and (3) incipient studies assessing physiological correlations during mediumistic communications (i.e. hypoactivation of brain regions responsible for cognitive processing and writing planning during psychography compared to a control task; electroencephalographic (EEG) changes and a slight predominance of the sympathetic nervous system).

Discussion

There is a paucity of empirical data available in this controversial research field. New studies employing rigorous design (e.g. triple-blind protocols to test accuracy of mediumistic communications), and sensitive methods are required.

Mediumship; after-death communication; spiritual experiences; mental health; psychophysiology

Introduction

Although there is no formal consensus on the definition of mediumship, it could be roughly defined as the alleged ability to communicate with deceased personalities on a regular basis1Rock AJ. Introduction: the medium and the message. In: Rock AJ. The survival hypothesis: essays on mediumship. 1st ed. Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company Inc.; 2013. p. 19., or as a set of experiences in which an individual, alleged “medium”, maintains regular contact or stays under the control of someone else who has already died or of another immaterial being2Menezes Jr. A, Alminhana L, Moreira-Almeida A. Perfil sociodemográfico e de experiências anômalas em indivíduos com vivências psicóticas e dissociativas em grupos religiosos. Rev Psiquiatr Clin. 2012;39(6):203-7..

At different times and among different cultures in human History there were reports of experiences that could be interpreted as mediumistic phenomena, for example the reception of the Ten Commandments by Moses at the Mount Sinai (Christianity and Judaism) and the reception of information and advice from superior and protector beings (dharmapalas) by Tibetan Buddhist monks3Plakun EM. Psychiatry in Tibetan Buddhism: madness and its cure seen through the lens of religious and national history. J Am Acad Psychoanal Dyn Psychiatry. 2008;36(3):415-30..

During the 19th century and in the early 20th century, many pioneers of modern Psychology and Psychiatry have studied cases of alleged mediums. This was the case of William James, who for many years studied the medium Leonora Piper4Sech Junior A, de Freitas Araujo S, Moreira-Almeida A. William James and psychical research: towards a radical science of mind. Hist Psychiatry. 2013;24(1):62-78.; Pierre Janet, who wrote a book about the “psychological automatism”5Crabtree A. “Automatism” and the emergence of dynamic psychiatry. J Hist Behav Sci. 2003;39(1):51-70.; Carl G. Jung, who received his doctorate degree after studying his cousin (S.W.), an alleged medium6Stein M. Introdução. In: Stein M. Jung: o mapa da alma – uma introdução. 9ª ed. São Paulo: Cultrix; 2012. p. 13., among others. At that time, other renowned scientists also dedicated their time and effort to study mediumistic phenomena, such as William Crookes7Kidd IJ. Was Sir William Crookes epistemically virtuous? Stud Hist Philos Biol Biomed Sci. 2014;48 Pt A:67-74.and Charles Richet8Alvarado CS, Machado FM, Zangari W, Zingrone NL. Perspectivas históricas da influência da mediunidade na construção de ideias psicológicas e psiquiátricas. Rev Psiquitr Clin. 2007;34(1):42-53..

Talking to “spirits” or being possessed by “spirits” are cultural phenomena found in almost every society worldwide and their relationship with mental illness has been the topic of extensive investigations in the fields of cross-cultural psychiatry and anthropology9Hunter J, Luke D, editors. Talking with the spirits: ethnographies from between the worlds. Brisbane, Australia: Daily Grail Publishing; 2014.,1010 Bourguignon E, editor. Religion, altered states of consciousness, and social change. Columbus (OH): Ohio State University Press; 1973.. Observational studies demonstrate a high prevalence (around 80 percent) of individuals who believe in the survival of the soul after death in general population1111 Greeley AM, Hout M. Americans’ increasing belief in life after death: religious competition and acculturation. American Sociological Review. 1999;64(6):813-35.,1212 Flannelly KJ, Ellison CG, Galek K, Koenig HG. Beliefs about life-after-death, psychiatric symptomology and cognitive theories of psychopathology. J Psychology and Theology. 2008;36(2):94-103., and this finding is congruent with the significant popularity that many cultural products covering this topic have (e.g.: books, films, novels, TV series)1313 Wilson K. A skeptic’s view of mediumship. In: Rock AJ. The survival hypothesis: essays on mediumship. 1st ed. Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company Inc.; 2013. p. 189..

At present, mediumship is considered to be a form of dissociation1414 Seligman R. Embodied experience : reconsidering the pathways to mediumship and mental health. Ethos. 2005;33(1):71-99.,1515 Peres JF, Moreira-Almeida A, Caixeta L, Leao F & Newberg A. Neuroimaging during trance state: a contribution to the study of dissociation. PLoS One. 2012;7(11).. Dissociation can be defined as “the occurrence of experiences and behaviors that are thought to exist apart from, or to have been disconnected from, the mainstream of one’s conscious awareness, behavioral repertoire, and/or self-concept”1616 Krippner S. Cross-cultural treatment perspectives on dissociative disorders. In: Lynn SJ, Rhue JW, editors. Dissociation: Clinical and theoretical perspectives. New York: Guilford; 1994. p. 338-61.. The majority of culture-bound dissociation is considered normal, does not lead to distress or impairment, it often arises in willing individuals in appropriate (frequently religious) contexts, and is usually experienced as beneficial. In subjects with pathologic dissociation, the experiences occur in an unsolicited, unruly and socially nontolerated manner, causing considerable distress, with adverse effects on occupational activities and other impairments1717 Somer E. Culture-bound dissociation: a comparative analysis. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2006;29(1):213-26..

Many societies where culture-bound dissociation is observed are characterized by an authoritarian male hierarchy, being oppressive for women and children. Some scholars affirm that in many of these societies, the dissociative experiences are more common in women and they propose that dissociation would be a psychological mechanism with an emotion-regulatory role. This is seen as a socially sanctioned (and even valued) behavior that would allow cathartic expression of forbidden and disowned feelings by individuals living under characteristically oppressive conditions1414 Seligman R. Embodied experience : reconsidering the pathways to mediumship and mental health. Ethos. 2005;33(1):71-99.,1717 Somer E. Culture-bound dissociation: a comparative analysis. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2006;29(1):213-26..

Self-hypnosis is one of the proposed psychophysiological mechanisms involved in dissociation. After carefully analyzing mediumship in the context of Candomblé, an Afro-Brazilian religion, Seligman (2005)1414 Seligman R. Embodied experience : reconsidering the pathways to mediumship and mental health. Ethos. 2005;33(1):71-99.concluded that mediums would be individuals with a psychophysiological tendency to somatization and to dissociation that, submitted to emotional distress, find in the religion system (and in the assumption of a new social role as mediums) a way for a positive transformation of their identities, leading to a moderation of their somatic and emotional afflictions and to a global therapeutic effect1414 Seligman R. Embodied experience : reconsidering the pathways to mediumship and mental health. Ethos. 2005;33(1):71-99.,1717 Somer E. Culture-bound dissociation: a comparative analysis. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2006;29(1):213-26..

Much of the controversy surrounding mediumship research derives from de fact that it stresses the “mind-brain problem”1818 Moreira-Almeida, A. Research on mediumship and the mind-brain relationship In: Moreira-Almeida A, Santos FS. Exploring frontiers of the mind-brain relationship. 1st ed. New York: Springer; 2012. p. 199.. The explanation for how the human brain generates mind is one of the greatest philosophical and scientific challenges of the past, the present, and probably of the future1919 Beauregard M, Schwartz GE, Miller L, Dossey L, Moreira-Almeida A, Schlitz M, et al. Manifesto for a post-materialist science. Explore (NY). 2014;10(5):272-4.. The French seventeenth century philosopher René Descartes put forth the theory of Substance Dualism, supporting the idea that mind (subjective) and body (objective) are different natural substances and that it is the bond of mind to the brain that allows it to think. Mind and brain being independent entities, consciousness could continue to exist (and perhaps communicate) despite the death of the physical body. On the other hand, the dominant materialist perspective argues that consciousness is a byproduct of brain activity, being extinguished with the death of the physical body. A better understanding of mind could only be possible through an increase on the knowledge about brain functioning2020 Chalmers D. The conscious mind: in search of a fundamental theory. New York: Oxford University Press Inc.; 1996.

21 Searle JR. Why I am not a property dualist. Journal of Consciousness Studies. 2002;9(12):57-64.
-2222 Brandão ML. Consciência. In: Brandão ML. Psicofisiologia. 3ª ed. São Paulo: Atheneu; 2012. p. 189..

Contemporary dualist philosophers and scientists sustain that materialism has left a range of questions unanswered, as, for example, how human subjective and affective experiences exert such a decisive influence over body functions. For many dualist and post-materialist scientists the evidences stemming from high quality studies about near-death experiences and about mediumistic communication are strong indicators that consciousness has a non-local character and could survive the death of the physical body1919 Beauregard M, Schwartz GE, Miller L, Dossey L, Moreira-Almeida A, Schlitz M, et al. Manifesto for a post-materialist science. Explore (NY). 2014;10(5):272-4..

Currently, the three most common explanations for mediumship are: (1) fraud (conscious or unconscious), (2) after-death communication, and (3) telepathy. The first, the materialist model, excludes the possibility that any aspect of the personality can survive physical death and rejects extra-sensorial perceptions (ESP) in general. Thus, if a mediumistic information seems accurate this can only result of deliberate fraud, coincidence or “cold reading” (fishing of information) by the medium. The second explanation, the spiritualist model, argues that the mind (or soul) of the medium communicates with the mind (or soul) of the deceased person through some form of telepathy, obtaining information. From the third perspective, generally defended by investigators of ESP, when an alleged medium is asked to communicate with a deceased person, the medium’s answers are given based on clairvoyant access to physical traits of the deceased personality and on telepathic access to the knowledge of the consultant (super-psi hypothesis). The supporters of this explanation, in general, also consider the survival as a plausible hypothesis requiring evidence-based proof2323 Tart CT. Mediumship: experimental approach to postmortem survival. In: Tart CT. The end of materialism: how evidence of the paranormal is bringing science and spirit together. 1st ed. Oakland, California: New Harbinger Publications Inc.; 2009. p. 257-76.. This variety of possible explanations, hard to differentiate one from another, poses significant challenges and complexity for the studies aiming to prove the reality of mediumistic phenomena.

In fact, the last decade has been particularly productive for this field of research and the study of mediumship can help the understanding of the human mind-brain relationship and provide objective data to the scientific community and to the general population. Within this context, the aim of this review is to summarize and discuss the recent results found on studies investigating mediumship. Furthermore, we aim to discuss the psychophysiology underlying mediumship and future perspectives for this study topic.

Methods

Literature review and selection of studies

The Literature search strategy was carried out in three phases, as described below.

Phase 1 (primary Literature search): two researchers (MAVBJ and IHSO) independently screened the list of citations (full text were retrieved for further analysis whenever necessary) to exclude studies that did not address the issue at hand. Any disagreement between the researchers were discussed with a third reviewer (GL), and resolved by reaching a consensus.

Articles in English, Portuguese and Spanish dealing with quantitative investigation of mediumship, published within the time range from January 1st, 2000 up to June 10th, 2015, were selected – hence focusing only on the contemporary scientific aspects of the topic.

Review articles, qualitative studies (either anthropological, theological or ethnographic), studies investigating altered states of consciousness caused by psychoactive substances and letters to the editor were excluded. Regarding the studies testing accuracy of mediumistic information, works that have not applied rigorous designs aiming to eliminate information leakage (i.e. those not strictly adhering to protocols that minimize as much as possible the probability of the alleged medium having access to information about the deceased personality via other common means or via fishing of information) were also excluded. Regarding psychophysiologic correlates of alleged mediumship, due to extreme paucity of available data, case reports were not excluded. All articles not fulfilling the inclusion criteria and which met the exclusion criteria were omitted from the final analysis.

The following databases were evaluated: Web of Science, PubMed and Lilacs. For the Web of Science database, filters were used to limit the search to fields of Psychiatry, Psychology, Neurosciences/Neurology, Religion and General Internal Medicine, as this database also indexes citations in arts and humanities, which are beyond the scope of this review.

The keywords used were: “mediumship”, “mediumistic” and “after-death communication”. Although a few important concepts and theories accrued from anthropological and ethnographic studies about Possession Trance are cited, this is not the focus of this review.

Phase 2 (secondary Literature search): with the aim of identifying studies that could have been missed in the primary search, references lists of initially included articles were screened and the same three keywords were evaluated in Google Scholar. Only the first 100 references from Google Scholar were evaluated. The same inclusion and exclusion criteria were applied in both phase 1 and phase 2.

Phase 3 (critical review of studies): the included articles were evaluated regarding number of participants, methods applied, comparisons and main findings. The studies were grouped in the following three categories based on their methodology and type of outcomes evaluated: (1) mediumship and psychopathology, (2) testing the accuracy of information provided by mediums and (3) psychophysiological correlates of mediumship. These categories were adopted based on the classification proposed by Jinks (2013)2424 Jinks T. The future of the field of mediumship. In: Rock AJ. The survival hypothesis: essays on mediumship. 1st ed. Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company Inc.; 2013.for quantitative research on mediumship.

Table 1andfigure 1summarize the search and selection strategy.

Table 1
Selection of studies – phase 1

Figure 1
PRISMA (The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis) flow diagram.

The last section of this review consist of future perspectives for the study of alleged mediumship, especially in its psychophysiological aspects, and due to paucity of available empirical data on this field, it was based on authors’ opinions and on recent reviews dealing with other dissociative and spiritual experiences.

Results and discussion

The phase 1 (primary Literature search) of the search led to the retrieval of a total of 150 articles. The application of the exclusion criteria resulted in the exclusion of 140 articles and the inclusion of 10 articles in this phase. As shown intable 1, the most frequent reasons for excluding articles, in descending order, were: historical perspective studies (34 articles), literature reviews (25 articles) and ethnographic studies (24 articles).

In phase 2 (secondary Literature search) of the search, screening of references lists and evaluating the keywords in Google Scholar led to the inclusion of 9 additional articles.

In phase 3 (critical review of studies), all 19 studies found were evaluated in terms of number of participants, methods applied, comparisons and main findings.

Tables 2,3and4briefly describe the 19 included studies. The main results are as follows:

Table 2
Studies on mediumship and psychopathology

Table 3
Studies on the accuracy of mediumistic information

Table 4
Studies involving psychophysiological correlates of mediumship

(1) Mediumship and psychopathology (Table 2)

Most studies suggest that mediums have good mental health, are socially well adapted and occupationally active. However, one must notice that all but one study2525 Roxburgh EC, Roe CA. A survey of dissociation, boundary-thinness and psychological wellbeing in spiritualist mental mediumship. J Parapsychol. 2011;75(2):279-99.took place in Brazil, so caution is necessary when trying to extrapolate these conclusions to other countries, since mediums have an established social role in Brazil2626 Moreira-Almeida A, Neto FL, Cardeña E. Comparison of Brazilian spiritist mediumship and dissociative identity disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2008;196(5):420-4., which may not be the case in other societies. It is known that a society’s dominant ideas about questions like the dualism, the soul and paranormality influences the way dissociative experiences are viewed and whether they are tolerated or not1717 Somer E. Culture-bound dissociation: a comparative analysis. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2006;29(1):213-26..

Our findings corroborate with the theory proposed by Seligman1414 Seligman R. Embodied experience : reconsidering the pathways to mediumship and mental health. Ethos. 2005;33(1):71-99.that affirms that mediumship has a therapeutic role to certain individuals under psychological distress, who find in the religious system a way for a positive transformation of their identities. The scores of mental health and control of the dissociative experiences are clearly different if the individuals are beginning to participate in the religion, as in the studies of Menezes Jr. et al.2Menezes Jr. A, Alminhana L, Moreira-Almeida A. Perfil sociodemográfico e de experiências anômalas em indivíduos com vivências psicóticas e dissociativas em grupos religiosos. Rev Psiquiatr Clin. 2012;39(6):203-7.and Alminhana et al.2727 Alminhana LO, Menezes Jr. A, Moreira-Almeida A. Personalidade, religiosidade e qualidade de vida em indivíduos que apresentam experiências anômalas em grupos religiosos. J Bras Psiquiatr. 2013;62(4):268-74., or if they have many years of practice as socially sanctioned mediums, as it was the case in all the other studies, being significantly worse in the former case.

However, the psychosocial stressors theory may not fully explain the Brazilian Spiritist participants, since this group has one of the country’s best socioeconomic status and best education level among all the other religious groups2828 Neri MC. Novo mapa das religiões. 1st ed. Rio de Janeiro: Fundação Getúlio Vargas/CPS; 2011.. For instance, in the study of Moreira-Almeida (2004)2929 Moreira-Almeida A. Fenomenologia das experiências mediúnicas, perfil e psicopatologia de médiuns espíritas [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Medicina, Departamento de Psiquiatria; 2004., the financial situation of Spiritist mediums was not different from mentally healthy control participants, highlighting that other possible explanation for this phenomenon may be considered1414 Seligman R. Embodied experience : reconsidering the pathways to mediumship and mental health. Ethos. 2005;33(1):71-99..

(2) Accuracy of information provided by mediums (Table 3)

Quantitative research on alleged mediums has been done for more than a hundred years, and meanwhile the designs of studies have been progressively improved as to eliminate information leakage and telepathy as explanations for the reception of accurate information by mediums2323 Tart CT. Mediumship: experimental approach to postmortem survival. In: Tart CT. The end of materialism: how evidence of the paranormal is bringing science and spirit together. 1st ed. Oakland, California: New Harbinger Publications Inc.; 2009. p. 257-76.. Research protocols reproduce the habitual situation of a friend or relative (the named “sitters”) of a deceased person consulting a claimed medium looking for postmortem information about this person, but they apply a “triple blind” design3030 Beischel J, Schwartz GE. Anomalous information reception by research mediums demonstrated using a novel triple-blind protocol. Explore (NY). 2007;3(1):23-7.. That is, mediums are blind to the identities of the consultant (“sitter”) and of his/her related-deceased person; the researcher interacting with the mediums (acting as a proxy for the consultant) also remains blind to the identities of the consultants and his/her related-deceased person; and consultants who score the accuracy of the “readings” remain blind about the information source (reading really intended for him/her vs decoy reading)1818 Moreira-Almeida, A. Research on mediumship and the mind-brain relationship In: Moreira-Almeida A, Santos FS. Exploring frontiers of the mind-brain relationship. 1st ed. New York: Springer; 2012. p. 199..

In the present review, only studies considered to have sufficient control against information leakage (i.e. which strictly adhered to triple-blind protocols) were included. Hence the works of Roy & Robertson3131 Roy AE, Robertson TJ. Results of the application of the Robertson-Roy protocol to a series of experiments with mediums and participants. J Soc Psychical Res. 2004;68(1):161-74.and Schwartz et al.3232 Schwartz GE, Geoffrion S, Jain S, Lewis S, Russek LG. Evidence of anomalous information retrieval between two mediums: replication in a double-blind design. J Soc Psych Res. 2003;67:115-30., although consist of important contributions as exploratory methods, were excluded (they applied methods where mediums had direct contact and talked to the consultants, among other shortcomings). Data from experiment 2 of the study of Kelly & Arcangel3333 Kelly EW, Arcangel D. An investigation of mediums who claim to give information about deceased persons. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2011;199(1):11-7.was also excluded from this review because of lack of strict adherence to triple-blind protocol (mediums received pictures from the deceased individuals, many of the sitters were colleagues or friends of one of the investigators, and this investigator knew some of the deceased individuals). Altogether the results of the well-controlled studies included in this review show: (a) in two studies3030 Beischel J, Schwartz GE. Anomalous information reception by research mediums demonstrated using a novel triple-blind protocol. Explore (NY). 2007;3(1):23-7.,3434 Beischel J, Boccuzzi M, Biuso M, Rock AJ. Anomalous information reception by research mediums under blinded conditions II: replication and extension. Explore (NY). 2015;11(2):136-42.(totaling 28 mediums and 102 readings) the sitters’ accuracy ratings of specific information from target readings were statistically higher than for decoy readings; and (b) in three3333 Kelly EW, Arcangel D. An investigation of mediums who claim to give information about deceased persons. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2011;199(1):11-7.,3535 O’Keeffe C, Wiseman R. Testing alleged mediumship: methods and results. Br J Psychol. 2005;96(Pt 2):165-79.,3636 Jensen CG, Cardeña E. A controlled long-distance test of a professional medium. Eur J Parapsychol. 2009;24(1):53-67.studies (totaling 10 mediums and 44 readings) there was no statistical difference in the fit scores assigned by sitters for the target or decoy readings.

Interestingly, the “triple-blind” design was used in all these studies, but there were slight methodological differences between them that can have influenced the outcomes. For example, in the studies with positive results, the sitters were asked to decide which was the fittest between only two readings, while in “negative studies” the sitters needed to decide among three to seven readings. Moreover, “positive” but not “negative” studies have used a pairing procedure of the deceased persons to optimize differences (in categories like age at passing, hobbies, physical description, cause of death). Hence, it seems that, for methodological reasons, choosing the target reading was less difficult for sitters of the “positive” than for “negative” studies.

Different from the others, the study of Rocha et al.3737 Rocha AC, Paraná D, Freire ES, Lotufo Neto F, Moreira-Almeida A. Investigating the fit and accuracy of alleged mediumistic writing: a case study of Chico Xavier’s letters. Explore (NY). 2014;10(5):300-8.did not use a controlled design, choosing a retrospective approach. Authors screened published and unpublished letters that were psychographed (allegedly written under the influence of a spirit) by a Brazilian medium, Chico Xavier, who died in 2002. Then, a set of 13 letters was selected because these letters contained much specific and objective information about a deceased person, and living relatives and friends of this person agreed to participate in interviews with the investigators to verify the fit and accuracy of the mediumistic information conveyed. Among 71 items of specific information extracted from the letters, a high percentage (97,2%) was scored as having a “clear and precise fit”. As authors acknowledge, one significant limitation of this type of study is that the scoring may have been subject to memory bias, as letters were written more than forty years before. However, three “drop-in” communications (“situations where allegedly a deceased personality communicates via the medium without the request of relatives or friends”3737 Rocha AC, Paraná D, Freire ES, Lotufo Neto F, Moreira-Almeida A. Investigating the fit and accuracy of alleged mediumistic writing: a case study of Chico Xavier’s letters. Explore (NY). 2014;10(5):300-8.) were identified in the letters. Pieces of specific information from these communications were objectively verified, and they proved to be true.

(3) Psychophysiological correlates of mediumship (Table 4)

The most common findings concerning psychophysiological correlates of mediumship were: increased noradrenalin levels during possession trance in a controlled study3838 Kawai N, Honda M, Nakamura S, Samatra P, Sukardika K, Nakatani Y, et al. Catecholamines and opioid peptides increase in plasma in humans during possession trances. Neuroreport. 2001;12(16):3419-23., increased muscle tone in a case report3939 Krippner S. Learning from the spirits: Candomblé, Umbanda, and Kardecismo in Recife, Brazil. Anthropology of Consciousness. 2008;19(1):1-32., increased heart rate3838 Kawai N, Honda M, Nakamura S, Samatra P, Sukardika K, Nakatani Y, et al. Catecholamines and opioid peptides increase in plasma in humans during possession trances. Neuroreport. 2001;12(16):3419-23.,3939 Krippner S. Learning from the spirits: Candomblé, Umbanda, and Kardecismo in Recife, Brazil. Anthropology of Consciousness. 2008;19(1):1-32.and increased spectral power in various EEG frequency bands during mediumistic communication and possession trance4040 Oohashi T, Kawai N, Honda M, Nakamura S, Morimoto M, Nishina E, et al. Electroencephalographic measurement of possession trance in the field. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;113(3):435-45.,4141 Delorme A, Beischel J, Michel L, Boccuzzi M, Radin D, Mills PJ. Electrocortical activity associated with subjective communication with the deceased. Front Psychol. 2013;4:834.. None of the studies included in this review has demonstrated an ictal pattern on EEG during the experience. These results could point to a state of mental and physical arousal during the phenomena4242 Heller W, Nitschke JB, Etienne MA, Miller GA. Patterns of regional brain activity differentiate types of anxiety. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106(3):376-85.,4343 Wallin BG, Esler M, Dorward P, Eisenhofer G, Ferrier C, Westerman R, et al. Simultaneous measurements of cardiac noradrenaline spillover and sympathetic outflow to skeletal muscle in humans. J Physiol. 1992;453:45-58..

On the other hand, the only study using functional neuroimaging, in this case a Single Positron Emission Tomography (SPECT), showed a reduction of cerebral blood flow (CBF) in left culmen, left hippocampus, left inferior occipital gyrus, left anterior cingulated cortex, right superior temporal gyrus and right pre-central gyrus (brain areas associated with memory, language and writing planning) during mediumistic communication (psychography) as compared to a control task. There was also a surprising negative correlation between the CBF in these areas and the linguistic complexity of the written text produced during psychography1515 Peres JF, Moreira-Almeida A, Caixeta L, Leao F & Newberg A. Neuroimaging during trance state: a contribution to the study of dissociation. PLoS One. 2012;7(11).. Taken together with the case series report of a slowing of background EEG activity in some mediums during psychophony (allegedly speaking under the influence of a spirit) and psychography4444 Hageman JJ, Peres JFP, Moreira-Almeida A, Caixeta L, Wickramasekera II I, Krippner S. The neurobiology of trance and mediumship in Brazil. In: Kripnner S, Friedman H. Mysterious minds: the neurobiology of psychics, mediums and other extraordinary people. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger; 2010. p. 85-111.these data could suggest the activation of fewer neuronal populations during the dissociative experience.

Considering the dominant current psychobiological explanation for the possession trance and mediumship, there is a number of authors, which propose the role of self-hypnosis1414 Seligman R. Embodied experience : reconsidering the pathways to mediumship and mental health. Ethos. 2005;33(1):71-99.,1717 Somer E. Culture-bound dissociation: a comparative analysis. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2006;29(1):213-26.. Hypnosis is seen as a state of focused attention and concentration, inner absorption and a relative suspension of external awareness. There is evidence for a correlation between hypnotizability and number of dissociative symptoms and that patients with dissociative disorders have higher rate of hypnotizability. Although differences occur according to the phase of hypnotic procedure (relaxation – induction – suggestion – waking up) and to which actions are suggested to the individual, many studies show increased theta power during hypnosis, indicating an intensification of attention processes. Regarding functional neuroimaging, the most characteristic finding of the hypnotic state is the hypoactivity of the “extrinsic brain network”, which encompasses lateral fronto-parietal regions and which has been linked to cognitive processes of external sensory input. The reduced activity of this network can reflect a blocking of the sensorial systems to receive stimulus. A structural Magnetic Ressonance study has shown neuro-anatomical differences between high and low hypnotizable individuals: larger (32%) rostrum of the corpus callosum in highs than in lows. This area is involved in the information transfer between pre-frontal cortices and in attention allocation. The authors suggest that high hypnotizable subjects would have more efficient frontal attentional systems, with greater ability to monitor performance and to inhibit undesirable stimulus for conscious awareness4545 Vanhaudenhuyse A, Laureys S, Faymonville ME. Neurophysiology of hypnosis. Neurophysiol Clin. 2014;44(4):343-53..

Although it is not possible to say that spontaneous dissociative symptoms, like mediumship, share strictly the same neurophysiological processes of hypnosis, evidence suggest that many parallels can be drawn between them4545 Vanhaudenhuyse A, Laureys S, Faymonville ME. Neurophysiology of hypnosis. Neurophysiol Clin. 2014;44(4):343-53.. There are already some reports of research groups using hypnosis on healthy individuals to “model” dissociative symptoms (e.g. “automatic writing”) and more easily study them in the neurophysiology and neuroimaging lab4646 Deeley Q, Walsh E, Oakley DA, Bell V, Koppel C, Mehta MA, et al. Using hypnotic suggestion to model loss of control and awareness of movements: an exploratory FMRI study. PLoS One. 2013;8(10):e78324.,4747 Deeley Q, Oakley DA, Walsh E, Bell V, Mehta MA, Halligan PW. Modelling psychiatric and cultural possession phenomena with suggestion and fMRI. Cortex. 2014;53:107-19.. This strategy might bring valuable complementary data for the understanding of the neurobiology of alleged mediumship.

Future perspectives

Mediumship is a controversial research topic with ongoing experiments. Regarding its relation to psychopathology, in practical terms, it is important to avoid the risk of a “category fallacy”. In other words, here both risks exist and should be avoided: pathologizing normal religious experiences and considering potentially dangerous clinical conditions as a common religious/spiritual experience. Thus, if such cases of mediumship and possession come to medical attention, besides a comprehensive assessment of the sociocultural implications and of the possible secondary gains for the individual, a standard psychiatric evaluation should not be neglected. In this evaluation, not only the patient but also family and community members should be interviewed, so that the extent of impairment of daily life by the dissociative experiences can be inferred. Possible psychiatric comorbidities should also be sought. The most commonly used type of therapy for possession disorders is psychotherapy, and some scholars advise it should focus on the stressing factors that can initiate the dissociative phenomena4848 During EH, Elahi FM, Taieb O, Moro MR, Baubet T. A critical review of dissociative trance and possession disorders: etiological, diagnostic, therapeutic, and nosological issues. Can J Psychiatry. 2011;56(4):235-42.,4949 Littlewood R. Possession states. Psychiatry. 2004;3(8):8-10..

Concerning studies on the accuracy of information provided by mediums, their design must reach a balance between control against information leakage and the provision of a research environment that optimizes the phenomenon for both the medium and for the alleged communicating deceased person3636 Jensen CG, Cardeña E. A controlled long-distance test of a professional medium. Eur J Parapsychol. 2009;24(1):53-67.,5050 Beischel J. Contemporary methods used in laboratory-based mediumship research. J Parapsychol, 2007;71:37-68.

51 Beischel J. Advances in quantitative mediumship research. In: Rock AJ. The survival hypothesis: essays on mediumship. 1st ed. Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company Inc.; 2013.
-5252 Peres JFP, Newberg A. Neuroimagem e mediunidade: uma promissora linha de pesquisa. Rev Psiquiatr Clin. 2013;40(6):225-32.. Apparently, among the reviewed studies, the protocol proposed by Beischel is the most successful in meeting these conditions, and it requires replication by other research groups to broaden the database and to eventually demonstrate or not the scientific evidence of mediumship. Notwithstanding, some investigators affirm that even accurate results of mediumship studies do not prove the postmortem survival hypothesis and that the best explanation would still be some form of telepathy between the medium and other “living agent psi”5353 Sudduth M. Is postmortem survival the best explanation of the data of mediumship? In: Rock AJ. The survival hypothesis: essays on mediumship. 1st ed. Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company Inc.; 2013..

Studies evaluating physiological correlations during mediumistic communications are still very incipient. Here, as for other types of psychophysiologic studies, the employment of a process-based approach is recommended5454 Beischel J, Rock AJ. Adressing the survival versus psi debate through process-focused mediumship research. J Parapsychol. 2009;73:71-90.. Before initiating any data collection, a detailed definition of the phenomenon, based on systematic observation, is necessary. This should include a definition, as precise as possible, of when the mediumistic communication begins and when it ends. Proper initial definitions about the process under investigation are necessary if one aims to obtain valid physiological research data about it5555 O’Connor K. Towards a process paradigm in psychophysiology. Int J Psychophysiol. 1990;9:209-23..

Moreover, because spiritual and religious experiences are complex and multidimensional, and because mediums usually describe them in terms of changes in perception, cognition and affect5252 Peres JFP, Newberg A. Neuroimagem e mediunidade: uma promissora linha de pesquisa. Rev Psiquiatr Clin. 2013;40(6):225-32., the measurement of the subjective elements of the phenomena is highly advisable. Posteriorly, a possible correlation among these psychological, affective and cognitive elements with specific brain regions and brain functions can be explored5656 Newberg AB. The neuroscientific study of spiritual practices. Front Psychol. 2014;5:215.,5757 Rock AJ, Krippner S. States of consciousness redefined as patterns of phenomenal properties: an experimental application. In: Cvetkovic D, Cosic I. States of consciousness: experimental insights into meditation, waking, sleep and dreams. 1st ed. Berlin Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag; 2011. p. 264-5.. Some examples of instruments that can be used for this purpose are: the Phenomenology of Consciousness Inventory (PCI)5858 Pekala RJ. Quantifying consciousness: an empirical approach. 1st ed. New York: Plenum Press; 1991., the Assessment Schedule for Altered States of Consciousness (ASASC)5959 van Quekelberghe R, Altstotter-Gleich C, Hertweck E. Assessment schedule for altered states of consciousness: a brief report. J Parapsychol. 1991;55:37790.and Hartmann’s Boundary Questionnaire6060 Hartmann E, Ebldo R, Gargl M. Personality and dreaming : the dreams of people with very thick or very thin boundaries. Dreaming. 1991;1(4):311-24., among many others. These questionnaires quantify different phenomenological elements of consciousness and can be useful for helping to distinguish qualitatively different ASC5757 Rock AJ, Krippner S. States of consciousness redefined as patterns of phenomenal properties: an experimental application. In: Cvetkovic D, Cosic I. States of consciousness: experimental insights into meditation, waking, sleep and dreams. 1st ed. Berlin Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag; 2011. p. 264-5.. Likewise, considering that mediumistic phenomena usually involve dissociative experiences, it is also advisable to conduct a baseline assessment of the personality trait absorption. For this purpose, questionnaires such as the Tellegen Absorption Scale6161 Tellegen A, Atkinson G. Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. J Abnorm Psychol. 1974;83(3):268-77.or the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility6262 Shor RE, Orne EC. Norms on the Harvard group scale of hypnotic susceptibility, form A. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1963;11:39-47.could be used. Evidence suggests that individual differences in these characteristics are associated with significant differences in neurological findings6363 Reiser EM, Schulter G, Weiss EM, Fink A, Rominger C, Papousek I. Decrease of prefrontal-posterior EEG coherence: loose control during social-emotional stimulation. Brain Cogn. 2012;80(1):144-54.,6464 Passamonti L, Terracciano A, Riccelli R, Donzuso G, Cerasa A, Vaccaro M, et al. A. Increased functional connectivity within mesocortical networks in open people. Neuroimage. 2015;104:301-9..

Concerning the possibility of studying mediumship through functional neuroimaging technique, one important aspect is the selection of the control task. Usually the participant acts as his/her own control, being asked to perform two similar tasks, one with and another without spiritual (mediumistic in this case) connotation. This allows the control task to be comparable to mediumistic practice with respect to a range of elements (open or closed eyes, talking or not talking, listening or not listening) and potentially optimizes de identification of characteristic neurophysiological correlates of the mediumistic experience5656 Newberg AB. The neuroscientific study of spiritual practices. Front Psychol. 2014;5:215.. The decision of which specific method to use depends on availability and financial possibilities5252 Peres JFP, Newberg A. Neuroimagem e mediunidade: uma promissora linha de pesquisa. Rev Psiquiatr Clin. 2013;40(6):225-32.but SPECT (Single-Photon Emission Computed Tomography) and PET (Positron Emission Tomography) techniques are usually preferred because images can be captured after the completion of the event under investigation (not simultaneously as it is necessary for functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging [MRI]). In addition, the noise during MRI may disturb mediums, interfering with the phenomenon5656 Newberg AB. The neuroscientific study of spiritual practices. Front Psychol. 2014;5:215..

The electroencephalography (EEG) can also offer important information about the degree of brain activation during the mediumistic practice, particularly of cortical regions. Unlike neuroimaging techniques, which provide excellent spatial location of cerebral functioning but less precise timing information, the EEG provides greater time precision (accuracy of milliseconds, in real time) with much lesser spatial accuracy6565 Lane RD, Waldstein SR, Chesney MA, Jennings JR, Lovallo WR, Kozel PJ, et al. The rebirth of neuroscience in psychosomatic medicine, Part I: historical context, methods, and relevant basic science. Psychosom Med. 2009;71(2):117-34.. However, because EEG recording requires stillness to avoid muscle artifacts, and as mediumship usually involves talking and moving, there might be some technical difficulties.

The objective measurement of physiological parameters that may vary according to the emotions experienced (e.g. heart rate variability – HRV)6666 Lane RD, McRae K, Reiman EM, Chen K, Ahern GL, Thayer JF. Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. Neuroimage. 2009;44(1):213-22.,6767 van der Kruijs SJ, Bodde NM, Carrette E, Lazeron RH, Vonck KE, Boon PA, et al. Neurophysiological correlates of dissociative symptoms. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2014;85(2):174-9.could also bring relevant contributions to this research field. These parameters can be correlated with subjective self-reports and with neuroimaging findings, bringing to sight a more global physiological picture of the mediumistic phenomenon5656 Newberg AB. The neuroscientific study of spiritual practices. Front Psychol. 2014;5:215.. Measurements related to the immune and endocrine systems may also contribute to broaden this understanding6868 Matsunaga M, Isowa T, Kimura K, Miyakoshi M, Kanayama N, Murakami H, et al. Associations among central nervous, endocrine, and immune activities when positive emotions are elicited by looking at a favorite person. Brain Behav Immun. 2008;22(3):408-17.,6969 Liou CH, Hsieh CW, Hsieh CH, Chen DY, Wang CH, Chen JH, et al. Detection of nighttime melatonin level in Chinese Original Quiet Sitting. J Formos Med Assoc. 2010;109(10):694-701..

Finally, it should be taken into account that different mediumistic modalities may require different study designs. Psychography and psychopictography (allegedly painting under the influence of a spirit) may adapt more easily to an experimental protocol employing functional neuroimaging. In regard of psychophony, although it may represent a larger universe of potential research participants (great prevalence of “spirit release therapy” sessions employing psychophonic mediums in countries such as Brazil7070 Lucchetti G, Aguiar PR, Braghetta CC, Vallada CP, Moreira-Almeida A, Vallada H. Spiritist psychiatric hospitals in Brazil: integration of conventional psychiatric treatment and spiritual complementary therapy. Cult Med Psychiatry. 2012;36(1):124-35.,7171 Lucchetti ALG. Descrição da terapia complementar religiosa em centros espíritas da cidade de São Paulo com ênfase na abordagem sobre problemas de saúde mental [dissertação]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Medicina, Programa de Psiquiatria; 2013.and Puerto Rico7272 Hohmann AA, Richeport M, Marriott BM, Canino GJ, Rubio-Stipec M, Bird H. Spiritism in Puerto Rico. Results of an island-wide community study. Br J Psychiatry. 1990;156:328-35.), it offers obstacles to neuroimaging studies as it usually requires team work. In these cases, protocols involving EEG and ECG may be preferred as they allow researchers to record the event “in loco” using portable equipment.

Limitations

The present study has some limitations, which should be highlighted. First, the searches were limited to English, Spanish and Portuguese; therefore potentially relevant articles published in other languages were not included. Second, although the terms searched are the most frequently used ones, and although three databases were used, potentially relevant articles not indexed in these databases or described with other terms may have been missed.

Conclusions

The evidence analyzed in this review suggests: (1) mediumship seems to be associated with good mental health, mainly when individuals have many years of work as socially sanctioned mediums (2) there was heterogeneous evidence regarding the accuracy of information provided by alleged mediums, what may be due to different study methodologies, and (3) there is a paucity of studies investigating the physiologic correlates of mediumship. These results emphasize the need for more studies in this field to further elucidate mediumistic experiences, what could broaden the understanding of the mind-brain relationship.

References

  • 1
    Rock AJ. Introduction: the medium and the message. In: Rock AJ. The survival hypothesis: essays on mediumship. 1st ed. Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company Inc.; 2013. p. 19.
  • 2
    Menezes Jr. A, Alminhana L, Moreira-Almeida A. Perfil sociodemográfico e de experiências anômalas em indivíduos com vivências psicóticas e dissociativas em grupos religiosos. Rev Psiquiatr Clin. 2012;39(6):203-7.
  • 3
    Plakun EM. Psychiatry in Tibetan Buddhism: madness and its cure seen through the lens of religious and national history. J Am Acad Psychoanal Dyn Psychiatry. 2008;36(3):415-30.
  • 4
    Sech Junior A, de Freitas Araujo S, Moreira-Almeida A. William James and psychical research: towards a radical science of mind. Hist Psychiatry. 2013;24(1):62-78.
  • 5
    Crabtree A. “Automatism” and the emergence of dynamic psychiatry. J Hist Behav Sci. 2003;39(1):51-70.
  • 6
    Stein M. Introdução. In: Stein M. Jung: o mapa da alma – uma introdução. 9ª ed. São Paulo: Cultrix; 2012. p. 13.
  • 7
    Kidd IJ. Was Sir William Crookes epistemically virtuous? Stud Hist Philos Biol Biomed Sci. 2014;48 Pt A:67-74.
  • 8
    Alvarado CS, Machado FM, Zangari W, Zingrone NL. Perspectivas históricas da influência da mediunidade na construção de ideias psicológicas e psiquiátricas. Rev Psiquitr Clin. 2007;34(1):42-53.
  • 9
    Hunter J, Luke D, editors. Talking with the spirits: ethnographies from between the worlds. Brisbane, Australia: Daily Grail Publishing; 2014.
  • 10
    Bourguignon E, editor. Religion, altered states of consciousness, and social change. Columbus (OH): Ohio State University Press; 1973.
  • 11
    Greeley AM, Hout M. Americans’ increasing belief in life after death: religious competition and acculturation. American Sociological Review. 1999;64(6):813-35.
  • 12
    Flannelly KJ, Ellison CG, Galek K, Koenig HG. Beliefs about life-after-death, psychiatric symptomology and cognitive theories of psychopathology. J Psychology and Theology. 2008;36(2):94-103.
  • 13
    Wilson K. A skeptic’s view of mediumship. In: Rock AJ. The survival hypothesis: essays on mediumship. 1st ed. Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company Inc.; 2013. p. 189.
  • 14
    Seligman R. Embodied experience : reconsidering the pathways to mediumship and mental health. Ethos. 2005;33(1):71-99.
  • 15
    Peres JF, Moreira-Almeida A, Caixeta L, Leao F & Newberg A. Neuroimaging during trance state: a contribution to the study of dissociation. PLoS One. 2012;7(11).
  • 16
    Krippner S. Cross-cultural treatment perspectives on dissociative disorders. In: Lynn SJ, Rhue JW, editors. Dissociation: Clinical and theoretical perspectives. New York: Guilford; 1994. p. 338-61.
  • 17
    Somer E. Culture-bound dissociation: a comparative analysis. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2006;29(1):213-26.
  • 18
    Moreira-Almeida, A. Research on mediumship and the mind-brain relationship In: Moreira-Almeida A, Santos FS. Exploring frontiers of the mind-brain relationship. 1st ed. New York: Springer; 2012. p. 199.
  • 19
    Beauregard M, Schwartz GE, Miller L, Dossey L, Moreira-Almeida A, Schlitz M, et al. Manifesto for a post-materialist science. Explore (NY). 2014;10(5):272-4.
  • 20
    Chalmers D. The conscious mind: in search of a fundamental theory. New York: Oxford University Press Inc.; 1996.
  • 21
    Searle JR. Why I am not a property dualist. Journal of Consciousness Studies. 2002;9(12):57-64.
  • 22
    Brandão ML. Consciência. In: Brandão ML. Psicofisiologia. 3ª ed. São Paulo: Atheneu; 2012. p. 189.
  • 23
    Tart CT. Mediumship: experimental approach to postmortem survival. In: Tart CT. The end of materialism: how evidence of the paranormal is bringing science and spirit together. 1st ed. Oakland, California: New Harbinger Publications Inc.; 2009. p. 257-76.
  • 24
    Jinks T. The future of the field of mediumship. In: Rock AJ. The survival hypothesis: essays on mediumship. 1st ed. Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company Inc.; 2013.
  • 25
    Roxburgh EC, Roe CA. A survey of dissociation, boundary-thinness and psychological wellbeing in spiritualist mental mediumship. J Parapsychol. 2011;75(2):279-99.
  • 26
    Moreira-Almeida A, Neto FL, Cardeña E. Comparison of Brazilian spiritist mediumship and dissociative identity disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2008;196(5):420-4.
  • 27
    Alminhana LO, Menezes Jr. A, Moreira-Almeida A. Personalidade, religiosidade e qualidade de vida em indivíduos que apresentam experiências anômalas em grupos religiosos. J Bras Psiquiatr. 2013;62(4):268-74.
  • 28
    Neri MC. Novo mapa das religiões. 1st ed. Rio de Janeiro: Fundação Getúlio Vargas/CPS; 2011.
  • 29
    Moreira-Almeida A. Fenomenologia das experiências mediúnicas, perfil e psicopatologia de médiuns espíritas [tese]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Medicina, Departamento de Psiquiatria; 2004.
  • 30
    Beischel J, Schwartz GE. Anomalous information reception by research mediums demonstrated using a novel triple-blind protocol. Explore (NY). 2007;3(1):23-7.
  • 31
    Roy AE, Robertson TJ. Results of the application of the Robertson-Roy protocol to a series of experiments with mediums and participants. J Soc Psychical Res. 2004;68(1):161-74.
  • 32
    Schwartz GE, Geoffrion S, Jain S, Lewis S, Russek LG. Evidence of anomalous information retrieval between two mediums: replication in a double-blind design. J Soc Psych Res. 2003;67:115-30.
  • 33
    Kelly EW, Arcangel D. An investigation of mediums who claim to give information about deceased persons. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2011;199(1):11-7.
  • 34
    Beischel J, Boccuzzi M, Biuso M, Rock AJ. Anomalous information reception by research mediums under blinded conditions II: replication and extension. Explore (NY). 2015;11(2):136-42.
  • 35
    O’Keeffe C, Wiseman R. Testing alleged mediumship: methods and results. Br J Psychol. 2005;96(Pt 2):165-79.
  • 36
    Jensen CG, Cardeña E. A controlled long-distance test of a professional medium. Eur J Parapsychol. 2009;24(1):53-67.
  • 37
    Rocha AC, Paraná D, Freire ES, Lotufo Neto F, Moreira-Almeida A. Investigating the fit and accuracy of alleged mediumistic writing: a case study of Chico Xavier’s letters. Explore (NY). 2014;10(5):300-8.
  • 38
    Kawai N, Honda M, Nakamura S, Samatra P, Sukardika K, Nakatani Y, et al. Catecholamines and opioid peptides increase in plasma in humans during possession trances. Neuroreport. 2001;12(16):3419-23.
  • 39
    Krippner S. Learning from the spirits: Candomblé, Umbanda, and Kardecismo in Recife, Brazil. Anthropology of Consciousness. 2008;19(1):1-32.
  • 40
    Oohashi T, Kawai N, Honda M, Nakamura S, Morimoto M, Nishina E, et al. Electroencephalographic measurement of possession trance in the field. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002;113(3):435-45.
  • 41
    Delorme A, Beischel J, Michel L, Boccuzzi M, Radin D, Mills PJ. Electrocortical activity associated with subjective communication with the deceased. Front Psychol. 2013;4:834.
  • 42
    Heller W, Nitschke JB, Etienne MA, Miller GA. Patterns of regional brain activity differentiate types of anxiety. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106(3):376-85.
  • 43
    Wallin BG, Esler M, Dorward P, Eisenhofer G, Ferrier C, Westerman R, et al. Simultaneous measurements of cardiac noradrenaline spillover and sympathetic outflow to skeletal muscle in humans. J Physiol. 1992;453:45-58.
  • 44
    Hageman JJ, Peres JFP, Moreira-Almeida A, Caixeta L, Wickramasekera II I, Krippner S. The neurobiology of trance and mediumship in Brazil. In: Kripnner S, Friedman H. Mysterious minds: the neurobiology of psychics, mediums and other extraordinary people. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger; 2010. p. 85-111.
  • 45
    Vanhaudenhuyse A, Laureys S, Faymonville ME. Neurophysiology of hypnosis. Neurophysiol Clin. 2014;44(4):343-53.
  • 46
    Deeley Q, Walsh E, Oakley DA, Bell V, Koppel C, Mehta MA, et al. Using hypnotic suggestion to model loss of control and awareness of movements: an exploratory FMRI study. PLoS One. 2013;8(10):e78324.
  • 47
    Deeley Q, Oakley DA, Walsh E, Bell V, Mehta MA, Halligan PW. Modelling psychiatric and cultural possession phenomena with suggestion and fMRI. Cortex. 2014;53:107-19.
  • 48
    During EH, Elahi FM, Taieb O, Moro MR, Baubet T. A critical review of dissociative trance and possession disorders: etiological, diagnostic, therapeutic, and nosological issues. Can J Psychiatry. 2011;56(4):235-42.
  • 49
    Littlewood R. Possession states. Psychiatry. 2004;3(8):8-10.
  • 50
    Beischel J. Contemporary methods used in laboratory-based mediumship research. J Parapsychol, 2007;71:37-68.
  • 51
    Beischel J. Advances in quantitative mediumship research. In: Rock AJ. The survival hypothesis: essays on mediumship. 1st ed. Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company Inc.; 2013.
  • 52
    Peres JFP, Newberg A. Neuroimagem e mediunidade: uma promissora linha de pesquisa. Rev Psiquiatr Clin. 2013;40(6):225-32.
  • 53
    Sudduth M. Is postmortem survival the best explanation of the data of mediumship? In: Rock AJ. The survival hypothesis: essays on mediumship. 1st ed. Jefferson, North Carolina: McFarland & Company Inc.; 2013.
  • 54
    Beischel J, Rock AJ. Adressing the survival versus psi debate through process-focused mediumship research. J Parapsychol. 2009;73:71-90.
  • 55
    O’Connor K. Towards a process paradigm in psychophysiology. Int J Psychophysiol. 1990;9:209-23.
  • 56
    Newberg AB. The neuroscientific study of spiritual practices. Front Psychol. 2014;5:215.
  • 57
    Rock AJ, Krippner S. States of consciousness redefined as patterns of phenomenal properties: an experimental application. In: Cvetkovic D, Cosic I. States of consciousness: experimental insights into meditation, waking, sleep and dreams. 1st ed. Berlin Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag; 2011. p. 264-5.
  • 58
    Pekala RJ. Quantifying consciousness: an empirical approach. 1st ed. New York: Plenum Press; 1991.
  • 59
    van Quekelberghe R, Altstotter-Gleich C, Hertweck E. Assessment schedule for altered states of consciousness: a brief report. J Parapsychol. 1991;55:37790.
  • 60
    Hartmann E, Ebldo R, Gargl M. Personality and dreaming : the dreams of people with very thick or very thin boundaries. Dreaming. 1991;1(4):311-24.
  • 61
    Tellegen A, Atkinson G. Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. J Abnorm Psychol. 1974;83(3):268-77.
  • 62
    Shor RE, Orne EC. Norms on the Harvard group scale of hypnotic susceptibility, form A. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1963;11:39-47.
  • 63
    Reiser EM, Schulter G, Weiss EM, Fink A, Rominger C, Papousek I. Decrease of prefrontal-posterior EEG coherence: loose control during social-emotional stimulation. Brain Cogn. 2012;80(1):144-54.
  • 64
    Passamonti L, Terracciano A, Riccelli R, Donzuso G, Cerasa A, Vaccaro M, et al. A. Increased functional connectivity within mesocortical networks in open people. Neuroimage. 2015;104:301-9.
  • 65
    Lane RD, Waldstein SR, Chesney MA, Jennings JR, Lovallo WR, Kozel PJ, et al. The rebirth of neuroscience in psychosomatic medicine, Part I: historical context, methods, and relevant basic science. Psychosom Med. 2009;71(2):117-34.
  • 66
    Lane RD, McRae K, Reiman EM, Chen K, Ahern GL, Thayer JF. Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. Neuroimage. 2009;44(1):213-22.
  • 67
    van der Kruijs SJ, Bodde NM, Carrette E, Lazeron RH, Vonck KE, Boon PA, et al. Neurophysiological correlates of dissociative symptoms. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2014;85(2):174-9.
  • 68
    Matsunaga M, Isowa T, Kimura K, Miyakoshi M, Kanayama N, Murakami H, et al. Associations among central nervous, endocrine, and immune activities when positive emotions are elicited by looking at a favorite person. Brain Behav Immun. 2008;22(3):408-17.
  • 69
    Liou CH, Hsieh CW, Hsieh CH, Chen DY, Wang CH, Chen JH, et al. Detection of nighttime melatonin level in Chinese Original Quiet Sitting. J Formos Med Assoc. 2010;109(10):694-701.
  • 70
    Lucchetti G, Aguiar PR, Braghetta CC, Vallada CP, Moreira-Almeida A, Vallada H. Spiritist psychiatric hospitals in Brazil: integration of conventional psychiatric treatment and spiritual complementary therapy. Cult Med Psychiatry. 2012;36(1):124-35.
  • 71
    Lucchetti ALG. Descrição da terapia complementar religiosa em centros espíritas da cidade de São Paulo com ênfase na abordagem sobre problemas de saúde mental [dissertação]. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Medicina, Programa de Psiquiatria; 2013.
  • 72
    Hohmann AA, Richeport M, Marriott BM, Canino GJ, Rubio-Stipec M, Bird H. Spiritism in Puerto Rico. Results of an island-wide community study. Br J Psychiatry. 1990;156:328-35.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Sep-Oct 2015

History

  • Received
    30 June 2015
  • Accepted
    8 Oct 2015
Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo Rua Ovídio Pires de Campos, 785 , 05403-010 São Paulo SP Brasil, Tel./Fax: +55 11 2661-8011 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: archives@usp.br