Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Evaluation of sensory discomfort caused by stimulation with virtual reality in volunteers with and without kinetosis

ABSCTRACT

Purpose

to compare the symptoms of motion sickness caused by virtual reality stimulation in volunteers with and without history of the disease.

Methods

qualitative and analytical, observational, cross-sectional, and prospective study, approved by Research Ethics Committee, 3.443.429/19, with volunteers with and without history of motion sickness who were subjected to immersion in VR with the use of virtual reality glasses. Before and after sensory stimulation, the participant had respiratory rate (RF), heart rate (HR) and systolic blood pressure (PAs) and diastolic blood pressure (PAd) measured. On the first day, the volunteer was exposed to a video that simulated a person in a car, with a predominance of lateral visual flow. A week later, an animation of a roller coaster, with a predominance of frontal visual flow. During the 10-minute experiment, a score from 0 to 10 was given every 30 seconds for the intensity of the discomfort felt by the volunteer. A post-questionnaire was conducted to assess motion sickness symptoms.

Results

individuals with motion sickness history had a higher intensity of symptoms in the car (p = 0.026) and roller coaster experiment (p = 0.035). There was no correlation between motion sickness and the variables HR,FR,PA. Patients with motion sickness gave higher scores of discomfort throughout the experiments, mainly in the roller coaster experience.

Conclusion

individuals with motion sickness present more intense symptoms when subjected to stimuli by VR compared to controls without disease.

Keywords:
Motion sickness; Virtual reality; Nausea; Dizziness; Patient health questionnaire

RESUMO

Objetivo

comparar os sintomas da cinetose provocados por estímulo de realidade virtual, em voluntários com e sem histórico da doença.

Métodos

estudo analítico qualitativo e quantitativo, observacional transversal, prospectivo, realizado com voluntários com e sem histórico de cinetose, submetidos à imersão em realidade virtual com o uso de óculos de realidade aumentada. Antes e após a estimulação sensorial, o participante tinha a frequência respiratória (FR), a frequência cardíaca (FC) e pressão arterial sistólica (PAs) e diastólica (PAd) medidas. No primeiro dia, o voluntário foi exposto a um vídeo que simulava uma pessoa dentro de um carro, com predomínio de fluxo visual lateral. Após uma semana, uma animação de montanha russa, com predomínio de fluxo visual frontal. Durante a estimulação sensorial de dez minutos, uma nota de 0 a 10 era dada a cada 30 segundos para a intensidade do desconforto sentido pelo participante. Após, um questionário foi realizado para avaliação dos sintomas de cinetose.

Resultados

indivíduos com cinetose apresentaram maior intensidade de sintomas, tanto no experimento do carro (p=0,026), como na montanha russa (p=0,035). Não houve correlação entre cinetose e as variáveis FC, FR e PA. Os pacientes com cinetose atribuíram maiores notas de desconforto no curso das experiências, principalmente na experiência da montanha russa.

Conclusão

indivíduos com cinetose apresentam sintomas mais intensos quando submetidos a estímulos por realidade virtual, se comparados a indivíduos sem a doença.

Palavras-chave:
Enjoo devido ao movimento; Realidade virtual; Náusea; Tontura; Questionário de saúde do paciente

INTRODUCTION

Motion sickness is a complex syndrome, characterized by the feeling of queasiness and nausea caused by sensory conflict(11 Cha YH, Golding JF, Keshavarz B, Furman J, Kim JS, Lopez-Escamez JA, et al. Motion sickness diagnostic criteria: Consensus Document of the Classification Committee of the Bárány Society. J Vestib Res. 2021;31(5):327-44. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200005. PMid:33646187.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200005...

2 Leung AK, Hon KL. Motion sickness: an overview. Drugs Context. 2019 Dec 13;8:2019-9-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.7573/dic.2019-9-4. PMid:32158479.
http://dx.doi.org/10.7573/dic.2019-9-4...

3 Gupta AK, Kumar BV, Rajguru R, Parate KD. Assessment of sea sickness in naval personnel: incidence and management. Indian J Occup Environ Med. 2021 Abr-Jun;25(2):119-24. PMid:34421250.
-44 Koch A, Cascorbi I, Westhofen M, Dafotakis M, Klapa S, Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP. The neurophysiology and treatment of motion sickness. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2018 Out 12;115(41):687-96. http://dx.doi.org/10.3238/arztebl.2018.0687. PMid:30406755.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3238/arztebl.2018.0...
). Other symptoms may be involved, such as pallor, sweating, stomach pain, drowsiness, and headache(11 Cha YH, Golding JF, Keshavarz B, Furman J, Kim JS, Lopez-Escamez JA, et al. Motion sickness diagnostic criteria: Consensus Document of the Classification Committee of the Bárány Society. J Vestib Res. 2021;31(5):327-44. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200005. PMid:33646187.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200005...
,55 Nooij SAE, Bockisch CJ, Bülthoff HH, Straumann D. Beyond sensory conflict: the role of beliefs and perception in motion sickness. PLoS One. 2021 Jan 19;16(1):e0245295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245295. PMid:33465124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0...
).

Despite its highly prevalence in the population, with a greater predominance in women(11 Cha YH, Golding JF, Keshavarz B, Furman J, Kim JS, Lopez-Escamez JA, et al. Motion sickness diagnostic criteria: Consensus Document of the Classification Committee of the Bárány Society. J Vestib Res. 2021;31(5):327-44. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200005. PMid:33646187.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200005...
,66 Klosterhalfen S, Kellermann S, Pan F, Stockhorst U, Hall G, Enck P. Effects of ethnicity and gender on motion sickness susceptibility. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2005;76(11):1051-7. PMid:16313141.), the neural mechanisms responsible for motion sickness are still not fully understood(55 Nooij SAE, Bockisch CJ, Bülthoff HH, Straumann D. Beyond sensory conflict: the role of beliefs and perception in motion sickness. PLoS One. 2021 Jan 19;16(1):e0245295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245295. PMid:33465124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0...

6 Klosterhalfen S, Kellermann S, Pan F, Stockhorst U, Hall G, Enck P. Effects of ethnicity and gender on motion sickness susceptibility. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2005;76(11):1051-7. PMid:16313141.
-77 Stanney K, Fidopiastis C, Foster L. Virtual reality is sexist: but it does not have to be. Front Robot AI. 2020 Jan 31;7:4. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2020.00004. PMid:33501173.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2020.000...
).

Motion sickness arises from an incongruity between the stimuli from the vestibular, visual, and proprioceptive systems in the face of an unusual movement of the body, or a distorted spatial perception, which generates a conflict sensory input in the brain(88 Li CC, Zhang ZR, Liu YH, Zhang T, Zhang XT, Wang H, et al. Multi-dimensional and objective assessment of motion sickness susceptibility based on machine learning. Front Neurol. 2022 Abr 1;13:824670. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2022.824670. PMid:35432161.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2022.824...
,99 Nürnberger M, Klingner C, Witte OW, Brodoehl S. Mismatch of visual-vestibular information in virtual reality: is motion sickness part of the brains attempt to reduce the prediction error? Front Hum Neurosci. 2021 Out 29;15:757735. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757735. PMid:34776909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757...
).

The increase of motion sickness symptoms coincides with the modern world and the technological revolution. In addition to perceiving its occurrence in everyday environments, such as in means of transport, motion sickness also occurs in visual and bodily immersion in simulators and virtual reality experiences(99 Nürnberger M, Klingner C, Witte OW, Brodoehl S. Mismatch of visual-vestibular information in virtual reality: is motion sickness part of the brains attempt to reduce the prediction error? Front Hum Neurosci. 2021 Out 29;15:757735. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757735. PMid:34776909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757...

10 Gavgani AM, Walker FR, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. A comparative study of cybersickness during exposure to virtual reality and “classic” motion sickness: are they different? J Appl Physiol. 2018;125(6):1670-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00338.2018. PMid:30284516.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.0...

11 Gavgani AM, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. Effects of visual flow direction on signs and symptoms of cybersickness. PLoS One. 2017;12(8):e0182790. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182790. PMid:28777827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0...

12 Lim HK, Ji K, Woo YS, Han DU, Lee DH, Nam SG, et al. Test-retest reliability of the virtual reality sickness evaluation using electroencephalography (EEG). Neurosci Lett. 2021 Jan 19;743:135589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135589. PMid:33359731.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020....

13 Gavgani AM, Nesbitt KV, Blackmore KL, Nalivaiko E. Profiling subjective symptoms and autonomic changes associated with cybersickness. Auton Neurosci. 2017;203:41-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016.12.004. PMid:28010995.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016....
-1414 Mittelstaedt JM. Individual predictors of the susceptibility for motion-related sickness: a systematic review. J Vestib Res. 2020;30(3):165-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200702. PMid:32623410.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200702...
).

With the advancement of technology and the growing market for virtual reality head-mounted displays (HMD), motion sickness has become an important complaint in a considerable part of its users, so much so that motion sickness experienced during or after immersion in virtual environments has its specific term in English, cybersickness or virtual reality sickness(99 Nürnberger M, Klingner C, Witte OW, Brodoehl S. Mismatch of visual-vestibular information in virtual reality: is motion sickness part of the brains attempt to reduce the prediction error? Front Hum Neurosci. 2021 Out 29;15:757735. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757735. PMid:34776909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757...

10 Gavgani AM, Walker FR, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. A comparative study of cybersickness during exposure to virtual reality and “classic” motion sickness: are they different? J Appl Physiol. 2018;125(6):1670-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00338.2018. PMid:30284516.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.0...

11 Gavgani AM, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. Effects of visual flow direction on signs and symptoms of cybersickness. PLoS One. 2017;12(8):e0182790. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182790. PMid:28777827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0...

12 Lim HK, Ji K, Woo YS, Han DU, Lee DH, Nam SG, et al. Test-retest reliability of the virtual reality sickness evaluation using electroencephalography (EEG). Neurosci Lett. 2021 Jan 19;743:135589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135589. PMid:33359731.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020....

13 Gavgani AM, Nesbitt KV, Blackmore KL, Nalivaiko E. Profiling subjective symptoms and autonomic changes associated with cybersickness. Auton Neurosci. 2017;203:41-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016.12.004. PMid:28010995.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016....
-1414 Mittelstaedt JM. Individual predictors of the susceptibility for motion-related sickness: a systematic review. J Vestib Res. 2020;30(3):165-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200702. PMid:32623410.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200702...
).

The virtual experience has become popular not only in the entertainment world, but also in the area of medical care, specifically in therapeutic practices for various clinical conditions prevalent in the population, such as alcoholism and smoking(1515 Metcalf M, Rossie K, Stokes K, Tallman C, Tanner B. Virtual reality cue refusal video game for alcohol and cigarette recovery support: summative study. JMIR Serious Games. 2018;6(2):e7. http://dx.doi.org/10.2196/games.9231. PMid:29661748.
http://dx.doi.org/10.2196/games.9231...
).

The motion sickness felt by volunteers is one of the obstacles to immersion in virtual environments and, therefore, understanding the conditions that predispose anyone to develop such symptoms is essential for the delivery of good virtual reality services in society(99 Nürnberger M, Klingner C, Witte OW, Brodoehl S. Mismatch of visual-vestibular information in virtual reality: is motion sickness part of the brains attempt to reduce the prediction error? Front Hum Neurosci. 2021 Out 29;15:757735. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757735. PMid:34776909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757...

10 Gavgani AM, Walker FR, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. A comparative study of cybersickness during exposure to virtual reality and “classic” motion sickness: are they different? J Appl Physiol. 2018;125(6):1670-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00338.2018. PMid:30284516.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.0...

11 Gavgani AM, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. Effects of visual flow direction on signs and symptoms of cybersickness. PLoS One. 2017;12(8):e0182790. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182790. PMid:28777827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0...

12 Lim HK, Ji K, Woo YS, Han DU, Lee DH, Nam SG, et al. Test-retest reliability of the virtual reality sickness evaluation using electroencephalography (EEG). Neurosci Lett. 2021 Jan 19;743:135589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135589. PMid:33359731.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020....

13 Gavgani AM, Nesbitt KV, Blackmore KL, Nalivaiko E. Profiling subjective symptoms and autonomic changes associated with cybersickness. Auton Neurosci. 2017;203:41-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016.12.004. PMid:28010995.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016....
-1414 Mittelstaedt JM. Individual predictors of the susceptibility for motion-related sickness: a systematic review. J Vestib Res. 2020;30(3):165-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200702. PMid:32623410.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200702...
).

For a better study of motion sickness, as well as to evaluate the possibilities of therapies and drug treatments, some motion sickness provocation models were used(1616 Sevinc V, Berkman MI. Psychometric evaluation of simulator sickness questionnaire and its variants as a measure of cybersickness in consumer virtual environments. Appl Ergon. 2020 Jan;82:102958. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2019.102958. PMid:31563798.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2019....

17 Felipe L, Staggs A, Hunnicutt S. Can type of dizziness influence the vestibular caloric test result? J Prim Care Community Health. 2021 Jan-Dez;12:21501327211030120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/21501327211030120. PMid:34720013.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/21501327211030...

18 Gimmon Y, Schubert MC. Vestibular testing-rotary chair and dynamic visual acuity tests. Adv Otorhinolaryngol. 2019;82:39-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000490270. PMid:30947187.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000490270...
-1919 Janky KL, Patterson J. The relationship between rotary chair and video head impulse testing in children and young adults with cochlear implants. Am J Audiol. 2020 Dez 9;29(4):898-906. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2020_AJA-20-00079. PMid:33201726.). One of the first models was labyrinthine stimulation with air and water, which attempts to simulate labyrinthine functioning, although the endolymphatic current velocity achieved in the caloric test was very low and nonphysiological(1717 Felipe L, Staggs A, Hunnicutt S. Can type of dizziness influence the vestibular caloric test result? J Prim Care Community Health. 2021 Jan-Dez;12:21501327211030120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/21501327211030120. PMid:34720013.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/21501327211030...
).

In an attempt to simulate more intense movements, some studies used the swivel chair, initially without strict control of speed and acceleration, which complicated the analyze of the results, especially in case-control studies, which required to determine the effect of an intervention. With the emergence of computerized swivel chairs, an attempt was made to meet this need, but the high cost of the device made its popularization difficult(1818 Gimmon Y, Schubert MC. Vestibular testing-rotary chair and dynamic visual acuity tests. Adv Otorhinolaryngol. 2019;82:39-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000490270. PMid:30947187.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000490270...
,1919 Janky KL, Patterson J. The relationship between rotary chair and video head impulse testing in children and young adults with cochlear implants. Am J Audiol. 2020 Dez 9;29(4):898-906. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2020_AJA-20-00079. PMid:33201726.).

If the effectiveness of the virtual experience in developing symptoms in volunteers with motion sickness is proven, virtual reality could serve as a model for provoking motion sickness symptoms, in order to be used in studies that study the disease, in its drug treatments and rehabilitation proposals.

This study aimed to compare the symptoms of motion sickness provoked by the virtual reality stimulus in volunteers with and without a history of the disease. Furthermore, we intended to verify the influence of stimuli with different predominance of visual flow (lateral and frontal) in the generation of symptoms.

METHODS

The experiment

Analytical qualitative and quantitative, observational, cross-sectional, prospective study, carried out with volunteers after approval by the Ethics Committee for Research on Human Beings of Santa Casa de São Paulo -CEP-SC, under protocol number 3,443,429, in 2019.

Volunteers over 18 years of age, with or without a history of motion sickness, were included. The sample was for convenience and students and professors from a Health Sciences University were selected between 2019 and 2020.

After signing the Informed Consent Form (ICF), the individuals were clinically evaluated by an otorhinolaryngologist and divided into two groups: control group (group without motion sickness), with no history of discomfort or discomfort due to movement; and the study group (group with motion sickness), which showed history and symptoms of discomfort or malaise due to movement. Volunteers were matched by age and gender.

Volunteers with a history of psychiatric, neurological, and other vestibular diseases (excluding motion sickness) were excluded; history of otologic surgeries; visual difficulties that interfere with immersion in virtual reality; musculoskeletal alterations that could harm balance and the end of the experiment.

In the initial assessment, the volunteers answered the question: “Do you consider yourself a person with motion sickness, or do you feel sick or dizzy when you are in means of transport?.” The answer could range from “no;” “a few times;” “sometimes;” “most of the time;” “nausea or dizziness doing specific activities, such as reading, using a cell phone, when in a means of transport, such as a car, bus.” Volunteers who answered “sometimes,” “most of the time” and “doing specific activities such as reading, using cell phones” were considered to have motion sickness.

The volunteers were submitted to the questionnaire translated and culturally adapted to Brazilian Portuguese by França & Branco-Barreiro(2020 França SR, Branco-Barreiro FCA. Susceptibilidade à cinetose no idoso com doença vestibular. RECES. 2013;5(1):30-5.) of the Motion Sickness Susceptibility Questionnaire - Short Form (MSSQ-Short), a simplification of the questionnaire originally created by Reason & Brand(2121 Reason JT, Brand JJ. Motion sickness. Londres: Academic Press; 1975.). (Annex 1).

The MSSQ questionnaire assesses, in a scoring system, the situations responsible for causing motion sickness and considered nine different types of transportation and entertainment that the participant had used (1) in childhood, prior 12 years of age (Motion Sickness A - MSA), and (2) in the current age, in the last ten years (Motion Sickness B - MSB). For each means of transport or entertainment, the person answered between: “I have never experienced it;” “I never felt sick;” “I rarely felt sick;” “Sometimes I felt sick,” or “I was always felt sick.” Each answer received a score, respectively: 0, 1, 2 and 3. The formula used to calculate the score for these questionnaires is described as:

M S A = c h i l d h o o d t o t a l s c o r e × 9 / 9 n u m b e r o f u n u s e d m e a n s o f t r a n s p o r t (1)
M S B = a d u l t t o t a l s c o r e × 9 / 9 n u m b e r o f u n u s e d m e a n s o f t r a n s p o r t (2)

Soon after completing the questionnaire, respiratory rate (RR), heart rate (HR) and systolic (SBP) and diastolic (DBP) blood pressure were measured using the OMRON HEM-7113 digital sphygmomanometer. RR up to 25 mmHg(2222 Neumar RW, Shuster M, Callaway CW, Gent LM, Atkins DL, Bhanji F, et al. Part 1: executive summary: 2015 american heart association guidelines update for cardiopulmonary resuscitation and emergency cardiovascular care. Circulation. 2015 Nov 3;132(18, Suppl 2):S315-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/CIR.0000000000000252. PMid:26472989.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/CIR.0000000000...
), HR up to 75 bpm, BP < 130 mmHg and BP < 85 mmHg were considered normal values(2323 Kohlmann O Jr, Costa Guimarães A, Carvalho MHC, Chaves HC Jr, Machado CA, Praxedes JN, et al. III Consenso Brasileiro de Hipertensão Arterial. Arq Bras Endocrinol Metabol. 1999;43(4):257-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-27301999000400004.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-27301999...
).

The volunteer started the virtual immersion in orthostatic position, and was exposed to a 10 minutes recorded video, through the HMD VR BOX virtual glasses, that simulated a person inside a car overlooking the streets of São Paulo. The video was recorded with an iPhone 8 Plus, edited in the Adobe Premiere Pro CC 2017 editing program and converted into a stereoscopic image using the VRPlayer application.

Every 30 seconds, the volunteer quantified the overall discomfort felt, using a increasing scale from 0 to 10, with a score of 10 corresponding to the maximum discomfort. Each individual had their own follow-up table with these notes.

In case of extreme discomfort, the participant had the right to report and stop the experiment if necessary. During the entire assessment, an evaluator stood next to the volunteer to prevent falls.

At the end of the experiment, respiratory rate (RR), heart rate (HR), systemic blood pressure (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure (DBP) were measured using the same equipment mentioned above.

The volunteers answered a symptom evaluation questionnaire that contained 16 statements related to the clinical state of motion sickness. The volunteers were asked to assign a score on an increasing scale from 1 to 9 for each symptom identified during the experiment, with 1 being the minimum score and 9 being the maximum intensity score for the determined symptom. The total evaluation of the motion sickness is based on the percentage film calculated from out of 144 possible points(1212 Lim HK, Ji K, Woo YS, Han DU, Lee DH, Nam SG, et al. Test-retest reliability of the virtual reality sickness evaluation using electroencephalography (EEG). Neurosci Lett. 2021 Jan 19;743:135589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135589. PMid:33359731.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020....
).

In the second experiment, at least a week after the first, the participant had their vital signs measured and the whole process was repeated; however, now based on the subject’s exposure to a virtual animation (a roller coaster ride 3D ROLLER COASTER) - TOP15 VR 3D Side By Side SBS Google Cardboard VR Box Gear Oculus Rift), with predominantly frontal visual flow, also lasting ten minutes, with individual assessment every 30 seconds, and completion of the motion sickness symptoms questionnaire at the end.

Statistical analysis

The results were submitted to statistical analysis, in which a significance level of 5% (p = 0.05) and a confidence interval (CI) of 95% were considered. SPSS V13 and Excel Office 2010 software were used. The Shapiro-Wilk test was applied to test the normality of the variables.

The Chi-square test was used to compare the gender variable. MSSQ results and variables (HR, RR, SBP, DBP, motion sickness symptoms assessment questionnaire) were compared using Pearson’s correlation, which was considered weak when from 0 to 0.4; medium, between 0.4 and 0.7 and strong when greater than 0.7.

The difference between the mean scores reported in the follow-up of the experiment of each volunteer in the case group compared to the control group was analyzed using the Mann-Whitney test.

RESULTS

The study consisted of a total sample of 42 volunteers (21 from the control group and 21 from the study group), of which 18 were female (42.85%) and 24 were male (57.15%); the age ranged from 18 to 55 years, with a mean of 25.71 (±10.08).

The MSSQ score was compared with the variables and revealed no correlation between the score of the questionnaire prior the experiment and the variables (HR, RR, SBP, DBP) of the car and roller coaster experiments, showing that all volunteers started with the same pattern in these variables.

The car video stimulation showed a weak correlation between patients with motion sickness (assessment with MSSQ of the last 10 years) and post-experiment symptoms (p=0.026 / r=0.0344).

The roller coaster stimulation revealed weak correlation between patients with motion sickness, according to the MSSQ questionnaires of childhood and the last 10 years, with motion sickness symptoms, respectively p=0.043 / r=0.314 and p=0.035 / r=0.326. (Table 1)

Table 1
Relationship between the presence of motion sickness (according to the Motion Sickness Susceptibility Questionnaire in childhood and the last ten years) and variables: heart rate, respiratory rate, blood pressure and presence of post-exposure symptoms

The association between gender and motion sickness was analyzed. The results showed that gender was not a variable that contributed to the intensification of motion sickness (p=0.582).

There was no difference in the variation of HR, RR, SBP and DBP pre- and post-experiment in any of the evaluated groups (Table 1).

Two volunteers with motion sickness asked to stop the experiment at 7 minutes and 30 seconds in the car video experiment, and one subject in the control group asked to stop the roller coaster experiment at 5 minutes and 30 seconds.

The scores attributed by the volunteers during the follow-up of the experiment showed a significant difference between the groups with and without motion sickness, for both experiments (Figure 1). The Mann-Whitney test showed a significant statistical difference in the following moments: minutes 3’30” (p=0,023), 4’ (p=0,029), 4’30”(p=0,026) e 10’ (p=0,046) in the car experiment, and minutes 4’30” (p=0,032), 5’30” (p=0,020), 6’ (p=0,033), 7’ (p=0,013), 8’ (p=0,042), 8’30” (p=0,024), 9’ (p=0,001), 9’30” (p=0,006), 10’ (p=0,012) in the roller coaster experiment.

Figure 1
Comparative graph between groups with and without motion sickness for grades assigned during exposure to virtual reality. In the upper field, exposure to the car video (side visual predominance) and in the lower field, the roller coaster video (front visual predominance)

DISCUSSION

The correlation between pre- and post-experiment questionnaires was weak, demonstrating that virtual reality provoked a symptomatic reaction in the cases and controls. The difference between the two groups was observed by evaluating the scores given by the volunteers in reference to the intensity of the symptoms during the collection.

No correlation was observed between the increased prevalence or intensity of motion sickness in relation to gender or age, contrary to what some studies suggest, which report that women and younger individuals are more susceptible to this disease(66 Klosterhalfen S, Kellermann S, Pan F, Stockhorst U, Hall G, Enck P. Effects of ethnicity and gender on motion sickness susceptibility. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2005;76(11):1051-7. PMid:16313141.

7 Stanney K, Fidopiastis C, Foster L. Virtual reality is sexist: but it does not have to be. Front Robot AI. 2020 Jan 31;7:4. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2020.00004. PMid:33501173.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2020.000...
-88 Li CC, Zhang ZR, Liu YH, Zhang T, Zhang XT, Wang H, et al. Multi-dimensional and objective assessment of motion sickness susceptibility based on machine learning. Front Neurol. 2022 Abr 1;13:824670. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2022.824670. PMid:35432161.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2022.824...
).

According to other studies in the scientific literature(44 Koch A, Cascorbi I, Westhofen M, Dafotakis M, Klapa S, Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP. The neurophysiology and treatment of motion sickness. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2018 Out 12;115(41):687-96. http://dx.doi.org/10.3238/arztebl.2018.0687. PMid:30406755.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3238/arztebl.2018.0...
,1010 Gavgani AM, Walker FR, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. A comparative study of cybersickness during exposure to virtual reality and “classic” motion sickness: are they different? J Appl Physiol. 2018;125(6):1670-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00338.2018. PMid:30284516.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.0...

11 Gavgani AM, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. Effects of visual flow direction on signs and symptoms of cybersickness. PLoS One. 2017;12(8):e0182790. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182790. PMid:28777827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0...
-1212 Lim HK, Ji K, Woo YS, Han DU, Lee DH, Nam SG, et al. Test-retest reliability of the virtual reality sickness evaluation using electroencephalography (EEG). Neurosci Lett. 2021 Jan 19;743:135589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135589. PMid:33359731.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020....
), motion sickness symptoms are not necessarily related to variations in heart rate, respiratory rate, and blood pressure measurements. Motion sickness has been characterized as a visceral symptom with nausea and, eventually, vomiting.

One of the studies(1313 Gavgani AM, Nesbitt KV, Blackmore KL, Nalivaiko E. Profiling subjective symptoms and autonomic changes associated with cybersickness. Auton Neurosci. 2017;203:41-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016.12.004. PMid:28010995.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016....
) believes that the most reliable parameter to quantify an individual’s motion sickness is the frequency of skin conductance levels in the frontal region of the head. Therefore, the evaluation of post-exposure symptoms, as performed in the present study, becomes the preferred way of analyzing and measuring the effects of stimulation, when evaluating cost, accessibility, and morbidity in the methods.

It is worth remembering that many factors are involved in the onset of symptoms within a virtual environment and that they must be studied further for this observed correlation to be strong. First, one must consider the model of the HMD glasses, which create the environment for the virtual experience. The equipment involves not only lenses, but also a visual field completely sealed to external light and with total immersion for a better experience. In addition, it is necessary to consider variables such as exposure time, image resolution and the intensity of light and ambient sound, which interfere with the volunteers’ perception of symptoms(1010 Gavgani AM, Walker FR, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. A comparative study of cybersickness during exposure to virtual reality and “classic” motion sickness: are they different? J Appl Physiol. 2018;125(6):1670-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00338.2018. PMid:30284516.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.0...
,1111 Gavgani AM, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. Effects of visual flow direction on signs and symptoms of cybersickness. PLoS One. 2017;12(8):e0182790. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182790. PMid:28777827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0...
).

Genetic variants, hormonal profile, and even psychological aspects - such as the individual’s expectation regarding future exposure to any event that may generate motion sickness - can also interfere with the onset of symptoms associated with this condition(55 Nooij SAE, Bockisch CJ, Bülthoff HH, Straumann D. Beyond sensory conflict: the role of beliefs and perception in motion sickness. PLoS One. 2021 Jan 19;16(1):e0245295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245295. PMid:33465124.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0...
,99 Nürnberger M, Klingner C, Witte OW, Brodoehl S. Mismatch of visual-vestibular information in virtual reality: is motion sickness part of the brains attempt to reduce the prediction error? Front Hum Neurosci. 2021 Out 29;15:757735. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757735. PMid:34776909.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757...
). In addition to the concern with age standardization and initial objective evaluation parameters (RR, HR, and BP), virtual reality stimulation allows greater control of the environment, light exposure, and perfect standardization of stimuli, both in time and intensity.

During the experiment, the scores assigned by the volunteers in reference to the intensity of motion sickness symptoms were collected every 30 seconds. The difference between the means of the two groups in the car experiment was relevant at four specific points. This difference increased to nine intervals in the roller coaster experiment, indicating that the difference in the intensity of symptoms felt by the groups with and without motion sickness was greater in the roller coaster experience with frontal visual flow, when compared to the stimulus provoked by the car video, with predominance of lateral visual flow. A study(1111 Gavgani AM, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. Effects of visual flow direction on signs and symptoms of cybersickness. PLoS One. 2017;12(8):e0182790. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182790. PMid:28777827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0...
) has already proven that a roller coaster ride, facing forward, is more stimulating than backward, and therefore, considering the influence of the visual flow in the appearance of more intense symptoms is important.

Still, it must be considered that two volunteers gave up on the car experiment after 7 minutes and 30 seconds, an interval in which the difference in the average scores of both becomes significant, as they are more concentrated in half the time for the car and more towards the end of the roller coaster experiment. Habituation is a phenomenon that occurs in motion sickness, however, the average time in which it occurs within a single experiment is not known(1313 Gavgani AM, Nesbitt KV, Blackmore KL, Nalivaiko E. Profiling subjective symptoms and autonomic changes associated with cybersickness. Auton Neurosci. 2017;203:41-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016.12.004. PMid:28010995.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016....
). However, as pointed out by some authors, the response of an individual to motion sickness is due to three main factors: sensitivity to the stimulus, adaptation constant (which is the rate of adaptation to the stimulus) and the decay constant of symptoms(44 Koch A, Cascorbi I, Westhofen M, Dafotakis M, Klapa S, Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP. The neurophysiology and treatment of motion sickness. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2018 Out 12;115(41):687-96. http://dx.doi.org/10.3238/arztebl.2018.0687. PMid:30406755.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3238/arztebl.2018.0...
).

With this, it is possible to formulate different hypotheses for the car and roller coaster experiment. In the first one, the volunteers must have sensitivity to the stimulus and, thus, present a peak in the intensity of the symptoms in the middle of the path, reaching, therefore, a constant of adaptation and decay of the symptoms and consequent reduction of the discomfort. In the second experiment, the progression of the difference in the average scores started close to the middle of the experiment and was maintained until the end of the full ten minutes. Thus, it can be inferred that the sensitivity to the stimulus was high, but there was a longer delay reaching the symptom decay constant and the adaptation constant, prolonging the volunteers’ discomfort interval.

Most existing studies on the subject have small samples, as the research depends on the inclusion of volunteers who, not always, wish to be submitted to known unpleasant stimuli(22 Leung AK, Hon KL. Motion sickness: an overview. Drugs Context. 2019 Dec 13;8:2019-9-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.7573/dic.2019-9-4. PMid:32158479.
http://dx.doi.org/10.7573/dic.2019-9-4...
,33 Gupta AK, Kumar BV, Rajguru R, Parate KD. Assessment of sea sickness in naval personnel: incidence and management. Indian J Occup Environ Med. 2021 Abr-Jun;25(2):119-24. PMid:34421250.,1010 Gavgani AM, Walker FR, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. A comparative study of cybersickness during exposure to virtual reality and “classic” motion sickness: are they different? J Appl Physiol. 2018;125(6):1670-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00338.2018. PMid:30284516.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.0...
,1111 Gavgani AM, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. Effects of visual flow direction on signs and symptoms of cybersickness. PLoS One. 2017;12(8):e0182790. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182790. PMid:28777827.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0...
,1414 Mittelstaedt JM. Individual predictors of the susceptibility for motion-related sickness: a systematic review. J Vestib Res. 2020;30(3):165-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200702. PMid:32623410.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200702...
). The inclusion of volunteers was also a difficulty in the present study. In addition, a limitation of all studies on motion sickness is the lack of an objective gold standard method that establishes a criterion for classifying people who actually have or do not have the disease. In this study, the only subject in the study who dropped out of the roller coaster experiment also reported discomfort in the car but had not reported previous motion sickness, leading to the consideration of whether exposure to virtual reality is a promising method for diagnosing individuals with motion sickness.

Knowledge on the subject is still incipient and needs further case-control studies; but it is shown that subjects with a history of motion sickness have more symptoms after exposure to virtual reality than patients without the disease and that, when comparing cases and controls, the frontal visual stimulus was more effective in discriminating those with and without motion sickness.

CONCLUSION

Individuals with motion sickness present more intense symptoms when submitted to virtual reality stimuli, when compared to individuals without the disease. The roller coaster experiment with predominance of frontal visual flow caused greater intensity of symptoms than the car experiment with predominance of lateral flow.

Annex 1. Motion Sickness Susceptibility Questionnaire (MSSQ)

Name: HR:

Age: RR:

Gender: () Male () Female BP:

Date:

The questionnaire is intended to predict an individual’s susceptibility to motion sickness, and which types of movements are more intense in causing symptoms of nausea. Please place an “X” in one of the blanks for each motion sickness-generating transport and entertainment.

A. In your childhood (before age 12), how often did you feel sick?

Transport and entertainment generating motion sickness Never experienced Never felt sick Rarely felt sick Sometimes felt sick Always felt sick
Cars
Bus or Vans
Trains
Planes
Small boats
Ships or ferries
Swings in playgrounds
Merry-go-round in playgrounds
Rides and amusement parks

B. In the last 10 years, how often did you feel sick?

Transport and entertainment generating motion sickness Never experienced Never felt sick Rarely felt sick Sometimes felt sick Always felt sick
Cars
Bus or Vans
Trains
Planes
Small boats
Ships or ferries
Swings in playgrounds
Merry-go-round in playgrounds
Rides and amusement parks

The score is calculated based on a point system:

Answer Score
Not applicable - never used 0
Never felt sick 0
Rarely felt sick 1
Sometimes felt sick 2
Always felt sick 3
  • Study carried out at Departamento de Otorrinolaringologia, Irmandade da Santa Casa de Misericórdia de São Paulo - ISCMSP - São Paulo (SP), Brasil and at Faculdade de Ciências Médicas, Santa Casa de São Paulo - FCMSCSP - São Paulo (SP), Brasil.
  • Funding: None.

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Cha YH, Golding JF, Keshavarz B, Furman J, Kim JS, Lopez-Escamez JA, et al. Motion sickness diagnostic criteria: Consensus Document of the Classification Committee of the Bárány Society. J Vestib Res. 2021;31(5):327-44. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200005 PMid:33646187.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200005
  • 2
    Leung AK, Hon KL. Motion sickness: an overview. Drugs Context. 2019 Dec 13;8:2019-9-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.7573/dic.2019-9-4 PMid:32158479.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.7573/dic.2019-9-4
  • 3
    Gupta AK, Kumar BV, Rajguru R, Parate KD. Assessment of sea sickness in naval personnel: incidence and management. Indian J Occup Environ Med. 2021 Abr-Jun;25(2):119-24. PMid:34421250.
  • 4
    Koch A, Cascorbi I, Westhofen M, Dafotakis M, Klapa S, Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP. The neurophysiology and treatment of motion sickness. Dtsch Arztebl Int. 2018 Out 12;115(41):687-96. http://dx.doi.org/10.3238/arztebl.2018.0687 PMid:30406755.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3238/arztebl.2018.0687
  • 5
    Nooij SAE, Bockisch CJ, Bülthoff HH, Straumann D. Beyond sensory conflict: the role of beliefs and perception in motion sickness. PLoS One. 2021 Jan 19;16(1):e0245295. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245295 PMid:33465124.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0245295
  • 6
    Klosterhalfen S, Kellermann S, Pan F, Stockhorst U, Hall G, Enck P. Effects of ethnicity and gender on motion sickness susceptibility. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2005;76(11):1051-7. PMid:16313141.
  • 7
    Stanney K, Fidopiastis C, Foster L. Virtual reality is sexist: but it does not have to be. Front Robot AI. 2020 Jan 31;7:4. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2020.00004 PMid:33501173.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2020.00004
  • 8
    Li CC, Zhang ZR, Liu YH, Zhang T, Zhang XT, Wang H, et al. Multi-dimensional and objective assessment of motion sickness susceptibility based on machine learning. Front Neurol. 2022 Abr 1;13:824670. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2022.824670 PMid:35432161.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2022.824670
  • 9
    Nürnberger M, Klingner C, Witte OW, Brodoehl S. Mismatch of visual-vestibular information in virtual reality: is motion sickness part of the brains attempt to reduce the prediction error? Front Hum Neurosci. 2021 Out 29;15:757735. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757735 PMid:34776909.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.757735
  • 10
    Gavgani AM, Walker FR, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. A comparative study of cybersickness during exposure to virtual reality and “classic” motion sickness: are they different? J Appl Physiol. 2018;125(6):1670-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00338.2018 PMid:30284516.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00338.2018
  • 11
    Gavgani AM, Hodgson DM, Nalivaiko E. Effects of visual flow direction on signs and symptoms of cybersickness. PLoS One. 2017;12(8):e0182790. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182790 PMid:28777827.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182790
  • 12
    Lim HK, Ji K, Woo YS, Han DU, Lee DH, Nam SG, et al. Test-retest reliability of the virtual reality sickness evaluation using electroencephalography (EEG). Neurosci Lett. 2021 Jan 19;743:135589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135589 PMid:33359731.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2020.135589
  • 13
    Gavgani AM, Nesbitt KV, Blackmore KL, Nalivaiko E. Profiling subjective symptoms and autonomic changes associated with cybersickness. Auton Neurosci. 2017;203:41-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016.12.004 PMid:28010995.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.autneu.2016.12.004
  • 14
    Mittelstaedt JM. Individual predictors of the susceptibility for motion-related sickness: a systematic review. J Vestib Res. 2020;30(3):165-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200702 PMid:32623410.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/VES-200702
  • 15
    Metcalf M, Rossie K, Stokes K, Tallman C, Tanner B. Virtual reality cue refusal video game for alcohol and cigarette recovery support: summative study. JMIR Serious Games. 2018;6(2):e7. http://dx.doi.org/10.2196/games.9231 PMid:29661748.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.2196/games.9231
  • 16
    Sevinc V, Berkman MI. Psychometric evaluation of simulator sickness questionnaire and its variants as a measure of cybersickness in consumer virtual environments. Appl Ergon. 2020 Jan;82:102958. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2019.102958 PMid:31563798.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2019.102958
  • 17
    Felipe L, Staggs A, Hunnicutt S. Can type of dizziness influence the vestibular caloric test result? J Prim Care Community Health. 2021 Jan-Dez;12:21501327211030120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/21501327211030120 PMid:34720013.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/21501327211030120
  • 18
    Gimmon Y, Schubert MC. Vestibular testing-rotary chair and dynamic visual acuity tests. Adv Otorhinolaryngol. 2019;82:39-46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000490270 PMid:30947187.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000490270
  • 19
    Janky KL, Patterson J. The relationship between rotary chair and video head impulse testing in children and young adults with cochlear implants. Am J Audiol. 2020 Dez 9;29(4):898-906. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2020_AJA-20-00079. PMid:33201726.
  • 20
    França SR, Branco-Barreiro FCA. Susceptibilidade à cinetose no idoso com doença vestibular. RECES. 2013;5(1):30-5.
  • 21
    Reason JT, Brand JJ. Motion sickness. Londres: Academic Press; 1975.
  • 22
    Neumar RW, Shuster M, Callaway CW, Gent LM, Atkins DL, Bhanji F, et al. Part 1: executive summary: 2015 american heart association guidelines update for cardiopulmonary resuscitation and emergency cardiovascular care. Circulation. 2015 Nov 3;132(18, Suppl 2):S315-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/CIR.0000000000000252 PMid:26472989.
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/CIR.0000000000000252
  • 23
    Kohlmann O Jr, Costa Guimarães A, Carvalho MHC, Chaves HC Jr, Machado CA, Praxedes JN, et al. III Consenso Brasileiro de Hipertensão Arterial. Arq Bras Endocrinol Metabol. 1999;43(4):257-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-27301999000400004
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0004-27301999000400004

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    06 Jan 2023
  • Date of issue
    2023

History

  • Received
    24 May 2022
  • Accepted
    14 Oct 2022
Academia Brasileira de Audiologia Rua Itapeva, 202, conjunto 61, CEP 01332-000, Tel.: (11) 3253-8711, Fax: (11) 3253-8473 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@audiologiabrasil.org.br