Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Phonological awareness assessment in Williams syndrome

Abstract:

PURPOSES:

assess the performance of children and adolescents with Williams Syndrome in tasks of phonological awareness and analyze it in terms of age, education and indicators of intellectual skills.

METHODS:

twenty two children and adolescents (11 boys and 11 girls), aged 7 to 18 years old, took Block Design and Vocabulary subtests of Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children to estimate intellectual skills, and the Phonological Awareness Test by Oral Production.

RESULTS:

participants obtained estimate IQ values compatible with intellectual disability. Phonological awareness results indicated that participants with the syndrome had a lower performance in nine out of ten subtests of the Phonological Awareness Test compared to standard scores for children with same age and education level. No significant correlations were found among phonological awareness, intellectual skill indicators, age and level of education.

CONCLUSIONS:

results corroborate the findings of international researches that suggest difficulties in phonological awareness processes in Williams Syndrome. Considering the relevance of these findings, it is necessary to include continuous stimulation programs, including early intervention for preschoolers with Williams Syndrome, adaptation of teaching methods and curriculum.

Keywords:
Williams Syndrome; Intellectual Disability; Awareness

Resumo:

OBJETIVOS:

avaliar o desempenho de crianças e adolescentes com Síndrome de Williams em tarefas de consciência fonológica e analisar esse desempenho em função da idade, escolaridade e indicadores de habilidade intelectual.

MÉTODOS:

vinte e duas crianças e adolescentes (11 meninos e 11 meninas) com idades entre 7 e 18 anos realizaram os subtestes cubos e vocabulário das Escala Wechsler de Inteligência para estimativa de habilidades intelectuais e responderam a Prova de Consciência Fonológica por produção oral.

RESULTADOS:

os participantes obtiveram valores do quociente de inteligência estimado compatível com rebaixamento intelectual. Nas habilidades de consciência fonológica os resultados mostraram que os participantes com a síndrome obtiveram desempenho rebaixado em nove dos dez subtestes da prova de consciência fonológica em relação à pontuação padronizada em função da idade, assim como do nível escolar. Não foram encontradas correlações significantes entre consciência fonológica, indicadores de habilidade intelectual, idade e escolaridade.

CONCLUSÃO:

os resultados corroboram os encontrados em pesquisas internacionais sugerindo rebaixamento em consciência fonológica na Síndrome de Williams. Dada a relevância desses achados é necessário incluir programas de estimulação contínua, inclusive com intervenções precoces dirigidas a crianças com Síndrome de Williams em idade pré-escolar, adequação de métodos de ensino e de currículo adaptado.

Descritores:
Síndrome de Williams; Deficiência Intelectual; Consciência

Introduction

Williams Syndorme (WS) is a genetic disorder caused by a multigenic hemizygous deletion in chromosome 7q11.23 that, in 95% of the cases, encompasses from 1,5 to 1,6 Mb (deletion of approximately 26 genes), with prevalence above 1:7.500 live borns11 Porter MA, Dobson-Stone C, Kwok JBJ, Schofield PR, Beckett W, Beckett W et al. A Role for Transcription Factor GTF2IRD2 in Executive Function in Williams-Beuren Syndrome. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e474-570. 44 Collins RT, Aziz PF, Gleason MM, Kaplan PB, Shah MJ. Abnormalities of cardiac repolarization in Williams syndrome. Am J Cardiol. 2010; 1;106(7):1029-33.. WS is diagnosed through clinical assessment, usually during childhood, based on phenotypic traits such as prominent cheek, upturned nose, long philtrum and cardiovascular symptoms, especially supravalvular aortic stenosis. Other typical alterations are neurobehavioral dysfunctions, growth alterations, gastrointestinal and renal dysfunctions55 Greer J, Riby DM, Hamiliton C, Riby LM. Attentional lapse and inhibition control in adults with Williams Syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(11):4170-7. 77 Pober BR. Williams-Beuren syndrome. N Engl J Med. 2010;21;362(3):239-52.. The definite diagnosis must be confirmed by genetic-molecular exams. Although there are more severe cases, most people with WS present mild to moderate intellectual disability (ID)88 Honjo RS. Detecção da microdeleção 7q11.23 por MLPA(r) e estudo clínico dos pacientes com síndrome de Williams-Beuren [tese]. São Paulo (SP): Universidade de São Paulo; 2012..

In the last 10 years, the WS has been receiving considerable attention due to its specific cognitive profile99 Martens MA, Wilson SJ, Reutens DC. Research Review: Williams syndrome: a critical review of the cognitive, behavioral, and neuroanatomical phenotype. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(6):576-608. 1313 Nunes MM, Honjo RS, Dutra RL, Amaral VS, Amaral VAS, Oh HK et al. Assessment of Intellectual and VisuoSpatial Abilities in Children and Adults with Williams Syndrome. Universitas Psychologica. 2013;12(2):581-9.. In terms of neurocognitive phenotype, WS is often described as a profile of "peaks and valleys". People with WS are relatively proficient in concrete language skills and have a better performance in expressive language than in receptive; this contrasts sharply with the difficulties in syntactic-pragmatic language skills, structural and functional linguistic limitations, deficits in executive and visuospatial skills (work memory and planning) and severely impaired learning1212 Osório A, Cruz R, Sampaio A, Garayzábal E, Martínez-Regueiro R, Gonçalves ÓF et al. How executive functions are related to intelligence in Williams syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2012;33(4):1169-75. 1414 Mervis CB. Language and Literacy Development of Children with Williams Syndrome. Top Lang Disord. 2009;29(2):149-69..

Studies on phonological awareness in individuals with WS have shown that the phonological process plays an important role for the acquisition of reading skills1515 Laing E, Hulme C, Grant J, Karmiloff-Smith A. Learning to read in Williams syndrome: looking beneath the surface of atypical reading development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001;42(6):729-39. 1717 Becerra AM, John AE, Peregrine E and Mervis CB. Reading Abilities of 9 - 17-Year- Olds with Williams Syndrome: Impact of Reading Method. Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders; Madison, WI. 2008.. Phonological awareness is an intentional reflection on speech that has different levels, i.e., the segmentation of spoken language can happen in different units: a sentence can be segmented in words, words can be segmented in syllables and syllables can be segmented in phonemes1818 Seabra AG, Capovilla FC. Problemas de leitura e escrita: Como identificar, prevenir e remediar numa abordagem fônica. 6.ed. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas. 2012. 1919 Seabra AG, Dias NM. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas, 2012.. Indeed, knowing that the language has a specific system of sounds, that these units are repeated in different spoken words and that orthography can be converted in phonology is a relevant aspect for reading acquisition. Thus, phonological processing is required for the acquisition of reading skills through decoding letters and groups of letters in their corresponding sounds. This skill is, in its turn, key to the capacity of reading words fluently1919 Seabra AG, Dias NM. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas, 2012. 2121 Maluf MR. Do conhecimento implícito à consciência metalinguística indispensável na alfabetização. In: Guimarães SRK, Maluf MR. Aprendizagem da linguagem escrita: contribuições da pesquisa. São Paulo: Vetor Editora. 2010. p. 17-32..

Considering the relevance of phonological awareness for the acquisition of written language and the scarce number of studies that assess phonological awareness in children with WS, the present study had the objective of assessing the performance of children and adolescents with WS in phonological awareness tests and analyzing this performance in terms of age, education level and intellectual ability (estimate intelligence quotient - IQ).

Methods

The present study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Universidade Presbiteriana Mackenzie and was registered under CEP/UPM nº 1191/11/2009 and CAAE nº 0090.0.272.000.09. According to ethical norms for researches with humans, all participants and their legal guardians were instructed in advance about the objectives of the study and signed an informed consent form.

Participants

22 children and adolescents diagnosed with WS took part of this study, 11 of them (50%) were male and 11 (50%) female, ranging from 7 to 18 years old (M=11,6; SD=3,7). Concerning school attendance, 12 (54,6%) are enrolled in a mainstream public school, 5 (22,7%) in private schools and 5 (22,7%) special education schools. Each child was assessed individually in a 90'-session with intervals between tasks.

Tools and Procedures

To assess the sample's intellectual potential we used Block Design and Vocabulary subtests of Wechsler Intelligence Scales, WISC-III2222 Wechsler, D WISC III: Escala de inteligência Wechsler para crianças: manual. 3.ed. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2002. or WAIS-III2323 Nascimento E. Adaptação e validação do teste WAIS-III para um contexto brasileiro [tese]. Brasília (DF): Universidade de Brasília; 2000.depending on the age, in order to obtain the estimate IQ2424 Mello CB, Argollo N, Shayer BPM, Abreu N, Godinho K, Durán P et al. Abbreviated version of the WISC-III: correlation between estimated IQ and global IQ of brazilian children. Psicologia Teoria e Pesquisa. 2011;27(2):149-55..

After that, participants took the Phonological Awareness Test by Oral Production (PAT-OP)1919 Seabra AG, Dias NM. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas, 2012., which consists of ten subtests with 4 items each. Initially, the examiner presented two examples in order to explain what the child was expected to do:

1) syllabic synthesis: the child has to unite the syllables spoken by the examiner and speak the resulting word;

2) phonemic synthesis: the child has to join phonemes spoken by the examiner;

3) rhyme: the child has to choose, out of three words, two that end with the same sound;

4) alliteration: the child has to choose, out of three words, two that start with the same sound;

5) syllabic segmentation: the child has to divide a word spoken by the examiner into its component syllables;

6) phonemic segmentation: the child has to divide a word spoken by the examiner into its component phonemes;

7) syllabic manipulation: the child has to add and subtract syllables and say the word that is formed;

8) phonemic manipulation: the child has to add or subtract phonemes and say the word that is formed;

9) syllabic transposition: the child as to invert syllables and say the word that is formed;

10) phonemic transposition: the child has to invert phonemes and say the word that is formed.

In order to assess the response patterns for each participant, the total number of correct answers in the test was analyzed. Individual results were then compared to standardized data2525 Dias NM, Trevisan BT, Seabra AG. Dados normativos da Prova de Consciência Fonológica por produção Oral. In: Alessandra G. Seabra; Natália Martins Dias, organizador. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem Oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas; 2012. p. 109-15. obtained from the performance of children enrolled in pre-school and elementary school, with conversion of raw scores into standard scores (Mean = 100; Standard Deviation = 15). We used Wilcoxon analysis to compare mean results according to level of education (WS group X Normative group) and Pearson correlation analysis to describe associations among age, education and indicators of intellectual abilities. Results with p values ≤0,05 were considered significant.

Results

Table 1 presents the general profile of the 22 children and adolescents with WS in terms of gender, age, education, intellectual abilities (estimate IQ) and total performance at the PAT-OP. Children and adolescents that formed the sample obtained estimate IQ values compatible with mild to moderate intellectual disability according to the standardization of the test.

The comparison of PAT-OP results of children with WS and normative data according to age2525 Dias NM, Trevisan BT, Seabra AG. Dados normativos da Prova de Consciência Fonológica por produção Oral. In: Alessandra G. Seabra; Natália Martins Dias, organizador. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem Oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas; 2012. p. 109-15.revealed that 3 participants had "low" classification while 19 were classified as "very low". However, considering that most individuals with WS do not study at the grade corresponding to their age, we have opted to consider the mean age equivalent to each grade. Thus, we considered the following: 7 years old = 1st grade; 8 years old = 2nd grade; 9 years old = 3rd grade; 10 years old = 4th grade; 11 years old = 5th grade; 12 years old = 6th grade; 13 years old = 7th grade; and 14 years old = 8th grade. Then, we proceeded to comparing normative data according to grade, so that participants with WS were compared to younger children but which study at the same grade. Concerning the classification at PAT-OP, the comparison of performances of children with WS with normative data according to education2525 Dias NM, Trevisan BT, Seabra AG. Dados normativos da Prova de Consciência Fonológica por produção Oral. In: Alessandra G. Seabra; Natália Martins Dias, organizador. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem Oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas; 2012. p. 109-15. revealed that one participant presented low classification while the remaining 21 presented "very low" classification. These findings indicate that children with WS had performances below the expected at PAT-OP considering standard scores according to age and level of education, even when compared to younger children. Table 1 shows the classification of participants according to age and level of education.

Table 1:
Characterization of participants, including gender, education, IQ, total score at pat-op and classification considering normative data

Special education (SE) students were compared to the grade that corresponds to their age: 8y10m with 1st grade; 11y with 5thgrade; 13y7m with 7th grade; and above 14y with 8th grade.

Table 2 shows that phonemic aspects of phonological awareness present a higher degree of difficulty in syllabic aspects.

Table 2:
Descriptive analysis of phonological awareness aspects

In the studied sample, the group presented relatively high percentages of aspects of syllabic synthesis, rhyme, alliteration and syllabic segmentation; unlike aspects of phonemic synthesis, segmentation, manipulation and transposition, which were relatively low.

Table 3summarizes results of the group with WS at PAT-OP for each school grade. Wilcoxon analysis revealed a significant difference (Z = - 4,110; p< 0,001) between the performance of the WS group and the expected result according to level of education based on PAT-OP's normative data2525 Dias NM, Trevisan BT, Seabra AG. Dados normativos da Prova de Consciência Fonológica por produção Oral. In: Alessandra G. Seabra; Natália Martins Dias, organizador. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem Oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas; 2012. p. 109-15..

Table 3:
Mean total correct answers in pat-op for the group with Williams Syndrome and normative group

A Pearson correlation analysis was also made of total performance at PAT-OP and age (months), years of study / education (1st to 8th) and indicators of intellectual abilities. As it can be seen on Table 4, no significant correlations were observed.

Table 4:
Pearson correlation analysis of results in the phonological awareness test and factors age (months), education (1st to 8th grade) and indicators of intellectual skills

Discussion

Compared to normative data, results obtained by individuals with WS were lower in almost all PAT-OP subtests. Only at the syllabic synthesis subtest did individuals with WS present adequate results in relation to normative data indexes. This information suggests individuals with WS acquire syllabic awareness before phonemic awareness, as it is the case for people with typical development1818 Seabra AG, Capovilla FC. Problemas de leitura e escrita: Como identificar, prevenir e remediar numa abordagem fônica. 6.ed. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas. 2012.. Moreover, syllabic skills seem to be less impaired than general intelligence skills, as shown in Table 1.

However, it is important to point that this is the simplest subtest, seeing that syllabic analysis and other supra-segmental skills tend to develop more naturally once that syllables are units that require less analysis effort1818 Seabra AG, Capovilla FC. Problemas de leitura e escrita: Como identificar, prevenir e remediar numa abordagem fônica. 6.ed. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas. 2012.. Lower scores were found in phonemic synthesis, phonemic manipulation and phonemic transposition. This can be explained by the fact that phonemes are the smallest units of the language (which can also explain the difficulty to perceive them) and that phonetic and phonemic segments require a high degree of analytical capacity. Nevertheless, possible relations between phonemic awareness and syllabic skills in children with WS are still not well established22 Mervis CB, Velleman S. Children with Williams Syndrome: Language, Cognitive, and Behavioral Characteristics and their Implications for Intervention. Perspect Lang Learn Educ. 2011;1;18(3):98-107.. The impaired phonological awareness skills found in the group with WS can be part of the typical language phenotype of the syndrome, in which there is often sophisticated domain of syntax and vocabulary, while metacognitive deficits are also present, once the individuals themselves do not understand the immediate implications of phrases and sentences they say1414 Mervis CB. Language and Literacy Development of Children with Williams Syndrome. Top Lang Disord. 2009;29(2):149-69. 1515 Laing E, Hulme C, Grant J, Karmiloff-Smith A. Learning to read in Williams syndrome: looking beneath the surface of atypical reading development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001;42(6):729-39. 2626 Piattelli-Palmarini M. Speaking of learning: how do we acquire our marvellous facility for expressing ourselves in words? Nature. 2001;411:887-8..

Both age and level of education influence the development of phonological awareness, that is, these two factors contribute to the development of metaphonolofical competences2727 Maluf MR, Zanella MS, Pagnez KSMM. Metalinguistic abilities and written language in brazilian research. Boletim de Psicologia. 2006;LVI(124):67-92.. However, Pearson correlation analysis of performance and variables such as age, level of education (1st to 8th grade) and IQ did not reveal significant correlations. The findings corroborate previous studies2828 Laing E, Hulme C, Grant J, Karmiloff-Smith A. Learning to read in Williams syndrome: looking beneath the surface of atypical reading development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(6):729-39. 2929 Menghini D, Verucci L, Vicari, S. Reading and phonological awareness in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(1):29-37.. According to normative data2525 Dias NM, Trevisan BT, Seabra AG. Dados normativos da Prova de Consciência Fonológica por produção Oral. In: Alessandra G. Seabra; Natália Martins Dias, organizador. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem Oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas; 2012. p. 109-15.there is a progression in scores related to higher level of education in all PAT-OP subtests and that determines a growth in total correct answers. Individuals with WS, on the other hand, present irregularity in all analyzed items; this shows that, unlike the control group, for this group there is no relation between level of education and number of correct answers at PAT-OP.

Data from the present study was analyzed without considering the participants' ability to read and write. Phonological awareness depends on the writing system that is being taught and phonemic awareness appears to be achieved only with the introduction of an alphabetic system1818 Seabra AG, Capovilla FC. Problemas de leitura e escrita: Como identificar, prevenir e remediar numa abordagem fônica. 6.ed. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas. 2012. 2121 Maluf MR. Do conhecimento implícito à consciência metalinguística indispensável na alfabetização. In: Guimarães SRK, Maluf MR. Aprendizagem da linguagem escrita: contribuições da pesquisa. São Paulo: Vetor Editora. 2010. p. 17-32. 2525 Dias NM, Trevisan BT, Seabra AG. Dados normativos da Prova de Consciência Fonológica por produção Oral. In: Alessandra G. Seabra; Natália Martins Dias, organizador. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem Oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas; 2012. p. 109-15. 2727 Maluf MR, Zanella MS, Pagnez KSMM. Metalinguistic abilities and written language in brazilian research. Boletim de Psicologia. 2006;LVI(124):67-92.. This capacity gradually develops as the child experiments ludic situations and is formally instructed in grapho-phonemic activities1717 Becerra AM, John AE, Peregrine E and Mervis CB. Reading Abilities of 9 - 17-Year- Olds with Williams Syndrome: Impact of Reading Method. Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders; Madison, WI. 2008.. Thus, we can interpret that, when children become aware, analyze and manipulate segments or pieces of speech, besides having perceived that oral language is made of words, syllables and phonemes, they have developed or are developing and using their phonological awareness1818 Seabra AG, Capovilla FC. Problemas de leitura e escrita: Como identificar, prevenir e remediar numa abordagem fônica. 6.ed. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas. 2012. 2121 Maluf MR. Do conhecimento implícito à consciência metalinguística indispensável na alfabetização. In: Guimarães SRK, Maluf MR. Aprendizagem da linguagem escrita: contribuições da pesquisa. São Paulo: Vetor Editora. 2010. p. 17-32. 2525 Dias NM, Trevisan BT, Seabra AG. Dados normativos da Prova de Consciência Fonológica por produção Oral. In: Alessandra G. Seabra; Natália Martins Dias, organizador. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem Oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas; 2012. p. 109-15.. It is still possible to consider that the learning process is not the same and does not occur at the same time for all children, and this difference can be directly related to intrinsic factors, which, in their turn, depend on environmental, socioeconomic and cultural factors.

Conclusion

Data from this study corroborate the findings of literature that suggest that individuals with WS present difficulties in phonological processing tasks. These results have important implications for intervention programs and educational planning aimed at teaching writing and reading skills for children with WS. It is necessary, for instance, to consider in such programs the fact that this population presents intellectual disability, which is particularly important in Brazilian context where inclusion process present severe flaws for the education of children with special needs. It is also necessary to include continuous stimulations programs, with early interventions for children with WS in preschool age, adapted teaching methods and curriculum.

Further research is required to broaden the characterization of the profile of phonological processing skills in WS. Some aspects that could be contemplated in further studies are: larger sample; more detailed metalinguistic analysis; assessment of reading and writing skills for those who attend elementary school and effects of interventions using phonic method to develop phonological awareness skills.

Referências

  • 1
    Porter MA, Dobson-Stone C, Kwok JBJ, Schofield PR, Beckett W, Beckett W et al. A Role for Transcription Factor GTF2IRD2 in Executive Function in Williams-Beuren Syndrome. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e474-570.
  • 2
    Mervis CB, Velleman S. Children with Williams Syndrome: Language, Cognitive, and Behavioral Characteristics and their Implications for Intervention. Perspect Lang Learn Educ. 2011;1;18(3):98-107.
  • 3
    Karmiloff-Smith A, D'Souza D, Dekker TM, Van Herwegen J, Xu F, Rodic M et al. Genetic and environmental vulnerabilities in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;16;109(Suppl 2):17261-5.
  • 4
    Collins RT, Aziz PF, Gleason MM, Kaplan PB, Shah MJ. Abnormalities of cardiac repolarization in Williams syndrome. Am J Cardiol. 2010; 1;106(7):1029-33.
  • 5
    Greer J, Riby DM, Hamiliton C, Riby LM. Attentional lapse and inhibition control in adults with Williams Syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(11):4170-7.
  • 6
    Lima SFB, Teixeira MCTV, Carreiro LRR, Seraceni M, Kim CA, Segin M et al. Manejo comportamental de crianças e adolescentes com Síndrome de Williams: guia para professores, pais e cuidadores. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científica; 2013.
  • 7
    Pober BR. Williams-Beuren syndrome. N Engl J Med. 2010;21;362(3):239-52.
  • 8
    Honjo RS. Detecção da microdeleção 7q11.23 por MLPA(r) e estudo clínico dos pacientes com síndrome de Williams-Beuren [tese]. São Paulo (SP): Universidade de São Paulo; 2012.
  • 9
    Martens MA, Wilson SJ, Reutens DC. Research Review: Williams syndrome: a critical review of the cognitive, behavioral, and neuroanatomical phenotype. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(6):576-608.
  • 10
    Mervis CB, Velleman SL. Children with Williams Syndrome: Language, Cognitive, and Behavioral Characteristics and their Implications for Intervention. Perspect Lang Learn Educ. 2011;18(3):98-107.
  • 11
    Teixeira, MCTV, Segin M, Martin MAF, Lellis VRR, Araujo MV, Schwartzman JS et al. Protocolo de avaliação neuropsicológica, comportamental e clínica para rastreamento de indicadores de desatenção e hiperatividade na síndrome de Williams-Beuren. Cad. pos-grad. disturb. desenvolv. 2010;10(1):59-75.
  • 12
    Osório A, Cruz R, Sampaio A, Garayzábal E, Martínez-Regueiro R, Gonçalves ÓF et al. How executive functions are related to intelligence in Williams syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2012;33(4):1169-75.
  • 13
    Nunes MM, Honjo RS, Dutra RL, Amaral VS, Amaral VAS, Oh HK et al. Assessment of Intellectual and VisuoSpatial Abilities in Children and Adults with Williams Syndrome. Universitas Psychologica. 2013;12(2):581-9.
  • 14
    Mervis CB. Language and Literacy Development of Children with Williams Syndrome. Top Lang Disord. 2009;29(2):149-69.
  • 15
    Laing E, Hulme C, Grant J, Karmiloff-Smith A. Learning to read in Williams syndrome: looking beneath the surface of atypical reading development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001;42(6):729-39.
  • 16
    Menghini D, Verucci L, Vicari S. Reading and phonological awareness in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(1):29-37.
  • 17
    Becerra AM, John AE, Peregrine E and Mervis CB. Reading Abilities of 9 - 17-Year- Olds with Williams Syndrome: Impact of Reading Method. Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders; Madison, WI. 2008.
  • 18
    Seabra AG, Capovilla FC. Problemas de leitura e escrita: Como identificar, prevenir e remediar numa abordagem fônica. 6.ed. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas. 2012.
  • 19
    Seabra AG, Dias NM. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas, 2012.
  • 20
    Seabra AG, Dias NM. Métodos de alfabetização: delimitação de procedimentos e considerações para uma prática eficaz. Rev. Psicopedag 2011;28(87):306-20.
  • 21
    Maluf MR. Do conhecimento implícito à consciência metalinguística indispensável na alfabetização. In: Guimarães SRK, Maluf MR. Aprendizagem da linguagem escrita: contribuições da pesquisa. São Paulo: Vetor Editora. 2010. p. 17-32.
  • 22
    Wechsler, D WISC III: Escala de inteligência Wechsler para crianças: manual. 3.ed. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2002.
  • 23
    Nascimento E. Adaptação e validação do teste WAIS-III para um contexto brasileiro [tese]. Brasília (DF): Universidade de Brasília; 2000.
  • 24
    Mello CB, Argollo N, Shayer BPM, Abreu N, Godinho K, Durán P et al. Abbreviated version of the WISC-III: correlation between estimated IQ and global IQ of brazilian children. Psicologia Teoria e Pesquisa. 2011;27(2):149-55.
  • 25
    Dias NM, Trevisan BT, Seabra AG. Dados normativos da Prova de Consciência Fonológica por produção Oral. In: Alessandra G. Seabra; Natália Martins Dias, organizador. Avaliação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Linguagem Oral. São Paulo: Memnon Edições Científicas; 2012. p. 109-15.
  • 26
    Piattelli-Palmarini M. Speaking of learning: how do we acquire our marvellous facility for expressing ourselves in words? Nature. 2001;411:887-8.
  • 27
    Maluf MR, Zanella MS, Pagnez KSMM. Metalinguistic abilities and written language in brazilian research. Boletim de Psicologia. 2006;LVI(124):67-92.
  • 28
    Laing E, Hulme C, Grant J, Karmiloff-Smith A. Learning to read in Williams syndrome: looking beneath the surface of atypical reading development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(6):729-39.
  • 29
    Menghini D, Verucci L, Vicari, S. Reading and phonological awareness in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(1):29-37.
  • Support sources: CAPES, Mackpesquisa

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Oct 2015

History

  • Received
    05 May 2015
  • Accepted
    03 July 2015
ABRAMO Associação Brasileira de Motricidade Orofacial Rua Uruguaiana, 516, Cep 13026-001 Campinas SP Brasil, Tel.: +55 19 3254-0342 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistacefac@cefac.br