Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory (ASRI) Adaptation to Portuguese Context1 1 Paper derived from doctoral dissertation of the first author under the advisory of the second author in the Graduate Program of Health Psychology at the Universidad Miguel Hernández, Spain.

Adaptação do Inventário de Autorregulação na Adolescência Para o Contexto Português

Adaptación del Inventario de Autorregulación en la Adolescencia Para el Contexto Portugués

Abstracts

In this study we present the results of the adaptation of the Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory (ASRI) to Portugal. The measure was used with two samples of high school students to which ASRI was administered with Self-Regulation Scale as control measure. In the first study the measures were administered to 823 adolescents and the construct validity analysis was assessed with exploratory factor analysis. The results allow us to find an adequate structure with proper psychometric properties, in their construct and content validity, and reliability. A second study involved 435 adolescents, being tested three models using confirmatory factor analyses. The final version of the ASRI-2 presents an acceptable fit of the data in construct and concurrent validity, given its moderate or high correlation with academic success and substance use. The implications of this new approach to the self-regulation study are discussed and directions to further studies are suggested.

self-regulation; adolescence; adjustment; psychometrics


O objetivo deste estudo foi apresentar os resultados da adaptação do Inventário de Autorregulação na Adolescência (IARA) para Portugal. O instrumento foi testado com duas amostras de estudantes do ensino secundário com a Escala de Autorregulação como medida de controle. No primeiro estudo, os instrumentos foram testados junto de 823 adolescentes e a análise de validade de construto foi feita com recurso à análise factorial exploratória. Os resultados indicaram uma estrutura apropriada com propriedades psicométricas adequadas referentes à validade de conteúdo e construto, e fidelidade. Um segundo estudo envolveu 435 adolescentes, sendo testados três modelos com análise factorial confirmatória. A versão final do IARA-2 apresenta valores de ajustamento aceitáveis em termos de validade de contruto e concorrente, dadas as correlações moderadas a elevadas com o sucesso acadêmico e uso de substâncias. As implicações desta nova abordagem ao estudo da autorregulação são discutidas e direções para próximos estudos são sugeridas.

autorregulação; adolescência; ajustamento; psicometria


En este estudio se presentan los resultados de la adaptación del Inventario de Autorregulación en la Adolescencia (IARA) para Portugal. El instrumento fue administrado con dos muestras de estudiantes de secundarias públicas con la Escala de Autorregulación como medida de control. En el primer estudio, los instrumentos fueron testados con 823 adolescentes y la validez de constructo se evaluó con análisis factorial exploratorio. Los resultados indicaron una estructura apropiada con propiedades psicométricas adecuadas respecto a su validez de contenido y constructo y fidelidad. Un segundo estudio involucró 435 adolescentes, testando tres modelos con análisis factorial confirmatoria. La versión final del IARA-2 muestra valores de ajuste satisfactorios en términos de validez concurrente y de constructo, ante las correlaciones moderadas a altas con el éxito académico y uso de sustancias. Las implicaciones de este nuevo enfoque para el estudio de la autorregulación son discutidas y direcciones para futuros estudios son sugeridas.

autorregulación; adolescencia; ajuste; psicometría


Adolescence is characterized by the reconfiguration of relationships with peers and family, increased academic stresses, pressures to choose career paths, among other individual challenges (Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner, 2008Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Skinner, E. A. (2008). Adolescents coping with stress: Development and diversity. Prevention Researcher, 15(4), 3-7.). Thus, successful adaptation appears to depend upon the ways in which individuals manage their emotions, think constructively, regulate and direct their behaviours, and modify their contexts to change or diminish sources of stress (Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner, 2011)Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Skinner, E. A. (2011). Review: The development of coping across childhood and adolescence: An integrative review and critique of research. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35(1), 1-17. doi:10.1177/0165025410384923. In other words, adolescents’ self-regulatory abilities can play a central role in this phase, in human functioning, resilience, and overall well-being (Elliot, Thrash, & Murayama, 2011Elliot, A. J., Thrash, T. M., & Murayama, K. (2011). A longitudinal analysis of self-regulation and well-being: Avoidance personal goals, avoidance coping, stress generation, and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 79(3), 643-674. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00694.x; Garcia del Castillo & Dias, 2009Garcia del Castillo, J. A., & Dias, P. C. (2009). Auto-regulação, resiliência e consumo de substâncias na adolescência: Contributos da adaptação do questionário reduzido de auto-regulação. Psicologia, Saúde e Doenças, 10(2), 205-216.; Gardner, Dishion, & Connell, 2008Gardner, T. W., Dishion, T. J., & Connell, A. M. (2008). Adolescent self-regulation as resilience: Resistance to antisocial behavior within the deviant peer context. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(2), 273-284. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9176-6; Hofer, Busch, & Kärtner, 2011Hofer, J., Busch, H., & Kärtner, J. (2011). Self-regulation and well-being: The influence of identity and motives. European Journal of Personality, 25(3), 211-224. doi:10.1002/per.789). Several studies underline the role of this process in the explanation of learning, motivation, and academic success (McClelland & Cameron, 2011McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2011). Self-regulation and academic achievement in elementary school children. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 133, 29-44. doi:10.1002/cd.302; Zimmerman, 2008)Zimmerman, B. J. (2008). Investigating self-regulation and motivation: Historical background, methodological developments, and future prospects. American Educational Research Journal, 45(1), 166-183. doi:10.3102/0002831207312909 but also in physical exercise practice (Annesi & Gorjala, 2010Annesi, J. J., & Gorjala, S. (2010). Relations of self-regulation and self-efficacy for exercise and eating and BMI change: A field investigation. BioPsychoSocial Medicine, 4:10. doi:10.1186/1751-0759-4-10; Hagger, Wood, Stiff, & Chatzisarantis, 2010Hagger, M. S., Wood, C. W., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L., (2010). Self-regulation and self-control in exercise: The strength-energy model. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 3(1), 62-86. doi:10.1080/17509840903322815; MacDonald & Palfai, 2008)MacDonald, A., & Palfai, T. (2008). Predictors of exercise behavior among university student women: Utility of a goal-systems/self-regulation theory framework. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(4), 921-931. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.024., substance use prevention (Garcia del Castillo, Dias, & Castelar-Perim, 2012Garcia del Castillo, J. A., Dias, P. C., & Castelar-Perim, P. (2012). Autorregulação e consumo de substâncias na adolescência. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25(2), 238-247. doi:10.1590/S0102-79722012000200005; Garcia del Castillo, Dias, Diaz-Pérez, & Castillo-López, 2012Garcia del Castillo, J. A., Dias, P. C., Diaz-Pérez, J., & Garcia del Castillo-López, A. (2012). The mediating role of self-regulation in cigarette smoking and alcohol use among young people. Anales de Psicologia, 28(1), 1-10.; Gardner, Dishion, & Posner, 2006Gardner, T. W., Dishion, T. J., & Posner, M. I. (2006). Attention and adolescent tobacco use: A potential self-regulator dynamic underlying nicotine addiction. Addictive Behaviors, 31(3), 531-536. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.05.018; Wills & Dishion, 2004)Wills T. A., & Dishion, T. J. (2004). Temperament and adolescent substance use: A transactional analysis of emerging self-control. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 33(1), 69-81. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_7, or others.

Despite widespread agreement among researchers and theorists that adolescent self-regulation is an important factor in the overall adjustment in adolescence, there is less agreement among scholars regarding how self-regulation should be conceptualized and measured during this phase. In fact, many of these studies are based on models designed for adults, allowing the emergence of doubts and inconsistencies in their application to the adolescent population (Gibbons, Gerrard, Reimer, & Pomery, 2006Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., Reimer, R. A., & Pomery, E. A. (2006). Unintentional behavior: A subrational approach to health risk. In D. T. D. Ridder, & J. B. F. de Wit (Eds.), Self-regulation in health behavior (pp. 45-70). Chichester, United Kingdom: John Wiley & Sons.). From a dispositional approach, self-regulation is conceptualized as an individual variable, which includes the knowledge and previous experiences of a person about the necessary actions to achieve a goal or behaviour (Diehl, Semegon, & Schwarzer, 2006Diehl, M., Semegon, A. B., & Schwarzer, R. (2006). Assessing attention control in goal pursuit: A component of dispositional self-regulation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 86(3), 306-317. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa8603_06). This approach focuses at the individual, his personal abilities and skills (Gestsdottir, Bowers, von Eye, Napolitano, & Lerner, 2010Gestsdottir, S., Bowers, E., von Eye, A., Napolitano, C. M., & Lerner, R. M. (2010). Intentional self-regulation in middle adolescence: The emerging role of loss-based selection in positive youth development. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(7), 764-782. doi:10.1007/s10964-010-9537-2; Luszczynska, Diehl, Gutiérrez-Doña, Kuusinen, & Schwarzer, 2004Luszczynska, A., Diehl, M., Gutiérrez-Doña, B., Kuusinen, P., & Schwarzer, R. (2004). Measuring one component of dispositional self-regulation: Attention control in goal pursuit. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(3), 555-566. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.026). That doesn’t necessarily mean that the actions of individuals are unified, but conceptualized as a relatively stable and persistent process (Lerner et al., 2011Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., Bowers, E. P., Lewin-Bizan, S., Gestsdottir, S., & Urban, J. B. (2011). Self-regulation processes and thriving in childhood and adolescence: A view of the issues. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 133, 1-9. doi:10.1002/cd.300; Luszczynska et al., 2004)Luszczynska, A., Diehl, M., Gutiérrez-Doña, B., Kuusinen, P., & Schwarzer, R. (2004). Measuring one component of dispositional self-regulation: Attention control in goal pursuit. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(3), 555-566. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.026. This is also a tendency in the conceptualization of self-regulation, according to developmental theory (Gestsdottir & Lerner, 2008Gestsdottir, S., & Lerner, R. M. (2008). Positive development in adolescence: The development and role of intentional self-regulation. Human Development, 51(3), 202-224. doi:10.1159/000135757; Moilanen, 2007)Moilanen, K. L. (2007). The Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory: The development and validation of a questionnaire of short-term and long-term self-regulation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(6), 835-848. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9107-9, as an ability to activate, monitor, and inhibit behaviour, attention, emotions or cognitive processes in a flexible and adjustable way as a response to internal or external stimuli, for the achievement of desired outcomes (Demetriou, 2000Demetriou, A. (2000). Organization and development of self-understanding and self-regulation: Toward a general theory. In M. Boekaerst, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 209-251). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.; Moilanen, 2007)Moilanen, K. L. (2007). The Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory: The development and validation of a questionnaire of short-term and long-term self-regulation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(6), 835-848. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9107-9.

However, several questions have then been raised by theorists closer to the developmental areas. In a conceptual and methodological review of 123 measures of self-regulation from childhood to adulthood, Moilanen, Goodvin, Raffaelli, and Crockett (2009)Moilanen, K. L., Goodvin, R., Crockett, L., & Raffaelli, M. (2009). Approaches to the measurement of self-regulation from infancy through early adulthood. Unpublished manuscript, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE. verified that measures tend to present limited empirical scrutiny and to focus primarily on the regulation objectives at most immediate or short-term, which could be operationalized as the control of impulses, control of attention and emotional control or regulation of the immediate context. However, the time perspective is quite different over the course of life and there is need to distinguish and evaluate essential long-term components of self-regulation (Moilanen, 2007Moilanen, K. L. (2007). The Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory: The development and validation of a questionnaire of short-term and long-term self-regulation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(6), 835-848. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9107-9). Long-term self-regulation would involve impulse control or direction of the effort for a period of time, several weeks, months, or years. This is the case, for example, when an adolescent takes a course to achieve career goals, or keeps saving money to buy an expensive object. This crucial aspect of self-regulated behaviours that take place during adolescence has been verified in studies about school performance (Bilde, Vansteenkiste, & Lens, 2011Bilde, J., Vansteenkiste, M., & Lens, W. (2011). Understanding the association between future time perspective and self-regulated learning through the lens of self-determination theory. Learning and Instruction, 21(3), 332-344. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.03.002; Husman & Lens, 1999Husman, J., & Lens, W. (1999). The role of the future in student motivation. Educational Psychologist, 34(2), 113-125. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3402_4; Miller & Brickman, 2004)Miller, R. B., & Brickman, S. A. (2004). A model of future oriented motivation and self-regulation. Educational Psychology Review, 16(1), 9-33. doi:10.1023/B:EDPR.0000012343.96370.39 or health behaviours (Hall & Fong, 2007Hall, P. A., & Fong, G. T. (2007). Temporal self-regulation theory: A model for individual health behavior. Health Psychology Review, 1(1), 6-52. doi:10.1080/17437190701492437; Zimbardo & Boyd, 1999)Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271-1288. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1271, but has not been consistently represented in instruments. Given the developmental challenges that have not been consistently represented in assessment instruments, there is a need to promote research based on sound knowledge of developmental science (Silbereisen & Lerner, 2007)Silbereisen, R. K., & Lerner, R. M. (2007). Approaches to positive youth development. London, United Kingdom: SAGE..

Therefore, in this paper we present the results of the Portuguese adaptation of the Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory (ASRI), a developmentally appropriate measure to evaluate short and long-term self-regulation (Moilanen, 2007Moilanen, K. L. (2007). The Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory: The development and validation of a questionnaire of short-term and long-term self-regulation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(6), 835-848. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9107-9). This measure was originally tested with 169 students from the 6th, 8th and 10th grades of a US school with satisfactory validity and reliability. To this instrumental study, the requirements for the adaptation of psychological measures were followed (Borsa, Damásio, & Bandeira, 2012; Carretero-Dias & Pérez, 2007Borsa, J. C., Damásio, B. F., & Bandeira, D. R. (2012). Cross-cultural adaptation and validation of psychological instruments: Some considerations. Paidéia (Ribeirão Preto), 22(53), 423-432. doi:10.1590/1982-43272253201314), considering two studies with independent samples to evaluate the factor structure, with exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis (Henson & Roberts, 2006)Henson, R. K., & Roberts, J. K. (2006). Use of exploratory factor analysis in published research: Common errors and some comment on improved practice. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(3), 393-416. doi:10.1177/0013164405282485. Despite both might be used to the validity evaluation, the first one is specially used for theory-building, while the second one is used primarily for theory-testing (Henson & Roberts, 2006Henson, R. K., & Roberts, J. K. (2006). Use of exploratory factor analysis in published research: Common errors and some comment on improved practice. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(3), 393-416. doi:10.1177/0013164405282485; Matsunaga, 2010)Matsunaga, M. (2010). How to factor-analyse your data right: Do’s, don’ts, and how-to’s. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(1), 97-110.. That way, we start exploring the latent structure of the measure in Portuguese population to deepen our analysis and confirm the theoretical model in the second study.

Method - Study 1

The first study aims examine the latent factor from several observed variables (Henson & Roberts, 2006Henson, R. K., & Roberts, J. K. (2006). Use of exploratory factor analysis in published research: Common errors and some comment on improved practice. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(3), 393-416. doi:10.1177/0013164405282485), and that way explores validity of the ASRI in Portuguese adolescents. Given some limitations of the Exploratory Factor Analysis, we gathered data from a broader sample to which we applied the inventory and Schwarzer (1999)Schwarzer, R. (1999). Self-Regulation. Retrieved from http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/selfreg_g.htm
http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/sel...
Self-regulation Scale as control measure.

Participants

The sample of the study was composed by 823 adolescents from public high schools at the north of Portugal, 27.7% from the 10th grade (n = 228), 38.5% from the 11th grade (n = 317) and 33.8% from the 12th grade (n = 278). Individual ages varied from 14 to 20 years (M = 16.38, SD = 1.09), and the majority were of the female gender (61.2%, n = 503).

Instruments

Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory - ASRI (Moilanen, 2007Moilanen, K. L. (2007). The Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory: The development and validation of a questionnaire of short-term and long-term self-regulation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(6), 835-848. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9107-9). This is a 36 item measure that evaluates the degree to which adolescents are able to activate, monitor, maintain, inhibit, and adapt their emotions, thoughts, attention, and behaviour. The Inventory has a dimensional structure developed specifically to assess adolescent short and long term self-regulation. Short-term self-regulation dimension relates to impulse, attention and emotional regulation toward immediate objectives. Long-term self-regulation dimension involves the evaluation of adolescent efforts to control impulse, attention and emotional regulation toward objectives that cover a longer period of time (as career goals, saving money, etc.). Respondents rate how true each item is for them in a Likert-type response scale, ranging from 1 (not at all true for me) to 5 (really true for me).

Self-regulation Scale - SRS (Schwarzer, 1999Schwarzer, R. (1999). Self-Regulation. Retrieved from http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/selfreg_g.htm
http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/sel...
). This is an unidimentional measure with 10 items that evaluates attention control in goal-directed behaviour. Respondents answer using a closed answer response scale ranging from 1 (not all true) to 4 (totally true). This measure was adapted to Portuguese adolescents demonstrating good psychometric properties in validity and reliability (Cronbach’s α = .78) (Dias, Garcia del Castillo, & Schwarzer, 2008Dias, P. C., Garcia del Castillo, J. A., & Schwarzer, R. (2008). Estudo de adaptação da Escala de Auto-Regulação (Schwarzer, Diehl, & Schmitz, 1999) para a população portuguesa. In A. P. Noronha, C. Machado, L. Almeida, M. Gonçalves, S. Martins, & V. Ramalho (Coords.), Actas da XIII Conferência Internacional de Avaliação Psicológica: Formas e contextos [edição em cd-rom] (s.p.). Braga, Portugal: Psiquilibrios Edições.). In the present sample, the Cronbach’s α was .67.

Procedure

Data collection. With the authorization of the author of the original version of the ASRI, the measure was translated into Portuguese and back translated into English in order to verify its linguistic and cultural adaptation. The measure was then presented to a pilot group and the final version was tested in a non-probabilistic sample of high school students, by geographical proximity of the research center, in their classrooms, during normal class time.

Data analysis. The data was then coded and analysed with SPSS version 17. An Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) of Principal Components with varimax rotation was used to evaluate validity and Cronbach’s alpha to evaluate reliability. Concurrent validity was assessed correlating the inventory structure factors with the self-regulation scale (Schwarzer, 1999Schwarzer, R. (1999). Self-Regulation. Retrieved from http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/selfreg_g.htm
http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/sel...
).

Ethical Considerations

After the school direction and youth’s parents’ consent to this research, adolescents were informed by a researcher of the voluntary nature of the research, that all their answers were anonymous and confidential. After this explanation, students signed the Terms of Free, Prior and Informed Consent and were presented to the questionnaires. Students who had no parental consent or didn’t volunteer to participate in the research were given academic tasks to do during the data gathering.

Results

Examination of Validity

With the application of the Bartlett sphericity test and Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin, we observed values of 3727.34 (p ≤ .001) and .85, showing that the data was adequate for factorial analysis. The ASRI was submitted to EFA forcing the extraction of two factors, as suggested in the original study (Moilanen, 2007Moilanen, K. L. (2007). The Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory: The development and validation of a questionnaire of short-term and long-term self-regulation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(6), 835-848. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9107-9). The factor 1, with an eigenvalue of 4.87, explains 16.978% of the variance and is composed by most of the long-term self-regulation items. The second factor, with an eigenvalue of 2.47 that explains 8.53% of the variance, is composed by the most of the short-term items.

However, this first analysis suggested some limitations in some item loadings, being deleted all the items that presented loadings below .30, the items that didn’t differentiate between short and long-term self-regulation and items who presented high loadings in other than the original factor. The remaining 11 items for the short-term self-regulation and 11 items for the long-term were then studied with respect to their concurrent validity and reliability.

To ensure concurrent validity, was tested the relationship between the ASRI and the SRS. As we can see in Table 1, a moderate correlation was found between the SRS and short-term (r = .40, p < .01) and higher correlation with long-term self-regulation (r = .58, p < .01), according to Pestana and Gageiro (2005)Pestana, M. H., & Gageiro, J. M. (2005). Análise de dados para ciências sociais: A complementaridade do SPSS (4th ed.). Lisboa, Portugal: Edições Sílabo..

Table 1
: Correlations Between Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory and Control Measure

Reliability

Cronbach’s alpha provided internal consistency values of .72 in long-term self-regulation and .84 in short-term self-regulation.

Differential Studies

There was no correlation between self-regulation and age, whereas in the long (p = .926) or short-term (p = .839). In what concerns to gender, a significant difference was found only for long-term self-regulation [t (531.326) = 2.465, p = .014] with average higher scores within girls (M = 74.038, SD = 9.573 to M = 72.032, SD = 11.110).

Method - Study 2

The second study aimed to contribute to the development of the theoretical model that underlies the ASRI. Given the novelty of this measure, new items were written and discussed with specialists to a better comprehension of self-regulation in this developmental phase. Fourteen items theoretically related to short-term self-regulation and 13 to long-term dimension were mixed with the existing items of the ASRI and administered again with the SRS (Schwarzer, 1999Schwarzer, R. (1999). Self-Regulation. Retrieved from http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/selfreg_g.htm
http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/sel...
) to analyse concurrent validity. Confirmatory Factor Analysis was used to ensure that the psychometric properties of the measure are theoretically and empirically sound (Henson & Roberts, 2006Henson, R. K., & Roberts, J. K. (2006). Use of exploratory factor analysis in published research: Common errors and some comment on improved practice. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(3), 393-416. doi:10.1177/0013164405282485; Matsunaga, 2010)Matsunaga, M. (2010). How to factor-analyse your data right: Do’s, don’ts, and how-to’s. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(1), 97-110. with a sample between 200 and 500 adolescents, according to literature (Pilati & Laros, 2007)Pilati, R., & Laros, J. A. (2007). Modelos de equações estruturais em Psicologia: Conceitos e aplicações. Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 23(2), 205-216. doi:10.1590/S0102-37722007000200011.

Participants

The sample for this study was composed by 435 Portuguese public high school students, 52.2% from the 10th grade (n = 227), 36.8% from the 11th (n = 160) and 11% from the 12th (n = 48). The participants’ ages varied from 14 to 19 years (M = 16.06, SD = 1.00) being in majority female (57.5%, n = 248).

Instruments

Were used the 22 original items from the Adolescent Self-regulatory Inventory that resulted from the previous study, 11 items for the short-term self-regulation and 11 items for the long-term, with the 27 new items. As a control measure the Self-regulation Scale (Schwarzer, 1999Schwarzer, R. (1999). Self-Regulation. Retrieved from http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/selfreg_g.htm
http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/sel...
) was used. The measures were collected with a brief demographic questionnaire.

Procedure

Data collection. After analysing the data of the first study, the new items were mixed with the 22 ASRI items that resulted from the previous study. The measures were then tested again with a convenience sample of public high school students, in their classrooms, during the normal class hours.

Data analysis. The data was submitted to a Confirmatory Factor Analyses (CFA) conducted in M-Plus version 4.0 (Muthén & Muthén, 2004Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2004). Mplus user’s guide (3rd ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.) to evaluate the adequacy of the structure of the ASRI-2. The minimal requirements for good model fit were a non-significant χ2-fit statistic, a χ2-fit/df ratio of less than 3, a Comparative Fit Index (CFI) greater than.90, a Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation (RMSEA) ranging from.05 to.10, and a Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) close to 0 (Kleine, 2010)Kleine, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural equation modelling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford.. Validity was examined using SPSS v. 17.0. The construct validity of the ASRI-2 was evaluated through an examination of the correlations between the latent ASRI-2 scales and the latent comparison measures of self-regulation. Finally the concurrent validity was evaluated through an examination of correlations between the latent ASRI-2 scales and several indices of academic performance and substance use behaviors.

Ethical Considerations

As in the previous study, school and parents’ consent was gathered and adolescents were informed of the volunteer, anonymity and confidentiality of the collected data. After the explanation, the Terms of Free, Prior and Informed Consent was signed and the questionnaires presented to students with parental consent and who volunteered to this research.

Results

Confirmatory Factor Analyses

Single factor model. In this initial single-factor model, all items from the long-term and short-term scales were constrained to load on one factor. The single-factor model did not provide an adequate fit to the data, χ2 fit (860) = 2240.57, p < .001, χ2 fit/df ratio = 2.60, CFI = .61, RMSEA = .061 (90% confidence interval = .058 – .064), SRMR = .069. All indicators except for item 34 loaded on the single factor (λ = .13, p > .05), and item 16 loaded rather weakly (λ = .23, p < .05). Five items’ loadings exceeded 1.00, qualifying as Heywood cases (Heywood, 1931).

Two factor model. A two-factor solution in which the long-term and short-term self-regulation factors were allowed to correlate was also evaluated. Like the single factor model, the two-factor model did not provide a fully adequate fit to the data, χ2 fit (859) = 2186.02, p < .001, χ2 fit/df ratio = 2.54, CFI = .62, RMSEA = .060 (90% confidence interval = .057 – .053), SRMR = .070, but the two-factor model fit the data significantly better than the single-factor model, Δχ2 fit (1) = 54.55 p < .001. The long-term and short-term factors were strongly correlated (r = .82, p < .001). All items loaded significantly on their specified factor, although some items’ loadings were weaker than conventional standards. No Heywood cases emerged in the two-factor model, and no items loaded on both factors. The model modification indices indicated that the model fit could be improved by allowing several items to correlate.

Revised two-factor model. A final model was fit to the data, corresponding to the two factor model with six correlations between indicator residual variances (Table 2). Specifically, correlations were specified between items 19 and 29, items 15 and 25, items 17 and 29, items 10 and 13, 3 and 46, and 15 with 22. These changes collectively improved model fit: χ2 fit (853) = 1949.48, p < .001, χ2 fit/df ratio = 2.29, CFI = .69, RMSEA = .054 (90% C. I. = .051 – .058), SRMR = .07, Δχ2 fit (6) = 236.54, p < .001. The two ASRI-2 factors were positively correlated (r = .59, p < .001).

Table 2
: Confirmatory Factor Loadings of the ASRI-2

Examination of Validity

Construct and concurrent validity. As presented in Table 3, both ASRI-2 factors were positively and strongly correlated with the comparison measure. The correlations were strong with the Schwarzer (1999)Schwarzer, R. (1999). Self-Regulation. Retrieved from http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/selfreg_g.htm
http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/sel...
scale (short-term and long-term rs = .59 and .56).

Table 3
: Correlations Between Scales, Self-Reported Grades, and Self-Reported Behavior

Additional correlations were examined in order to confirm that adolescents’ responses on the ASRI-2 were statistically associated with constructs theoretically and empirically linked to self-regulation. Both short and long-term self-regulation factors were associated with academic performance and behavior: high levels of both dimensions of self-regulation were associated with studying a greater number of hours at home and better grades in math and Portuguese. Long-term ASRI-2 scores were more strongly associated with hours of studying at home and Portuguese grades. With two exceptions, both factors were also associated with substance use behavior. High levels of short- and long-term self-regulation were associated with current absence of substance use, such as tobacco use, fewer days of alcohol use, and fewer times they get drunk. Additionally, high levels of long-term self-regulation were associated with absence of alcohol use, and fewer cigarettes smoked.

Differential Studies

Considering self-regulation according to gender and age variables, we could find no relation with this second variable, whereas in the long (p = .962) or short-term (p = .570). As presented in study 1, a significant gender difference was found in long-term self-regulation [t (361) = -6.297, p < .01] with average higher scores within girls (M = 88.721, SD = 9.490 to M = 81.806, SD = 11.109).

Discussion

This paper aimed to present the Portuguese version and the improvement of a new measure for the evaluation of self-regulation in adolescence, the Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory (Moilanen, 2007Moilanen, K. L. (2007). The Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory: The development and validation of a questionnaire of short-term and long-term self-regulation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(6), 835-848. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9107-9). This is a relatively recent instrument, which is in translation and adaptation process to several countries, as a way to overcome limitations arising from the application of adult self-regulation models to adolescence (Gibbons et al., 2006Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., Reimer, R. A., & Pomery, E. A. (2006). Unintentional behavior: A subrational approach to health risk. In D. T. D. Ridder, & J. B. F. de Wit (Eds.), Self-regulation in health behavior (pp. 45-70). Chichester, United Kingdom: John Wiley & Sons.; Moilanen et al., 2009Moilanen, K. L., Goodvin, R., Crockett, L., & Raffaelli, M. (2009). Approaches to the measurement of self-regulation from infancy through early adulthood. Unpublished manuscript, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE.). In theoretical terms, the assumed perspective has been cited in other studies in this field (Gestsdottir et al., 2010Gestsdottir, S., Bowers, E., von Eye, A., Napolitano, C. M., & Lerner, R. M. (2010). Intentional self-regulation in middle adolescence: The emerging role of loss-based selection in positive youth development. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(7), 764-782. doi:10.1007/s10964-010-9537-2; Gestsdottir & Lerner, 2008Gestsdottir, S., & Lerner, R. M. (2008). Positive development in adolescence: The development and role of intentional self-regulation. Human Development, 51(3), 202-224. doi:10.1159/000135757; Lerner et al., 2011)Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., Bowers, E. P., Lewin-Bizan, S., Gestsdottir, S., & Urban, J. B. (2011). Self-regulation processes and thriving in childhood and adolescence: A view of the issues. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 133, 1-9. doi:10.1002/cd.300, which encourage researchers interested in this issue to continue the empirical studies.

Thus, the first study aimed to adapt the existing version of the ASRI to the Portuguese population, exploring the latent structure of the items with Exploratory Factor Analysis. With the elimination of items that could decrease the discriminative power of the instrument, a version with good psychometric properties was reached, either in its validity or fidelity, and with a positive correlation with an existing self-regulation scale adapted to Portuguese population (Dias et al., 2008Dias, P. C., Garcia del Castillo, J. A., & Schwarzer, R. (2008). Estudo de adaptação da Escala de Auto-Regulação (Schwarzer, Diehl, & Schmitz, 1999) para a população portuguesa. In A. P. Noronha, C. Machado, L. Almeida, M. Gonçalves, S. Martins, & V. Ramalho (Coords.), Actas da XIII Conferência Internacional de Avaliação Psicológica: Formas e contextos [edição em cd-rom] (s.p.). Braga, Portugal: Psiquilibrios Edições.).

The inclusion of new items, intentionally developed to evaluate short-term and long-term dimensions, increased the collected information about self-regulation and maintained the psychometric properties of the measure. Despite the need to introduce some modification indices on items with a closer meaning, Confirmatory Factor Analysis provided a theoretically and empirically satisfactory solution in line with the original two-factor model that distinguishes short and long-term self-regulation, maintaining the correlation with the control measure and equivalent results at the differential studies. Regarding gender, despite conflicting results in the literature, there is a tendency for girls to present higher self-regulation, what tends to be associated with evolutionary issues or least controlling parenting practices over boys (McCabe & Brooks-Gunn, 2007McCabe, L. A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2007). With a little help from my friends?: Self-regulation in groups of young children. Infant Mental Health Journal, 28(6), 584-605. doi:10.1002/imhj.20155; Raffaelli, Crockett, & Shen, 2005Raffaelli, M., Crockett, L. J., & Shen, Y. L. (2005). Developmental stability and change in self-regulation from childhood to adolescence. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166(1), 54-75. doi:10.3200/GNTP.166.1.54-76). Also in line with existing research, the results tend to reflect the stability of self-regulation over time (Raffaelli et al., 2005)Raffaelli, M., Crockett, L. J., & Shen, Y. L. (2005). Developmental stability and change in self-regulation from childhood to adolescence. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166(1), 54-75. doi:10.3200/GNTP.166.1.54-76.

The results also highlight the relation between self-regulation and academic and substance use variables, in line with previous research. They point out the positive correlation between self-regulation and hours of study and school grades (McClelland & Cameron, 2011McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2011). Self-regulation and academic achievement in elementary school children. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 133, 29-44. doi:10.1002/cd.302; Zimmerman, 2008)Zimmerman, B. J. (2008). Investigating self-regulation and motivation: Historical background, methodological developments, and future prospects. American Educational Research Journal, 45(1), 166-183. doi:10.3102/0002831207312909 but also a negative correlation with substance use (Garcia del Castillo, Dias, & Castelar-Perim, 2012Garcia del Castillo, J. A., Dias, P. C., & Castelar-Perim, P. (2012). Autorregulação e consumo de substâncias na adolescência. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25(2), 238-247. doi:10.1590/S0102-79722012000200005; Gardner et al., 2006Gardner, T. W., Dishion, T. J., & Posner, M. I. (2006). Attention and adolescent tobacco use: A potential self-regulator dynamic underlying nicotine addiction. Addictive Behaviors, 31(3), 531-536. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.05.018; Wills & Dishion, 2004)Wills T. A., & Dishion, T. J. (2004). Temperament and adolescent substance use: A transactional analysis of emerging self-control. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 33(1), 69-81. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_7. These results suggest that students with higher self-regulation might achieve better in school and involve less in smoking or alcohol use. Moreover, they suggest also a consistently higher correlation of long-term self-regulation with these variables, what might reinforce the bidimentional model and the determinant role of the time perspective in adolescent development (Hall & Fong, 2007Hall, P. A., & Fong, G. T. (2007). Temporal self-regulation theory: A model for individual health behavior. Health Psychology Review, 1(1), 6-52. doi:10.1080/17437190701492437; Miller & Brickman, 2004Miller, R. B., & Brickman, S. A. (2004). A model of future oriented motivation and self-regulation. Educational Psychology Review, 16(1), 9-33. doi:10.1023/B:EDPR.0000012343.96370.39; Zimbardo & Boyd, 1999)Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271-1288. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1271. These might be exciting results that open new lines of research to deepen our understanding of the role this process plays in resilience (Gardner et al., 2008)Gardner, T. W., Dishion, T. J., & Connell, A. M. (2008). Adolescent self-regulation as resilience: Resistance to antisocial behavior within the deviant peer context. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(2), 273-284. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9176-6 and positive youth development (Gestsdottir & Lerner, 2008)Gestsdottir, S., & Lerner, R. M. (2008). Positive development in adolescence: The development and role of intentional self-regulation. Human Development, 51(3), 202-224. doi:10.1159/000135757.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this study allowed us to contribute to improve our knowledge about self-regulation in a fundamental stage of human development. With the presented studies, we adapted an existing version of an instrument measure to evaluate self-regulation to Portuguese context, but also contributed to the testing of new items that we expect to contribute to a more comprehensive and reliable vision of this developmental process in adolescence. For this purpose, two convenience samples were used, from public schools in the north of Portugal. As we previous noticed, the sample has a higher percentage of girls, in accordance with official statistics of Ministry of Education for Secondary Education (Direção-Geral de Estatísticas da Educação e Ciência, 2013Direção-Geral de Estatísticas da Educação e Ciência. (2013). Educação em números: Portugal 2013. Lisboa, Portugal: DGEEC.) - about 44.4% boys and 55.6% girls in regular Secondary Education - that might be considered in the generalization of the data. Also pragmatic reasons for school selection, given geographical proximity of the researcher, may limit the generalization of our inferences. So future studies should retest this structure with more representative samples of the Portuguese adolescent population. Also the collection of data about parents’ socio-economic or educational level can be important for a more detailed analysis of this process.

In the end, the ASRI-2 consists of 43 items, with 19 for short-term and 24 for long-term self-regulation. Given the recent nature of the measure, future studies will test the stability of this structure, either in their validity or in reliability, particularly temporal reliability. Apart from the limitations, this study might have a substantial contribution to a better knowledge of self-regulation in adolescence, opening new lines for research and for the development of youth positive development programs.

References

  • Annesi, J. J., & Gorjala, S. (2010). Relations of self-regulation and self-efficacy for exercise and eating and BMI change: A field investigation. BioPsychoSocial Medicine, 4:10. doi:10.1186/1751-0759-4-10
  • Bilde, J., Vansteenkiste, M., & Lens, W. (2011). Understanding the association between future time perspective and self-regulated learning through the lens of self-determination theory. Learning and Instruction, 21(3), 332-344. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2010.03.002
  • Borsa, J. C., Damásio, B. F., & Bandeira, D. R. (2012). Cross-cultural adaptation and validation of psychological instruments: Some considerations. Paidéia (Ribeirão Preto), 22(53), 423-432. doi:10.1590/1982-43272253201314
  • Carretero-Dios, H., & Pérez, C. (2007). Standards for the development and review of instrumental studies: Considerations about test selection in psychological research. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 7, 863-882.
  • Demetriou, A. (2000). Organization and development of self-understanding and self-regulation: Toward a general theory. In M. Boekaerst, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 209-251). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Dias, P. C., Garcia del Castillo, J. A., & Schwarzer, R. (2008). Estudo de adaptação da Escala de Auto-Regulação (Schwarzer, Diehl, & Schmitz, 1999) para a população portuguesa. In A. P. Noronha, C. Machado, L. Almeida, M. Gonçalves, S. Martins, & V. Ramalho (Coords.), Actas da XIII Conferência Internacional de Avaliação Psicológica: Formas e contextos [edição em cd-rom] (s.p.). Braga, Portugal: Psiquilibrios Edições.
  • Diehl, M., Semegon, A. B., & Schwarzer, R. (2006). Assessing attention control in goal pursuit: A component of dispositional self-regulation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 86(3), 306-317. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa8603_06
  • Direção-Geral de Estatísticas da Educação e Ciência. (2013). Educação em números: Portugal 2013 Lisboa, Portugal: DGEEC.
  • Elliot, A. J., Thrash, T. M., & Murayama, K. (2011). A longitudinal analysis of self-regulation and well-being: Avoidance personal goals, avoidance coping, stress generation, and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 79(3), 643-674. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00694.x
  • Garcia del Castillo, J. A., & Dias, P. C. (2009). Auto-regulação, resiliência e consumo de substâncias na adolescência: Contributos da adaptação do questionário reduzido de auto-regulação. Psicologia, Saúde e Doenças, 10(2), 205-216.
  • Garcia del Castillo, J. A., Dias, P. C., & Castelar-Perim, P. (2012). Autorregulação e consumo de substâncias na adolescência. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 25(2), 238-247. doi:10.1590/S0102-79722012000200005
  • Garcia del Castillo, J. A., Dias, P. C., Diaz-Pérez, J., & Garcia del Castillo-López, A. (2012). The mediating role of self-regulation in cigarette smoking and alcohol use among young people. Anales de Psicologia, 28(1), 1-10.
  • Gardner, T. W., Dishion, T. J., & Connell, A. M. (2008). Adolescent self-regulation as resilience: Resistance to antisocial behavior within the deviant peer context. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(2), 273-284. doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9176-6
  • Gardner, T. W., Dishion, T. J., & Posner, M. I. (2006). Attention and adolescent tobacco use: A potential self-regulator dynamic underlying nicotine addiction. Addictive Behaviors, 31(3), 531-536. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.05.018
  • Gestsdottir, S., Bowers, E., von Eye, A., Napolitano, C. M., & Lerner, R. M. (2010). Intentional self-regulation in middle adolescence: The emerging role of loss-based selection in positive youth development. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(7), 764-782. doi:10.1007/s10964-010-9537-2
  • Gestsdottir, S., & Lerner, R. M. (2008). Positive development in adolescence: The development and role of intentional self-regulation. Human Development, 51(3), 202-224. doi:10.1159/000135757
  • Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., Reimer, R. A., & Pomery, E. A. (2006). Unintentional behavior: A subrational approach to health risk. In D. T. D. Ridder, & J. B. F. de Wit (Eds.), Self-regulation in health behavior (pp. 45-70). Chichester, United Kingdom: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Hagger, M. S., Wood, C. W., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L., (2010). Self-regulation and self-control in exercise: The strength-energy model. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 3(1), 62-86. doi:10.1080/17509840903322815
  • Hall, P. A., & Fong, G. T. (2007). Temporal self-regulation theory: A model for individual health behavior. Health Psychology Review, 1(1), 6-52. doi:10.1080/17437190701492437
  • Henson, R. K., & Roberts, J. K. (2006). Use of exploratory factor analysis in published research: Common errors and some comment on improved practice. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(3), 393-416. doi:10.1177/0013164405282485
  • Hofer, J., Busch, H., & Kärtner, J. (2011). Self-regulation and well-being: The influence of identity and motives. European Journal of Personality, 25(3), 211-224. doi:10.1002/per.789
  • Heywood, H. B. (1931). On finite sequences of real numbers. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 134(824), 486-501.
  • Husman, J., & Lens, W. (1999). The role of the future in student motivation. Educational Psychologist, 34(2), 113-125. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3402_4
  • Kleine, R. B. (2010). Principles and practice of structural equation modelling (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford.
  • Lerner, R. M., Lerner, J. V., Bowers, E. P., Lewin-Bizan, S., Gestsdottir, S., & Urban, J. B. (2011). Self-regulation processes and thriving in childhood and adolescence: A view of the issues. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 133, 1-9. doi:10.1002/cd.300
  • Luszczynska, A., Diehl, M., Gutiérrez-Doña, B., Kuusinen, P., & Schwarzer, R. (2004). Measuring one component of dispositional self-regulation: Attention control in goal pursuit. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(3), 555-566. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.026
  • MacDonald, A., & Palfai, T. (2008). Predictors of exercise behavior among university student women: Utility of a goal-systems/self-regulation theory framework. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(4), 921-931. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.024.
  • Matsunaga, M. (2010). How to factor-analyse your data right: Do’s, don’ts, and how-to’s. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(1), 97-110.
  • McCabe, L. A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2007). With a little help from my friends?: Self-regulation in groups of young children. Infant Mental Health Journal, 28(6), 584-605. doi:10.1002/imhj.20155
  • McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2011). Self-regulation and academic achievement in elementary school children. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 133, 29-44. doi:10.1002/cd.302
  • Miller, R. B., & Brickman, S. A. (2004). A model of future oriented motivation and self-regulation. Educational Psychology Review, 16(1), 9-33. doi:10.1023/B:EDPR.0000012343.96370.39
  • Moilanen, K. L. (2007). The Adolescent Self-Regulatory Inventory: The development and validation of a questionnaire of short-term and long-term self-regulation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36(6), 835-848. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9107-9
  • Moilanen, K. L., Goodvin, R., Crockett, L., & Raffaelli, M. (2009). Approaches to the measurement of self-regulation from infancy through early adulthood Unpublished manuscript, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE.
  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2004). Mplus user’s guide (3rd ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
  • Pestana, M. H., & Gageiro, J. M. (2005). Análise de dados para ciências sociais: A complementaridade do SPSS (4th ed.). Lisboa, Portugal: Edições Sílabo.
  • Pilati, R., & Laros, J. A. (2007). Modelos de equações estruturais em Psicologia: Conceitos e aplicações. Psicologia: Teoria e Prática, 23(2), 205-216. doi:10.1590/S0102-37722007000200011
  • Raffaelli, M., Crockett, L. J., & Shen, Y. L. (2005). Developmental stability and change in self-regulation from childhood to adolescence. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166(1), 54-75. doi:10.3200/GNTP.166.1.54-76
  • Schwarzer, R. (1999). Self-Regulation Retrieved from http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/selfreg_g.htm
    » http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/selfreg_g.htm
  • Silbereisen, R. K., & Lerner, R. M. (2007). Approaches to positive youth development London, United Kingdom: SAGE.
  • Wills T. A., & Dishion, T. J. (2004). Temperament and adolescent substance use: A transactional analysis of emerging self-control. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 33(1), 69-81. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_7
  • Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271-1288. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1271
  • Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Skinner, E. A. (2008). Adolescents coping with stress: Development and diversity. Prevention Researcher, 15(4), 3-7.
  • Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Skinner, E. A. (2011). Review: The development of coping across childhood and adolescence: An integrative review and critique of research. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35(1), 1-17. doi:10.1177/0165025410384923
  • Zimmerman, B. J. (2008). Investigating self-regulation and motivation: Historical background, methodological developments, and future prospects. American Educational Research Journal, 45(1), 166-183. doi:10.3102/0002831207312909
  • Support: Strategic Program of the Centre for Philosophical and Humanistic Studies, funded by Foundation for Science and Technology (PEst-OE/FIL/UI0683/2014)
  • 1
    Paper derived from doctoral dissertation of the first author under the advisory of the second author in the Graduate Program of Health Psychology at the Universidad Miguel Hernández, Spain.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    May-August 2014

History

  • Received
    8 May 2013
  • Reviewed
    24 Apr 2014
  • Reviewed
    20 May 2014
  • Accepted
    2 June 2014
Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Filosofia Ciências e Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Av.Bandeirantes 3900 - Monte Alegre, 14040-901 Ribeirão Preto - São Paulo - Brasil, Tel.: (55 16) 3315-3829 - Ribeirão Preto - SP - Brazil
E-mail: paideia@usp.br