Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Verbal fluency in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment in individuals with low educational level and its relationship with reading and writing habits

Fluência verbal na doença de Alzheimer e comprometimento cognitivo leve na baixa escolaridade e sua relação com hábitos de leitura e de escrita

ABSTRACT.

Verbal fluency (VF) has contributed to building cognitive maps as well as differentiating healthy populations from those with dementia.

Objectives:

To compare the performance of healthy controls and patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) in two semantic VF tasks (animals/clothes) and a phonemic VF task (letter P). Also, to analyze the relationship between the frequency of reading and writing habits (FRWH) and VF in individuals with low educational level.

Methods:

Sixty-seven older adults aged 60-80 years and with 2-8 years of schooling were divided into three groups: controls (n=25), older adults with MCI (n=24), and older adults with AD (n=18). We analyzed the type, mean size, and number of clusters, switches, intersections, and returns. A post-hoc single-factor ANOVA analysis was conducted to verify differences between groups.

Results:

Total words in the phonemic VF and the animal category discriminated the three groups. Regarding the animal category, AD patients performed worse than controls in the total number of words, taxonomic clusters, returns, and number of words remembered. We found a moderate correlation between FRWH and total number of words in the phonemic fluency.

Conclusions:

Semantic (animate) and phonemic (total words) VF differentiated controls and clinical groups from each other - the phonemic component was more related to FRWH than the semantic one. The phonemic VF seems to be more related to cognitive reserve. VF tasks, considering total words and cluster analyses, are a valuable tool to test healthy and cognitively impaired older adults who have a low educational level.

Keywords:
fluency; Alzheimer’s Disease; Mild Cognitive Impairment; reading; writing; habits; schooling

RESUMO.

A tarefa de fluência verbal (FV) contribui para um mapeamento cognitivo e diferenciação entre populações saudáveis e com demência.

Objetivos:

Comparar o desempenho em duas tarefas de FV semântica (animais/roupas) e uma fonêmica (letra P) entre controles saudáveis e pacientes com Comprometimento Cognitivo Leve (CCL) e Doença de Alzheimer (DA). Além disso, analisar a relação entre frequência de hábitos de leitura e escrita (FHLE) e a FV nesses grupos de baixa escolaridade.

Métodos:

Sessenta e sete adultos idosos foram divididos em três grupos: controles (n=25), idosos com CCL (n=24) e idosos com DA (n=18), 60-80 anos de idade e 2-8 anos de escolaridade. Avaliaram-se tipo, tamanho médio e quantidade de agrupamentos, alternâncias, intersecções e retornos. Conduziu-se uma análise ANOVA de um fator com post hoc para verificar diferenças entre grupos.

Resultados:

O total de palavras na FV fonêmica e a categoria animais discriminaram os três grupos. Na categoria animais, pacientes com DA demonstraram desempenho inferior ao dos controles no número total de palavras, agrupamentos taxonômicos, retornos e número de palavras evocadas. Houve correlação moderada entre FHLE e número total de palavras na FV fonêmica.

Conclusões:

O componente semântico (animado) e o fonêmico (total de palavras) da FV diferenciaram os controles e os grupos clínicos entre si; o fonêmico relacionou-se mais com a FHLE do que o semântico. A FV fonêmica parece ser mais relacionada à reserva cognitiva. Tarefas de FV, considerando o total de palavras e as análises de clusters, são ferramentas valiosas para testar adultos idosos saudáveis e com declínio cognitivo na baixa escolaridade.

Palavras-chave:
fluência; Doença de Alzheimer; Comprometimento Cognitivo Leve; leitura; escrita; hábitos; escolaridade

INTRODUCTION

This study aimed at verifying whether healthy older adults and those with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) presented differences in semantic and phonemic verbal fluency (VF) tasks, as well as exploring the relationship between VF and frequency of reading and writing habits (FRWH), analyzing if lifelong cognitive stimulation through the individuals’ reading and writing habits affected their VF performance. VF ability is often used to assess cognitive decline, such as in MCI and AD,11. Clark LJ, Gatz M, Zheng L, Chen YL, McCleary C, Mack WJ. Longitudinal verbal fluency in normal aging, preclinical, and prevalent Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2009;24(6):461-8. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317509345154
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1177/...
,22. Wajman JR, Cecchini MA, Bertolucci PH, Mansur LL. Quanti-qualitative components of the semantic verbal fluency test in cognitively healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia subtypes. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(6):533-42. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
being widely present in clinical and research settings since it is easy and quick to administer. Studies show that VF can be useful in differentiating MCI and AD.33. Adlam A-L, Patterson K, Rogers TT, Nestor PJ, Salmond CH, Acosta-Cabronero J, et al. Semantic dementia and fluent primary progressive aphasia: two sides of the same coin? Brain. 2006;129(Pt 11):3066-80. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl285
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1093/...
Additionally, VF measures two important cognitive components usually linked to cognitive decline: verbal and executive skills.44. Shao Z, Janse E, Visser K, Meyer AS. What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Front Psychol. 2014;5:772. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3389/...
Uttering words during the task requires lexical access and retrieval, which, in turn, demand the recruitment of executive functions related to sub-constructs of inhibition and self-monitoring, as well as attention. Two types of VF tasks are typically administered: phonemic fluency (PF), or orthographic and letter fluency, and semantic fluency (SF), or category fluency.

The systematic search for words throughout the semantic system results in clustering organization, that is, the strategy of listing words that can be put into the same category. For example, when asked to produce names of animals, individuals usually organize their production in category subsets, namely, domestic animals, farm animals, wild animals, etc. Thus, measuring the size and quality of these clusters is an efficient way of evaluating verbal memory and storage capacity.55. Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Leach L, Freedman M. Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998;4(2):137-43. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798001374
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
Additionally to clustering, switching analyses provide interesting evidence to understand the lexico-semantic system, as they measure the ability to switch between these subsets when the individual can no longer produce words following a specific subcategory. Therefore, together with total raw scores, theses analyses can efficiently differentiate clinical groups, as in the study by Wajman et al.,22. Wajman JR, Cecchini MA, Bertolucci PH, Mansur LL. Quanti-qualitative components of the semantic verbal fluency test in cognitively healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia subtypes. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(6):533-42. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
in which cluster analyses were sensitive enough to discriminate between controls, MCI, AD, and frontotemporal dementia patients. Also, Troyer et al.55. Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Leach L, Freedman M. Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998;4(2):137-43. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798001374
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
revealed that AD patients tend to produce smaller cluster sizes and have decreased switching ability when compared to healthy individuals. Finally, only a few studies included analyses of returns. Returning and reapplying not only imply that the individual has to remember previously uttered words, thus having to use episodic and working memory capacity, but also demand attention and cognitive flexibility to keep producing words within the same category.66. Hermes-Pereira A, Gonçalves AB, Holz M, Gonçalves HA, Kochhann R, Joanette Y, et al. Influence of age and education on the processing of clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2018;12(4):360-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...

Some authors have argued that SF tends to be more sensitive for AD diagnosis compared to PF.77. Henry JD, Crawford JR, Phillips LH. Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(9):1212-22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
,88. Caramelli P, Carthery-Goulart MT, Porto CS, Charchat-Fichman H, Nitrini R. Category fluency as a screening test for Alzheimer disease in illiterate and literate patients. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2007;21(1):65-7. https://doi.org/10.1097/wad.0b013e31802f244f
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1097/...
,99. Rodríguez-Aranda C, Waterloo K, Johnsen SH, Eldevik P, Sparr S, Wikran GC, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of verbal fluency in early Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging. Brain Lang. 2016;155-156:24-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.03.001
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
This assumption could result from the fact that the neural correlates that underlie semantic processing are usually associated with degraded areas found in AD.99. Rodríguez-Aranda C, Waterloo K, Johnsen SH, Eldevik P, Sparr S, Wikran GC, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of verbal fluency in early Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging. Brain Lang. 2016;155-156:24-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.03.001
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
The SF for animals is widely used and has shown good diagnostic sensitivity among studies.1010. Radanovic M, Diniz BS, Mirandez RM, Novaretti TM, Flacks MK, Yassuda MS, et al. Verbal fluency in the detection of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease among Brazilian Portuguese speakers: the influence of education. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(6):1081-7. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1041610209990639
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
,1111. Pakhomov SVS, Eberly LE, Knopman DS. Recurrent perseverations on semantic verbal fluency tasks as an early marker of cognitive impairment. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2018;40(8):832-40. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2018.1438372
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...

Another issue that has been highly discussed is the interaction between educational level and cognitive functions, including language. Higher educational levels are associated with better performance in neuropsychological assessments,1212. Capitani E, Barbarotto R, Laiacona M. Does education influence the age‐related cognitive decline? A further inquiry. Dev Neuropsychol. 1996;12(2):231-40. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649609540648
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
,1313. Santos Nogueira D, Azevedo Reis E, Vieira A. Verbal fluency tasks: effects of age, gender, and education. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2016;68(3):124-33. https://doi.org/10.1159/000450640
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1159/...
,1414. Parente MA, Scherer LC, Zimmermann N, Fonseca R. Evidências do papel da escolaridade na organização cerebral. Neuropsicol Lat Am. 2009;1(1):72-80.,1515. Parente MA, Fonseca RP, Scherer LC. Literacy as a determining factor for brain organization: from Lecours’ contribution to the present day. Dement Neuropsychol. 2008;2(3):164. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN20300002
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
,1616. Passos VM, Giatti L, Bensenor I, Tiemeier H, Ikram MA, Figueiredo RC, et al. Education plays a greater role than age in cognitive test performance among participants of the Brazilian Longitudinal Study of Adult Health (ELSA-Brasil). BMC Neurol. 2015;15(1):191. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-015-0454-6
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1186/...
which is also true for VF tasks.1717. Brucki SM, Rocha MS. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(12):1771-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
PF tasks seem to be particularly more influenced by educational level than SF ones,1717. Brucki SM, Rocha MS. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(12):1771-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
which rely more on verbal ability and semantic memory than PF.1818. Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;14(2):167-77. Hermes-Pereira et al.66. Hermes-Pereira A, Gonçalves AB, Holz M, Gonçalves HA, Kochhann R, Joanette Y, et al. Influence of age and education on the processing of clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2018;12(4):360-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
reported data from healthy individuals on the effect of age and educational levels on semantic, phonemic, and unconstrained VF. Their results also included the analysis of clusters and switches. The authors found that age affects the total switches and total taxonomic clusters for SF, but not for PF. The educational level played an important role on the performance of all VF tasks, influencing ten out of the 25 variables analyzed, such as the number of switches and phonemic clusters for PF, and the number of switches, taxonomic clusters, and mean cluster size for SF. Santos Nogueira et al.1313. Santos Nogueira D, Azevedo Reis E, Vieira A. Verbal fluency tasks: effects of age, gender, and education. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2016;68(3):124-33. https://doi.org/10.1159/000450640
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1159/...
also identified a positive effect of education on VF, suggesting that it compensates for age-related decline.

Educational level might also differ according to the type of school - ten years of formal education in the public system can be qualitatively different from the same length of time in private schools. Thus, our analysis included the measurement of FRWH, which has proven to be a good tool to evaluate the maintenance of cognitive function. For instance, Pawlowski et al.1919. Pawlowski J, Remor E, de Mattos Pimenta Parente MA, de Salles JF, Fonseca RP, Bandeira DR. The influence of reading and writing habits associated with education on the neuropsychological performance of Brazilian adults. Read Writ. 2012;25(9):2275-89. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9357-8
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/...
investigated the relationship between educational level, FRWH, and the performance in linguistic and neuropsychological tasks. Their results associated higher schooling level and higher FRWH better performance of healthy older individuals in neuropsychological tests, including VF. Moreover, their results revealed that FRWH had a stronger influence on a better VF performance than education. Likewise, Cotrena et al.2020. Moraes AL, Guimarães LS, Joanette Y, Parente MA, Fonseca RP, Almeida RM. Efeito da idade, escolaridade, hábitos de leitura e escrita, processamento semântico e sintomas de depressão na fluência verbal. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2013;26(4):680-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722013000400008
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
also highlighted the influence of daily cognitive stimulation, as that provided by FRWH, on executive processing. Conversely, Moraes et al.2121. Cotrena C, Branco LD, Cardoso CO, Wong CEI, Fonseca RP. The predictive impact of biological and sociocultural factors on executive processing: the role of age, education, and frequency of reading and writing habits. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2016;23(2):75-84. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2015.1012760
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
specifically investigated the influence of age, education, and FRWH on semantic, phonemic, and unconstrained VF tasks and found that education was the most influential variable in all VF scores in healthy aging, while FRWH specifically had a greater effect on PF. A recent investigation also found that FRWH predicted the performance of all fluency tasks in a sample similar to the one in this study.2222. Kochhann R, Holz MR, Beber BC, Chaves ML, Fonseca RP. Reading and writing habits as a predictor of verbal fluency in elders. Psychol Neurosci. 2018;11(1):39-49. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000125
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/...

To the best of our knowledge, no research has compared clustering and switching in VF tasks in individuals with cognitive decline and healthy controls who have a low educational levels, correlating the results to FRWH. In addition, VF has been mainly explored considering the total score of valid words. Thus, this study aims at assessing PF and SF in healthy older adults and those with MCI and AD, who have a low educational level, in a sample of Brazilian Portuguese speakers, and correlating their performance to FRWH. We hypothesized that the tasks would differentiate AD, MCI, and control groups in terms of the quantitative (number of words generated in each category) and qualitative measurements (clustering, switching, return, and intersection analyses). Additionally, FWRH would be related to the performance of individuals in all VF tasks.

METHODS

Ethical and data collection procedures

This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, under protocol no. 560.073, as part of a larger project. All participants were asked to sign an Informed Consent Form to authorize data collection. Data were collected individually in a quiet room.

Study sample

This study assessed 67 participants. Convenience samples of patients were recruited in the outpatient department of a hospital that treats older adults with low schooling and low or medium socioeconomic level, while controls were recruited at community centers. Their age varied between 60 and 80 years, with years of schooling ranging from 2 to 8 years. For controls, the inclusion criteria were absence of cognitive decline or dementia, as evaluated by the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), with cut-off points established by Kochhann et al.,2323. Kochhann R, Varela JS, Lisboa CS, Chaves ML. The Mini Mental State Examination: Review of cutoff points adjusted for schooling in a large Southern Brazilian sample. Dement Neuropsychol. 2010;4(1):35-41. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642010DN40100006
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
and socioeconomic classes B2, C1, and C2 (corresponding to middle class), following the guidelines for socioeconomic status assessment by Associação Brasileira de Empresas e Pesquisa (ABEP). The exclusion criteria for clinical and healthy groups were presence of depressive symptoms determined by the Brazilian version of the Geriatric Depression Scale,2424. Almeida OP, Almeida SA. Confiabilidade da versão brasileira da Escala de Depressão em Geriatria (GDS) versão reduzida. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 1999;57(2B):421-6. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X1999000300013
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
and non-corrected visual or hearing problems.

AD diagnostic criteria followed the guidelines of the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS/ADRDA),2525. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack Jr CR, Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):263-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
selecting participants with a Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) of 1.2626. Macedo Montaño MBM, Ramos LR. Validade da versão em português da Clinical Dementia Rating. Rev Saude Publica. 2005;39(6):912-7. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0034-89102005000600007
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
MCI diagnostic guidelines were based on Albert et al.2727. Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):270-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
and selected participants with a CDR of 0.5. The MCI group included the amnestic and multiple domain types. AD and MCI participants underwent a neuropsychological evaluation that consisted of the Brazilian version of the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination-Revised (ACE-R)2828. Carvalho VA, Caramelli P. Brazilian adaptation of the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R). Dement Neuropsychol. 2007;1(2):212-6. https://doi.org/10.1590/s1980-57642008dn10200015
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
and the MMSE,2929. Chaves MLF, Izquierdo I. Differential diagnosis between dementia and depression : a study of efficiency increment. Acta Neurol Scand. 1992;85(6):378-82. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.1992.tb06032.x
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/...
with the cut-off points suggested by Kochhann et al.2222. Kochhann R, Holz MR, Beber BC, Chaves ML, Fonseca RP. Reading and writing habits as a predictor of verbal fluency in elders. Psychol Neurosci. 2018;11(1):39-49. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000125
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/...
Furthermore, participants were administered the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview 6.0 adapted for the Brazilian population to rule out any psychiatric disorders.3030. Azevedo Marques JM, Zuardi AW. Validity and applicability of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview administered by family medicine residents in primary health care in Brazil. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2008;30(4):303-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2008.02.001
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
Participants were also evaluated in terms of their FRWH.1919. Pawlowski J, Remor E, de Mattos Pimenta Parente MA, de Salles JF, Fonseca RP, Bandeira DR. The influence of reading and writing habits associated with education on the neuropsychological performance of Brazilian adults. Read Writ. 2012;25(9):2275-89. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9357-8
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/...
,3030. Azevedo Marques JM, Zuardi AW. Validity and applicability of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview administered by family medicine residents in primary health care in Brazil. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2008;30(4):303-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2008.02.001
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
The FRWH questionnaire assessed the weekly frequency of reading and writing activities with different types of texts, with ratings classified as: daily (4 points); a few days a week (3 points); once a week (2 points); rarely (1 point), and never (0 points) - maximum frequency score of 28 points.1919. Pawlowski J, Remor E, de Mattos Pimenta Parente MA, de Salles JF, Fonseca RP, Bandeira DR. The influence of reading and writing habits associated with education on the neuropsychological performance of Brazilian adults. Read Writ. 2012;25(9):2275-89. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9357-8
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/...
,3131. Holz M, Kochhan R, Cardoso CO, Zimmerman N, Pagliarin KC, Pimenta MA, et al. Frequência de hábitos de leitura e de escrita: normas, aplicação, pontuação e interpretação de uma medida sociocultural-linguística de avaliação neuropsicológica. In: Zimmermann N, Fonseca RP, editors. Avaliação de linguagem e funções executivas em adultos. v.2. 1.ed. São Paulo: MEMNON, 2017. p.161-73.

Participants were divided into three groups: controls=25, MCI=24, and AD=18. One group performed tasks of the “clothes” SF criterion (12 MCI, 9 AD, and 12 controls). Data from the “animal” SF criterion were gathered from 12 MCI, 9 AD, and 13 controls. The PF task was administered with the same phoneme for all 67 participants.

Instruments

SF tasks consisted of generating words for two different categories (animals and clothes) during one minute for each category, while the PF task asked participants to say as many words as possible during one minute using the phoneme /p/.

Clustering and switching analyses

Clustering and switching analyses were conducted according to the methods proposed by Troyer3232. Troyer AK. Normative data for clustering and switching on verbal fluency tasks. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22(3):370-8. https://doi.org/10.1076/1380-3395(200006)22:3;1-v;ft370
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1076/...
and Abwender et al.,3333. Abwender DA, Swan JG, Bowerman JT, Connolly SW. Qualitative analysis of verbal fluency output: Review and comparison of several scoring methods. Assessment. 2001;8(3):323-38. https://doi.org/10.1177/107319110100800308
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1177/...
Gonçalves et al.’s guidelines,3434. Gonçalves HA, Fonseca FP, Cargnin C, Fonseca RP. Análise quantitativa e qualitativa das estratégias de evocação de palavras em tarefas de fluência verbal para crianças. In: Fonseca RP, Prando ML, Zimmermann N, editors. Avaliação de Linguagem e Funções Executivas Em Crianças. São Paulo: Memnon; 2016. and Hermes-Pereira et al.66. Hermes-Pereira A, Gonçalves AB, Holz M, Gonçalves HA, Kochhann R, Joanette Y, et al. Influence of age and education on the processing of clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2018;12(4):360-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
We considered clusters as groups of two or more words belonging to the same category, which could be phonemic, semantic (taxonomic or thematic), or mixed (phonemic and semantic). The criteria for grouping each type of cluster were:

Phonemic clusters: words beginning with the same first two letters or sound, as well as those that either rhyme or are homonyms.

Taxonomic clusters: words belonging to the same semantic subcategory. For the animal category, we followed the taxonomic cluster guidelines proposed by Brucki and Rocha1717. Brucki SM, Rocha MS. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(12):1771-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
(domestic and farm animals, wild animals, birds, fish, and reptiles); for the clothes category, we assessed clusters according to Gonçalves et al.3434. Gonçalves HA, Fonseca FP, Cargnin C, Fonseca RP. Análise quantitativa e qualitativa das estratégias de evocação de palavras em tarefas de fluência verbal para crianças. In: Fonseca RP, Prando ML, Zimmermann N, editors. Avaliação de Linguagem e Funções Executivas Em Crianças. São Paulo: Memnon; 2016. (underwear, beachwear, shoes, accessories, and formal attire).

Thematic clusters: words that are usually associated with a specific event or situation, for example, naming all things starting with the letter P that can be found in a kitchen.

Cluster size was calculated as the number of words produced for the same category minus one. Perseverations and intrusions were considered in this calculation. Switches consisted of the number of transitions between clusters: returns were counted every time the participants returned to a previously employed strategy for forming clusters. Finally, we analyzed intersections, which correspond to the ability to use a word as a trigger to start another cluster when the previously used strategy was no longer productive.

Data analysis

We used the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test to assess data distribution. Performance of the three groups was compared in each category task by ANOVA, with age as a covariate. Bonferroni post-hoc test verified the differences among groups. Data were analyzed regarding the total number of words generated, number of switches and clusters, thematic clusters, category clusters, phonemic clusters, mixed clusters, intersections, and returns.

FRWH was correlated with the total words produced in the PF and the total words in the first 15 s in SF. Since data were not normally distributed, we adopted Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient.

RESULTS

Table 1 describes the sociodemographic data of the sample. We found differences among age groups and FRWH categories. Table 2 presents the comparison of VF task performances among groups. The clothes semantic category and the phonemic VF (except total words) showed no significant statistical difference among all three groups; however, in all variables, the control group had higher mean values than the MCI group, which, in turn, had higher mean values than the AD group (C>MCI>AD). We identified statistical differences distinguishing the three groups in the total words generated in the PF. Regarding the animal category scores, AD participants performed worse than controls in the total number of clusters in the first 15 s, total taxonomic clusters, total switches, and returns. MCI and AD individuals performed worse than controls in the total number of words. We detected a moderate correlation between FRWH and total words in PF (rho=0.42; p<0.01) and a weak correlation between FRWH and total words in the first 15 s in SF (rho=0.34; p<0.01).

Table 1.
Participants’ data on sociodemographic characteristics and reading/writing habits.
Table 2.
Participant’s verbal fluency performance.

DISCUSSION

Due to the small sample analyzed in this study, results must be considered cautiously. Nonetheless, overall, our results indicate that the SF animal category seems to be more sensitive for diagnostic purposes (for low-educated individuals) than the clothes category. Specifically, while we found no differences among groups in the SF clothes category, animal fluency was effective in differentiating AD patients to older adult controls, considering the total number of generated words, total number of clusters in the first 15 s, total number of taxonomic clusters, and number of returns.

Some authors have argued that SF is more sensitive in discriminating dementia patients1010. Radanovic M, Diniz BS, Mirandez RM, Novaretti TM, Flacks MK, Yassuda MS, et al. Verbal fluency in the detection of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease among Brazilian Portuguese speakers: the influence of education. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(6):1081-7. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1041610209990639
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
,1111. Pakhomov SVS, Eberly LE, Knopman DS. Recurrent perseverations on semantic verbal fluency tasks as an early marker of cognitive impairment. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2018;40(8):832-40. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2018.1438372
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
compared to PF, possibly because semantic processes are usually more prone to degradation in dementia than phonemic ones, which might take longer to show decline. Additionally, SF is more dependent on lexico-semantic processing, which seems to be impaired in AD from the onset.11. Clark LJ, Gatz M, Zheng L, Chen YL, McCleary C, Mack WJ. Longitudinal verbal fluency in normal aging, preclinical, and prevalent Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2009;24(6):461-8. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317509345154
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1177/...

Corroborating the findings of Troyer et al.,55. Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Leach L, Freedman M. Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998;4(2):137-43. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798001374
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
our results indicate that the total number of words generated was not as sensitive in differentiating dementia groups as the cluster analysis. Importantly, the total number of words is a good indicator of cognitive impairment, as both MCI and AD differed from controls. Also, Herrera-García3535. Herrera-García JD, Rego-García I, Guillén-Martínez V, Carrasco-García M, Valderrama-Martín C, Vílchez-Carrillo R, et al. Discriminative validity of an abbreviated Semantic Verbal Fluency Test. Dement Neuropsychol. 2019;13(2):203-9. https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn13-020009
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
and Troyer et al.55. Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Leach L, Freedman M. Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998;4(2):137-43. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798001374
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
showed the advantage of clustering and switching analysis for the differential diagnosis of cognitively impaired individuals. Wajman et al.22. Wajman JR, Cecchini MA, Bertolucci PH, Mansur LL. Quanti-qualitative components of the semantic verbal fluency test in cognitively healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia subtypes. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(6):533-42. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
found no differences in the average number of words produced in SF for MCI, AD, Lewy body dementia, and the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. However, when assessing the switching component, they could identify controls among dementia subtypes, excluding MCI, which is, in fact, the most difficult to discriminate from controls. Moreover, the mean cluster size evaluation allowed them to differentiate MCI from controls. Our results regarding the switching component contrast with those of previous studies,22. Wajman JR, Cecchini MA, Bertolucci PH, Mansur LL. Quanti-qualitative components of the semantic verbal fluency test in cognitively healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia subtypes. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(6):533-42. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
,55. Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Leach L, Freedman M. Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998;4(2):137-43. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798001374
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
,3535. Herrera-García JD, Rego-García I, Guillén-Martínez V, Carrasco-García M, Valderrama-Martín C, Vílchez-Carrillo R, et al. Discriminative validity of an abbreviated Semantic Verbal Fluency Test. Dement Neuropsychol. 2019;13(2):203-9. https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn13-020009
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
since we found an indication of a significant difference that was ruled out after a post-hoc analysis.

Herrera-García et al.3535. Herrera-García JD, Rego-García I, Guillén-Martínez V, Carrasco-García M, Valderrama-Martín C, Vílchez-Carrillo R, et al. Discriminative validity of an abbreviated Semantic Verbal Fluency Test. Dement Neuropsychol. 2019;13(2):203-9. https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn13-020009
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
reported that the assessment of words generated in the first 30 seconds of SF for animals might be enough to diagnose subjective cognitive impairment. Their findings show that individuals with subjective cognitive impairment might present an increased number of switches and smaller cluster sizes for the animal category. Differently from our results, they did not find differences in PF indices among cognitively impaired participants and controls. Also, Troyer et al.55. Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Leach L, Freedman M. Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998;4(2):137-43. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798001374
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
identified no differences in the number of switches in the phonemic task between AD and controls, although the number of words (similar to our study) and the cluster size differed among these groups. For the SF, our findings corroborate those of Troyer et al.55. Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Leach L, Freedman M. Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998;4(2):137-43. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798001374
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
and Wajman et al.:22. Wajman JR, Cecchini MA, Bertolucci PH, Mansur LL. Quanti-qualitative components of the semantic verbal fluency test in cognitively healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia subtypes. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(6):533-42. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
smaller cluster sizes - albeit not statistically significant in our study - and total number of words for AD, as well as less frequent switches. The switching component is related to the frontal lobe functioning, which is not the first degraded area for dementia of the Alzheimer’s type, which primarily damages the temporal lobes. Thus, we hypothesize that, since our patients are still on the onset of dementia, they might not present a significant executive impairment, as shown in the switching analysis.

The semantic categories chosen for this study are representative of living (animals) and non-living (clothes) things, providing some new evidence to the debate over which category is best preserved and which is most compromised. Our study corroborates the main trend of results,3636. Duong A, Whitehead V, Hanratty K, Chertkow H. The nature of lexico-semantic processing deficits in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(10):1928-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.034
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
,3737. Taler V, Voronchikhina A, Gorfine G, Lukasik M. Knowledge of semantic features in mild cognitive impairment. J Neurolinguistics. 2016;38:56-70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.11.002
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
,3838. Zannino GD, Perri R, Pasqualetti P, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo G a. (Category-specific) semantic deficit in Alzheimer’s patients: the role of semantic distance. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(1):52-61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.008
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
,3939. Tyler L, Moss H. Towards a distributed account of conceptual knowledge. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5(6):244-52. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01651-x
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
,4040. Devlin JT, Gonnerman LM, Andersen ES, Seidenberg MS. Category-specific semantic deficits in focal and widespread brain damage: a computational account. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998;10(1):77-94. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998563798
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1162/...
indicating that living categories are more impaired and sensitive in differentiating groups compared to non-living categories. Also, when we look at the raw scores of both categories, we can see the consistent decline from healthy aging to MCI to AD.

Besides analyzing VF among cognitively impaired groups and controls, we assessed the relationship between FRWH and VF ability. Previously, Wajman et al.22. Wajman JR, Cecchini MA, Bertolucci PH, Mansur LL. Quanti-qualitative components of the semantic verbal fluency test in cognitively healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia subtypes. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(6):533-42. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
found a small correlation coefficient between schooling and the number of words generated in SF tasks and the number of clusters produced. Other studies also identified the beneficial effect of education on VF.66. Hermes-Pereira A, Gonçalves AB, Holz M, Gonçalves HA, Kochhann R, Joanette Y, et al. Influence of age and education on the processing of clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2018;12(4):360-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.159...
,1313. Santos Nogueira D, Azevedo Reis E, Vieira A. Verbal fluency tasks: effects of age, gender, and education. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2016;68(3):124-33. https://doi.org/10.1159/000450640
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1159/...
To the best of our knowledge, no previous studies specifically focused on the relationship between FRWH and VF tasks performed by MCI and AD older adults with low educational level. The phonemic component of VF was more related to the FRWH than SF, as reported in previous studies.44. Shao Z, Janse E, Visser K, Meyer AS. What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Front Psychol. 2014;5:772. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3389/...
,1818. Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;14(2):167-77.,2222. Kochhann R, Holz MR, Beber BC, Chaves ML, Fonseca RP. Reading and writing habits as a predictor of verbal fluency in elders. Psychol Neurosci. 2018;11(1):39-49. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000125
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/...
No direct VF comparisons were made due to the different samples performing those tasks. Although not statistically significant, the number of generated words in PF was lower in all groups compared to SF. Bearing in mind that our sample consisted of low-educated participants, we expected that they would generate more words in the semantic task than in the phonemic one.

This study presents some limitations. Firstly, the sample size is reduced, and groups differed in age and FRWH, which might have affected the results, even though our analyses were controlled for age. Further studies should aim to control for FRWH. Secondly, we only used the standard time set for VF tasks - one-minute interval -, while a two-minute protocol might be advisable.4141. Zimmerman N, Branco L, Ska B, Gasparetto EL, Joanette Y, Fonseca R. Verbal fluency in right brain damage: dissociations among production criteria and duration. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2014;21(4):260-8. https://doi.org/10.1080/09084282.2013.802693
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...

Finally, the clinical implications of this study highlight the screening role of VF tasks in differentiating healthy and cognitively impaired (as in MCI and AD) individuals. Moreover, FRWH must be considered when evaluating results together with schooling and more deeply investigated at history taking during clinical and neuropsychological assessments for dementia diagnosis and prognosis, taking into account the cognitive reserve factor, and for more specific and accurate intervention planning. This study also showed the importance and sensitivity of clustering, switching, and return analyses for diagnostic purposes. Even with a rather small sample, we hope to have contributed to a more detailed VF analysis, considering the sociodemographic variables of the participants.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Clark LJ, Gatz M, Zheng L, Chen YL, McCleary C, Mack WJ. Longitudinal verbal fluency in normal aging, preclinical, and prevalent Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2009;24(6):461-8. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317509345154
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317509345154
  • 2
    Wajman JR, Cecchini MA, Bertolucci PH, Mansur LL. Quanti-qualitative components of the semantic verbal fluency test in cognitively healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia subtypes. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2019;26(6):533-42. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2018.1465426
  • 3
    Adlam A-L, Patterson K, Rogers TT, Nestor PJ, Salmond CH, Acosta-Cabronero J, et al. Semantic dementia and fluent primary progressive aphasia: two sides of the same coin? Brain. 2006;129(Pt 11):3066-80. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl285
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awl285
  • 4
    Shao Z, Janse E, Visser K, Meyer AS. What do verbal fluency tasks measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Front Psychol. 2014;5:772. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772
  • 5
    Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G, Leach L, Freedman M. Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998;4(2):137-43. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798001374
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798001374
  • 6
    Hermes-Pereira A, Gonçalves AB, Holz M, Gonçalves HA, Kochhann R, Joanette Y, et al. Influence of age and education on the processing of clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Dement Neuropsychol. 2018;12(4):360-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004
    » https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn12-040004
  • 7
    Henry JD, Crawford JR, Phillips LH. Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42(9):1212-22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001
  • 8
    Caramelli P, Carthery-Goulart MT, Porto CS, Charchat-Fichman H, Nitrini R. Category fluency as a screening test for Alzheimer disease in illiterate and literate patients. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2007;21(1):65-7. https://doi.org/10.1097/wad.0b013e31802f244f
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1097/wad.0b013e31802f244f
  • 9
    Rodríguez-Aranda C, Waterloo K, Johnsen SH, Eldevik P, Sparr S, Wikran GC, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of verbal fluency in early Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging. Brain Lang. 2016;155-156:24-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.03.001
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2016.03.001
  • 10
    Radanovic M, Diniz BS, Mirandez RM, Novaretti TM, Flacks MK, Yassuda MS, et al. Verbal fluency in the detection of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease among Brazilian Portuguese speakers: the influence of education. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(6):1081-7. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1041610209990639
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/s1041610209990639
  • 11
    Pakhomov SVS, Eberly LE, Knopman DS. Recurrent perseverations on semantic verbal fluency tasks as an early marker of cognitive impairment. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2018;40(8):832-40. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2018.1438372
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2018.1438372
  • 12
    Capitani E, Barbarotto R, Laiacona M. Does education influence the age‐related cognitive decline? A further inquiry. Dev Neuropsychol. 1996;12(2):231-40. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649609540648
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649609540648
  • 13
    Santos Nogueira D, Azevedo Reis E, Vieira A. Verbal fluency tasks: effects of age, gender, and education. Folia Phoniatr Logop. 2016;68(3):124-33. https://doi.org/10.1159/000450640
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1159/000450640
  • 14
    Parente MA, Scherer LC, Zimmermann N, Fonseca R. Evidências do papel da escolaridade na organização cerebral. Neuropsicol Lat Am. 2009;1(1):72-80.
  • 15
    Parente MA, Fonseca RP, Scherer LC. Literacy as a determining factor for brain organization: from Lecours’ contribution to the present day. Dement Neuropsychol. 2008;2(3):164. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN20300002
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642009DN20300002
  • 16
    Passos VM, Giatti L, Bensenor I, Tiemeier H, Ikram MA, Figueiredo RC, et al. Education plays a greater role than age in cognitive test performance among participants of the Brazilian Longitudinal Study of Adult Health (ELSA-Brasil). BMC Neurol. 2015;15(1):191. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-015-0454-6
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-015-0454-6
  • 17
    Brucki SM, Rocha MS. Category fluency test: effects of age, gender and education on total scores, clustering and switching in Brazilian Portuguese-speaking subjects. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(12):1771-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002
    » https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0100-879X2004001200002
  • 18
    Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1999;14(2):167-77.
  • 19
    Pawlowski J, Remor E, de Mattos Pimenta Parente MA, de Salles JF, Fonseca RP, Bandeira DR. The influence of reading and writing habits associated with education on the neuropsychological performance of Brazilian adults. Read Writ. 2012;25(9):2275-89. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9357-8
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9357-8
  • 20
    Moraes AL, Guimarães LS, Joanette Y, Parente MA, Fonseca RP, Almeida RM. Efeito da idade, escolaridade, hábitos de leitura e escrita, processamento semântico e sintomas de depressão na fluência verbal. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2013;26(4):680-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722013000400008
    » https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0102-79722013000400008
  • 21
    Cotrena C, Branco LD, Cardoso CO, Wong CEI, Fonseca RP. The predictive impact of biological and sociocultural factors on executive processing: the role of age, education, and frequency of reading and writing habits. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2016;23(2):75-84. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2015.1012760
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2015.1012760
  • 22
    Kochhann R, Holz MR, Beber BC, Chaves ML, Fonseca RP. Reading and writing habits as a predictor of verbal fluency in elders. Psychol Neurosci. 2018;11(1):39-49. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000125
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000125
  • 23
    Kochhann R, Varela JS, Lisboa CS, Chaves ML. The Mini Mental State Examination: Review of cutoff points adjusted for schooling in a large Southern Brazilian sample. Dement Neuropsychol. 2010;4(1):35-41. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642010DN40100006
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642010DN40100006
  • 24
    Almeida OP, Almeida SA. Confiabilidade da versão brasileira da Escala de Depressão em Geriatria (GDS) versão reduzida. Arq Neuro-Psiquiatr. 1999;57(2B):421-6. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X1999000300013
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X1999000300013
  • 25
    McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack Jr CR, Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):263-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005
  • 26
    Macedo Montaño MBM, Ramos LR. Validade da versão em português da Clinical Dementia Rating. Rev Saude Publica. 2005;39(6):912-7. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0034-89102005000600007
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/S0034-89102005000600007
  • 27
    Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):270-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008
  • 28
    Carvalho VA, Caramelli P. Brazilian adaptation of the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R). Dement Neuropsychol. 2007;1(2):212-6. https://doi.org/10.1590/s1980-57642008dn10200015
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/s1980-57642008dn10200015
  • 29
    Chaves MLF, Izquierdo I. Differential diagnosis between dementia and depression : a study of efficiency increment. Acta Neurol Scand. 1992;85(6):378-82. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.1992.tb06032.x
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.1992.tb06032.x
  • 30
    Azevedo Marques JM, Zuardi AW. Validity and applicability of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview administered by family medicine residents in primary health care in Brazil. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2008;30(4):303-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2008.02.001
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2008.02.001
  • 31
    Holz M, Kochhan R, Cardoso CO, Zimmerman N, Pagliarin KC, Pimenta MA, et al. Frequência de hábitos de leitura e de escrita: normas, aplicação, pontuação e interpretação de uma medida sociocultural-linguística de avaliação neuropsicológica. In: Zimmermann N, Fonseca RP, editors. Avaliação de linguagem e funções executivas em adultos. v.2. 1.ed. São Paulo: MEMNON, 2017. p.161-73.
  • 32
    Troyer AK. Normative data for clustering and switching on verbal fluency tasks. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22(3):370-8. https://doi.org/10.1076/1380-3395(200006)22:3;1-v;ft370
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1076/1380-3395(200006)22:3;1-v;ft370
  • 33
    Abwender DA, Swan JG, Bowerman JT, Connolly SW. Qualitative analysis of verbal fluency output: Review and comparison of several scoring methods. Assessment. 2001;8(3):323-38. https://doi.org/10.1177/107319110100800308
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1177/107319110100800308
  • 34
    Gonçalves HA, Fonseca FP, Cargnin C, Fonseca RP. Análise quantitativa e qualitativa das estratégias de evocação de palavras em tarefas de fluência verbal para crianças. In: Fonseca RP, Prando ML, Zimmermann N, editors. Avaliação de Linguagem e Funções Executivas Em Crianças. São Paulo: Memnon; 2016.
  • 35
    Herrera-García JD, Rego-García I, Guillén-Martínez V, Carrasco-García M, Valderrama-Martín C, Vílchez-Carrillo R, et al. Discriminative validity of an abbreviated Semantic Verbal Fluency Test. Dement Neuropsychol. 2019;13(2):203-9. https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn13-020009
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-57642018dn13-020009
  • 36
    Duong A, Whitehead V, Hanratty K, Chertkow H. The nature of lexico-semantic processing deficits in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(10):1928-35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.034
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.034
  • 37
    Taler V, Voronchikhina A, Gorfine G, Lukasik M. Knowledge of semantic features in mild cognitive impairment. J Neurolinguistics. 2016;38:56-70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.11.002
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.11.002
  • 38
    Zannino GD, Perri R, Pasqualetti P, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo G a. (Category-specific) semantic deficit in Alzheimer’s patients: the role of semantic distance. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(1):52-61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.008
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.008
  • 39
    Tyler L, Moss H. Towards a distributed account of conceptual knowledge. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5(6):244-52. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01651-x
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01651-x
  • 40
    Devlin JT, Gonnerman LM, Andersen ES, Seidenberg MS. Category-specific semantic deficits in focal and widespread brain damage: a computational account. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998;10(1):77-94. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998563798
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998563798
  • 41
    Zimmerman N, Branco L, Ska B, Gasparetto EL, Joanette Y, Fonseca R. Verbal fluency in right brain damage: dissociations among production criteria and duration. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2014;21(4):260-8. https://doi.org/10.1080/09084282.2013.802693
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/09084282.2013.802693
  • 1
    This study was conducted at the Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
  • Funding: The first author acknowledges the Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq) for their financial support through a master’s scholarship. This study was financially supported by CNPq Universal, grant no. 479989/2013-3, and the Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio Grande do Sul (FAPERGS) Program for Internationalization of Graduate Education, grant no. 0429-2551/14-5.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    18 Sept 2020
  • Date of issue
    Jul-Sep 2020

History

  • Received
    12 Oct 2019
  • Accepted
    14 May 2020
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia, Departamento de Neurologia Cognitiva e Envelhecimento R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices, Torre Norte, São Paulo, SP, Brazil, CEP 04101-000, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistadementia@abneuro.org.br | demneuropsy@uol.com.br