Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Behaviour of personality-screened juvenile Oreochromis niloticus in different challenges: Bold fish present more exploratory capacity and learning to face imposed challenges

Abstract

The investigation of cultivation and management techniques that take into account behaviour and animal welfare becomes important due to their direct relationship with animal performance. This study aimed to classify juvenile Oreochromis niloticus by personality and compare their performance at exploration and competition tasks. The new object test was used to select bold and shy animals, which were then tested to see which first approached an object. The food competition test involved placing three feed pellets inside a cylinder and measuring the time taken until the pellet was eaten. The new object test found bold animals with a shorter mean time to approach the object than shy animals approaching 65% of the first object when compared to shy. The food competition test found that the bold animals ingested the first pellet more times and with a shorter average time of ingestion than shy animals, no difference in the second intake, whereas the results for the third were same as those for the first. Shy animals had higher intake of the second pellet. Bold animals explored their environment faster in both challenges, while shy individuals exhibited ability to learn from the challenges imposed in the study.

Key words
Stress; new object; feeding; personality

INTRODUCTION

Nile tilapia, Oreochromis niloticus, is the most cultivated fish species in the world (Paredes-Trujillo et al. 2021PAREDES-TRUJILLO A, VELÁZQUEZ-ABUNADER I, PAPIOL V, RODOLFO E & VIDAL-MARTÍNEZ V M. 2021. Negative effect of ectoparasite burdens on the condition factor from farmed tilapia Oreochromis niloticus in the Yucatan, Mexico. Veterinary Parasitology, 292: 109393.). It has a fast growth rate, high resistance to diseases and stress, and the capacity to efficiently convert a wide variety of natural and artificial foods into high quality proteins (Bosu et al. 2016BOSU A, DAS M, HOSSAIN S & MONIRUZZAMAN M. 2016. Evaluation of commercial feed on growth performance of Tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) in Mymensingh. Inter Jour Nat Soc Scien 3: 73-82.). The species presents competitive dominance and can negatively affect some native species (Champneys et al. 2020CHAMPNEYS T, GENNER MJ & IOANNOU CC. 2020. Invasive Nile tilapia dominates a threatened indigenous tilapia in competition over shelter. Hydrobio 1-16.). Wing et al. (2021)WING JD, CHAMPNEYS TS & IOANNOU CC. 2021. The impact of turbidity on foraging and risk taking in the invasive Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) and a threatened native cichlid (Oreochromis amphimelas). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 75: 1-13. found O. niloticus to have a competitive foraging advantage over another tilapia species (O. amphimelas). Mesquita et al. (2016)MESQUITA FO, TORRES IFA & LUZ RK. 2016. Behaviour of proactive and reactive tilapia Oreochromis niloticus in a T-maze. Appl Anim Behav Sci 181: 200-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.022.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016....
evaluated the behaviour of O. niloticus, classifying personalities as proactive and reactive using a T-maze. They showed that the two personalities learned to locate food efficiently during training, with similar decreases in search time on successive trials. In addition, proactive animals adapted more quickly to training and fed more efficiently in the T-maze than reactive individuals. However, there are no studies in the literature that have compared the competitive ability of Nile tilapia with different personality traits. Furthermore, it is necessary to investigate cultivation and management techniques that consider animal behaviour so that greater animal welfare can be guaranteed, which is directly related to animal performance (Torres et al. 2018TORRES IFA, FERREIRA AS, SOUZA E SILVA W, MESQUITA FO & LUZ RK. 2018. Effect of environmental color on learning of Nile tilapia. Appl Anim Behav Sci 209: 104-108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.08.021.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018....
).

The term “boldness” in behavioural studies is defined by placing an individual’s level of risk acceptance during behaviours, such as foraging, exploration, and defense, along a boldness-shyness continuum (Budaev 1997BUDAEV SV. 1997. Alternative styles in the European wrasse, Symphodus ocellatus: boldness-related schooling tendency. Enviro Biol Fish 49: 71-78., Coleman & Wilson 1998COLEMAN K & WILSON DS. 1998. Shyness and boldness in pumpkinseed sunfish: individual differences are context-specific. Anim Behav 56: 927-936., Wilson et al. 1994WILSON DS, CLARK AB, COLEMAN K & DEARSTYNE T. 1994. Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. Trends Ecol Evol 9: 442-446. https://doi.org/10.1016/0169-5347(94)90134-1.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0169-5347(94)901...
). The terms used to refer to extreme behaviours in such situations are “bold” and “shy”, to reflect bolder and shyer personalities, respectively. An environment which presents risks to a bold fish can lead it to perform general tasks, such as looking for food, whereas shy individuals tend to reduce swimming activity and, consequently, reduce their exploratory behaviour (Brydges et al. 2008BRYDGES NM, HEATHCOTE RJP & BRAITHWAITE VA. 2008. Habitat stability and predation pressure influence learning and memory in populations of three-spined sticklebacks. Anim Behav 75: 935-942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.08.005.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.0...
). The classification of individuals according to personality is done through trials that measure their level of boldness, such as the exploration of a new environment or approaching a new object, for example (Mesquita et al. 2016MESQUITA FO, TORRES IFA & LUZ RK. 2016. Behaviour of proactive and reactive tilapia Oreochromis niloticus in a T-maze. Appl Anim Behav Sci 181: 200-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.022.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016....
).

Studies have shown advantages in doing personality screening because personality relates to learning, survival and resistance to diseases (Moiron et al. 2020MOIRON M, LASKOWSKI KL & NIEMELA PT. 2020. Individual differences in behaviour explain variation in survival: a meta-analysis. Ecol Let 23: 399-408., Ariyomo & Watt 2012ARIYOMO TO & WATT PJ. 2012. The effect of variation in boldness and aggressiveness on the reproductive success of zebrafish. Anim Behav 83: 41-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.004.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.1...
). For instance, personality and level of boldness can affect an individual’s choice of a sexual partner during couple formation, its level of aggressiveness, and its reproductive success (Moscicki & Hurd 2015MOSCICKI MK & HURD PL. 2015. Sex, boldness and stress experience affect convict cichlid, Amatitlania nigrofasciata, open field behaviour. Anim Behav 107: 105-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.05.027.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.0...
), with bold individuals tending to have greater reproductive success than shy individuals (Wilson et al. 2010WILSON ADM, GODIN JGJ & WARD AJW. 2010. Boldness and reproductive fitness correlates in the eastern mosquitofish, gambusia holbrooki. Ethol 116: 96-104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01719.x.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009...
). Using a meta-analytic approach, Moiron et al. (2020)MOIRON M, LASKOWSKI KL & NIEMELA PT. 2020. Individual differences in behaviour explain variation in survival: a meta-analysis. Ecol Let 23: 399-408. demonstrated that behavioural variation explains about 6% of the variation in survival for the different species of animals analysed. Furthermore, bold individuals tend to recover their appetite more quickly after stressful situations (Mas-Muñoz et al. 2011MAS-MUÑOZ J, KOMEN H, SCHNEIDER O, VISCH SW & SCHRAMA JW. 2011. Feeding behaviour, swimming activity and boldness explain variation in feed intake and growth of sole (Solea solea) reared in captivity. PLoS ONE 6: 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021393.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.002...
, Basic et al. 2012BASIC D, WINBERG S, SCHJOLDEN J, KROGDAHL Å & HOGLUND E. 2012. Context-dependent responses to novelty in Rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss), selected for high and low post-stress cortisol responsiveness. Physiol Behav 105: 1175-1181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.12.021.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.1...
). However, personality classification methods can vary among species, thus studies with target species are required.

Thus, the aim of the present study was to screen O. niloticus by personality and compare their performance at exploration and competition tasks to identify differences between personalities in the face of challenges.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

The experiments were conducted in the larviculture laboratory Laqua (Laboratório de Aquacultura) of Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Brazil, under protocol number 373/2013 of Comitê de Ética em Experimentação Animal. A total of 324 O. niloticus (40.1 ± 5.3 g, 13.5 ± 1.1 cm) were used for personality classification. The animals were placed in 16 tanks (200 L), with a stocking density of 5.41 g/L, in a recirculating aquaculture system with controlled temperature (28ºC) and pH (7.2 - 7.8) (model HI9146 Hanna instruments) and ammonia (<0.004 ppm) (Labcon kit), constant aeration (dissolved oxygen > 5 mg/L) (YSI 6920VZ2 multiparameter probe), 12h/12h photoperiod (digital timer, group Key West DNI), and feed (36% crude protein, 2.6 mm diameter, Laguna/Socil, São Paulo, Brazil) offered until apparent satiety twice a day (8:00am and 4:00pm). The animals were kept in this system for two weeks, with fasting for the last two days prior to personality screening.

Personality screening

Personality screening was done according to the new environment methodology of Mesquita et al. (2016)MESQUITA FO, TORRES IFA & LUZ RK. 2016. Behaviour of proactive and reactive tilapia Oreochromis niloticus in a T-maze. Appl Anim Behav Sci 181: 200-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.022.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016....
. A tank (200L) was filled to ¾ with water, in which a divider was placed creating two zones — a light zone corresponding to 60% of total volume and a dark zone corresponding to 40%. The dark zone was completely closed form the entrance of light. The division possessed a circular form (5-cm radius) where there was a door mechanism that could be opened and closed, as necessary, to allowed access to the light zone (Figure 1a).

Figure 1
Tanks used in trials. a - Tank used for animal screening by personality; b - Tank used for new object test; c - Tank used for food competition test.

Six animals were randomly chosen and placed in the dark zone for 10 minutes to acclimatize to the new environment (Mesquita et al. 2016MESQUITA FO, TORRES IFA & LUZ RK. 2016. Behaviour of proactive and reactive tilapia Oreochromis niloticus in a T-maze. Appl Anim Behav Sci 181: 200-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.022.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016....
). The door mechanism was then opened allowing the animals to explore the new environment for 20 minutes, after which the door was closed. When the door was opened, 1 to 2 ml of a solution of water and dissolved feed was added in the light zone to stimulate animals in the dark zone to leave. Animals that remained in the dark zone were classified as shy animals while those that passed through the hole to the light zone were classified as bold animals (Mesquita et al. 2016MESQUITA FO, TORRES IFA & LUZ RK. 2016. Behaviour of proactive and reactive tilapia Oreochromis niloticus in a T-maze. Appl Anim Behav Sci 181: 200-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.022.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016....
). The water parameters of the trial tank were the same as those of the culture tanks. This trial was repeated 54 times until 120 animals were obtained with 60 bold and 60 shy. A total water change was performed before each trial to avoid problems from the stimulus solution used in the previous trial. Once classified, the animals were microchipped (microchip Partners) to monitor individual performance. Microchips were implanted intramuscularly in the dorsal region of the animals (close to the dorsal fin) using a syringe suitable for the application. The animals were anesthetized with a solution of 80 mg/L of eugenol for microchip implantation, as suggested by Ribeiro et al. (2015)RIBEIRO PAP, MIRANDA-FILHO KC, MELO DCH & LUZ RK. 2015. Efficiency of eugenol as anesthetic for the early life stages of Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus). An Acad Bras Ciênc 87: 529-535..

The animals were maintained in six tanks (200L each tank) in a recirculation system with each tank containing 20 animals of shared personality (“bold”-T1, “bold”-T2, “bold”-T3, “shy”-T1, “shy”-T2, “shy”-T3).

New object test

New object trials were performed immediately after classification in a 200L tank that was ¾ full of water. The water parameters were kept similar to those maintained in the culture system. Each trial used one randomly-chosen bold animal and one randomly-chosen shy animal, with each animal being used only once. The animals were transferred to the trial tank where they were allowed to acclimatize for 10 minutes. A coloured Lego block (the object) measuring 5x4x2 cm was then placed in the tank using a fishing line and observations were made for 20 minutes (Figure 1b). The time it took for the first animal to approach the object was recorded, and the animal was captured and had its microchip read, marking the end of the trial. Testing was repeated for 60 pairs of bold and shy animals.

Food competition test

Food competition trials were performed after the completion of the new object trials, in a 200L tank that was filled with water. A transparent cylinder (6-cm radius, 52 cm in height) with a square hole (4x4 cm) near mid-length was placed in the central region of the tank (Figure 1c).

For these trials, bold animals were marked with a green elastomer to visually differentiate them from shy animals in the tank, thus avoiding the need to remove animals from the tank for identification. Shy animals were submitted to the same marking procedure, but with a neutral substance, to avoid direct influences from marking only bold individuals. Two days after marking, the animals of both experimental groups were fasted. Each trial involved placing a randomly-chosen bold animal and a randomly-chosen shy animal in the tank, with each animal being used only once. The animals were allowed to acclimatize for 10 minutes, after which a feed pellet (36% crude protein, 2.6 mm diameter, Laguna/Socil, São Paulo, Brazil) was placed inside the cylinder. Thus, to acquire the pellet a fish had to enter the cylinder, pass through the lower part and swim to the surface of the water column where the pellet remained.

The time it took for a fish to consume the pellet, and whether the fish was marked by an elastomer (bold) or not (shy), were recorded. Next, a second feed pellet was placed inside the cylinder and the information described above was again recorded. Finally, a third feed pellet was placed inside the cylinder and the information again recorded. The timer was reset to zero at each inclusion of a pellet. The total time allowed to consume the three pellets was 20 minutes, and whether none, one or only two pellets were taken was also recorded.

Statistical analysis

All data were submitted to the Shapiro-Wilk normality test. Data of average time(s) of approach to the new object and average time(s) spent for the consumption of the different pellets that met normality were submitted to the Student’s t-test at the 5% significance level. Non-normal data were transformed into Log 10 to meet normality and then submitted to the Student’s t-test at the 5% significance level. Consumption time data for pellets 1, 2 and 3 of different personalities and consumption of each pellet by different personalities were submitted to ANOVA followed by the Tukey test (p <0.05).

RESULTS

New object test

Data for the new object trials are given in Table I and reveal that bold animals had a significantly shorter mean approach time to the new object than did shy animals (p <0.05). Of the 60 trials performed with bold and shy pairs, the bold individual approached the object first in 65% of the trials, the shy approached first in 21.6% of the trials, and neither bold nor shy approached the object in 13.4% of the trials.

Table I
Mean time (s) spent by Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) of different personalities to approach of the new object.

Food competition test

The proportion of bold animals that ingested the first pellet (58.4%) was greater than that of shy animals (33.3%) and the pellet was not consumed in 8.3% of the trials (Figure 2). The number of second pellets ingested by the two personalities did not differ significantly, while bold animals had a higher mean ingestion (43.4%) of the third pellet than did shy animals (16.6%), while the third pellet was not ingested in 40% of the trials (p <0.05). There were trials in which all three pellets were ingested by bold or shy animals, but without significant differences (p >0.05). Bold animals had a higher consumption of the first pellet, shy animals had higher consumption of second, but this consumption was reduced again for the third pellet. There was a higher rate of non-consumption for the third pellet than for the others (Figure 2).

Figure 2
Mean values (± standard deviation) for consumption percentage of each pellet by Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) of different personalities. Different uppercase letters indicate significant difference in consumption among pellets for each personality between the different pellets. Lowercase letters indicate significant difference in consumption between personalities for each pellet. The results were submitted to ANOVA followed by the Tukey test (p <0.05).

Data for the food competition trials are given in Table II and reveal that bold animals had a significantly shorter mean first-consume time (first pellet) than the shy animals (p <0.001), while personalities did not differ significantly for mean second-consume time (second pellet; p = 0.1733) or mean third-consume time (third pellet; p = 0.3403). Figure 3 shows the mean time for consuming the first, second and third pellet, which did not differ significantly for bold animals. Shy animals, however, exhibited a significant reduction in consume time from the first to the third pellet (p <0.05).

Figure 3
Mean values (± standard deviation) for consumption time among first, second and third pellets for Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) of different personalities. Different letters indicate significant differences between results by ANOVA followed by the Tukey test (p <0.05).
Table II
Mean time (s) that Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) of different personalities spent to consume the different pellets in the food competition test.

DISCUSSION

In general, the comparison of the performance of bold and shy O. niloticus juveniles in the exploration and competition tasks, to identify differences between personalities in the face of challenges, presented interesting and important results for the development of this research.

The results of the new object test revealed that shy animals took longer to approach the object than bold animals. This result can be explained by the fact that shy animals are more afraid and slower at exploring environments than are bold animals, which explore environments quickly and superficially (Mesquita et al. 2016MESQUITA FO, TORRES IFA & LUZ RK. 2016. Behaviour of proactive and reactive tilapia Oreochromis niloticus in a T-maze. Appl Anim Behav Sci 181: 200-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.022.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016....
). In addition, bold animals can be considered a threat to shy animals, which in turn can be more sensitive to stressors (Hoglund et al. 2008HOGLUND E, GJOEN HM, POTTINGER TG & OVERLI O. 2008. Parental stress-coping styles affect the behaviour of rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss at early developmental stages. J Fish Biol 73: 1764-1769.). Thus, the presence of a bold individual as a social stressor may have discouraged shy animals from exploring the environment and, consequently, reduced their swimming activity (Brydges et al. 2008BRYDGES NM, HEATHCOTE RJP & BRAITHWAITE VA. 2008. Habitat stability and predation pressure influence learning and memory in populations of three-spined sticklebacks. Anim Behav 75: 935-942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.08.005.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.0...
, Archard & Braithwaite 2011ARCHARD GA & BRAITHWAITE VA. 2011. Variation in aggressive behaviour in the poeciliid fish Brachyrhaphis episcopi: Population and sex differences. Behav Proc 86: 52-57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.09.002.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.09...
). When shy animals were the first to approach the object, they spent longer time than did bold animals arrived first. Shy animals tend to spend more time on tasks (Torres et al. 2018TORRES IFA, FERREIRA AS, SOUZA E SILVA W, MESQUITA FO & LUZ RK. 2018. Effect of environmental color on learning of Nile tilapia. Appl Anim Behav Sci 209: 104-108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.08.021.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018....
), since they explore environments more carefully (Mesquita et al. 2016MESQUITA FO, TORRES IFA & LUZ RK. 2016. Behaviour of proactive and reactive tilapia Oreochromis niloticus in a T-maze. Appl Anim Behav Sci 181: 200-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.022.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016....
).

In contrast, bold animals are more inattentive. Thus, bold individuals who did not approach an object may have been inattentive with the presence of the shy co-specific, leaving the initial task (Lego approach) in the background (Vaz-Serrano et al. 2011VAZ-SERRANO J, RUIZ-GOMEZ ML, GJøEN HM, SKOV PV, HUNTINGFORD FA, OVERLI Ø & HOGLUND E. 2011. Physiology & Behavior Consistent boldness behaviour in early emerging fry of domesticated Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar): Decoupling of behavioural and physiological traits of the proactive stress coping style. Physiol Behav 103: 359-364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.02.025.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.0...
). The new object test is also considered a test for personality classification (Dahlbom et al. 2011DAHLBOM SJ, LAGMAN D, LUNDSTEDT-ENKEL K, SUNDSTROM LF & WINBERG S. 2011. Boldness predicts social status in zebrafish (Danio rerio). PLoS ONE 6: e23565. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023565.). The results of the present new object test corroborate the initial personality classification carried out using the new environment method, since bold animals had a lower mean exploration time of the new object than did shy animals. The fact that shy individuals are more meticulous and slower can also explain the results of the food competition test, since it took more time for shy animals to feed than it did for bold animals. Therefore, shy animals may have spent more time exploring the environment looking for food. Furthermore, the fact that shy individuals exhibit greater signs of stress when exposed to a stress or challenge (Koolhaas 2008KOOLHAAS J.M. 2008. Coping style and immunity in animals: Making sense of individual variation. Brain Behav Immun 22: 662-667. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2007.11.006.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2007.11.00...
, Andersson et al. 2011ANDERSSON AM, SILVA PIM, STEFFENSEN JF & HOGLUND E. 2011. Effects of maternal stress coping style on offspring characteristics in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). Horm Behav 60: 699-705. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.09.008.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.09....
), disfavours the activation of the hunger center (Andersen et al. 1991ANDERSEN DE, REID SD & MOON TW. 1991. Metabolic effects associated with chronically elevated cortisol in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). Can J Fish Aquat Sci 48: 1811-1817.), potentially resulting in decreased interest in foraging. In contrast, bold individuals exhibit greater motivation to eat immediately after handling (Overli et al. 2007OVERLI O, SORENSEN C, PULMAN KGT, POTTINGER TG, KORZAN W, SUMMERS CH & NILSSON E. 2007. Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles : Relationships between physiological, behavioral, and cognitive traits in non-mammalian vertebrates. Neurosc. Biobeh Rev 31: 396-412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.006.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006...
).

In the case of the present study, fasting for two days, handling before trials and exposure to a new environment were potential stressors for animals with greater effects on shy individuals. For the number of pellets ingested, bold animals consumed a greater number of first pellets than did shy animals. These results, once again, corroborate the greater willingness of bold animals to eat after handling (Overli et al. 2007OVERLI O, SORENSEN C, PULMAN KGT, POTTINGER TG, KORZAN W, SUMMERS CH & NILSSON E. 2007. Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles : Relationships between physiological, behavioral, and cognitive traits in non-mammalian vertebrates. Neurosc. Biobeh Rev 31: 396-412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.006.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006...
) than their shy co-specifics that tend to conserve energy, inhibit hunger by physiological factors related to stress and inhibit foraging behaviour. Greater feeding motivation for bold fish may be explained by a higher metabolic rate (Jolles et al. 2019JOLLES JW, BRIGGS HD, ARAYA-AJOY YG & BOOGERT NJ. 2019. Personality, plasticity and predictability in sticklebacks: bold fish are less plastic and more predictable than shy fish. Anim Behav 154: 193-202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.06.022.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.0...
), and thus greater energy requirements (Mesquita 2011MESQUITA FO. 2011. Coping styles and learning in fish: developing behavioural tools for welfare-friendly aquaculture. PhD thesis (Unpublished). http://theses.gla.ac.uk/2785/.
http://theses.gla.ac.uk/2785/...
). In contrast, the number of second pellets consumed by the two personalities (bold and shy fish) did not differ significantly, demonstrating an ability by shy animals to observe and make decisions in the second trial (Torres et al. 2018TORRES IFA, FERREIRA AS, SOUZA E SILVA W, MESQUITA FO & LUZ RK. 2018. Effect of environmental color on learning of Nile tilapia. Appl Anim Behav Sci 209: 104-108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.08.021.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018....
). Bold and shy animals differed with regard to the third pellet. During the first and second pellets, the bold animals learned that the food consume environment was safe, and thus explored the environment further (Brydges et al. 2008BRYDGES NM, HEATHCOTE RJP & BRAITHWAITE VA. 2008. Habitat stability and predation pressure influence learning and memory in populations of three-spined sticklebacks. Anim Behav 75: 935-942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.08.005.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.0...
) until they felt the need to feed, at which point they returned to the feeding place. The feed pellets provide an olfactory stimulus, and so when placed in the water they are recognized by the animals as food, thus encouraging searching. The longer time taken by shy animals to consume the first pellet than the other pellets can be explained by it being the first time the animals experienced the trial. The decrease in the amount of time taken by shy animals to consume the second and third pellets can be explained by the fact that, as a species, O. niloticus is a fast learner of simple tasks, such as eating during the second repetition of a task (Torres et al. 2018TORRES IFA, FERREIRA AS, SOUZA E SILVA W, MESQUITA FO & LUZ RK. 2018. Effect of environmental color on learning of Nile tilapia. Appl Anim Behav Sci 209: 104-108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.08.021.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018....
). Therefore, after ingesting the first pellet, shy animals may have associated the feeding place with food, resulting in reduced ingestion time for the second and third pellets.

Bold animals, on the other hand, took less time to associate the task than did shy animals. However, the exploratory instinct of these animals lead them to explore new ways of eating and the environment (Brydges et al. 2008BRYDGES NM, HEATHCOTE RJP & BRAITHWAITE VA. 2008. Habitat stability and predation pressure influence learning and memory in populations of three-spined sticklebacks. Anim Behav 75: 935-942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.08.005.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.0...
), which explains the fact that they maintained a pattern for capture time among all pellets offered. In addition, bold animals have the ability to store information longer during exploratory activities (Dammhahn & Almeling 2012DAMMHAHN M & ALMELING L. 2012. Is risk taking during foraging a personality trait? A field test for cross-context consistency in boldness. Anim Behav 84: 1131-1139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.08.014.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.0...
, Jones & Godin 2010JONES KA & GODIN JGJ, 2010. Are fast explorers slow reactors? Linking personal type and anti-predator behavior. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 277: 625-632. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1607.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1607...
, Kareklas et al. 2016KAREKLAS K, ARNOTT G, ELWOOD RW & HOLLAND RA. 2016. Plasticity varies with boldness in a weaklyelectric fish. Front Zool 13: 1-7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-016-0154-0.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-016-0154-...
). Thus, bold animals recognize the place of food as a place without risks, leading them to a lessened fear in relation to food.

CONCLUSION

In conclusion, performance in exploration tasks and competition in the face of challenges imposed by different personalities demonstrated that bold O. niloticus exhibited better exploratory abilities than did shy individuals. In addition, shy O. niloticus exhibited a good ability to learn from the challenges proposed in the study. Overall, the study confirmed the advantages of doing personality screening that is related to learning.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was supported by Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq-Brasil), Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES-Brasil), and Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de Minas Gerais (FAPEMIG). R. K. Luz received a research grant and a research fellowship from Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq n°. 308547/2018-7).

REFERENCES

  • ANDERSEN DE, REID SD & MOON TW. 1991. Metabolic effects associated with chronically elevated cortisol in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). Can J Fish Aquat Sci 48: 1811-1817.
  • ANDERSSON AM, SILVA PIM, STEFFENSEN JF & HOGLUND E. 2011. Effects of maternal stress coping style on offspring characteristics in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). Horm Behav 60: 699-705. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.09.008.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.09.008
  • ARCHARD GA & BRAITHWAITE VA. 2011. Variation in aggressive behaviour in the poeciliid fish Brachyrhaphis episcopi: Population and sex differences. Behav Proc 86: 52-57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.09.002.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.09.002
  • ARIYOMO TO & WATT PJ. 2012. The effect of variation in boldness and aggressiveness on the reproductive success of zebrafish. Anim Behav 83: 41-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.004.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.004
  • BASIC D, WINBERG S, SCHJOLDEN J, KROGDAHL Å & HOGLUND E. 2012. Context-dependent responses to novelty in Rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss), selected for high and low post-stress cortisol responsiveness. Physiol Behav 105: 1175-1181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.12.021.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.12.021
  • BOSU A, DAS M, HOSSAIN S & MONIRUZZAMAN M. 2016. Evaluation of commercial feed on growth performance of Tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) in Mymensingh. Inter Jour Nat Soc Scien 3: 73-82.
  • BRYDGES NM, HEATHCOTE RJP & BRAITHWAITE VA. 2008. Habitat stability and predation pressure influence learning and memory in populations of three-spined sticklebacks. Anim Behav 75: 935-942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.08.005.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.08.005
  • BUDAEV SV. 1997. Alternative styles in the European wrasse, Symphodus ocellatus: boldness-related schooling tendency. Enviro Biol Fish 49: 71-78.
  • CHAMPNEYS T, GENNER MJ & IOANNOU CC. 2020. Invasive Nile tilapia dominates a threatened indigenous tilapia in competition over shelter. Hydrobio 1-16.
  • COLEMAN K & WILSON DS. 1998. Shyness and boldness in pumpkinseed sunfish: individual differences are context-specific. Anim Behav 56: 927-936.
  • DAHLBOM SJ, LAGMAN D, LUNDSTEDT-ENKEL K, SUNDSTROM LF & WINBERG S. 2011. Boldness predicts social status in zebrafish (Danio rerio). PLoS ONE 6: e23565. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023565.
  • DAMMHAHN M & ALMELING L. 2012. Is risk taking during foraging a personality trait? A field test for cross-context consistency in boldness. Anim Behav 84: 1131-1139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.08.014.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.08.014
  • HOGLUND E, GJOEN HM, POTTINGER TG & OVERLI O. 2008. Parental stress-coping styles affect the behaviour of rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss at early developmental stages. J Fish Biol 73: 1764-1769.
  • JOLLES JW, BRIGGS HD, ARAYA-AJOY YG & BOOGERT NJ. 2019. Personality, plasticity and predictability in sticklebacks: bold fish are less plastic and more predictable than shy fish. Anim Behav 154: 193-202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.06.022.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.06.022
  • JONES KA & GODIN JGJ, 2010. Are fast explorers slow reactors? Linking personal type and anti-predator behavior. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 277: 625-632. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1607.
    » https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1607
  • KAREKLAS K, ARNOTT G, ELWOOD RW & HOLLAND RA. 2016. Plasticity varies with boldness in a weaklyelectric fish. Front Zool 13: 1-7. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-016-0154-0.
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/s12983-016-0154-0
  • KOOLHAAS J.M. 2008. Coping style and immunity in animals: Making sense of individual variation. Brain Behav Immun 22: 662-667. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2007.11.006.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2007.11.006
  • MAS-MUÑOZ J, KOMEN H, SCHNEIDER O, VISCH SW & SCHRAMA JW. 2011. Feeding behaviour, swimming activity and boldness explain variation in feed intake and growth of sole (Solea solea) reared in captivity. PLoS ONE 6: 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021393.
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021393
  • MESQUITA FO, TORRES IFA & LUZ RK. 2016. Behaviour of proactive and reactive tilapia Oreochromis niloticus in a T-maze. Appl Anim Behav Sci 181: 200-204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.022.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.05.022
  • MESQUITA FO. 2011. Coping styles and learning in fish: developing behavioural tools for welfare-friendly aquaculture. PhD thesis (Unpublished). http://theses.gla.ac.uk/2785/
    » http://theses.gla.ac.uk/2785/
  • MOIRON M, LASKOWSKI KL & NIEMELA PT. 2020. Individual differences in behaviour explain variation in survival: a meta-analysis. Ecol Let 23: 399-408.
  • MOSCICKI MK & HURD PL. 2015. Sex, boldness and stress experience affect convict cichlid, Amatitlania nigrofasciata, open field behaviour. Anim Behav 107: 105-114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.05.027.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.05.027
  • OVERLI O, SORENSEN C, PULMAN KGT, POTTINGER TG, KORZAN W, SUMMERS CH & NILSSON E. 2007. Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles : Relationships between physiological, behavioral, and cognitive traits in non-mammalian vertebrates. Neurosc. Biobeh Rev 31: 396-412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.006.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.006
  • PAREDES-TRUJILLO A, VELÁZQUEZ-ABUNADER I, PAPIOL V, RODOLFO E & VIDAL-MARTÍNEZ V M. 2021. Negative effect of ectoparasite burdens on the condition factor from farmed tilapia Oreochromis niloticus in the Yucatan, Mexico. Veterinary Parasitology, 292: 109393.
  • RIBEIRO PAP, MIRANDA-FILHO KC, MELO DCH & LUZ RK. 2015. Efficiency of eugenol as anesthetic for the early life stages of Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus). An Acad Bras Ciênc 87: 529-535.
  • TORRES IFA, FERREIRA AS, SOUZA E SILVA W, MESQUITA FO & LUZ RK. 2018. Effect of environmental color on learning of Nile tilapia. Appl Anim Behav Sci 209: 104-108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.08.021.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2018.08.021
  • VAZ-SERRANO J, RUIZ-GOMEZ ML, GJøEN HM, SKOV PV, HUNTINGFORD FA, OVERLI Ø & HOGLUND E. 2011. Physiology & Behavior Consistent boldness behaviour in early emerging fry of domesticated Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar): Decoupling of behavioural and physiological traits of the proactive stress coping style. Physiol Behav 103: 359-364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.02.025.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.02.025
  • WILSON ADM, GODIN JGJ & WARD AJW. 2010. Boldness and reproductive fitness correlates in the eastern mosquitofish, gambusia holbrooki. Ethol 116: 96-104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01719.x.
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01719.x
  • WILSON DS, CLARK AB, COLEMAN K & DEARSTYNE T. 1994. Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. Trends Ecol Evol 9: 442-446. https://doi.org/10.1016/0169-5347(94)90134-1.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0169-5347(94)90134-1
  • WING JD, CHAMPNEYS TS & IOANNOU CC. 2021. The impact of turbidity on foraging and risk taking in the invasive Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) and a threatened native cichlid (Oreochromis amphimelas). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 75: 1-13.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    03 Feb 2023
  • Date of issue
    2023

History

  • Received
    14 July 2021
  • Accepted
    18 Nov 2021
Academia Brasileira de Ciências Rua Anfilófio de Carvalho, 29, 3º andar, 20030-060 Rio de Janeiro RJ Brasil, Tel: +55 21 3907-8100 - Rio de Janeiro - RJ - Brazil
E-mail: aabc@abc.org.br