Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Thalamus and language: interface with attention, memory and executive functions

Tálamo e linguagem: interface com atenção, memória e funções executivas

Abstracts

Subcortical structures are in a strategic functional position within the cognitive networks. Their lesion can interfere with a great number of functions. We studied six patients with thalamic vascular lesions (three left sided, two right sided and one bilateral), to characterize their repercussion in the communicative abilities and the interface between language alterations and other cognitive abilities, as attention, memory and frontal executive. All patients were evaluated through a functional interview (discourse analysis), and the following batteries: Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, Boston Naming Test, Token Test, Benton Visual Retention Test, Trail Making, Wisconsin Card Sorting and frontal scripts. All patients performed MRI and five underwent SPECT. Results show that these patients present impairment in several cognitive domains, especially attention and executive functions (working memory, planning and self-monitoring); those with right lesions have an additional visuospatial impairment. Such alterations interfere with language abilities, and this fact must be considered in the rehabilitation efforts.

thalamus; language; attention; memory; prefrontal cortex


As estruturas subcorticais ocupam posições funcionais estratégicas nas redes cognitivas. Sua lesão pode interferir com grande número de funções. Estudamos seis pacientes com lesão vascular talâmica (três à esqueda, dois à direita e um bilateral), para caracterizar a repercussão da lesão nas suas habilidades comunicativas e a interface entre alterações de linguagem e outras habilidades cognitivas, como atenção, memória e executivas frontais. Os pacientes foram avaliados através de entrevista funcional (análise do discurso), testes de Boston para Diagnóstico da Afasia, Nomeação Boston, Token, Múltipla Escolha de Benton, Trail Making, Wisconsin Card Sorting e scripts frontais. Todos os pacientes realizaram ressonância magnética encefálica (RM) e cinco realizaram SPECT. Os resultados mostram que os pacientes apresentam prejuízo em diversos domínios cognitivos, especialmente atenção e funções executivas (memória operacional, planejamento e automonitoração); nos lesados à direita, ocorre prejuízo visuoespacial associado. Tais alterações repercutem nas habilidades de linguagem, o que deve ser levado em conta para reabilitação.

tálamo; linguagem; atenção; memória; córtex pré-frontal


Thalamus and language

Interface with attention, memory and executive functions

Tálamo e linguagem:

Interface com atenção, memória e funções executivas

Marcia RadanovicI; Mariana AzambujaII; Letícia Lessa MansurIII; Cláudia Sellitto PortoIV; Milberto ScaffV

Department of Neurology and Department of Physiotherapy, Speech Pathology and Occupational Therapy, University of São Paulo School of Medicine, São Paulo SP Brazil (FMUSP)

IMD, PhD, Neurologist

IIPhonoaudiologist, Neurolinguistic Specialist

IIIPhonoaudiologist, Master and PhD in Linguistics

IVPsychologist, Master in Experimental Pathophysiology

VMD, PhD, Full Professor of Neurology

Address to correspondence Address to correspondence Dra. Márcia Radanovic Rua Cristiano Viana 163 /92 05411-000 São Paulo SP FAX : 55 11 3088 9708 E-mail: radano@tecway.com.br

ABSTRACT

Subcortical structures are in a strategic functional position within the cognitive networks. Their lesion can interfere with a great number of functions. We studied six patients with thalamic vascular lesions (three left sided, two right sided and one bilateral), to characterize their repercussion in the communicative abilities and the interface between language alterations and other cognitive abilities, as attention, memory and frontal executive. All patients were evaluated through a functional interview (discourse analysis), and the following batteries: Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, Boston Naming Test, Token Test, Benton Visual Retention Test, Trail Making, Wisconsin Card Sorting and frontal scripts. All patients performed MRI and five underwent SPECT. Results show that these patients present impairment in several cognitive domains, especially attention and executive functions (working memory, planning and self-monitoring); those with right lesions have an additional visuospatial impairment. Such alterations interfere with language abilities, and this fact must be considered in the rehabilitation efforts.

Keywords: thalamus, language, attention, memory, prefrontal cortex.

RESUMO

As estruturas subcorticais ocupam posições funcionais estratégicas nas redes cognitivas. Sua lesão pode interferir com grande número de funções. Estudamos seis pacientes com lesão vascular talâmica (três à esqueda, dois à direita e um bilateral), para caracterizar a repercussão da lesão nas suas habilidades comunicativas e a interface entre alterações de linguagem e outras habilidades cognitivas, como atenção, memória e executivas frontais. Os pacientes foram avaliados através de entrevista funcional (análise do discurso), testes de Boston para Diagnóstico da Afasia, Nomeação Boston, Token, Múltipla Escolha de Benton, Trail Making, Wisconsin Card Sorting e scripts frontais. Todos os pacientes realizaram ressonância magnética encefálica (RM) e cinco realizaram SPECT. Os resultados mostram que os pacientes apresentam prejuízo em diversos domínios cognitivos, especialmente atenção e funções executivas (memória operacional, planejamento e automonitoração); nos lesados à direita, ocorre prejuízo visuoespacial associado. Tais alterações repercutem nas habilidades de linguagem, o que deve ser levado em conta para reabilitação.

Palavras-chave: tálamo, linguagem, atenção, memória, córtex pré-frontal.

The study of the role of subcortical structures in cognitive processes, largely impelled by neuroimaging development has brought interest in the frontostriatal circuits and the thalamus, and their participation in the linguistic processing. Fisher, in 19591, was the first author to describe aphasia in left thalamic hemorrhagic lesions. Aphasia with decreased speech output (although fluent), anomia, paraphasias (that could deteriorate into jargon), comprehension less affected than production, and no repetition problems (or minimum) are described following ventrolateral (VL) and ventral anterior (VA) nuclei lesions; a fluent aphasia, with non-words, may occur especially in lesions of the pulvinar and the posterior lateral (PL) nucleus2. Left dorsomedial (DM) lesions are related to anomia, verbal memory deficits and a frontal syndrome3,4. Thalamic aphasia is more frequently found in lesions of the dominant tuberothalamic (polar) and interpeduncular profunda (paramedian thalamic) arteries, and since they are small vessels, there is a low probability that some associated cortical dysfunction may exist5. Thalamic lesions may provoke an association of disorders: disfluency and conditions similar to transcortical aphasia in lesions involving the left VL, pulvinar and paramedian nuclei6,7 or leading to a disconnection between the frontal area – VA; arousal disturbances (intrusions, contaminations, non-words); comprehension alterations, because of interruption in thalamus - frontal area - superior longitudinal fascicle - Wernicke's area connections; hypophonia, by interruption of pallido-nigral afferents to the thalamus8.

The attempts to explain the mechanisms by which thalamic lesions lead to language disturbances are based upon theories such as cortical activation and integration, modulation and monitoration of the formulated language9-12. Some authors hypothesized about the integrative circuits involving the cerebral cortex, the thalamus and the brainstem, especially the ascending reticular activating system, with the participation of verbal memory13-15. Cappa and Vignolo16 attributed the handling of words as semantic units to the thalamus, excluding its participation in phonemic processes, which could explain the few repetition disturbances in subjects with thalamic aphasia. Nadeau and Crosson17 suggested that the mediothalamic-frontocortical system (including the frontal lobes – inferior thalamic peduncle – nucleus reticularis and centromedian nucleus) could participate in the control of lexical selection processes, its failure leading to a loss of any difference in the activation among a certain lexical item and others semantically related, thus resulting in anomia and semantic paraphasias.

The interaction between language and other cognitive domains is an important factor to be considered, especially in lesions affecting structures that have extensive reciprocal connections with the cerebral cortex, such as the thalamic nuclei. Based on a preliminary study18, we noted a great interference of factors such as memory and attention in the linguistic performance of patients with thalamic lesions. Damage of short-term memory (phonological loop) can be expressed clinically by sentence comprehension deficits19, either due to the absence of a phonological memory and/or semantic or syntactic memory. Likewise, the short-term memory deficit may impair sentence planning, leading to verbal production and vocabulary acquisition disturbances20. Primary attentional disorders can also disturb linguistic processing. Attention deficits can occur in frontal lesions, and in these, we frequently find verbal fluency alterations, compromising speech initiative in left hemisphere lesions, and of pragmatic aspects of speech in right hemisphere lesions21. Neglect dyslexia also was described following thalamic lesions17.

The aphasias caused by subcortical lesion usually escape the possibility of classification and the use of linguistic tests alone may not be sufficient to identify the language disturbance, which can lead to therapeutic inefficacy. Our purpose in this study was to describe the profile of language disturbances found after right and left thalamic lesions, and to verify the interrelation between these findings and other cognitive impairments, such as attention, memory and executive functions.

METHOD

We studied six patients with vascular thalamic lesions; two patients had right-sided lesions, three had left-sided lesions and in one there were bilateral lesions. All patients were right-handed and Portuguese native speakers. The selection of cases was based on anatomical features (site of lesion in neuroimaging performed when the patient was admitted in the hospital). Patient 6, although presenting bilateral lesion, was included when he suffered a right thalamic stroke, and began to demonstrate the described clinical picture. Exclusion criteria were: delay in language acquisition, antecedents of psychiatric and/or neurological disease, alcoholism, hearing deficiency, associated cortical involvement (based on clinical history and neurological examination). Demographic data are shown in Table 1; neuroimaging features are shown in Table 1 and Figure 1.


All the participants of this study signed an agreement form that was included in the project approved by the Commission of Ethics for Analysis of Research Projects - Hospital das Clínicas – FMUSP.

Instruments

The language evaluation was performed using a functional interview (narrative, argumentative and procedural discourses), the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (including the Boston Naming Test)22 and the Token Test23. These tests evaluate the phonological, morphological, syntactic and semantic aspects that are present in the processes of language comprehension (word discrimination, commands and text interpretation) and language production (naming, repetition of words and phrases, text production). They take into account the effects related to the modalities of stimuli input and answer output and also those task-specific, in order to control linguistic and non-linguistic variables. The Brazilian reference values to BDAE and Token Test were based on previous studies conducted by the authors24,25.

An additional test of frontal scripts, translated into Portuguese from Allain's et al. original publication26, was applied in four patients (two with left lesion and two with right lesion) and nine normal individuals, for comparison (normal subjects had an average of 2.6 ordination mistakes and no intrusions). This task investigates the abilities of information recovery using common action sequences and it is sensitive to frontal dysfunction. The subjects are asked to establish the hierarchical organization of six scripts (each one containing from 10 to 17 actions) obeying to the same temporal sequence in which they are usually executed. The actions of each script are presented in mixed cards that must be organized by the subject. The title of each script is presented in a separated card, remaining accessible during the task execution. In the three last scripts some distractors elements are presented, containing actions that do not belong to the general plan and that must be excluded. The ordination and intrusion mistakes are, then, counted. Six scripts were applied in our patients ("Going to the supermarket", "Making a cake", "Going to the movies", "Going to a wedding", "Going to a restaurant", "Taking the subway"), totalizing 76 actions. Patient 3, who had moderate comprehension deficit, and Patient 4, who was illiterate, could not perform this test. The language results are shown in Tables 2 and 3.

Attention, memory and executive functions were evaluated through the Benton Visual Retention (BVRT), Trail Making and Wisconsin Card Sorting (WCST) tests. The BVRT consists of several series of line drawings that must be reproduced by the patient, and it was designed to assess visual perception and short-term visual memory27. The Trail Making test measures visual skills (searching and sequencing) and the ability to make alternating conceptual shifts, as it requires the patient to draw a line connecting 25 circles in a certain order, as rapidly and accurately as possible28. The WCST assesses abstract ability, conceptual set shifting and the capacity of "learning", as the subject is asked to sort a series of printed cards according to criteria that are changed without previous notice (from colors to forms, numbers, etc)29. In the neuropsychological tests, we adopted a performance of 90% as a cut-off score for normality, because there are no standardized values for the Brazilian population, especially low-educated. All patients were able to comprehend the instructions for test accomplishment. The clinical evaluations were performed within a minimum interval of six weeks after symptom installation. All patients performed SPECT and cranial MRI, after a minimum interval of two months from onset of symptoms.

The patients were tested by a speech therapist and a neuropsychologist, both experienced, according to the procedures recommended by Lezak30,31. Their language and neurological evaluation were reviewed and analyzed by two authors (respectively LLM and MR).

RESULTS

Language: Five patients persistently complained of memory and word finding difficulties that interfered in their daily activities (exception: Patient 5).

Left lesions

Patient 1: This patient presented moderate comprehension deficit in more complex linguistics tasks (Complex Ideational Material and Reading Sentences and Paragraphs in the BDAE and especially in the fourth and fifth blocks in the Token Test), semantic paraphasias and anomia. This patient had difficulties in changing semantic categories and sequences in formal tests (from letters to numbers, days of the week to months), showing perseverations; her performance in the animal fluency was reduced. In the scripts, she had two ordination mistakes and no intrusions. During the examination, she often complained that she could not remember the instructions or that she forgot the original stimulus while searching for the correct answer. Besides, she was disorganized and inattentive during the search for answers in reading tasks, having difficulties to choose just one answer.

Patient 2 had a normal performance in the formal tests and made no mistakes in the frontal scripts.

Patient 3 showed moderate to severe comprehension, repetition and naming deficits, with phonemic paraphasias. His reading and writing were also defective.

Right lesions

Patient 4 had interpretation difficulties, emphasizing descriptive aspects in the Cookie Theft narrative. In the scripts he had 20 ordination mistakes and no intrusions.

Patient 5 had a slight anomia for visual presented stimuli, bad performance in all blocks of the Token test and severe difficulties in the integration of the several set elements in the Cookie Theft picture. Although these impairments are difficult to interpret (the patient is illiterate), we consider them as relevant because the patient had subjective complaints in these specific skills, and we could observe anomia in spontaneous speech.

Patient 6: This patient showed scores below normal for repetition, related to sentence extension, and Visual Confrontation Naming, as well as deficits in narrative organization, with digressions and perseverations. In the Token test, he showed bad performance in the fourth and fifth blocks. In the scripts, he had 21 ordination mistakes and 4 intrusions.

Neuropsychological examination: All six patients had performances below normal in the attention and memory tests. All of them, except case two, presented performance below normal in tests of executive functions. The group with right lesions had worse performance in the visual discrimination test. The results are exposed in Table 4.

DISCUSSION

The interaction between language and other cognitive functions is receiving greater attention by researchers in Neuropsychology. Subcortical lesions can interfere with cortical networks in several ways, due to the complexity of cortico-striato-thalamic-cortical loops and also because small changes in the lesion sites can completely change the connections affected and their functional repercussion on the cerebral cortex (through deafferentation mechanisms)4,12,32,33. Language alterations in subcortical lesions are often mild, although the patients persistently complain of naming difficulties or that "the thoughts are confused". These deficits are rarely as disabling as those found in cortical lesions (unless the patient has some professional activity which requires a higher level of cognitive/verbal abilities: academic students, lawyers), but they have a significant impact in the subjects' quality of life. We will try to describe the relation between language alterations and the attention, memory and executive function disorders found in our patients.

The comprehension deficits in thalamic aphasias are mild to moderate, and the written comprehension tends to be more preserved than the oral comprehension16,34. Verbal memory alterations can contribute to the low scores in these tasks35. The concomitant attention and memory deficits found in cases 1 and 3 suggest that these functions can be implied in the comprehension impairment, as we note a worsening of performance related to the stimulus extension and syntactic complexity (both processes are linked to working memory capacity). Patient 1 also presented some findings that suggest an additional deficit of attention, mnesic and planning/monitoring abilities such as impairment in changing categories and sequences (from days of the week to months, for example) with perseverations, reduction in animal fluency, difficulties in scanning strategies and concentration. Furthermore, her complaint that she could not remember the task or the stimuli presented also point to attention and working memory deficits. It is interesting to notice that Patient 2, who did not present any dysfunction in formal tests, had attention and memory alterations, but not of executive dysfunction. Although she performed well, she showed a great delay in answers. This patient complained that she could not talk and write as she did before (especially names), and that "when I read anything long, I do not remember the beginning", being unable to attend her college courses. Her complaint supports the existence of a working memory alteration.

Repetition disturbances were found in Patient 3 and in Patient 6, who had bilateral lesions. It is our impression that attention and working memory dysfunction can also explain these repetition alterations, if we consider that there is a correlation between the decline of repetition capacity and increases in sentence length. Perseverations were found in patients 1 and 6. Their existence in thalamic aphasia is considered as related to attention and vigilance mechanisms11,15, and may be present in VA lesions8.

Paraphasias were found in Patients 1 and 3. Patient 1 had semantic paraphasias, especially in naming tests, where she benefited from phonemic cues. Patient 3 showed phonemic paraphasias. These patients also presented anomia. These findings sustain the most consistent theoretical model about the thalamus role as a "floodgate", responsible for the best lexical selection12,17, with the participation of attention and working memory36. A category-specific anomia37 and proper name anomia38 were already found after dominant thalamic lesions. The difficulties evidenced in the Token Test also point to a working memory deficit, as they become more evident in those blocks which demand larger mnesic span and the analysis of more extensive sentences.

Patients with right lesions were different from those with left lesions, as a group, as they had impairment in visuospatial, discursive and script tasks. In Patient 5, the visuospatial alteration led to low scores in Visual Confrontation Naming and in the Token Test associated to an inability to adequately distinguish the elements in the Cookie Theft picture. These impairments are difficult to interpret because this patient is illiterate, and there are not reference values for Brazilian illiterates in most neuropsychological tests. In patient 6, the Visual Confrontation Naming task was impaired too. These patients with right thalamic damage were those who had the worst performances in the Benton visual discrimination test.

The ability to elaborate a discourse macrostructure and to integrate information in a context are impaired in patients with right hemisphere lesions, who present difficulties in extracting sentence meaning, discarding irrelevant information and integrating the apprehended meaning within a coherent whole, in order to generate an organized narrative. Contrarily, they have a tendency to fix on individual, isolated and/or less important elements, without performing a connection between them to form a whole39. The coherent discourse comprehension, in MRI studies, provokes larger activation in the right frontal lobe than the reading of non-related sentences40.

The impact of the executive system dysfunction in language is a complex issue. Alexander et al., in 198921, tried to correlate language deficits and frontal lesions. They considered four levels in their theoretic model: motor, linguistic-cognitive, activation and formulation, the latter being the most important in our discussion. Thus, in left frontal lesions (especially prefrontal and those in the posterior lateral convexity), the formulation impairment reflects an inability in language structuration and organization (when the subject tries to deal with elements that are more complex or abstract) and of conceptual association. In the right frontal lesions (also prefrontal or of its projections to the posterior convexity), formulation difficulties are more contextual, as goal selection and self-monitoring, leading to tangenciality, inadequate topic changes, improper discourse and confabulation. Frontal symptoms like concrete thinking, deficiency in behavior planning and monitoring, perseveration, apathy and impairment in conceptualization can certainly compromise the discursive ability, generating problems with its macrostructure, organization, cohesion, and inferences as those found in our cases. The attention and working memory deficits can also be inserted in the context of frontal alterations.

An interesting finding was the difference in performance between individuals with left and right lesions in the frontal scripts. According to the cognitive theory, scripts are a class of organized structures that are stored as knowledge nets representing events, stories and frequent activities41. Thus, the frontal dysexecutive syndrome also includes impairment in the temporal and sequential processing of this pre-existing mental plan42, although the exact nature of this storage process is still unknown. Studies with functional MRI showed bilateral prefrontal dorsolateral activation in tasks involving frontal scripts, thus evidencing the participation of these areas in the analysis of action sequences43.

Most errors in our patients occurred in the actions ordination, and not in the choice of which actions belonged to the scripts. This fact puts into evidence a specific inability in performing the temporal-sequential ordination discussed above. An intriguing fact is the preponderance of this dysfunction in patients with right lesion, since most studies point to the participation of bilateral prefrontal cortices in this task. We can speculate that the great demand for spatial movement in most scripts (going to the supermarket, taking the subway) generated an additional disadvantage for these patients. Patient 2, that did not present any mistakes, also did not have executive dysfunction in WCST. Disorders of thought that are independent of primary language disturbances were described in dominant thalamic lesions, and seem to reflect a primary disorganization that can rely on frontal executive mechanisms44.

Many authors have proposed that the pathophysiological mechanisms underlying subcortical aphasia (and other cognitive symptoms) are associated with cerebral blood flow and metabolism alterations45-48. These abnormalities reflect the remote consequences of focal damage (diaschisis of the ipsilesional cortex) and it may explain why these changes are sometimes transient and their improvement follows the pattern of the cortical reperfusion49,50. Considering that the anterior and medial thalamus project to the frontal cortex and the lateral thalamus projects to posterior cortex47, most thalamic lesions will lead to some cortical involvement in associative areas, and thus provoke cognitive symptoms.

The greatest difficulty we found in our study was to correlate the clinical data with the SPECT findings. Hypoperfusion patterns were heterogeneous even when the patients had similar clinical pictures. Compensatory mechanisms may account for these discrepancies46. Two patients that showed normal perfusion studies (Patients 1 and 6) were symptomatic. Patient 2, whose results in tests were normal, refused to undergo the procedure. In case 3, there was a relative overlapping between the hypoperfusion pattern (left posterior) and his clinical picture; this patient presented the largest lesion, that affected all the thalamus and extended to the adjacent posterior periventricular white matter, suggesting a more severe deafferentation mechanism. In Patient 5, there was diffuse hypoperfusion in the right hemisphere, making any detailed analysis impossible. In Patient 4, there was an apparent contralateral functional repercussion (left posterior parietal), but of little clinical repercussion. We believe that the small number of cases is the main reason for these inconclusive results.

The study of subcortical-induced language alterations provides very complex and diverse types of clinical syndromes, which helps us to understand how other cognitive processes interact with language and vice-versa. Based on our results, we agree with the interpretation that the causes for a loss of efficiency in the language use caused by thalamic lesions are multifactorial and transcend the boundaries of linguistic processing itself7,11,13-15. Lesions in both hemispheres can generate dysfunctions that, although different in nature, become disabling. From an anatomical view, the amnesic syndrome (resembling that caused by the temporal medial lesion) appears more specifically after lesions of the mammillo-thalamic tract (MTT); different memory deficit patterns can occur after lesions of other thalamic structures, even when the MTT is preserved. The dysexecutive syndrome can be found after lesions in DM, midline, intralaminar (IL) or any combination of these nuclei51. As our patients had widespread lesions, frequently in more than one arterial territory, and taking into account the complexity and diversity of the thalamic vascularization, is not possible for us to make precise anatomical correlations. We can speculate that MTT, IL and the ventral portion of DM are damaged in Patients 1 and 2, in the latter possibly existing a VA lesion; in Patients 4, 5 and 6, the posterior lesions predominate, probably in pulvinar, PL, posterior portion of DM and lateral geniculate. Patient 3 had a total thalamic lesion.

In many cases, the alterations objectively found in language batteries can be slight and disproportionate to the patient's complaints. Executive, attention, mnesic and visuospatial dysfunctions, caused by the thalamus insertion in the respective cognitive networks, are elements that contribute to language deficits, and must be adequately evaluated and measured to improve the efficiency of therapeutic approaches.

18. Radanovic M, Scaff M. Speech and language disturbances due to subcortical lesions. Brain Lang, in press.

Received 15 March 2002, received in final form 31 July 2002.

Accepted 20 August 2002.

  • 1. Fisher CM. The pathologic and clinical aspects of thalamic hemorrhage. Trans Am Neurol Assoc 1959;84:56-59.
  • 2. Graff-Radford NR. Syndromes due to acquired thalamic damage. In Feinberg TE, Farah MJ (ed.)Behavioral neurology and neuropsychology. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1997:433-443.
  • 3. Sandson TA, Daffner KR, Carvalho PA, Mesulam M-M. Frontal lobe dysfuntion following infarction of the left-sided medial thalamus. Arch Neurol 1991;48:1300-1303.
  • 4. Mesulam M-M. Behavioral neuroanatomy. In Mesulam M-M (ed). Principles of behavioral and cognitive neurology 2.Ed. New York: Oxford Univ Press, 2000:1-120.
  • 5. Crosson B, Nadeau SE. The role of subcortical structures in linguistic processes. recent developments. In Stemmer B, Whitaker HA (eds.) Handbook of neurolinguistics. San Diego: Academic Press, 1998:431-445.
  • 6. Riklan M, Cooper IS. Psychometric studies of verbal functions following thalamic lesions in humans. Brain Lang 1975;2:45-64.
  • 7. Lazzarino LG, Nicolai A, Valassi F Biasizzo E. Language disturbances from mesencephalo-thalamic infarcts: identification of thalamic nuclei by CT-reconstruction. Neuroradiology 1991;33:300-304.
  • 8. Gorelick PB, Hier DB, Benevento L, Levitt S, Tan W. Aphasia after left thalamic infarction. Arch Neurol 1984;41:1296-1298.
  • 9. Samra K, Riklan M, Levita E, et al. Language and speech correlates of anatomically verified lesions in thalamic surgery for parkinsonism. J Speech Hear Res 1969;12:510-540.
  • 10. Darley FL, Brown JR, Swenson WM. Language changes after neurosurgery for parkinsonism. Brain Lang 1975;2:65-69.
  • 11. Luria AR On quasi-aphasic speech disturbances in lesions of the deep structures of the brain. Brain Lang 1977; 4: 432-459.
  • 12. Wallesch C-W, Papagno C. Subcortical aphasia. In Rose FC, Whurr R, Wyke MA (ed) Aphasia. London: Whurr 1988: 256-287.
  • 13. Reynolds AF, Turner PT, Harris AB, Ojemann GA, Davis LE. Left thalamic hemorrhage with dysphasia: a report of five cases. Brain Lang 1979;7:62-73.
  • 14. Metter EJ, Riege WH, Hanson WR, et al. Comparison of metabolic rates, language, and memory in subcortical aphasias. Brain Lang 1983;19: 33-47.
  • 15. Ojemann G. Brain organization for language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping. Behav Brain Sci 1983;6:189-230.
  • 16. Cappa SF, Vignolo LA. "Transcortical " features of aphasia following left thalamic hemorrhage. Cortex 1979;15:121-130.
  • 17. Nadeau SE, Crosson B. Subcortical aphasia. Brain Lang 1997;58: 355-402.
  • 19. Vallar G, Baddeley AD. Fractionation of working memory: neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-term store. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 1984;23:151-161.
  • 20. Baddeley A, Gathercole S, Papagno C. The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychol Rev 1998;105:158-173.
  • 21. Alexander MP, Benson DF, Stuss DT. Frontal lobes and language. Brain Lang 1989;37:656-691.
  • 22. Goodglass H, Kaplan E. The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. 2 .Ed. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger, 1983.
  • 23. DeRenzi E, Vignolo LA. The Token Test: a sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. Brain 1962;85:665-678.
  • 24. Radanovic M, Mansur LL. Performance of a Brazilian population sample in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: a pilot study. Braz J Med Biol Res 2002;35:305-317.
  • 25. Radanovic M. Disturbances of speech and language in subcortical lesions.Thesis. São Paulo University School of Medicine. São Paulo, 1999.
  • 26. Allain P, Le Gall D, Etcharry-Bouyx F, Aubin G, Emile J. Mental representation of knowledge following frontal-lobe lesion: dissociations on tasks using scripts. J Clin Exper Neuropsychol 1999;21:643-665.
  • 27. Benton AL. he revised visual retention test. 4.Ed. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1974.
  • 28. Reitan RM. Validity of the Trail-Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept Mot Skills 1958;8:271-276.
  • 29. Grant DA, Berg E. The Wisconsin Card Sort Test Random Layout: directions for administration and scoring. Madison, Wisconsin: Wells Printing, 1980.
  • 30. Lezak MD. The neuropsychological examination: procedures. In Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assesment. 3.Ed. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press, 1995:110-143.
  • 31. Lezak MD. The neuropsychological examination: interpretation. In Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assesment. 3.Ed. Oxford: Oxford Univ Press, 1995:144-169.
  • 32. Naeser MA, Palumbo CL, Helm-Estabrooks N, Stiassny-Eder D, Albert ML. Severe nonfluency in aphasia. Brain 1989;112:1-38.
  • 33. Alexander MP. Speech and language deficits after subcortical lesions of the left hemisphere: a clinical, CT, and PET study. In Vallar G (ed). Neuropsychological disorders associated with subcortical lesions. New York: Oxford Univ Press, 1992:455-477.
  • 34. Graff-Radford NR, Eslinger PJ, Damasio AR, Yamada T. Nonhemorrhagic infarction of the thalamus: behavioral, anatomic, and physiologic correlates. Neurology 1984;34:14-23.
  • 35. Puel M, Cardebat D, Demonet JF, et al. Le role du thalamus dans les aphasies sous-corticales. Rev Neurol (Paris) 1986;142:431-440.
  • 36. Crosson B. Subcortical mechanisms in language: lexical-semantic mechanisms and the thalamus. Brain Cogn 1999;40:414-438.
  • 37. Crosson B, Moberg PJ, Boone JR, Rothi LJG, Raymer A. Category-specific naming deficit for medical terms after dominant thalamic/capsular hemorrhage. Brain and Lang 1997;60:407-442.
  • 38. Lucchelli F, DeRenzi E. Proper name anomia. Cortex 1992; 28: 221-230.
  • 39. Joanette Y, Goulet P, Hannequin D. Alterações da comunicação verbal nos destros com lesões cerebrais no hemisfério direito. In Mansur LL, Rodrigues N (eds.), Temas em neurolingüística. São Paulo: Tec Art, 1993:23-30.
  • 40. Kircher TT, Brammer M, Tous Andreu N, Williams SC, McGuire PK. Engagement of right temporal cortex during processing of linguistic context. Neuropsychologia 2001;39:798-809.
  • 41. Schank R, Abelson P. Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, 1977.
  • 42. Shallice T, Burgess PW. Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain 1991;114:727-741.
  • 43. Crozier S, Sirigu A, Lehéricy S, van der Moortele P-F, Pillon B, Grafman J et al. Distinct prefrontal activations in processing sequence at the sentence and script level: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 1999;37: 1469-1476.
  • 44. Chatterjee A, Yapundich R, Mennemeier M, et al. Thalamic thought disorder: on being " a bit addled". Cortex 1997;33:419-440.
  • 45. Perani D, Vallar G, Cappa S, Messa C, Fazio F. Aphasia and neglect after subcortical stroke. Brain 1987;110:1211-1229.
  • 46. Démonet JF, Puel M, Celsis P, Cardebat D. "Subcortical" aphasia: some proposed pathophysiological mechanisms and their rCBF correlates revealed by SPECT. J Neuroling 1991;6:319-344.
  • 47. Metter EJ. Role of subcortical structures in aphasia: evidence from studies of resting cerebral glucose metabolism. In Vallar G. Neuropsychological disorders associated with subcortical lesions. New York: Oxford Univ Press, 1992:478-500.
  • 48. Okuda B, Tanaka H, Tachibana H, Kawabata K, Sugita M. Cerebral blood flow in subcortical global aphasia: perisylvian cortical hypoperfusion as a crucial role. Stroke 1994;25:1495-1499.
  • 49. Olsen TS, Bruhn P, Öberg RGE. Cortical hypoperfusion as a possible cause of "subcortical aphasia". Brain 1986;109:393-410.
  • 50. Vallar G, Perani D, Cappa SF, Messa C, Lenzi GL, Fazio F. Recovery from aphasia and neglect after subcortical stroke: neuropsychological and cerebral perfusion study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988; 51:1269-1276.
  • 51. Van der Werf YD, Witter MP, Uylings HBM, Jolles J. Neuropsychology of infarctions in the thalamus: a review. Neuropsychologia 2000;38: 613-627.
  • Address to correspondence
    Dra. Márcia Radanovic
    Rua Cristiano Viana 163 /92
    05411-000 São Paulo SP
    FAX : 55 11 3088 9708
    E-mail:
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      16 Apr 2003
    • Date of issue
      Mar 2003

    History

    • Received
      15 Mar 2002
    • Accepted
      20 Aug 2002
    Academia Brasileira de Neurologia - ABNEURO R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices Torre Norte, 04101-000 São Paulo SP Brazil, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: revista.arquivos@abneuro.org