Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Psychometric properties of the Negative Stereotypes Towards Aging Questionnaire (CENVE) among a sample of Portuguese adults

Abstract

Stereotyped beliefs about old age and the aging process have proven to strongly promote negative behaviors toward the elderly, with unfavorable influences on their mental and physical health. Therefore, it is important to assess negative aging attitudes with brief but reliable and validated measurement instruments. The psychometric properties of the Portuguese version of the Negative Stereotypes Towards Aging Questionnaire (15 items self-reported) are explored and described in a sample of 302 participants (213 females and 89 males) between the ages of 18 and 59 years old. Confirmatory Factor Analysis shows a one-factor structure, similar to the Spanish version. The internal reliability and mean inter-item correlation showed satisfactory psychometric proprieties. Factorial ANOVA reveals that differences in agism beliefs are mainly related to educational level, with lower negative stereotypes in more educated adults. No differences were found concerning gender. This instrument may be a useful tool to assess negative stereotypes toward old age and the aging process.

Keywords:
Aging; Assessment; Negative stereotypes; Validation

Background

Demographic aging is rapidly accelerating worldwide, and this shift has profound implications for today's societies (World Health Organization, 2015World Health Organization (2015). World Report on Ageing and Health. Geneva: World Health Organization.). Although it is well known that the number of aging people is increasing with time, much less is known about what these individuals may have to deal with in the number of generalizations associated with the aging process. Stereotypes concerning aging have been shown to predict health, illness, and other behavioral outcomes in later life. Positive age stereotypes are associated with positive effects, whereas negative age stereotypes are related to negative effects (Meisner, 2012Meisner, B. A. (2012). A meta-analysis of positive and negative age stereotype priming effects on behavior among older adults. The Journal of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67(1), 13–17. doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbr062.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbr062...
). Although there is a tendency for a more positive image and greater evidence supporting that older adults can contribute to society in many different ways, some pervasive misconceptions, attitudes, and stereotypes still prevail (Dionigi, 2015Dionigi, R. A. (2015). Stereotypes of aging: their effects on the health of older adults. Journal of Geriatrics, 2015, 954027, 9. doi: https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/954027.
https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/954027...
).

Stereotypes are unchallenged myths, overstated beliefs or tendentious, preconceived ideas, often negative, associated with a category that are widespread, and mainly used to define and limit people or groups of people in society (Fiske & Tablante, 2015Fiske, S. T., & Tablante, C. B. (2015). Stereotyping: Processes and content. In E. Borgida, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology: attitudes and social cognition, (vol. 1, pp. 457–507). Washington, DC: APA.; Helmes & Pachana, 2016Helmes, E., & Pachana, N. A. (2016). Dimensions of stereotypical attitudes among older adults: analysis of two countries. Geriatric Gerontology International, 16, 1226–1230. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12613.
https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12613...
). They have important influences on behavior and operate through a variety of mechanisms (Helmes & Pachana, 2016Helmes, E., & Pachana, N. A. (2016). Dimensions of stereotypical attitudes among older adults: analysis of two countries. Geriatric Gerontology International, 16, 1226–1230. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12613.
https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12613...
). Stereotypes of aging include assumptions and generalizations about older people without regard for individual differences or unique circumstances (Fiske & Tablante, 2015Fiske, S. T., & Tablante, C. B. (2015). Stereotyping: Processes and content. In E. Borgida, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology: attitudes and social cognition, (vol. 1, pp. 457–507). Washington, DC: APA.; Moreira, 2012Moreira, J. O. (2012). Mudanças na percepção sobre o processo de envelhecimento: reflexões preliminares. [Changes in perception of the aging process: preliminary thoughts]. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 28(4), 451–456. doi: https://doi.org/10.1590/S010237722012000400003.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S010237722012000...
; Wachelke & Contarello, 2011Wachelke, J., & Contarello, A. (2011). Italian students’ social representation on aging: an exploratory study of a representational system. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 24(3), 551–560. doi: https://doi.org/10.1590/S010279722011000300016.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S010279722011000...
).

Agism is the stereotyping of and discrimination against individuals or groups based on their age (Swift, Abrams, Lamont, & Drury, 2017Swift, H. J., Abrams, D., Lamont, R. A., & Drury, L. (2017). The risks of ageism model: how ageism and negative attitudes toward age can be a barrier to active aging. Social Issues and Policy Review, 11(1), 195–231. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12031.
https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12031...
). It can take many forms, including prejudicial attitudes, discriminatory practices, or institutional policies and practices that perpetuate stereotypical beliefs (Smith, Bergeron, Cowart, Ahn, Towne, et al., 2016Smith, M. L., Bergeron, C. D., Cowart, C., Ahn, S., Towne, S. D., Ory, M. G., … Chaney, J. D. (2016). Factors associated with ageist attitudes among college students. Geriatrics & Gerontology International. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12894.
https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12894...
). Agism includes cognitive, behavioral, and emotional manifestations (Ayalon & Tesch-Römer, 2017Ayalon, L., & Tesch-Römer, C. (2017). Taking a closer look at ageism: self- and other-directed ageist attitudes and discrimination. European Journal of Ageing, 14(1), 1–4. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s1043301604099.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s1043301604099...
) and reinforces social inequalities (Rippon, Kneale, de Oliveira, Demakakos, & Steptoe, 2014Rippon, I., Kneale, D., de Oliveira, C., Demakakos, P., & Steptoe, A. (2014). Perceived age discrimination in older adults. Age and Ageing, 43(3), 379–386. doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/aft146.
https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/aft146...
).

These negative stereotypes have serious consequences both for seniors and for society in general (World Health Organization, 2015World Health Organization (2015). World Report on Ageing and Health. Geneva: World Health Organization.). Agism and negative attitudes can influence the potential for healthy and active aging (Nelson, 2016Nelson, T. D. (2016). Promoting healthy aging by confronting ageism. American Psychologist, 71(4), 276–282. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040221.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040221...
; Rippon et al., 2014Rippon, I., Kneale, D., de Oliveira, C., Demakakos, P., & Steptoe, A. (2014). Perceived age discrimination in older adults. Age and Ageing, 43(3), 379–386. doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/aft146.
https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/aft146...
). In fact, stereotypes about aging have a significant influence on older people themselves and can deleteriously impact their health, reduce autonomy, independence, and quality of life (Swift et al., 2017Swift, H. J., Abrams, D., Lamont, R. A., & Drury, L. (2017). The risks of ageism model: how ageism and negative attitudes toward age can be a barrier to active aging. Social Issues and Policy Review, 11(1), 195–231. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12031.
https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12031...
). Research shows that older adults are often stereotyped as feeble, incompetent, sick and useless, disinterested and apathetic, socially isolated, depressed, rigid, out of touch, burdensome, dependent, and cognitively impaired (Cook, 2011Cook, J. (2011). The socioeconomic contribution of older people in the UK. Working Older People, 15(4), 141–146. doi: https://doi.org/10.1108/13663661111191257.
https://doi.org/10.1108/1366366111119125...
). For older people who accept and incorporate these stereotypes, negative self-perceptions of their aging process may hinder them (Lamont, Swift, & Abrams, 2015Lamont, R. A., Swift, H. J., & Abrams, D. (2015). A review and meta-analysis of agebased stereotype threat: negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage. Psychology and Aging, 30(1), 180–193. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038586.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038586...
; Sánchez, Trianes, & Blanca, 2009Sánchez, C., Trianes, M. V., & Blanca, M. J. (2009). Negative aging stereotypes and their relation with psychosocial variables in the elderly population. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 40, 385–390. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2008.03.007.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2008.0...
).

Several studies have stated individual differences about agism. Regarding sex differences, research has noted that women have less ageist attitudes compared to men (Bodner, Bergman, & Cohen-Fridel, 2012Bodner, E., Bergman, Y. S., & Cohen-Fridel, S. (2012). Different dimensions of ageist attitudes among men and women: a multigenerational perspective. International Psychogeriatrics, 24(6), 895–901. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610211002936.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S104161021100293...
). Concerning age, researchers have found that there is a changing pattern across the life span; a study of Bodner et al., (2012)Bodner, E., Bergman, Y. S., & Cohen-Fridel, S. (2012). Different dimensions of ageist attitudes among men and women: a multigenerational perspective. International Psychogeriatrics, 24(6), 895–901. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610211002936.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S104161021100293...
noted that these types of attitudes change as people age as they found that a middle-age group revealed higher negative levels when compared to younger and older participants.

Given the practical implications of this issue, in the last few decades, many researchers have analyzed the implications of agism and negative stereotypes, with specific regard to negative influences on the mental and physical health of older people (Ayalon & Tesch-Römer, 2017Ayalon, L., & Tesch-Römer, C. (2017). Taking a closer look at ageism: self- and other-directed ageist attitudes and discrimination. European Journal of Ageing, 14(1), 1–4. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s1043301604099.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s1043301604099...
; Dionigi, 2015Dionigi, R. A. (2015). Stereotypes of aging: their effects on the health of older adults. Journal of Geriatrics, 2015, 954027, 9. doi: https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/954027.
https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/954027...
; Nelson, 2016Nelson, T. D. (2016). Promoting healthy aging by confronting ageism. American Psychologist, 71(4), 276–282. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040221.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040221...
; World Health Organization, 2015World Health Organization (2015). World Report on Ageing and Health. Geneva: World Health Organization.). Several investigations note a clear and direct threat to the cognition of the elderly, namely, when they believe and incorporate these negative stereotypes themselves (Marquet, Missotten, & Adam, 2016Marquet, M., Missotten, P., & Adam, S. (2016). Âgisme et surestimation des difficultés cognitives des personnes âgées: une revue de la question. [Aging and overestimation of the elderly cognitive difficulties: a review of the question]. Geriatric Psychology Neuropsychiatric Vieil, 14(2), 177–186. doi: https://doi.org/10.1684/pnv.2016.0609.
https://doi.org/10.1684/pnv.2016.0609...
; Nelson, 2016Nelson, T. D. (2016). Promoting healthy aging by confronting ageism. American Psychologist, 71(4), 276–282. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040221.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040221...
). For example, stereotypes about age-related cognitive decline may have a detrimental effect on older adults regarding their cognitive performance. Whether they are consciously or unconsciously perceived by the elderly, beliefs in stereotypes can operate through various mechanisms such as increased anxiety and the adoption of a vigilant attitude (Marquet et al., 2016Marquet, M., Missotten, P., & Adam, S. (2016). Âgisme et surestimation des difficultés cognitives des personnes âgées: une revue de la question. [Aging and overestimation of the elderly cognitive difficulties: a review of the question]. Geriatric Psychology Neuropsychiatric Vieil, 14(2), 177–186. doi: https://doi.org/10.1684/pnv.2016.0609.
https://doi.org/10.1684/pnv.2016.0609...
). Research also shows that the will to live is decreased, and seniors are less interested in engaging in healthy preventive behaviors. Negative age stereotypes also have significant effects on the physical well-being of older persons. Recovery from illness is impaired, cardiovascular reactivity to stress is increased, and longevity is decreased (Nelson, 2016Nelson, T. D. (2016). Promoting healthy aging by confronting ageism. American Psychologist, 71(4), 276–282. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040221.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040221...
).

These negative stereotypes have important social and economic implications (Lamont et al., 2015Lamont, R. A., Swift, H. J., & Abrams, D. (2015). A review and meta-analysis of agebased stereotype threat: negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage. Psychology and Aging, 30(1), 180–193. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038586.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038586...
), which are particularly relevant given the aging population. Stereotypes can be a barrier to developing good policies and can seriously impact the quality of health and social care that older people receive.

Despite the importance of this issue, instruments to assess negative stereotypes about old age are scarce. Polizzi (2003)Polizzi, K. G. (2003). Assessing attitudes toward elderly: Polozzi's refined version of the aging semantic differential. Educational Gerontology, 29(3), 197–216. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/713844306.
https://doi.org/10.1080/713844306...
notes that difficulties persist in making an accurate assessment of attitudes toward the elderly. According to this researcher, the Aging Semantic Differential, one of the most widely used instruments to assess the stereotypic attitudes toward older adults, developed by Rosencranz and McNevin (1969)Rosencranz, H. A., & McNevin, T. E. (1969). A factor analysis of attitudes toward the aged. The Gerontologist, 9(1), 55–59. doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/9.1.55.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/9.1.55...
employs adjectives not necessarily descriptive of attitudes today and is outdated. In the same sense, some weaknesses in the Kogan Attitudes Toward Old People Scale developed by Kogan (1961)Kogan, N. (1961). Attitudes toward old people: the development of a scale and an examination of correlates. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62(1), 44–54. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048053.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048053...
are also identified, and the need for further refinement is suggested due to the failure associated with two of its subscales (Iwasaki & Jones, 2008Iwasaki, M., & Jones, J. A. (2008). Attitudes toward older adults: a reexamination of two major scales. Gerontology & Geriatrics Education, 29(2), 139–157. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/02701960802223209.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0270196080222320...
).

From previous research, we can also mention the questionnaire Facts on Aging Quizzes (FAQ 1 and FAQ 2) developed by Palmore (1988)Palmore, E. B. (1988). The facts on aging quiz, (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.. This questionnaire has been used in some studies. However, researchers have suggested that given the areas addressed (e.g., physical, mental, and social facts) and its reliability, these instruments may not be suited for evaluative use (Snyder & Tyler, 2001Snyder, C. W., & Tyler, C. V. (2001). The Palmore facts on aging quiz––are we using it correctly? Gerontology & Geriatrics Education, 21(3), 21–29. doi: https://doi.org/10.1300/J021v21n03_04.
https://doi.org/10.1300/J021v21n03_04...
).

These identified fragilities justify the relevance of the Negative Stereotypes Towards Aging Questionnaire (Cuestionario de Evaluación de Estereotipos Negativos Hacia la Vejez – CENVE) developed by Blanca, Sánchez, and Trianes (2005)Blanca, M. J., Sánchez, C., & Trianes, M. V. (2005). Cuestionario de evaluación de estereótipos negativos hacia la vejez [Negative Stereotypes Towards Aging Questionnaire]. Revista Multidisciplinar de Gerontotologia, 15(4), 212–220.. This questionnaire is based on the Palmore Questionnaire (Palmore, 1988Palmore, E. B. (1988). The facts on aging quiz, (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.) and the Montorio and Izal questionnaire (Montorio & Izal, 1991Montorio, I., & Izal, M. (1991). Cuestionario sobre estereotipos hacia la vejez. [Questionnaire on Stereotypes Towards Old Age] [work not published]. Madrid: Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.). It consists of 15 items in the original form, corresponding to negative stereotypes of old age, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The first psychometric study was conducted with a sample of 757 noninstitutionalized elderly between the ages of 65 and 96 years old. The analysis showed three factors (health, motivational-social, and character-personality), which can vary between 5 and 20 points with higher scores indicating a high degree of belief in negative stereotypes about old age and low scores indicating a weak belief in negative stereotypes regarding old age.

To validate the questionnaire with nonelderly subjects, a second study was conducted by Menéndez, Cuevas-Toro, Pérez-Padilla, and Lorence (2016)Menéndez, S., Cuevas-Toro, A. M., Pérez-Padilla, J., & Lorence, B. (2016). Evaluación de los estereotipos negativos hacia la vejez en jóvenes y adultos. [Assessment of negative stereotypes about old age in young people and adults]. Revista Española de Geriatría y Gerontología, 51(6), 323–328. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.003.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.0...
with a total of 350 adults under the age of 65. The results did not confirm the original factorial structure in three factors, suggesting a one-dimensional structure.

To adapt the CENVE to the Portuguese population, an exploratory study was conducted by Authors (2015)Authors (2015). Negative ageing stereotypes: psychometric properties of a Portuguese version of CENVE. In M. T. Ramiro Sánchez, & T. Ramiro Sánchez (Eds.), Avances en Psicología Clínica, 2015 [Advances in Clinical Psychology], (pp. 868–876). Granada: Associación Española de Psicología Conductual. with a sample of 240 adults between the ages of 20 and 59 years old. The factor analysis conducted did not reveal the same structure as the original version (Blanca et al., 2005Blanca, M. J., Sánchez, C., & Trianes, M. V. (2005). Cuestionario de evaluación de estereótipos negativos hacia la vejez [Negative Stereotypes Towards Aging Questionnaire]. Revista Multidisciplinar de Gerontotologia, 15(4), 212–220.). Our results confirm a one-dimensional solution presented by Menéndez et al. (2016)Menéndez, S., Cuevas-Toro, A. M., Pérez-Padilla, J., & Lorence, B. (2016). Evaluación de los estereotipos negativos hacia la vejez en jóvenes y adultos. [Assessment of negative stereotypes about old age in young people and adults]. Revista Española de Geriatría y Gerontología, 51(6), 323–328. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.003.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.0...
to assess the negative stereotypes related to behavior and the physical and psychological health of older adults. All of the items were important for consistency and reliability.

Increasing our awareness about stereotypes and age discrimination is fundamental to developing appropriate policies and strategies for future interventions to combat agism. The aims of the current study are to analyze (1) the psychometric properties of the Portuguese CENVE version, including using Confirmatory Factor Analysis to examine its factor structure, and (2) the differences in negative aging stereotypes concerning sociodemographic characteristics.

Method

Participants

The sample was composed of 302 adults between the ages of 18 and 59 (M = 37.11, SD = 12.31), mostly women (70.65%) and most without any previous aging training (92.41%). Concerning their educational level, 25.69% completed primary school, 42.71% completed high school, and 31.60% had university studies. Most of the participants were employed (64.79%) or students (20.42%), 49.48% were married or living together, and 39.45% were single.

Instrument

To evaluate the negative aging stereotypes, we used the Portuguese version of the CENVE (Cuestionario de Evaluación de Estereotipos Negativos Hacia la Vejez; Blanca et al., 2005Blanca, M. J., Sánchez, C., & Trianes, M. V. (2005). Cuestionario de evaluación de estereótipos negativos hacia la vejez [Negative Stereotypes Towards Aging Questionnaire]. Revista Multidisciplinar de Gerontotologia, 15(4), 212–220.), composed of 15 items, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The original version was structured in three dimensions of old age stereotypes referring to health (5 items, e.g., “Cognitive impairment—memory loss, disorientation, confusion, etc.—is an inevitable part of aging”), motivation (5 items, e.g., “Older people have fewer friends than younger people”), and personality (5 items, e.g., “As people get older they become more rigid and inflexible”). Higher scores reveal a more negative aging stereotype. This instrument is free access; therefore, no authorization was requested to original authors.

Participants also provided data concerning their sociodemographic characteristics: age, gender, educational level, work status, marital status, and previous aging training.

Procedures

The original translation of the CENVE into the European Portuguese language was previously conducted (Authors, 2015Authors (2015). Negative ageing stereotypes: psychometric properties of a Portuguese version of CENVE. In M. T. Ramiro Sánchez, & T. Ramiro Sánchez (Eds.), Avances en Psicología Clínica, 2015 [Advances in Clinical Psychology], (pp. 868–876). Granada: Associación Española de Psicología Conductual.). During the translation and retroversion of the RPQ, appropriate procedures were followed. The cultural adaptation was particularly considered, taking into account clarity, common language use, and conceptual equivalence of the scale. The questionnaire was then independently back-translated into Spanish. The original and the back-translated items were compared for non-equivalence of meaning, and items were revised when any discrepancies in meaning were detected until no semantic differences were identified between the Spanish and the Portuguese versions. Using a snowball sampling technique, graduate students from the Psychology Department of the University of Algarve were contacted and asked to participate in this study by answering and recruiting five adults to the CENVE.

Data analysis

The data were analyzed using SPSS (version 22; IBM Corp. released 2013. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 22.0. Armonk, NY) and EQS 6.3 (Bentler & Wu, 2015Bentler, P., & Wu, E. (2015). Supplement to EQS6.3 for windows user's guide. Temple City: Multivariate Software, Inc.). The factor structure of the CENVE Portuguese version was assessed with Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) performed in EQS 6.3 (Bentler & Wu, 2015Bentler, P., & Wu, E. (2015). Supplement to EQS6.3 for windows user's guide. Temple City: Multivariate Software, Inc.; Byrne, 2006Byrne, B. (2006). Structural equation modeling with EQS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.) with the robust estimation methods. We calculated the goodness of fit indexes, including Satorra-Bentler chi-square/degrees of freedom (S-Bχ2/df), incremental fit index (IFI), comparative fit index (CFI), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). A chi-square/degrees of freedom value < 5 was considered acceptable, a value of 2 was considered good, and 1 equaled very good (West, Taylor, & Wu, 2012West, S., Taylor, A., & Wu, W. (2012). Model fit and model selection in structural equation modeling. In R. Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook of structural equation modeling (pp. 209–231). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.). An IFI/CFI ≥ .90 and RMSEA ≤ .10 indicated adequate fit, whereas an IFI/CFI ≥ .95 and RMSEA ≤ .06 indicated a good model fit (Byrne, 2006Byrne, B. (2006). Structural equation modeling with EQS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.).

The CFA was performed on the original scale items using a covariance matrix. Modification indexes were considered to determine if any suggestion of model modification would significantly improve the measurement model. Items with a standardized loading above .30 were retained because factor loadings are typically considered to be meaningful when they exceed that value. Cronbach's alpha values above .70 were considered to be good, mean inter-item correlations were considered good if they were between .15 and .50, and corrected item-total correlations were considered satisfactory if they were above .20 (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994Nunnally, J., & Bernstein, I. (1994). Psychometric theory, (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.).

To analyze differences associated with the main and interaction effects of group characteristics, a factorial ANOVA was performed using CENVE scores as dependent variables and age, gender, and educational level as fixed factors. As factorial ANOVA requires that independent variables are qualitative, 33 and 66 age percentiles were computed to establish three equivalent groups.

Results

Our first step in assessing the psychometric properties of the Portuguese version of CENVE was to replicate it using a CFA operating with the maximum likelihood (ML) method and the unifactorial structure proposed for this instrument by Menéndez et al. (2016)Menéndez, S., Cuevas-Toro, A. M., Pérez-Padilla, J., & Lorence, B. (2016). Evaluación de los estereotipos negativos hacia la vejez en jóvenes y adultos. [Assessment of negative stereotypes about old age in young people and adults]. Revista Española de Geriatría y Gerontología, 51(6), 323–328. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.003.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.0...
and Authors (2015)Authors (2015). Negative ageing stereotypes: psychometric properties of a Portuguese version of CENVE. In M. T. Ramiro Sánchez, & T. Ramiro Sánchez (Eds.), Avances en Psicología Clínica, 2015 [Advances in Clinical Psychology], (pp. 868–876). Granada: Associación Española de Psicología Conductual.. We were able to find support in all the goodness of fit indexes for the proposed one-factor model (Table 1).

Table 1
Goodness of fit indices for different ML models of CENVE

Table 2 shows descriptive and metric analysis; all items had averages greater than 2 (with the exception of item 15: M = 1.70, SD = 0.84) revealing values in the upper levels. Concerning the distribution, the results are mostly negative and near zero, revealing a left and platykurtic tendency, but near normal.

Table 2
Descriptive and metric analysis of CENVE items

The Cronbach's alpha revealed a good standardized coefficient (α = .86) and also the ordinal alpha (αordinal =.89), with all the items being important to total reliability. The item-total correlation was mostly above r > .45, showing a good discriminability of the scale items. All items had loadings above .45.

Table 3 summarizes the results of factorial ANOVA 2 (gender) × 3 (age group) × 3 (educational level). The corrected model obtained a statistical significant result explaining 13.3% of the variance but with moderate effect. Educational level was the only factor with a significant main effect. The Bonferroni post hoc test showed that participants with a primary educational level reported higher means of negative aging stereotypes (M =39.72, SD = 7.45) than high school (M =36.70, SD = 7.17) and university levels (M =34.32, SD =8.69).

Table 3
Relation between sociodemographic characteristics and CENVE scores

Discussion

The aim of this study was to contribute with an analysis of structural validity and internal consistency of the Portuguese CENVE version. Therefore, a CFA was conducted to confirm the one-factorial structural proposed by Menéndez et al. (2016)Menéndez, S., Cuevas-Toro, A. M., Pérez-Padilla, J., & Lorence, B. (2016). Evaluación de los estereotipos negativos hacia la vejez en jóvenes y adultos. [Assessment of negative stereotypes about old age in young people and adults]. Revista Española de Geriatría y Gerontología, 51(6), 323–328. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.003.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.0...
and Authors (2015)Authors (2015). Negative ageing stereotypes: psychometric properties of a Portuguese version of CENVE. In M. T. Ramiro Sánchez, & T. Ramiro Sánchez (Eds.), Avances en Psicología Clínica, 2015 [Advances in Clinical Psychology], (pp. 868–876). Granada: Associación Española de Psicología Conductual.. Our results provide additional evidence for a one-factorial model, finding support in all the goodness of fitness indexes. The item loadings were above .51, except item 01 (r = .46), and the item-total correlation was mostly above r > .40, showing a good discriminability of the scale items (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994Nunnally, J., & Bernstein, I. (1994). Psychometric theory, (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.).

Concerning the scores obtained in each level of negative aging stereotypes, the participants revealed punctuations situated in an intermediate position similar to Blanca et al. (2005)Blanca, M. J., Sánchez, C., & Trianes, M. V. (2005). Cuestionario de evaluación de estereótipos negativos hacia la vejez [Negative Stereotypes Towards Aging Questionnaire]. Revista Multidisciplinar de Gerontotologia, 15(4), 212–220. and slightly above Menéndez et al. (2016)Menéndez, S., Cuevas-Toro, A. M., Pérez-Padilla, J., & Lorence, B. (2016). Evaluación de los estereotipos negativos hacia la vejez en jóvenes y adultos. [Assessment of negative stereotypes about old age in young people and adults]. Revista Española de Geriatría y Gerontología, 51(6), 323–328. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.003.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.0...
. All of the items were important to the consistency and reliability. The internal consistency analysis revealed satisfactory values, with all scores exceeding the recommended minimum Cronbach's alpha of .70 (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994Nunnally, J., & Bernstein, I. (1994). Psychometric theory, (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.).

The factorial ANOVA performed between educational levels showed differences regarding aging stereotypes. Participants with primary educational levels reported higher means of negative aging stereotypes than high school and university levels. This result is comparable with the findings of Menéndez et al. (2016)Menéndez, S., Cuevas-Toro, A. M., Pérez-Padilla, J., & Lorence, B. (2016). Evaluación de los estereotipos negativos hacia la vejez en jóvenes y adultos. [Assessment of negative stereotypes about old age in young people and adults]. Revista Española de Geriatría y Gerontología, 51(6), 323–328. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.003.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.0...
and highlights the relevance of formal education in the changing of aging attitudes (Sousa, Cerqueira, & Galante, 2008Sousa, L., Cerqueira, M., & Galante, H. (2008). How images of old age vary with age: an exploratory study among the Portuguese population. Reviews in Clinical Gerontolology, 18(1), 77–90. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959259808002682.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S095925980800268...
). In fact, educational level was the only discriminant sociodemographic characteristic of aging stereotypes. Contrary to other studies (Menéndez et al., 2016Menéndez, S., Cuevas-Toro, A. M., Pérez-Padilla, J., & Lorence, B. (2016). Evaluación de los estereotipos negativos hacia la vejez en jóvenes y adultos. [Assessment of negative stereotypes about old age in young people and adults]. Revista Española de Geriatría y Gerontología, 51(6), 323–328. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.003.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.0...
), neither age nor sex (Laditka, Fisher, Laditka, & Segal, 2004Laditka, S. B., Fisher, M., Laditka, J., & Segal, D. (2004). Attitudes about aging and gender among young, middle age, and older college-based students. Educational Gerontology, 30(5), 403–421. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/03601270490433602.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0360127049043360...
) was significant.

From the results explored above, this instrument can be a useful tool in the assessment of negative stereotypes not only in a research scenario but also in an interventions context (e.g., programs to promote positive beliefs toward aging).

Some limitations concerning this research must be mentioned. First, due to the inexistence of adequately validated instruments, Portuguese CENVE convergent validity assessment was not possible. It is, therefore, necessary to conduct more validation studies of alternative measures of aging stereotypes among Portuguese samples. Second, due to the cross-sectional nature of the study, some psychometric properties could not be evaluated (e.g., test–retest reliability). Additionally, in the future, other analysis including discriminant measures must be conducted to improve negative stereotypes research.

Conclusions

Since research has reported that negative aging stereotypes can constitute barriers to the functionality of older adults and negatively influence their social status (Palmore, 1988Palmore, E. B. (1988). The facts on aging quiz, (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.), professionals must develop interventions that aim to change attitudes and beliefs starting at a young age. One must be aware that agism can take many forms (e.g., prejudicial attitudes, discriminatory practices, or institutional policies) and practices, perpetuating stereotypical beliefs (Smith et al., 2016Smith, M. L., Bergeron, C. D., Cowart, C., Ahn, S., Towne, S. D., Ory, M. G., … Chaney, J. D. (2016). Factors associated with ageist attitudes among college students. Geriatrics & Gerontology International. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12894.
https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12894...
). Therefore, interventions that are aimed at cognitive, behavioral, and emotional components (Ayalon & Tesch-Römer, 2017Ayalon, L., & Tesch-Römer, C. (2017). Taking a closer look at ageism: self- and other-directed ageist attitudes and discrimination. European Journal of Ageing, 14(1), 1–4. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s1043301604099.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s1043301604099...
) are important, and CENVE can be a useful tool to assess the levels of negative stereotypes before and after intervention.

  • Funding
    This paper is financed by the National Funds provided by FCT - Foundation for Science and Technology through project CIP-PSI/04345/2013.
  • Availability of data and materials
    The instrument used in this research is available and free access; therefore, do not request authorization from authors. Data are not available.
  • Publisher's Note
    Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
  • Published online: 27 January 2018

References

  • Authors (2015). Negative ageing stereotypes: psychometric properties of a Portuguese version of CENVE. In M. T. Ramiro Sánchez, & T. Ramiro Sánchez (Eds.), Avances en Psicología Clínica, 2015 [Advances in Clinical Psychology], (pp. 868–876). Granada: Associación Española de Psicología Conductual.
  • Ayalon, L., & Tesch-Römer, C. (2017). Taking a closer look at ageism: self- and other-directed ageist attitudes and discrimination. European Journal of Ageing, 14(1), 1–4. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s1043301604099
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s1043301604099
  • Bentler, P., & Wu, E. (2015). Supplement to EQS6.3 for windows user's guide Temple City: Multivariate Software, Inc.
  • Blanca, M. J., Sánchez, C., & Trianes, M. V. (2005). Cuestionario de evaluación de estereótipos negativos hacia la vejez [Negative Stereotypes Towards Aging Questionnaire]. Revista Multidisciplinar de Gerontotologia, 15(4), 212–220.
  • Bodner, E., Bergman, Y. S., & Cohen-Fridel, S. (2012). Different dimensions of ageist attitudes among men and women: a multigenerational perspective. International Psychogeriatrics, 24(6), 895–901. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610211002936
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610211002936
  • Byrne, B. (2006). Structural equation modeling with EQS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Cook, J. (2011). The socioeconomic contribution of older people in the UK. Working Older People, 15(4), 141–146. doi: https://doi.org/10.1108/13663661111191257
    » https://doi.org/10.1108/13663661111191257
  • Dionigi, R. A. (2015). Stereotypes of aging: their effects on the health of older adults. Journal of Geriatrics, 2015, 954027, 9. doi: https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/954027
    » https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/954027
  • Fiske, S. T., & Tablante, C. B. (2015). Stereotyping: Processes and content. In E. Borgida, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology: attitudes and social cognition, (vol. 1, pp. 457–507). Washington, DC: APA.
  • Helmes, E., & Pachana, N. A. (2016). Dimensions of stereotypical attitudes among older adults: analysis of two countries. Geriatric Gerontology International, 16, 1226–1230. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12613
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12613
  • Iwasaki, M., & Jones, J. A. (2008). Attitudes toward older adults: a reexamination of two major scales. Gerontology & Geriatrics Education, 29(2), 139–157. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/02701960802223209
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/02701960802223209
  • Kogan, N. (1961). Attitudes toward old people: the development of a scale and an examination of correlates. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62(1), 44–54. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048053
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048053
  • Laditka, S. B., Fisher, M., Laditka, J., & Segal, D. (2004). Attitudes about aging and gender among young, middle age, and older college-based students. Educational Gerontology, 30(5), 403–421. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/03601270490433602
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/03601270490433602
  • Lamont, R. A., Swift, H. J., & Abrams, D. (2015). A review and meta-analysis of agebased stereotype threat: negative stereotypes, not facts, do the damage. Psychology and Aging, 30(1), 180–193. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038586
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038586
  • Marquet, M., Missotten, P., & Adam, S. (2016). Âgisme et surestimation des difficultés cognitives des personnes âgées: une revue de la question. [Aging and overestimation of the elderly cognitive difficulties: a review of the question]. Geriatric Psychology Neuropsychiatric Vieil, 14(2), 177–186. doi: https://doi.org/10.1684/pnv.2016.0609
    » https://doi.org/10.1684/pnv.2016.0609
  • Meisner, B. A. (2012). A meta-analysis of positive and negative age stereotype priming effects on behavior among older adults. The Journal of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67(1), 13–17. doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbr062
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbr062
  • Menéndez, S., Cuevas-Toro, A. M., Pérez-Padilla, J., & Lorence, B. (2016). Evaluación de los estereotipos negativos hacia la vejez en jóvenes y adultos. [Assessment of negative stereotypes about old age in young people and adults]. Revista Española de Geriatría y Gerontología, 51(6), 323–328. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.regg.2015.12.003
  • Montorio, I., & Izal, M. (1991). Cuestionario sobre estereotipos hacia la vejez. [Questionnaire on Stereotypes Towards Old Age] [work not published]. Madrid: Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.
  • Moreira, J. O. (2012). Mudanças na percepção sobre o processo de envelhecimento: reflexões preliminares. [Changes in perception of the aging process: preliminary thoughts]. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 28(4), 451–456. doi: https://doi.org/10.1590/S010237722012000400003
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S010237722012000400003
  • Nelson, T. D. (2016). Promoting healthy aging by confronting ageism. American Psychologist, 71(4), 276–282. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040221
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040221
  • Nunnally, J., & Bernstein, I. (1994). Psychometric theory, (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
  • Palmore, E. B. (1988). The facts on aging quiz, (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.
  • Polizzi, K. G. (2003). Assessing attitudes toward elderly: Polozzi's refined version of the aging semantic differential. Educational Gerontology, 29(3), 197–216. doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/713844306
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/713844306
  • Rippon, I., Kneale, D., de Oliveira, C., Demakakos, P., & Steptoe, A. (2014). Perceived age discrimination in older adults. Age and Ageing, 43(3), 379–386. doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/aft146
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/aft146
  • Rosencranz, H. A., & McNevin, T. E. (1969). A factor analysis of attitudes toward the aged. The Gerontologist, 9(1), 55–59. doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/9.1.55
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/9.1.55
  • Sánchez, C., Trianes, M. V., & Blanca, M. J. (2009). Negative aging stereotypes and their relation with psychosocial variables in the elderly population. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 40, 385–390. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2008.03.007
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2008.03.007
  • Smith, M. L., Bergeron, C. D., Cowart, C., Ahn, S., Towne, S. D., Ory, M. G., … Chaney, J. D. (2016). Factors associated with ageist attitudes among college students. Geriatrics & Gerontology International doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12894
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12894
  • Snyder, C. W., & Tyler, C. V. (2001). The Palmore facts on aging quiz––are we using it correctly? Gerontology & Geriatrics Education, 21(3), 21–29. doi: https://doi.org/10.1300/J021v21n03_04
    » https://doi.org/10.1300/J021v21n03_04
  • Sousa, L., Cerqueira, M., & Galante, H. (2008). How images of old age vary with age: an exploratory study among the Portuguese population. Reviews in Clinical Gerontolology, 18(1), 77–90. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959259808002682
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959259808002682
  • Swift, H. J., Abrams, D., Lamont, R. A., & Drury, L. (2017). The risks of ageism model: how ageism and negative attitudes toward age can be a barrier to active aging. Social Issues and Policy Review, 11(1), 195–231. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12031
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12031
  • Wachelke, J., & Contarello, A. (2011). Italian students’ social representation on aging: an exploratory study of a representational system. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 24(3), 551–560. doi: https://doi.org/10.1590/S010279722011000300016
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S010279722011000300016
  • West, S., Taylor, A., & Wu, W. (2012). Model fit and model selection in structural equation modeling. In R. Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook of structural equation modeling (pp. 209–231). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
  • World Health Organization (2015). World Report on Ageing and Health. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    2018

History

  • Received
    17 Nov 2017
  • Accepted
    17 Jan 2018
Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia da Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul Rua Ramiro Barcelos, 2600 - sala 110, 90035-003 Porto Alegre RS - Brazil, Tel.: +55 51 3308-5691 - Porto Alegre - RS - Brazil
E-mail: prc@springeropen.com