Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The effects of easy and difficult business relationship evaluations on purchase intentions

Abstract

Purpose:

This research aims to investigate the different impacts of easy (fluency) and difficult (disfluency) business relationship evaluations on future purchase intentions. In addition, it seeks to investigate the mediating role of confidence in these evaluations and present a new form of fluency manipulation performed directly by satisfaction evaluations.

Design/methodology/approach:

This research includes a first experiment with a 3 (fluency: easy vs difficult vs control) x 2 (hypothetical scenario of satisfaction vs dissatisfaction) factorial design, using 180 undergraduate students, and a second single factor experiment (fluency: easy vs difficult vs control) with 326 consumers evaluating their financial services provider. Both used a between-subject design with the individuals being randomly distributed between the scenarios.

Findings:

Dissatisfied consumers who perceived ease in evaluating commercial relationships have increased confidence in these evaluations, negatively influencing their purchase intentions. Meanwhile, satisfied consumers are generally overconfident, being equally affected by perceived difficulty or ease due to a direct bias from the evaluation, which increases purchase intentions.

Originality/value:

The results demonstrate that fluency (perceived ease or difficulty) depends on the valence of the evaluations, directly affecting (positive valence) or indirectly affecting (negative valence) subsequent decisions. We also present a new, faster, and more practical way to manipulate fluency. Furthermore, we raise some ethical questions as these effects result in biased decisions.

Keywords:
fluency; bias; valence; confidence; purchase intention

Resumo

Objetivo:

Investigar os diferentes impactos que a facilidade (fluência) e a dificuldade (disfluência) nas avaliações de relacionamentos comerciais satisfatórios e insatisfatórios possuem nas intenções de compra futuras. Adicionalmente, investigar o papel mediador da confiança dessas avaliações e apresentar uma nova forma de manipulação de fluência realizada diretamente na forma de avaliações de satisfação.

Metodologia:

Esta pesquisa contempla um primeiro experimento com design fatorial 3 (fluência: fácil vs difícil vs controle) x 2 (cenário hipotético de satisfação vs insatisfação) com 180 estudantes de graduação e um segundo experimento single factor (fluência: fácil vs difícil vs controle) com 326 consumidores avaliando seus provedores de serviços financeiros. Ambos utilizaram um delineamento between-subjetcs com distribuição aleatória de sujeitos entre os cenários.

Resultados:

Consumidores insatisfeitos com facilidade percebida em avaliar relacionamentos comerciais têm um aumento de confiança nessas avaliações, fazendo que diminuam as intenções de compra. Por outro lado, consumidores satisfeitos têm em geral um excesso de confiança, sendo impactados de forma igual pela dificuldade ou facilidade percebida em razão de um viés direto das avaliações que aumenta as intenções de compra.

Contribuições:

Os resultados demonstram que a fluência (facilidade ou dificuldade percebida) depende da valência das avaliações para afetar diretamente as decisões subsequentes (valência positiva) ou indiretamente (valência negativa). Apresenta-se também uma nova maneira, mais rápida e prática de manipular a fluência. Ainda, como esses efeitos resultam em decisões enviesadas, são levantadas questões éticas.

Palavras-chave:
Fluência; viés; valência; confiança; intenção de compra

1 Introduction

Individuals usually seek to simplify decisions using “shortcuts” or heuristics that minimize cognitive efforts and avoid stress and strain in their decisions (Kahneman, 2011Kahneman, D. (2011). Rápido e Devagar: Duas formas de pensar. Rio de Janeiro: Objetiva.). However, these “shortcuts” usually produce some biases that influence attitudes and decisions. In relationship marketing, one of these biases has its origin in satisfaction evaluations, leading to a pattern of future expectations associated with higher repurchase rates (Oliver, 2010Oliver, R. L. (2010). Satisfaction, a behavioural perspective on the consumer (2. ed.). New York: Irwin McGraw-Hill.). Similarly, the development of loyalty can also be seen as a cognitive bias that directs attitudes and behaviors (Bloemer, Ruyter, & Peeters, 1998Bloemer, J., Ruyter, K., & Peeters, P (1998). Investigating drivers of bank loyalty: The complex relationship between image, service quality and satisfaction. International Journal of Bank Marketing, 16(7), 276-286.). The explanation for these biased behaviors is based on the positive memories experienced (Lazarus, 1991Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.), but it is also necessary to highlight the influence of fluency (Schwarz, 2004Schwarz, N. (2004). Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 332-348.) on the creation and reinforcement of these biases, intensifying the impact of intuitions and opinions (Aydin, 2016Aydin, A.E. (2016) Processing fluency: Examining its relationship with thinking style and purchase intention, Journal of Marketing Communications, 24(6), 588-598; Simons & Nelson, 2006, 2007).

At one end of the fluency continuum, individuals perceive ease in cognitive processing, creating comfort and pleasure (Labroo & Pocheptsova, 2016Labroo, A. A., & Pocheptsova, A. (2016). Metacognition and consumer judgment: Fluency is pleasant but disfluency ignites interest. Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 154-159.; Nunes, Ordani, & Valsevia, 2015Nunes, J. C., Ordani, A., & Valsesia, F. (2015). The power of repetition: Repetitive lyrics in a song increase processing fluency and drive market success. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(2), 187-199.) that would lead to biases such as: intensifying the effects of what is being interpreted (Claypool, Mackie, & Garcia-Marques, 2015Claypool, H. M., Mackie, D. M., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2015). Fluency and attitudes. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(7), 370-382.; Landwehr, Golla, & Reber, 2017Landwehr, J. R., Golla, B., & Reber, R. (2017). Processing fluency: An inevitable side effect of evaluative conditioning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 124-128.); evaluating and judging something more quickly (Schwarz, 2004Schwarz, N. (2004). Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 332-348.); a greater perception of veracity, familiarity, learning, intelligence, and value (Alter & Openhaimer, 2006Alter, A.L., & Oppenheimer, D.M. (2006) Predicting short-term stock fluctuations by using processing fluency. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(24), 9369-9372., 2008, 2009; Kornell, Rhodes, Castel, & Tauber, 2011Kornell, N., Rhodes, M. G., Castel, A. D., & Tauber, S. K. (2011). The ease-of-processing heuristic and the stability bias dissociating memory, memory beliefs, and memory judgments. Psychological Science, 22(6), 787-794; Miele & Molden, 2010Miele, D. B., & Molden, D. C. (2010). Naive theories of intelligence and the role of processing fluency in perceived comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3), 535-557.; Openhaimer, 2004; Yang, Huang, & Shanks, 2018). In addition, as evaluations get more intuitive, faster, and more confident (Kahneman, 2011Kahneman, D. (2011). Rápido e Devagar: Duas formas de pensar. Rio de Janeiro: Objetiva.), subsequent judgments and decisions show more biases (Aydin, 2016Aydin, A.E. (2016) Processing fluency: Examining its relationship with thinking style and purchase intention, Journal of Marketing Communications, 24(6), 588-598; Simons & Nelson, 2006, 2007). Perceived difficulty can be observed at the other end of the fluency continuum (Sanchez & Jaeger, 2015Sanchez, C., & Jaeger, A. J. (2015). If it’s hard to read, it changes how long you do it: Reading time as an explanation for perceptual fluency effects on judgment. Psychon B ull Rev, 22(1), 206-211.), linked to cognitive discomfort and thoughts about risk. However, if individuals could be stimulated to deliberate or to use their self-control (Kahneman, 2011), they would not develop overconfidence and would avoid using simple opinions and prior and/or erroneous beliefs - or even xenophobic thoughts - that could bias their decisions (Alter, 2013; Hernandez & Preston, 2013Hernandez, I. & Preston, J. L. (2013). Disfluency disrupts the confirmation bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(1), 178-182; Ryffel & Wirth, 2018Ryffel, F. A., & Wirth, W. (2018). How perceived processing fluency influences the illusion of knowing in learning from TV reports. Journal of Media Psychology, 32(1), 2-13).

Therefore, if consumers experienced easy and difficult assessments and judgments, would it be possible to lessen the impact of positive or even negative business experiences just by making the consumer doubt the evaluation? Is confidence in evaluations and subsequent decisions differently affected by positive and negative commercial experiences? In order to answer these questions, this study aims to verify the impact of fluency on commercial relationship evaluations and on subsequent future purchase intentions. In addition, the study also presents a new fluency manipulation (based on Oppenheimer, 2004Oppenheimer, D. M. (2004) Spontaneous discounting of availability in frequency judgment tasks. Psychological Science, 15(2), 100-105), not requiring previous tasks to affect later evaluations, but directly using easy or difficult satisfaction evaluations, and therefore enabling it to be applied in practice.

2 Literature Review

Despite the need and importance, in several situations consumers do not rationally and structurally judge the attributes, benefits, and costs of each purchase. Instead, they use a series of simplified rules (i.e. heuristics) without making in-depth comparisons and evaluations (Baron, 2008Baron, J. (2008). Thinking and deciding: Normative theory of utility and choice (4a ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.). These heuristics generate systematic errors, called biases, arising from factors such as lack of self-control and fluency (Kahneman, 2011Kahneman, D. (2011). Rápido e Devagar: Duas formas de pensar. Rio de Janeiro: Objetiva.). The latter is the subject of this study.

2.1 Effects of fluency on evaluations and judgments

Fluency can be understood as a continuum between ease (fluency) and difficulty (commonly called disfluency) of processing some type of information. The intensity of fluency can originate from the individual’s own characteristics or from the situation being evaluated and it can be classified as perceptual or conceptual fluency (Schwarz, 2004Schwarz, N. (2004). Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 332-348.). Perceptual fluency results from physical-visual characteristics, such as changes in brightness, contrast, or even in the print quality of texts. Conceptual fluency results from ease of processing a given stimulus, such as easy to pronounce names or even rhyming phrases. As an example, studies show that easy to pronounce stock names or symbols, therefore having greater fluency, create greater expectations of profitability for investors (Alter & Oppenheimer, 2006Alter, A.L., & Oppenheimer, D.M. (2006) Predicting short-term stock fluctuations by using processing fluency. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(24), 9369-9372.).

Stimuli perceived as easy have a greater tendency to be hedonically processed (Duke, Fiacconi, & Kohler, 2014Duke, D., Fiacconi, C. M., & Kohler, S. (2014). Parallel effects of processing fluency and positive affect on familiarity-based recognition decisions for faces. Front. Psychol, 5, 5-328). Thus, in general, people prefer music (rhythms and their lyrics), texts, and the like with simpler content, as they get cognitive comfort from the pleasure of being able to understand it without much effort (Bayliss, Constable, Tipper, & Kritikos, 2013Constable, M .D., Bayliss, A. P., Tipper, S. P., & Kritikos, A. (2013). Self-generated cognitive fluency as an alternative route to preference formation. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(1),; Maier & Dost, 2018Maier, E., & Dost, F. (2018). The positive effect of contextual image backgrounds on fluency and liking. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 40, 109-116.; Nunes, et al., 2015Nunes, J. C., Ordani, A., & Valsesia, F. (2015). The power of repetition: Repetitive lyrics in a song increase processing fluency and drive market success. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(2), 187-199.). Ease of processing is linked to perceived pleasure, reducing uncertainties and eliciting more positive responses when evaluating products (Labroo & Pocheptsova, 2016Labroo, A. A., & Pocheptsova, A. (2016). Metacognition and consumer judgment: Fluency is pleasant but disfluency ignites interest. Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 154-159.). This cognitive comfort can even increase the acceptance of an idea, topic, or even a product (Labroo, Dhar, & Schwarz, 2008). However, pleasure is not felt when processing all stimuli types, only positive ones (Albrecht & Carbon, 2014Albrecht, S., & Carbon, C-C. (2014) the fluency amplification model: Fluent stimuli show more intense but not evidently more positive evaluations. Acta Psychologica, 148, 195-203.). That is, there is no preference or pleasure in processing negative stimuli. In this sense, extreme ease of processing intensifies the effects of previously processed information, whether positive or negative, stimulating biases that influence future decisions (Claypool et al., 2015Claypool, H. M., Mackie, D. M., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2015). Fluency and attitudes. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(7), 370-382.; Landwehr et al., 2017Landwehr, J. R., Golla, B., & Reber, R. (2017). Processing fluency: An inevitable side effect of evaluative conditioning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 124-128.).

Although individuals have a preference for easier stimuli, difficulty (disfluency) generally helps them process information more carefully, acting as a warning to prevent more intuitive forms of reasoning (Alter, 2013Alter, L.A. (2013) The benefits of cognitive disfluency. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(6), 437-442.; Alter, Oppenheimer, Epley, & Eyre, 2007). Difficulty in cognitive processing is able to interrupt confirmatory biases, beliefs, previous expectations, and prejudices (Hernandez & Preston, 2013Hernandez, I. & Preston, J. L. (2013). Disfluency disrupts the confirmation bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(1), 178-182), leading to better event coding, better memory retrieval later (Diemand-Yauman, Oppenheimer, & Vaughan, 2011Diemand-Yauman, C., Oppenheimer, D. M., & Vaughan, E. B. (2011). Fortune favors the BOLD (and the Italicized): Effects of disfluency on educational outcomes. Cognition, 118(1), 111-5.; Oppenheimer & Alter, 2013), and better information processing. Thus, perceived difficulty provides superior results (Charness & Dave, 2017Charness, G. E., & Dave, C. (2017). Confirmation bias with motivated beliefs. Games and Economic Behavior, 104, 1-23.) by enforcing greater deliberation in the decision-making process (Weissgerber & Reinhard, 2017).

Heuristics and their biases are associated with System 1, where the process of seeking cognitive comfort leads to less and faster deliberation, producing overconfidence. On the other hand, in the case of System 2, there is slow thinking, more deliberation, and greater attention (Kahneman, 2011Kahneman, D. (2011). Rápido e Devagar: Duas formas de pensar. Rio de Janeiro: Objetiva.). Likewise, individuals who have previously had easy cognitive processing experiences overestimate their knowledge about these experiences, generating greater confidence (Ryffel & Wirth, 2018Ryffel, F. A., & Wirth, W. (2018). How perceived processing fluency influences the illusion of knowing in learning from TV reports. Journal of Media Psychology, 32(1), 2-13). Ease of processing can induce a greater perception of veracity (Silva, Garcia-Marques, & Reber, 2017Silva, R. R., Garcia-Marques, T., & Reber, R. (2017). The informative value of type of repetition: Perceptual and conceptual fluency influences on judgments of truth. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 51, 53-67.) and overconfidence, because individuals do not have to deal with inconsistencies or new interpretations in deliberations (Bajšanski, Žauhar, & Valerjev, 2019). Similarly, studies testing the effects of technical versus common words (or even easy versus difficult to read typefaces) point out that perceived ease is related to a greater perception of information confidence, while difficulty leads to a greater risk perception (Park, Herr, & Kim, 2016Park, Y., Herr, P. M., & Kim, B. C. (2016). The effect of disfluency on consumer perceptions of information security. Marketing Letters, 27(3), 525-535.).

Therefore, easily formed intuitions and opinions not only lead to results with greater perceived reliability, but also to greater confidence in the evaluation or judgment itself. Overconfident individuals do not engage in new information processing and are directly biased by these intuitions. However, if they have faced difficulties in forming these intuitions, this would signal to them that less intuitive alternatives should be considered, or even chosen (Simons & Nelson, 2006, 2007). As a result, individuals who easily formed prior choices of which product to buy would have greater confidence that these prior choices would be right, increasing subsequent purchase intentions (Aydin, 2016Aydin, A.E. (2016) Processing fluency: Examining its relationship with thinking style and purchase intention, Journal of Marketing Communications, 24(6), 588-598).

However, evidence indicates that this would not always happen because not only perceived ease or difficulty can affect confidence, but also mood states (Koch & Forgas, 2012Koch, A. S., & Forgas, J. P. (2012). Feeling good and feeling truth: The interactive effects of mood and processing fluency on truth judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(2), 481-485) and the evaluated valence of memories (Kensinger & Schacter, 2008Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Neural processes supporting young and older adults’ emotional memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(7), 1161-73.).

2.2 Effects of the valence of memories on evaluations and judgments

Positive mood states lead to judgments that are perceived as “more true,” causing the maintenance of confidence, while negative mood states lead to less confidence in information processing (Koch & Forgas, 2012Koch, A. S., & Forgas, J. P. (2012). Feeling good and feeling truth: The interactive effects of mood and processing fluency on truth judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(2), 481-485).

Confidence in what is being evaluated is negatively affected by negative recovered memories, involving more effort and need for control to avoid unwanted results (Caplan, Sommer, Madan, & Fujiwara, 2019Madan, C. R., Scott, S. M. E., & Kensinger, E. A. (2019). Positive emotion enhances association-memory. Emotion, 19(4), 733-740.). Negative emotions induce a more concrete adaptive processing state, leading individuals to process information with greater attention and for longer periods (Matovic, Kock, & Forgas, 2014Matovic, D., Kock, A. S., & Forgas, J. P. (2014). Can negative mood improve language understanding? Affective influences on the ability to detect ambiguous communication. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 44-49.). These effects relate to evidence that negative events are more accessible to memory, enhancing the recollection of details and central facts (Hostler, Wood, & Armitage, 2018Hostler, T. J., Wood, C., & Armitage, C. J. (2018). The influence of emotional cues on prospective memory: A systematic Review with meta-analyses, Cognition and Emotion, 32(8), 1578-1596.; Kensinger & Schacter, 2008Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Neural processes supporting young and older adults’ emotional memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(7), 1161-73.) that are essential and intrinsic to the events (Bookbinder & Brainerd, 2017Bookbinder, S. H., & Brainerd, C. J. (2017). Emotionally negative pictures enhance gist memory. Emotion, 17(1), 102-119; Kensinger, 2009) because they include more distinct memories (Brewin & Langley, 2019).

Negative emotions stimulate the ability to recapitulate episodes not only due to greater attention to coding and memories retrieval, but also due to the greater reactivation of these emotions (Bowen, Kark, & Kensinger, 2017). Thus, negative aspects, facts, and emotions result in greater attention and prevention in general (Prato & John, 1991Prato, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative Social Information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 380-391.), as well as a greater problem-solving focus (Orita & Hattori, 2018Orita, R. & Hattori, M. (2018). Positive and negative affects facilitate insight problem-solving in different ways: A study with Implicit hints. Japanese Psychological Research, Special issue: Implicit Cognition, 61(2), 94-106).

In contrast, positive memories lead to more general and peripheral attention (Talarico, Berntsen, & Rubin, 2009). These memories need more associations to be remembered (Madan, Scott, & Kensinger, 2019Madan, C. R., Scott, S. M. E., & Kensinger, E. A. (2019). Positive emotion enhances association-memory. Emotion, 19(4), 733-740.) and they therefore have more inconsistencies (recall errors). The recovery of positive memories seems to be associated with greater confidence in their reports (Kensinger, 2009). Positive emotions lead individuals to focus on more abstract aspects, with less focus on and less attention paid to external information, seeking to maintain the pleasant feelings (Bless & Fiedler, 2006Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (2006). Mood and the regulation of information processing and behaviour. In: J. Forgas (ed.) Affect in social thinking and behaviour (pp.65-84). New York: Psychology Press.). This greater confidence also relates to the way people have been treated (Lazarus, 1991Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.). People treated in a welcoming way, who had positive experiences, would feel safer and more “loose,” reducing their worries and their need to not make a fool of themselves (p. 421). These positive mood states lead to the relaxation of inhibitory control, resulting in a fundamental shift in attention (Rowe, Hirsh, & Anderson, 2007).

In general, situations with negative affect lead to memories with greater precision and focus than positive affect (Kensinger, 2009Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emocion Review, 1(2), 99-113.), and similarly, positive memories lead to less details and accuracy compared to negative memories (Bohn & Berntsen, 2007Bohn A, & Berntsen D. (2007). Pleasantness bias in flashbulb memories: Positive and negative flashbulb memories of the fall of the Berlin Wall among East and West Germans. Memory and Cognition, 35(3), 565- 577.; Brewin & Langley, 2018). With this lack of focus, in time individuals have a higher probability of remembering only the “essence” of the positive event - they would only “know” that something positive had happened, without any great precision or even forgetting details (Kensinger, 2009). Additionally, judgments focused on positive aspects are faster and more spontaneous than judgments focused on negative aspects (Herr & Page, 2004Herr, P. M. & Page, C. M. (2004). Asymmetric association of liking and disliking judgments: So what’s not to like? Journal of Consumer Research, 30(4), 588-601.). Positive evaluations are relatively more automatic, leading to lower quality and more biased decisions (Herr, Page, Pfeiffer, & Davis, 2012). Also, Sorbeck and Clore (2005) indicate that positive aspects lead to a greater disconnection between confidence and the vividness of memories. That is, even if positive memories are not as distinct and vivid in certain situations, they still lead to greater confidence. Thus, while negative moods lead people to focus on details, positive moods make them deliberate more, based more on schematics or heuristics (Rowe et al., 2007).

2.3 Fluency and the effects of the valence of memories on commercial relationship evaluations

Among some of the “shortcuts” used for decision making, individuals commonly use prices (Luppe & Angelo, 2010Luppe, M. R., & Angelo, C. F. de (2010). As decisões de consumo e a heurística da ancoragem: Uma análise da racionalidade do processo de escolha. RAM Revista de Administração Mackenzie, 11(6), 81-106.) and previous satisfaction evaluations - their own or from others - as anchors in buying decisions (Oliver, 2010Oliver, R. L. (2010). Satisfaction, a behavioural perspective on the consumer (2. ed.). New York: Irwin McGraw-Hill.). Such behavior is related to individuals’ tendency to reuse previous methods that have obtained satisfactory results (Baron, 2008Baron, J. (2008). Thinking and deciding: Normative theory of utility and choice (4a ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.). Consumers’ past purchase satisfaction (Oliver, 2010) or even information from others’ satisfactory experiences (Agnihotri, Dingus, Hu, & Krush, 2016Agnihotri, R., Dingus, R., Hu, M.Y., & Krush, m. (2016) Social media: Influencing customer satisfaction in B2B sales. Industrial Marketing Management, 53, 172-180.) create positive expectations for the decision maker because they develops a belief that their needs can be fulfilled with similar performance in the future. This creates an expectation of satisfaction (Sirgy, 1984; Oliver, 2010) and the anticipation of positive emotions (Patrick, Chun, & Macinnis, 2009Patrick, V. M., Chun, H. H., & Macinnis, D. J. (2009) Affective forecasting and self-control: Why anticipating pride wins over anticipating shame in a self-regulation context. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19(3), 537-545.). In this sense, satisfaction evaluations made before purchasing decisions serve as an anchor for substitution heuristics and thus, as opinions or intuitions, they will mostly be used, replacing complex questions that would bias subsequent decisions (Kahneman, 2011Kahneman, D. (2011). Rápido e Devagar: Duas formas de pensar. Rio de Janeiro: Objetiva.).

Satisfaction itself is an overall evaluation of expectations, benefits, value, and quality compared to the perceived costs of business relationships. It is an evaluation of the performance of these factors compared to expectations. Positive disconfirmation of expectations would lead the consumer to be satisfied and feel positive emotions and affect. Negative disconfirmation leads the consumer to be unsatisfied, feeling negative emotions and affect (Oliver, 2010Oliver, R. L. (2010). Satisfaction, a behavioural perspective on the consumer (2. ed.). New York: Irwin McGraw-Hill.). Commercial interactions with positive evaluations develop trust, commitment, and true loyalty (Grönroos, 2009Grönroos, C. (2009). Marketing: Gerenciamento e serviços. (Marques A. S., & Belhassof, C. M. Trad.) (3a ed.). Rio de Janeiro: Elsevier.; Larivière et al., 2016Larivière, B., Keiningham, T., Aksoy, L., Yalcin, A., Morgeson, F., & Mithas, S. (2016). Modeling heterogeneity in the satisfaction, loyalty intention, and shareholder value linkage: A cross-industry analysis at the customer and firm levels. Journal of Marketing Research, 53(1), 91-109.) - or even a bias that can influence attitudes and behaviors (Bloemer et al., 1998Bloemer, J., Ruyter, K., & Peeters, P (1998). Investigating drivers of bank loyalty: The complex relationship between image, service quality and satisfaction. International Journal of Bank Marketing, 16(7), 276-286.) - resulting in better financial results (Francisco-Mafezzolli & Prado, 2013; Fornell, Morgeson, & Hult, 2016Fornell, C., Morgeson , F. V., & Hult, G. T. (2016). An abnormally abnormal intangible: Stock returns on customer satisfaction. Journal of Marketing, 80(5), 122-125.).

As dissatisfied consumers have negative memories, emotions, and affections associated with their suppliers, they have more vivid and distinct memories (Brewin & Langley, 2019). Thus, decisions regarding these suppliers would be more focused on central aspects (Kensinger, 2009Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emocion Review, 1(2), 99-113.), that is, with attention paid to essential facts and intrinsic details (Bookbinder & Brainerd, 2017Bookbinder, S. H., & Brainerd, C. J. (2017). Emotionally negative pictures enhance gist memory. Emotion, 17(1), 102-119; Kensinger, 2009) for problem solving (Orita & Hattori, 2018Orita, R. & Hattori, M. (2018). Positive and negative affects facilitate insight problem-solving in different ways: A study with Implicit hints. Japanese Psychological Research, Special issue: Implicit Cognition, 61(2), 94-106). Remembering their negative episodes, these consumers focus on their negative memories. As they perceive higher fluency (perceived ease) in their satisfaction evaluations, they develop greater confidence in their own evaluations (Simons & Nelson; 2006, 2007; Aydin, 2016Aydin, A.E. (2016) Processing fluency: Examining its relationship with thinking style and purchase intention, Journal of Marketing Communications, 24(6), 588-598), intensifying the negative aspects (Claypool, et al., 2015Claypool, H. M., Mackie, D. M., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2015). Fluency and attitudes. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(7), 370-382.; Landwehr, et al., 2017Landwehr, J. R., Golla, B., & Reber, R. (2017). Processing fluency: An inevitable side effect of evaluative conditioning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 124-128.) of the commercial relationship, thus decreasing the purchase intentions for that supplier. However, even if they have less confidence because of perceived difficulty, this will only make them more vigilant and assume a general state of prevention, focusing on the problem (purchase decision) and beginning to doubt their intuitions (in that they should not buy from the supplier). Therefore, we propose that:

H1: Fluency (perceived ease) will increase a dissatisfied consumer’s confidence in their relationship evaluations, subsequently reducing their purchase intentions.

Satisfied consumers have positive memories, emotions, and affections associated with their commercial products/services suppliers and, therefore, they make more abstract decisions, more focused on more general aspects (Talarico et al. 2009) and less so on external information (Bless & Fiedler, 2006Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (2006). Mood and the regulation of information processing and behaviour. In: J. Forgas (ed.) Affect in social thinking and behaviour (pp.65-84). New York: Psychology Press.). In this case, they feel safer and more “relaxed,” with fewer worries (Lazarus, 1991Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.), less attention paid, and less self-control (Rowe et al. 2007). Satisfied consumers make more automatic judgments and thus more biased ones. Even if they cannot recall as many details, they have greater confidence in their evaluations (Kensinger, 2009Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emocion Review, 1(2), 99-113.; Sorbeck & Clore, 2005) and are more influenced by heuristics (Rowe et al. 2007). Therefore, even in disfluency situations (perceived difficulty) satisfied consumers do not experience a significant drop in confidence in their ability to evaluate their satisfaction. Therefore, if they have remembered positive facts/memories before a purchase decision, their interpretations and evaluation results will still be intensified (Claypool et al. 2015Claypool, H. M., Mackie, D. M., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2015). Fluency and attitudes. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(7), 370-382.; Landwehr et al. 2017Landwehr, J. R., Golla, B., & Reber, R. (2017). Processing fluency: An inevitable side effect of evaluative conditioning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 124-128.), despite the difficulty perceived. Therefore, we propose that:

H2: Commercial relationship evaluations (fluency or disfluency) will directly bias satisfied consumers to increase their subsequent purchase intentions.

3 Methodological Procedures

We used an experimental methodology to test the hypotheses (Figure 1), manipulating the independent variables and thus increasing the probability of an effect occurring (Shadish, Cook, & Campbell, 2002Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. In Cook, T. D. & Campbell (Ed.) Journal of the American Statistical Association, 100. Boston: Houghton Mifflin), measuring the manipulation (checking) and its effects on the dependent variables while at the same time controlling the other external variables (Malhotra, 2010Malhotra, N. K. (2010). Pesquisa de marketing: Uma orientação aplicada. Porto Alegre: Bookman.). We used non-probabilistic sampling for convenience in both experiments. The first experiment used a sample of undergraduate students while the second one used a general sample of several bank customers contacted through social networks and via e-mail.

Figure 1
Conceptual Framework for the Hypotheses

In both experiments, a new form of fluency manipulation was used based on Oppenheimer’s (2004Oppenheimer, D. M. (2004) Spontaneous discounting of availability in frequency judgment tasks. Psychological Science, 15(2), 100-105) manipulation, which manipulated fluency with texts using difficult words.

For the “easy” task (perceived ease), the individuals answered four overall satisfaction questions (Prado, 2004Prado, P. H. M. (2004). A avaliação do relacionamento sob a ótica do cliente: Um estudo em bancos de varejo. Tese (Doutorado em Administração) - Programa de Pós-Graduação em Administração de Empresas. Escola de Administração de Empresas de São Paulo, Fundação Getúlio Vargas, São Paulo.) before answering a purchase intention question. The four satisfaction questions to evaluate commercial relationships were: three questions adapted from Bettencourt (1997Bettencourt, L. A. (1997). Customer voluntary performance: Customer AX’s partners in service delivery. Journal of retailing, 73(3), 383-406.) and Fornell, Johnson, Anderson, Cha, & Bryant (1994Fornell, C., Johnson, M. D., Anderson, E. W., Cha, J., & Bryant, B. E. (1994). The American customer satisfaction: Nature, purpose and findings. Journal of Marketing, 58(4), 7-18.) (overall satisfaction, expectations distance, and global disconfirmation) and one affective evaluation question from Garbarino & Jonhson (1999Garbarino, E., & Johnson, M.S. (1999). The different roles of satisfaction, trust, and commitment in customer relationships. Journal of Marketing, 63(2), 70-97.). For the “difficult” task (perceived difficulty), instead of directly asking these four questions, the concept of each item (taken from Prado, 2004) was written in a question format and these four conceptual questions were asked before the individuals indicated their purchase intention. As an example, the first question from Prado (2004), “How would you rate your satisfaction regarding your relationship with ALPHA, the provider of residential telephony, internet, and cable TV services?” was adapted in the “difficult” task scenario to “Overall, how would you evaluate your commercial relationship with ALPHA, the provider of residential telephony, internet, and cable TV services - focusing on the services offered regarding the act or effect of satisfaction?” For the “control” group, the individuals indicated their purchase intentions and later completed the four-item scale from Prado (2004).

In both experiments, all groups (set of “easy” vs “difficult” commercial relationship evaluations before indicating the purchase intention, vs Control - without commercial relationship evaluations before indicating the purchase intention) answered the four standard satisfaction questions (Prado, 2004Prado, P. H. M. (2004). A avaliação do relacionamento sob a ótica do cliente: Um estudo em bancos de varejo. Tese (Doutorado em Administração) - Programa de Pós-Graduação em Administração de Empresas. Escola de Administração de Empresas de São Paulo, Fundação Getúlio Vargas, São Paulo.) at the end of the questionnaire to check manipulation (experiment 1) and unbiased evaluations of business relationships (experiment 2). Moreover, to verify that these manipulations not only resulted in greater difficulty, but were also convergent with the results of other studies, the results of the satisfaction evaluations were compared with Haddock (2002Haddock, G. (2002). It’s easy to like or dislike Tony Blair: Accessibility experiences and the favorability of attitude judgments. British Journal of Social Psychology, 93(2), 257-267.) and other authors. Haddock (2002) demonstrated that, in a task involving remembering one politician’s positive aspects, individuals in easy tasks (remembering few facts) developed more favorable opinions about the politician than those in difficult tasks (remembering more facts). Likewise, in tasks involving remembering one politician’s negative aspects, individuals in easy tasks (remembering few facts) developed less favorable opinions about the politician than those in difficult tasks (remembering more facts). Similar results were found when investigating personal use of bicycles (Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 1999Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). How often did I do it? Experienced ease of retrieval and frequency estimates of past behavior. Acta Psychologica, 103(1-2), 77-89.), aspects of childhood (Winkielman & Schwarz, 2001), and satisfaction evaluations (Viacava, Mantovani, Korelo, & Prado, 2015). Thus, we also expect unsatisfied consumers with perceived difficulty in commercial relationship evaluations to have higher satisfaction scores than those with evaluations they perceived as easy. Similarly, we expect satisfied consumers with perceived difficulty in commercial relationship evaluations to have lower satisfaction scores than those with evaluations they perceived as easy.

3.1 Experiment 1

Procedures: the experiment presented a 3 (fluency: “easy” vs “difficult” vs control) x 2 (positive scenario: “satisfaction” vs negative: “dissatisfaction”) factorial design. A total of 189 undergraduate students started the test but only 180 completed the survey and were therefore considered as valid (52.20% men, Mage = 22.14 years, SD = 4.36).

In a classroom at the university, the students randomly assigned to one of six conditions filled in a survey (Appendix A Appendix A - First experiment questionnaires - first experiment questionnaires) about commercial relationships (cable TV, home telephone, and internet providers). The students were given one of two scenarios (“satisfaction” vs “dissatisfaction”) consisting of fictional stories about a consumer named Eduardo. In the “positive” scenario, the customer had a three-year relationship that resulted in positive expectancy disconfirmation (general and attendance service quality, deadlines fulfilled, perceived cordiality, etc.) and the development of positive overall satisfaction. In the “negative” scenario, “Eduardo” also had a three-year relationship but the same situations resulted in negative expectancy disconfirmation and the development of negative overall satisfaction (“dissatisfaction”). After reading the scenarios, they answered two other unrelated surveys (“fillers”) averaging five minutes, so that the scenarios were not so evident and close to the evaluations phase (Su, Li, Zheng, Hu, Fan, & Luo, 2018).

Next, fluency (“easy” vs “difficult” vs “control”) was manipulated by the complexity of the satisfaction evaluations. For the “easy” task (perceived ease), the individuals answered four overall satisfaction questions (Prado, 2004Prado, P. H. M. (2004). A avaliação do relacionamento sob a ótica do cliente: Um estudo em bancos de varejo. Tese (Doutorado em Administração) - Programa de Pós-Graduação em Administração de Empresas. Escola de Administração de Empresas de São Paulo, Fundação Getúlio Vargas, São Paulo.) before indicating their purchase intentions. For the “difficult” task (perceived difficulty) they answered four adapted questions made for each concept item (taken from Prado, 2004) before indicating their purchase intention. The control group only indicated their purchase intention and, later, manipulation of the scenarios was checked (Figure 2 - experiment 1 flow).

Figure 2
Experiment 1 Flow.

As a dependent variable, all answered the purchase intention question (regarding a cable TV offer made by the same company described in the scenario, based on 1 - certainly NOT to 10 - definitely YES). Then they answered the fluency manipulation check, which questioned the general perceived difficulty (Graf, Mayer, & Landwehr, 2018Graf, L.K.M., Mayer, S., & Landwehr, J. R. (2018). Measuring processing fluency: One versus five items. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 28(3), 393-411.) to evaluate satisfaction (1 - very difficult to 10 - very easy). The students also reported their actual satisfaction with their own internet, cable, and phone provider services and how much they paid for them, followed by socio-demographic questions (these variables reveal non-significant relationships with the results). Before ending the experiment, all answered a four-item overall satisfaction scale (Prado, 2004Prado, P. H. M. (2004). A avaliação do relacionamento sob a ótica do cliente: Um estudo em bancos de varejo. Tese (Doutorado em Administração) - Programa de Pós-Graduação em Administração de Empresas. Escola de Administração de Empresas de São Paulo, Fundação Getúlio Vargas, São Paulo.) as a scenario manipulation check.

Manipulation checks: he one-way anova test revealed that individuals in the “easy” group perceived that it was easier to answer the four satisfaction items (F(2,177) = 12.127, p<0.001, ηp 2 = 0.121, Measy = 6.43, SD = 1.99, Mcontrol = 5.90, SD = 1.66, Mdifficult = 4.86, SD = 1.42 / p easyXdifficult = 0.005, p easyXcontrol = 0.196, p controlXdifficult 0.005). An independent t test using the average of the four satisfaction items (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.984) also reveals the expected results for the scenarios (t(1,178) = 16.745, p<0.001, Msatisfaction = 7.27, SD = 1.59, Mdissatisfaction = 3.19, SD = 1.67). The two-way anova test revealed a significant interaction between the independent variables (fluency: “easy” vs “difficult” / “dissatisfaction” vs “satisfaction”) (F(1,115) = 12.015, p = 0.001 ηp 2 = 0.095) and the average satisfaction (four items, Cronbach’s alpha = 0.970) questioned before purchase intentions, showing the same pattern as other related studies (Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 1999Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). How often did I do it? Experienced ease of retrieval and frequency estimates of past behavior. Acta Psychologica, 103(1-2), 77-89.; Haddock, 2002Haddock, G. (2002). It’s easy to like or dislike Tony Blair: Accessibility experiences and the favorability of attitude judgments. British Journal of Social Psychology, 93(2), 257-267.; Viacava et al., 2015; Winkielman & Schwarz, 2001). Perceived difficulty reduced the average satisfaction of the positive scenario (“satisfaction”: Measy = 7.55, SD = 1.43, Mdifficult = 6.47, SD = 1.36 / p=0.009) it increased the average satisfaction of the negative scenario (“dissatisfaction”: Measy = 2.88, SD = 1.46, Mdifficult = 3.74, SD=1.76 / p=0.027).

Results: the two-way anova test revealed a significant interaction of the independent variables (F(2,174) = 10.142, p<0.001, ηp 2 = 0.104) with purchase intention (PINT) (Figure 3), with a significant effect of the scenario (F(1,174) = 160.964, p<0.001, ηp 2 = 0.481, PINTsatisfaction = 7.21, SD = 1.67, PINTdissatisfaction = 3.82, SD = 2.06) and a non-significant effect of fluency (F(2,174) = 1.984, p = 0.141, ηp 2 = 0.022, PINTeasy = 5.34, SD = 2.95, PINTcontrol = 5.57, SD = 2.26, PINTdifficult = 5.75, SD = 2.23).

Figure 3
Purchase Intentions: Scenarios vs Fluency

Bonferroni post hoc tests in the positive scenario corroborated hypothesis 2, showing that commercial relationship evaluations, whether perceived as easy or difficult, lead to higher purchase intentions compared to the control group (“satisfaction” PINTeasy = 7.71, SD = 1.48, PINTcontrol = 6.44, SD = 1.98, PINTdifficult = 7.65, SD = 0.83 / p easyXcontrol = 0.011, p easyXdifficult = 1.000, p controlXdifficult = 0.037).

The results from the Bonferroni post hoc tests in the negative scenario corroborated hypothesis 1. Perceived ease leads to lower purchase intentions compared to the other groups (“dissatisfaction”: PINTeasy = 2.91, SD = 1.92, PINTcontrol = 4.48, SD = 2.13, PINTdifficult = 4.24, SD = 1.81 / p easyXcontrol = 0.002, p easyXdifficult = 0.010, p controlXdifficult = 0.999).

Discussion: Although the two hypotheses were initially corroborated, some limitations arose. First, there was a small, though significant difference in perceived difficulty between the fluency groups (“easy” vs “difficult”). Next, confidence in the evaluations was not measured (Aydin, 2016Aydin, A.E. (2016) Processing fluency: Examining its relationship with thinking style and purchase intention, Journal of Marketing Communications, 24(6), 588-598; Simmons & Nelson 2006Simmons, J.P., Nelson, L.D. (2006). Intuitive confidence: Choosing between intuitive and nonintuitive alternatives. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35(3), 409-428., 2007), so we can only deduce - not state - that individuals reinforce (or abandon) their intuitions/anchors before deciding on their purchase intentions. Last, and yet more important, business relationships can have more events (positive and/or negative ones) in their lifetimes (Oliver, 2010Oliver, R. L. (2010). Satisfaction, a behavioural perspective on the consumer (2. ed.). New York: Irwin McGraw-Hill.) and so the scenario manipulation may not fully incorporate this temporal view that results in overall satisfaction (Grönroos, 2009Grönroos, C. (2009). Marketing: Gerenciamento e serviços. (Marques A. S., & Belhassof, C. M. Trad.) (3a ed.). Rio de Janeiro: Elsevier.; Oliver, 2010).

3.2 Experiment 2

The objectives of experiment 2 were to check for congruence with the results of experiment 1, to resolve its limitations and to test the mediation effect of confidence. In the second experiment, the respondents’ actual business relationships were assessed through an online survey (Appendix B Appendix B - Second experiment questionnaire, disfluency/perceived difficulty - second experiment questionnaire, disfluency/perceived difficulty). In addition, the questions in the “difficult” manipulation were printed in bold letters to increase the perceived difficulty (Diemand-Yauman et al., 2011Diemand-Yauman, C., Oppenheimer, D. M., & Vaughan, E. B. (2011). Fortune favors the BOLD (and the Italicized): Effects of disfluency on educational outcomes. Cognition, 118(1), 111-5.).

Procedures: The experiment had a 3 (fluency: “easy” vs “difficult” vs control) factor design. We opted not to manipulate positive or negative commercial relationships but instead to use satisfaction evaluations of the respondents’ present financial service provider. A total of 361 undergraduate students started the test but only 326 completed the survey and were therefore considered valid (43.86% men, Mage = 29.32 years, SD = 8.72).

The individuals were invited by email and social media to participate in a Qualtrics survey about financial services companies. They started by answering socio-demographic questions and then were randomly assigned to one of the three conditions. Similarly to in experiment 1, in the “easy” task (perceived as easy), the individuals answered four overall satisfaction questions (Prado, 2004Prado, P. H. M. (2004). A avaliação do relacionamento sob a ótica do cliente: Um estudo em bancos de varejo. Tese (Doutorado em Administração) - Programa de Pós-Graduação em Administração de Empresas. Escola de Administração de Empresas de São Paulo, Fundação Getúlio Vargas, São Paulo.) before indicating their purchase intentions. For the “difficult” task (perceived as difficult) they answered four adapted questions made for each concept item (taken from Prado, 2004) - this time printed in bold letters - before indicating their purchase intentions. The control group first indicated their purchase intentions and then evaluated their commercial relationships. The purchase intention question was similar to the one used in experiment 1, but regarding an automobile insurance offer from their present financial services provider. Next, the respondents answered the fluency manipulation check (general perceived difficulty) and reported their confidence in their commercial relationship evaluations, followed by financial services questions regarding their use and buying frequency (these variables reveal non-significant relationships with the results). Following the same procedure from experiment 1, before ending the experiment, all the respondents answered a four-item overall satisfaction scale (Prado, 2004) regarding their present financial services provider (Figure 4 - experiment 2 flow).

Figure 4
Experiment 2 Flow.

Manipulation checks: the one-way anova test revealed that the individuals in the “difficult” group perceived it to be less easy to answer the four satisfaction questions (F(2,323) = 26.561, p<0.001, Measy = 7.47 SD=2.28, Mcontrol = 7.89, SD = 2.35, Mdifficult = 5.64, SD = 2.62 / p easyXcontrol = 0.398, p easyXdifficult <0.001, p controlXdifficult <0.001).

Results: to test hypotheses H1 and H2, we used the effects on purchase intentions -model 1 (bootstrapping with 5,000 resamples) of the conditional effects module (Hayes, 2013Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. New York: The Guilford Press. - SPSS 22). The fluency manipulation results (control: code “-1” vs “easy”: code “0” vs “difficult”: code “1”) revealed significant effects only for the “easy” task compared to the others (coef = -2.6559, t = -2.3188, p=0.0210, LLCI = -4.9094, ULCI = -0.4024 / “difficult” vs other groups, p = 0.9490). There was also a significant effect of average satisfaction (questions at the end of the survey) (coef = 0.5013, t=3.9692, p=0.0001, LLCI = 0.2528, ULCI = 0.7499) and an interaction effect between factors (fluency and average satisfaction) (coef = 0.4642, t = 2.6112, p = 0.0095, LLCI = -0.1144, ULCI = 0.8140) (Figure 5) and purchase intentions - with the same result pattern as in experiment 1. Easy and difficult questions (vs control group) made satisfied individuals increase their purchase intentions, corroborating hypothesis H2. Additionally, dissatisfied individuals with easy questions (vs difficult and control groups) developed lower purchase intentions, corroborating hypothesis H1.

Figure 5
Purchase Intentions: Average Satisfaction vs Fluency

Next, we tested whether the confidence in the commercial relationship evaluations could be mediating the effects on the consumers’ purchase intentions. First, we classified the individuals into groups according to the satisfaction averages (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.790) at the end of the questionnaires. Using Hart and Johnson’s (1999Hart, C. W., & Johnson, M. D. (1999). Growing the trust relationship. Marketing Management, 8(1), 9-19.) results, the individuals were grouped into: dissatisfied customers (average from 1 to 5.5, n = 89) and satisfied customers (6.5 to 10, n = 93) c excluding neutral-satisfied customers from the analyses (5.51 to 6.49, n = 39).

A two-way anova test revealed that valence (satisfied vs dissatisfied groups) had a significant effect on confidence in the evaluations (CONF) (F(1,268) = 70.121, p<0.001, ηp 2 = 0.078, CONFsatisfied = 7.91, SD = 1.66, CONFdissatisfied = 6.80, SD = 2.03). Fluency manipulation had a significant effect (F(2,268) = 22.742, p=0.001, ηp 2 = 0.050, CONFeasy = 7.77, SD = 1.72, CONFcontrol = 7.71, SD = 1.76, CONFdiffficult = 6.81, SD = 2.08) and an interaction effect (F(2,268) = 7.176, p = 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.051) without significant differences for satisfied customers, but with significant differences for dissatisfied customers (p easyXcontrol = 0.045, p easyXdifficult <0.001 p controlXdifficult 0.075) (Figure 6).

Figure 6
Confidence in Satisfaction Evaluations: Fluency vs Customer Groups

Then, with the sample divided between satisfied and dissatisfied customers, the mediating role of evaluation confidence was tested using model 4 (bootstrapping with 5,000 resamples) of the conditional effects module (Hayes, 2013Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. New York: The Guilford Press. - SPSS 22) (control: code “-1” vs “easy”: code “0” vs “difficult”: code “1”).

Satisfied customers with “easy” and “difficult” tasks had higher purchase intentions than the control group (coef = 1.0222, t = 3.0037, p = 0.0031, LLCI = 0.3496, ULCI = 1.6947) but there was no significant mediating effect of evaluation confidence (coef = -0.0056, t = -0.0559, p = 0.9555, LLCI = -0.2028, ULCI = 0.1917), therefore corroborating hypothesis H2 (commercial relationship evaluations - with fluency or disfluency - will directly bias satisfied consumers to increase their subsequent purchase intentions).

In contrast, the mediation analyses for dissatisfied customers revealed that their purchase intentions were only affected by evaluation confidence (coef =-0.3161, t = -2.7233, p = 0.0074, LLCI = -0.5459, ULCI = -0.0863). Dissatisfied customers with easy questions before the dependent variable had their confidence increased and then indicated lower purchase intentions (“easy” vs “control” vs “difficult”: coef = -0.8316, t = -1.6617, p = 0.0992, LLCI = -1.8225, ULCI = 0.1593) (F(2,120) = 7.6423, R2 = 0.1130 / indirect effects Coef = -0.4816, Se(boot) = 0.2020, LLCI = -0.9679, ULCI = -0.1612), thus corroborating hypothesis H1.

Discussion: the results from experiment 2, with respondents evaluating their business relationships with financial services companies, converged with the results of experiment 1, with undergraduate students evaluating scenarios (a home telephone, internet, and cable TV company). Both hypotheses were corroborated and it was further demonstrated that dissatisfied customers, despite experiencing a significant decrease in their evaluation confidence in difficult tasks, did not increase their purchase intentions compared to the control group. This is an important result and can only be verified using a control group, which is not pointed out in other studies, as they usually only make the comparison between fluency (perceived ease) and disfluency (perceived difficulty) tasks (Aydin, 2016Aydin, A.E. (2016) Processing fluency: Examining its relationship with thinking style and purchase intention, Journal of Marketing Communications, 24(6), 588-598; Kahneman, 2011Kahneman, D. (2011). Rápido e Devagar: Duas formas de pensar. Rio de Janeiro: Objetiva.; Schwarz, 2004Schwarz, N. (2004). Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 332-348.; Simmons & Nelson, 2006Simmons, J.P., Nelson, L.D. (2006). Intuitive confidence: Choosing between intuitive and nonintuitive alternatives. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35(3), 409-428., 2007). Furthermore, although other studies point to higher confidence in tasks perceived as easy, and less confidence in difficult tasks (Aydin, 2016; Epley & Norwick, 2006Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2006). The anchoring-and-Adjustment Heuristic. Psychological Science 17(4), 311-318., Simmons & Nelson, 2006, 2007; Kahneman, 2011), this experiment demonstrated that satisfied consumers maintain their overconfidence in their commercial relationship evaluations even in more difficult judgment tasks.

4 Final Considerations

4.1 Theoretical implications

This study demonstrated the implications of the ease and difficulty of satisfaction evaluations on subsequent purchase intentions. It also demonstrated that purchase intention results were influenced by two different mechanisms. Dissatisfied customers with a perception of ease in commercial relationship evaluations developed higher overconfidence, lowering their purchase intentions and thus corroborating the indirect effect hypothesis. Otherwise, satisfied customers with either easy or difficult evaluations showed overconfidence, therefore being directly affected by their evaluations with positive results.

Moreover, the study demonstrated congruent results with other fluency manipulations, but it corroborated the hypotheses by not only comparing difficult versus easy tasks (as in Aydin, 2016Aydin, A.E. (2016) Processing fluency: Examining its relationship with thinking style and purchase intention, Journal of Marketing Communications, 24(6), 588-598; Simons & Nelson 2006; 2007; among others), but also in comparisons with a control group. Fluency was directly manipulated conceptually by overall satisfaction evaluations, showing the same pattern of results as other studies. However, these other studies’ manipulations usually took longer or needed more effort to be concluded - such as in a fact-remembering task (easy: 6 facts vs difficult: 12 facts) (Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 1999Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). How often did I do it? Experienced ease of retrieval and frequency estimates of past behavior. Acta Psychologica, 103(1-2), 77-89.; Haddock, 2002Haddock, G. (2002). It’s easy to like or dislike Tony Blair: Accessibility experiences and the favorability of attitude judgments. British Journal of Social Psychology, 93(2), 257-267.; Viacava et al., 2015). Furthermore, other fluency manipulations, even more subtle ones, can be tested and can lead to similar results. It is possible to verify these effects by manipulating the perceived difficulty with very small letters (Su et al., 2018), using green, yellow, or light blue fonts on a white background (Reber & Schwarz, 1999Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truth. Consciousness and Cognition, 8(3), 338-342), with Brusch or Mistral typefaces (versus Arial) (Song & Schwarz, 2008), with italics (Diemand-Yauman et al., 2011Diemand-Yauman, C., Oppenheimer, D. M., & Vaughan, E. B. (2011). Fortune favors the BOLD (and the Italicized): Effects of disfluency on educational outcomes. Cognition, 118(1), 111-5.), or by inserting more technical or low-knowledge keywords (Oppenheimer, 2004Oppenheimer, D. M. (2004) Spontaneous discounting of availability in frequency judgment tasks. Psychological Science, 15(2), 100-105).

Some attention must be paid when trying to use these manipulations, altogether or with the intention of intensifying the effects. When the difficulty is set too high, individuals will stop thinking that “they” have difficulty completing the task, and will start thinking that “the task itself” is too difficult, which can lessen the effects (Schwarz, 2004Schwarz, N. (2004). Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 332-348.). In addition, making tasks too difficult and asking individuals to keep doing them could lead to ego depletion effects. Ego depletion is the momentary loss of cognitive capacity (depletion of cognitive resources) due to great effort and/or longer tasks. In ego depletion manipulations individuals are required to suppress their thoughts, inhibit their emotions, act against their principles, avoid reading words in a video, or try to solve great effort anagram tasks (or even unsolvable ones), which can also lead to greater purchase intentions (Baumeister & Tierney, 2011Baumeister, R.F., & Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Rediscovering the greatest human strength. New York, NY, US: Penguin Books.; Usta & Häubl, 2011).

4.2 Managerial implications

Despite the experiments’ results involving a small sample, we were able to verify that the perceived difficulty in satisfaction evaluations led to greater subsequent purchase intentions. If difficult tasks were used in practice (such as here or in other manipulations), satisfied consumers would not have their purchasing decisions negatively affected, while dissatisfied consumers would at least start to deliberate, and thus consider a less intuitive option (Epley & Norwick, 2006Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2006). The anchoring-and-Adjustment Heuristic. Psychological Science 17(4), 311-318.; Simons & Nelson 2006, 2007). However, here some ethical dilemmas begin. Although these tasks could not deplete all the consumers’ cognitive resources (Baumeister & Tierney, 2011Baumeister, R.F., & Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Rediscovering the greatest human strength. New York, NY, US: Penguin Books.) they do affect their ability to make decisions. These tasks do affect consumers’ confidence and in doing so they affect their opinions, intuitions, evaluations, and judgments. It is also important to emphasize that although individuals believe they are not being affected by biases (Santos & Barros, 2011Santos, J. O. dos, & Barros, C. A.S. (2011). O que determina a tomada de decisão financeira: Razão ou emoção?. Revista brasileira gestão de negócios, 13(38), 7-20.) these effects are present in the daily lives of consumers and decision makers (Kahneman, 2011Kahneman, D. (2011). Rápido e Devagar: Duas formas de pensar. Rio de Janeiro: Objetiva.). Hence, they can make decisions against their intuitions and even against their initial beliefs, with non-beneficial results for them. Nevertheless, we found that the main effect on purchase intentions was due to their commercial relationship evaluations, denoting the need to focus on better quality services that lead to satisfaction (Grönroos, 2009Grönroos, C. (2009). Marketing: Gerenciamento e serviços. (Marques A. S., & Belhassof, C. M. Trad.) (3a ed.). Rio de Janeiro: Elsevier.; Oliver, 2010Oliver, R. L. (2010). Satisfaction, a behavioural perspective on the consumer (2. ed.). New York: Irwin McGraw-Hill.).

4.3 Limitations and future studies

Firstly, the main limitation is the use of experiments with small samples and mainly in commercial relationship situations. Thus, further studies using different approaches or assessing different contexts are needed to verify this phenomenon. Here, a new fluency manipulation was demonstrated, but it is still possible to verify some of the fluency effects occurring in our daily lives (Alter & Oppenheimer, 2009Alter, A.L., & Oppenheimer, D.M. (2009) Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3), 219-235.), or to verify the influences of perceived ease or difficulty and overconfidence related to the individuals’ ability to process information (such as having difficulties in mathematics) as well as other characteristics. It is possible to verify the impact of factors such as age, the level of education (Mendes-da-Silva & Yu, 2009), greater intrinsic experience that affects self-confidence (Bortoli & Soares, 2019Bortoli, C., & Soares, R. O. (2019). Executivos com maior sofisticação financeira são mais confiantes e otimistas? Revista de administração contemporânea, 23(2), 268-287.), and the acceptance of anchors that could influence subsequent decisions (Tronco, Löbler, dos Santos, & Nishi, 2019). Verifying whether consumers’ purchase intentions are affected by their difficulty to calculate monthly interest and installments, or even differences in younger and older people’s overconfidence (Mendes-da-Silva & Yu, 2009), would help store owners to think about how to proceed in such situations.

Moreover, the different techniques to manipulate visual fluency (size or color typefaces, etc.) may produce results without managers’ awareness, for example, in eWOM (electronic word of mouth) results. Much has been studied about the motivations for making, receiving, or sharing eWOMs and the impact of perceived credibility (Chen, Yang, & Wang, 2016Chen, Y., Yang, S., & Wang, Z. (2016). Service cooperation and marketing strategies of Infomediary and online retailer with eWOM effect. Information and Technology Management 17(2), 109-118; Hennig-Thurau, Gwinner, Walsh, & Gremler, 2004Hennig‐Thurau, T., Gwinner, K. P., Walsh, G., & Gremler, D. D. (2004). Electronic word‐of‐mouth via consumer‐opinion platforms: What motivates consumers to articulate themselves on the Internet? Journal of Interactive Marketing, 18(1), 38-52.). Studies indicate that higher argument quality and writing clarity (general aspects) can positively influence credibility and eWOM acceptance (Cheung, Luo, Sia, & Chen 2009Cheung, M. Y., Luo, C., Sia, C. L., & Chen, H. (2009) Credibility of electronic word-of-mouth: Informational and normative determinants of on-line consumer recommendations, International Journal of Electronic Commerce, 13(4), 9-38.; Moran, Muzellec, & Nolan, 2014Moran, G., Muzellec, L., & Nolan, E. (2014) Consumer moments of truth in the digital context: How “search” and “e-word of mouth” can fuel consumer decision-making. Journal of Advertising Research, 54(2), 200-204). Therefore, visual fluency, or even conceptual fluency, may affect the credibility and thus the acceptance of eWOMs.

In addition, future studies could verify whether these effects are related to perceived pleasure or cognitive comfort when processing information (Duke et al., 2014Duke, D., Fiacconi, C. M., & Kohler, S. (2014). Parallel effects of processing fluency and positive affect on familiarity-based recognition decisions for faces. Front. Psychol, 5, 5-328; Labroo & Pocheptsova, 2016Labroo, A. A., & Pocheptsova, A. (2016). Metacognition and consumer judgment: Fluency is pleasant but disfluency ignites interest. Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 154-159.). However, despite the fact that dissatisfied customers may feel greater ease in their commercial evaluations (Claypool et al., 2015Claypool, H. M., Mackie, D. M., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2015). Fluency and attitudes. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(7), 370-382.; Landwehr et al., 2017Landwehr, J. R., Golla, B., & Reber, R. (2017). Processing fluency: An inevitable side effect of evaluative conditioning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 124-128.), they might not experience pleasure or comfort remembering negative commercial events (Maier & Dost, 2018Maier, E., & Dost, F. (2018). The positive effect of contextual image backgrounds on fluency and liking. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 40, 109-116.).

Finally, individuals may expect difficulties (or ease) in their decisions. If these expectations fit with the difficulty/ease encountered, this congruence would lead to better product evaluations (Jiang & Hong, 2014Jiang, Y., & Hong, J. (2014). It feels fluent, but not right: The interactive effect of expected and experienced processing fluency on evaluative judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 54, 147-152). These situations, involving evaluations or decisions, could be more complex and relevant in real-life environments due to low expertise (Payne et al., 1992Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., Coupey, E., & Johnson, E. J. (1992). A constructive process view of decision making: Multiple strategies in judgment and choice. Acta Psychologica, 80(1-3), 107-141.) - mainly due to low general knowledge about the same or other products.

Thus, it is possible to find different results when consumers are making decisions about the same or other products/services, depending on their involvement.

References

  • Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). How often did I do it? Experienced ease of retrieval and frequency estimates of past behavior. Acta Psychologica, 103(1-2), 77-89.
  • Agnihotri, R., Dingus, R., Hu, M.Y., & Krush, m. (2016) Social media: Influencing customer satisfaction in B2B sales. Industrial Marketing Management, 53, 172-180.
  • Albrecht, S., & Carbon, C-C. (2014) the fluency amplification model: Fluent stimuli show more intense but not evidently more positive evaluations. Acta Psychologica, 148, 195-203.
  • Alter, A.L., & Oppenheimer, D.M. (2006) Predicting short-term stock fluctuations by using processing fluency. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(24), 9369-9372.
  • Alter, A.L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N., & Eyre, R. N. (2007). Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(4), 569-576.
  • Alter, A.L., & Oppenheimer, D.M. (2009) Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3), 219-235.
  • Alter, A.L., & Oppenheimer, D.M. (2008) Easy on the mind, easy on the wallet: The roles of familiarity and processing fluency in valuation judgments. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 15(5), 985-990.
  • Alter, L.A. (2013) The benefits of cognitive disfluency. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(6), 437-442.
  • Aydin, A.E. (2016) Processing fluency: Examining its relationship with thinking style and purchase intention, Journal of Marketing Communications, 24(6), 588-598
  • Baron, J. (2008). Thinking and deciding: Normative theory of utility and choice (4a ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Bajšanski, I., Žauhar, V., & Valerjev, P. Confidence judgments in syllogistic reasoning: The role of consistency and response cardinality, Thinking & Reasoning, 25(1), 14-47.
  • Baumeister, R.F., & Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Rediscovering the greatest human strength. New York, NY, US: Penguin Books.
  • Bettencourt, L. A. (1997). Customer voluntary performance: Customer AX’s partners in service delivery. Journal of retailing, 73(3), 383-406.
  • Bohn A, & Berntsen D. (2007). Pleasantness bias in flashbulb memories: Positive and negative flashbulb memories of the fall of the Berlin Wall among East and West Germans. Memory and Cognition, 35(3), 565- 577.
  • Bortoli, C., & Soares, R. O. (2019). Executivos com maior sofisticação financeira são mais confiantes e otimistas? Revista de administração contemporânea, 23(2), 268-287.
  • Bowen, H. J., Kark, S. M. & Kensinger, E. A. (2018). NEVER forget: Negative emotional valence enhances recapitulation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(3), 870-891.
  • Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (2006). Mood and the regulation of information processing and behaviour. In: J. Forgas (ed.) Affect in social thinking and behaviour (pp.65-84). New York: Psychology Press.
  • Bloemer, J., Ruyter, K., & Peeters, P (1998). Investigating drivers of bank loyalty: The complex relationship between image, service quality and satisfaction. International Journal of Bank Marketing, 16(7), 276-286.
  • Bookbinder, S. H., & Brainerd, C. J. (2017). Emotionally negative pictures enhance gist memory. Emotion, 17(1), 102-119
  • Brewin, C. R., & Langley, K. M. R. (2009). Imagery retrieval may explain why recall of negative scenes contains more accurate detail. Memory & Cognition, 47(3), 420-427.
  • Caplan, J. B., Sommer, T., Madan, C.R., & Fujiwara, E. (2019). Reduced associative memory for negative information: Impact of confidence and interactive imagery during study. Cognition and Emotion, 33(8), 1745-1753
  • Charness, G. E., & Dave, C. (2017). Confirmation bias with motivated beliefs. Games and Economic Behavior, 104, 1-23.
  • Chen, Y., Yang, S., & Wang, Z. (2016). Service cooperation and marketing strategies of Infomediary and online retailer with eWOM effect. Information and Technology Management 17(2), 109-118
  • Cheung, M. Y., Luo, C., Sia, C. L., & Chen, H. (2009) Credibility of electronic word-of-mouth: Informational and normative determinants of on-line consumer recommendations, International Journal of Electronic Commerce, 13(4), 9-38.
  • Claypool, H. M., Mackie, D. M., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2015). Fluency and attitudes. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(7), 370-382.
  • Constable, M .D., Bayliss, A. P., Tipper, S. P., & Kritikos, A. (2013). Self-generated cognitive fluency as an alternative route to preference formation. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(1),
  • Diemand-Yauman, C., Oppenheimer, D. M., & Vaughan, E. B. (2011). Fortune favors the BOLD (and the Italicized): Effects of disfluency on educational outcomes. Cognition, 118(1), 111-5.
  • Duke, D., Fiacconi, C. M., & Kohler, S. (2014). Parallel effects of processing fluency and positive affect on familiarity-based recognition decisions for faces. Front. Psychol, 5, 5-328
  • Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2006). The anchoring-and-Adjustment Heuristic. Psychological Science 17(4), 311-318.
  • Fornell, C., Johnson, M. D., Anderson, E. W., Cha, J., & Bryant, B. E. (1994). The American customer satisfaction: Nature, purpose and findings. Journal of Marketing, 58(4), 7-18.
  • Fornell, C., Morgeson , F. V., & Hult, G. T. (2016). An abnormally abnormal intangible: Stock returns on customer satisfaction. Journal of Marketing, 80(5), 122-125.
  • Francisco-Maffezzolli, Eliane C., & Prado, Paulo H. M (2013). Avaliação de resultados no varejo de serviço sob a ótica do consumidor. Revista brasileira de gestão e negocios. 15(46), 112-128.
  • Garbarino, E., & Johnson, M.S. (1999). The different roles of satisfaction, trust, and commitment in customer relationships. Journal of Marketing, 63(2), 70-97.
  • Graf, L.K.M., Mayer, S., & Landwehr, J. R. (2018). Measuring processing fluency: One versus five items. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 28(3), 393-411.
  • Grönroos, C. (2009). Marketing: Gerenciamento e serviços. (Marques A. S., & Belhassof, C. M. Trad.) (3a ed.). Rio de Janeiro: Elsevier.
  • Haddock, G. (2002). It’s easy to like or dislike Tony Blair: Accessibility experiences and the favorability of attitude judgments. British Journal of Social Psychology, 93(2), 257-267.
  • Hart, C. W., & Johnson, M. D. (1999). Growing the trust relationship. Marketing Management, 8(1), 9-19.
  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. New York: The Guilford Press.
  • Hennig‐Thurau, T., Gwinner, K. P., Walsh, G., & Gremler, D. D. (2004). Electronic word‐of‐mouth via consumer‐opinion platforms: What motivates consumers to articulate themselves on the Internet? Journal of Interactive Marketing, 18(1), 38-52.
  • Hernandez, I. & Preston, J. L. (2013). Disfluency disrupts the confirmation bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(1), 178-182
  • Herr, P. M. & Page, C. M. (2004). Asymmetric association of liking and disliking judgments: So what’s not to like? Journal of Consumer Research, 30(4), 588-601.
  • Herr, P. M., Page, C. M., Pfeiffer, B. E., & Davis, F. D. (2012). Affective influences on evaluative processing. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(5), 833-845.
  • Hostler, T. J., Wood, C., & Armitage, C. J. (2018). The influence of emotional cues on prospective memory: A systematic Review with meta-analyses, Cognition and Emotion, 32(8), 1578-1596.
  • Jiang, Y., & Hong, J. (2014). It feels fluent, but not right: The interactive effect of expected and experienced processing fluency on evaluative judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 54, 147-152
  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Rápido e Devagar: Duas formas de pensar. Rio de Janeiro: Objetiva.
  • Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Neural processes supporting young and older adults’ emotional memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(7), 1161-73.
  • Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emocion Review, 1(2), 99-113.
  • Kensinger, E. A., & Mark, S. M. (2018). Emotion and memory. In Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. New York: John Wiley &Sons, inc.
  • Koch, A. S., & Forgas, J. P. (2012). Feeling good and feeling truth: The interactive effects of mood and processing fluency on truth judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(2), 481-485
  • Kornell, N., Rhodes, M. G., Castel, A. D., & Tauber, S. K. (2011). The ease-of-processing heuristic and the stability bias dissociating memory, memory beliefs, and memory judgments. Psychological Science, 22(6), 787-794
  • Labroo, A. A., Dhar, R., & Schwarz, N. (2008). Of frog wines and frowing watches: Semantic priming, perceptual fluency, and brand evaluation. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(6), 819-831.
  • Labroo, A. A., & Pocheptsova, A. (2016). Metacognition and consumer judgment: Fluency is pleasant but disfluency ignites interest. Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 154-159.
  • Landwehr, J. R., Golla, B., & Reber, R. (2017). Processing fluency: An inevitable side effect of evaluative conditioning. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 124-128.
  • Larivière, B., Keiningham, T., Aksoy, L., Yalcin, A., Morgeson, F., & Mithas, S. (2016). Modeling heterogeneity in the satisfaction, loyalty intention, and shareholder value linkage: A cross-industry analysis at the customer and firm levels. Journal of Marketing Research, 53(1), 91-109.
  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Luppe, M. R., & Angelo, C. F. de (2010). As decisões de consumo e a heurística da ancoragem: Uma análise da racionalidade do processo de escolha. RAM Revista de Administração Mackenzie, 11(6), 81-106.
  • Madan, C. R., Scott, S. M. E., & Kensinger, E. A. (2019). Positive emotion enhances association-memory. Emotion, 19(4), 733-740.
  • Maier, E., & Dost, F. (2018). The positive effect of contextual image backgrounds on fluency and liking. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 40, 109-116.
  • Malhotra, N. K. (2010). Pesquisa de marketing: Uma orientação aplicada. Porto Alegre: Bookman.
  • Matovic, D., Kock, A. S., & Forgas, J. P. (2014). Can negative mood improve language understanding? Affective influences on the ability to detect ambiguous communication. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 44-49.
  • Mendes-Da-Silva, W., & Yu, A. S. O. (2009). Análise empírica do senso de controle: Buscando entender o excesso de confiança. Revista de administração contemporânea, 13(2), 247-271.
  • Miele, D. B., & Molden, D. C. (2010). Naive theories of intelligence and the role of processing fluency in perceived comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3), 535-557.
  • Moran, G., Muzellec, L., & Nolan, E. (2014) Consumer moments of truth in the digital context: How “search” and “e-word of mouth” can fuel consumer decision-making. Journal of Advertising Research, 54(2), 200-204
  • Nunes, J. C., Ordani, A., & Valsesia, F. (2015). The power of repetition: Repetitive lyrics in a song increase processing fluency and drive market success. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(2), 187-199.
  • Oliver, R. L. (2010). Satisfaction, a behavioural perspective on the consumer (2. ed.). New York: Irwin McGraw-Hill.
  • Oppenheimer, D. M. (2004) Spontaneous discounting of availability in frequency judgment tasks. Psychological Science, 15(2), 100-105
  • Oppenheimer, D. M., & Alter, A. L. (2013). Disfluency sleeper effect: Disfluency today promotes fluency tomorrow. In C. Unkelbach & Rom. Greifender (Eds.), The experience of thinking: How the fluency of mental processes influences cognition and behaviour (pp. 85-97). New York: Psychology Press.
  • Orita, R. & Hattori, M. (2018). Positive and negative affects facilitate insight problem-solving in different ways: A study with Implicit hints. Japanese Psychological Research, Special issue: Implicit Cognition, 61(2), 94-106
  • Patrick, V. M., Chun, H. H., & Macinnis, D. J. (2009) Affective forecasting and self-control: Why anticipating pride wins over anticipating shame in a self-regulation context. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19(3), 537-545.
  • Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., Coupey, E., & Johnson, E. J. (1992). A constructive process view of decision making: Multiple strategies in judgment and choice. Acta Psychologica, 80(1-3), 107-141.
  • Park, Y., Herr, P. M., & Kim, B. C. (2016). The effect of disfluency on consumer perceptions of information security. Marketing Letters, 27(3), 525-535.
  • Prado, P. H. M. (2004). A avaliação do relacionamento sob a ótica do cliente: Um estudo em bancos de varejo. Tese (Doutorado em Administração) - Programa de Pós-Graduação em Administração de Empresas. Escola de Administração de Empresas de São Paulo, Fundação Getúlio Vargas, São Paulo.
  • Prato, F., & John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative Social Information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(3), 380-391.
  • Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truth. Consciousness and Cognition, 8(3), 338-342
  • Rowe G, Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson A.K. (2007). Positive affect increases the breadth of attentional selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(1), 383-388.
  • Ryffel, F. A., & Wirth, W. (2016). Heart versus mind: How affective and cognitive message frames change attitudes. Social Psychology, 47(1), 52-62.
  • Ryffel, F. A., & Wirth, W. (2018). How perceived processing fluency influences the illusion of knowing in learning from TV reports. Journal of Media Psychology, 32(1), 2-13
  • Sanchez, C., & Jaeger, A. J. (2015). If it’s hard to read, it changes how long you do it: Reading time as an explanation for perceptual fluency effects on judgment. Psychon B ull Rev, 22(1), 206-211.
  • Santos, J. O. dos, & Barros, C. A.S. (2011). O que determina a tomada de decisão financeira: Razão ou emoção?. Revista brasileira gestão de negócios, 13(38), 7-20.
  • Schwarz, N. (2004). Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 332-348.
  • Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. In Cook, T. D. & Campbell (Ed.) Journal of the American Statistical Association, 100. Boston: Houghton Mifflin
  • Silva, R. R., Garcia-Marques, T., & Reber, R. (2017). The informative value of type of repetition: Perceptual and conceptual fluency influences on judgments of truth. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 51, 53-67.
  • Simmons, J.P., Nelson, L.D. (2006). Intuitive confidence: Choosing between intuitive and nonintuitive alternatives. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35(3), 409-428.
  • Simmons, J. P., & Nelson, L. D. (2007). Intuitive confidence: When consumer choices are sensitive to matching prices. Association for Consumer Research, 34, 265-268
  • Sirgy, J. M. A. (1984) Social cognition model of consumer satisfaction/dissatisfaction: An experiment. Psychology & Marketing, 1(2), 27- 44.
  • Song, H., & Schwarz, N. (2008). If it’s hard to read, it’s hard to do: Processing fluency affects effort prediction and motivation. Psychological Science, 19(10), 986-988.
  • Storbeck J., & Clore G.L. (2005). With sadness comes accuracy; with happiness, false memory: Mood and the false memory effect. Psychological Science, 16(1), 785-791.
  • Su, N., Li, T., Zheng, J., Hu, X., Fan, T., & Lu, L. (2018). How font size affects judgments of learning: Simultaneous mediating effect of item-specific beliefs about fluency and moderating effect of beliefs about font size and memory. PLOS ONE 13, 1-14.
  • Talarico, J. M., Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2009). Positive emotions enhance recall of peripheral details. Cognition and Emotion, 23(2), 380-398.
  • Tronco, P. B., Lobler, M. L., Santos, L. G., & Nishi, J. M. (2019). Heurística da ancoragem na decisão de especialistas: Resultados sob teste de manipulação. Revista de Administração Contemporânea, 23(3), 331-350.
  • Usta, M., & Häubl, G. (2011). Self-regulatory strength and consumers’ relinquishment of decision control: When less effortful decisions are more resource depleting. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(2), 403-412.
  • Viacava, J. J. C, Mantovani, D., Korelo, J. C., & Prado, P. H. M. (2015). Vieses na satisfação: O efeito moderador da dificuldade e da obrigação sobre recordações positivas e negativas. REAd. Revista eletrônica administração. 21(2), 437-461.
  • Winkielman, P., & Schwarz, N. (2001). How pleasant was your childhood? Beliefs about memory shape inferences from experienced difficulty of recall. Psychological Science, 12(2), 176-179.
  • Yang, C., Huang, T. E., & Shanks, D. R. (2018). Perceptual fluency affects judgments of learning: The font size effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 99(1), 99-110
  • Weissgerber, C. S., & Reinhard, M. A. (2017). Is disfluency desirable for learning? Learning and Instruction, 49, 199-217.
  • Evaluation process:

    Double Blind Review

Appendix A - First experiment questionnaires

Appendix B - Second experiment questionnaire, disfluency/perceived difficulty

Responsible Editor:

Prof. Dr. Sebastián Molinillo

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    30 Nov 2020
  • Date of issue
    Oct-Dec 2020

History

  • Received
    03 Nov 2019
  • Accepted
    01 June 2020
Fundação Escola de Comércio Álvares Penteado Fundação Escola de Comércio Álvares Penteado, Av. da Liberdade, 532, 01.502-001 , São Paulo, SP, Brasil , (+55 11) 3272-2340 , (+55 11) 3272-2302, (+55 11) 3272-2302 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: rbgn@fecap.br