Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Longitudinal analysis of verbal episodic memory in patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis

Análise longitudinal da memória episódica verbal em pacientes com esclerose múltipla remitente-recorrente

ABSTRACT

Objective:

A 4.5-year follow-up study was conducted to characterize baseline verbal episodic memory (VEM) and its behavior and to assess the effects of relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis (RRMS) on this domain.

Methods:

Twenty-nine patients with RRMS underwent two neuropsychological assessments performed an average of 4.5 years apart. Twenty-six control participants underwent a single neuropsychological assessment. A significance level of p < 0.005 was adopted to denote a significant difference between the groups on the Mann Whitney and Wilcoxon paired statistical analyses.

Results:

No statistical difference was found in the results of the VEM tests between the first and second neuropsychological assessments of the patients. However, a statistical difference was evident between the patient and control groups in the results of the VEM tests.

Conclusion:

The patient group showed changes in the VEM relative to the control group. After approximately 4.5 years of disease, the patient performance on the VEM stabilized or improved.

Keywords:
multiple sclerosis; cognition; longitudinal studies

RESUMO

Objetivo:

Neste estudo, propomos a caracterização da Memória Episódica Verbal (MEV) basal e o seu comportamento após o período de 4,5 anos de doença, a fim de avaliar o efeito da EMRR neste domínio.

Métodos:

Vinte e nove pacientes com EMRR foram submetidos a duas avaliações neuropsicológicas realizadas entre um intervalo de tempo médio de 4,5 anos. Vinte e seis controles foram submetidos à avaliação neuropsicológica única. Considerou-se nível de significância p <0,005 para delinear diferença significante entre os grupos nas análises estatísticas Mann Whitney e Wilcoxon pareado.

Resultados:

Não houve diferença estatística nos resultados dos testes de MEV entre a primeira e segunda avaliação neuropsicológica realizada pelos pacientes. Houve discrepância estatística nos resultados dos testes de MEV entre o grupo dos pacientes e controles.

Conclusão:

O grupo de pacientes apresentou alterações de MEV quando comparado aos controles. Após 4,5 anos aproximadamente os pacientes estabilizaram ou melhoraram seu desempenho em MEV.

Palavras chave:
esclerose múltipla; cognição; estudos longitudinais

Patients with multiple sclerosis (MS) clinically present with a myriad of neurologic symptoms, including cognitive decline, regarded as having the greatest impact on key aspects of their daily living, such as managing domestic tasks, participating in society and holding down a job11. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 2008 Dec;7(12):1139-51. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70...
.

According to reviews of the scientific literature, one third of MS patients with the relapsing-remitting (RRMS) clinical form show cognitive impairments that are often milder than that found in the progressive subtypes of the disease22. Jongen PJ, Ter Horst AT, Brands AM. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Minerva Med. 2012 Apr;103(2):73-96.,33. Amato MP, Zipoli V, Portaccio E. Multiple sclerosis-related cognitive changes: a review of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. J Neurol Sci. 2006 Jun;245(1-2):41-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.08.019
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.08.01...
.

Cognitive impairments can vary, but an impact on episodic memory is evident in the early stages of the disease. A 10-year longitudinal study revealed that decline in information processing speed and verbal episodic memory (VEM) in early RRMS patients predicted progression to the secondary progressive MS clinical form. Given that secondary progressive MS is a more severe stage of the disease, assessing these cognitive domains in the early stages of MS is vital44. Moccia M, Lanzillo R, Palladino R, Chang KC, Costabile T, Russo C et al. Cognitive impairment at diagnosis predicts 10-year multiple sclerosis progression. Mult Scler. 2016 Apr;22(5):659-67. https://doi.org/10.1177/1352458515599075
https://doi.org/10.1177/1352458515599075...
.

A scientific review of the related literature11. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 2008 Dec;7(12):1139-51. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70...
reported that episodic memory is one of the most common deficits found in MS, occurring in 40–65% of patients. Evidence on the nature of the episodic memory disorder in MS patients is conflicting. Some authors hold that episodic memory problems are characterized by impairment in the retrieval of learned information over the course of time. Others have shown reduced assimilation of new information in patients relative to controls, yet once the information has been acquired, delayed recall and recognition ability in patients had proven to be similar to those of healthy controls55. Brissart H, Morele E, Baumann C, Debouverie M. Verbal episodic memory in 426 multiple sclerosis patients: impairment in encoding, retrieval or both? Neurol Sci. 2012 Oct;33(5):1117-23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-...
.

Numerous cross-sectional studies investigating the prevalence and pattern of cognitive dysfunction in MS are available in the literature. However, there is a dearth of studies on the evolution of the disease, from a cognitive viewpoint, over the course of time. The results of longitudinal studies in the literature are conflicting, reporting cognitive improvement, stability or decline in patients over time66. Strober LB, Rao SM, Lee JC, Fischer E, Rudick R. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis: An 18 year follow-up study. Mult Scler Relat Disord. 2014 Jul;3(4):473-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2014.03.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2014.03....
.

The objective of this study was to conduct a longitudinal assessment of VEM at two time points, consisting of a baseline assessment and another at 4.5 years of disease later, to assess the impact of RRMS on the VEM over this period. In order to characterize the VEM in RRMS patients at baseline, results were compared against those of the healthy controls. The characterization of sociodemographic and clinical variables, as well as correlation between attention and executive functions and VEM, were analyzed at the baseline assessment.

METHODS

Participants

The study sample comprised 29 patients with RRMS (19 women and 10 men), whereas the control group comprised 26 healthy subjects (17 women and nine men). Patients were recruited from the clinic for demyelinating diseases of the Department of Neurology of the Clínicas Hospital of the São Paulo University School of Medicine.

The study sample included patients diagnosed with RRMS based on the McDonald criteria (2010)77. Polman CH, Reingold SC, Banwell B, Clanet M, Cohen JA, Filippi M et al. Diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: 2010 revisions to the McDonald criteria. Ann Neurol. 2011 Feb;69(2):292-302. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.22366
https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.22366...
. All patients underwent an initial cognitive assessment to establish a baseline. In addition, only patients with an IQ within or above the mean expected IQ for age (mean = 106.60; SD = 7.65) were included, on the premise that patients of lower intelligence may have worse cognitive performance. Patient IQ was determined at baseline using the Vocabulary and Matrix Reasoning subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-III)88. Ringe WK, Saine KC, Lacritz LH, Hynan LS, Cullum CM. Dyadic short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III. Assessment. 2002 Sep;9(3):254-60. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191102009003004
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191102009003...
.

The exclusion criteria included: patients who had evolved to progressive clinical subtypes, patients with deficits that prevented assessment, patients with other central nervous system disease, patients with relapse less than 30 days before the study, patients using corticosteroids for the last 90 days before the examination, patients with a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score below the cut-off. Owing to the lack of normative MMSE data for the Brazilian population for the age stratum of the present sample, cut-off scores for the educational level were employed99. Brucki SM, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bertolucci PH, Okamoto IH. [Suggestions for utilization of the mini-mental state examination in Brazil]. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2003 Sep;61(3B):777-81. Portuguese. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2003000500014
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X200300...
.

The control group was formed by selecting healthy participants matched for age, educational level and gender with the patients in the study group. Volunteers using psychoactive or neuroleptic medications or with a history of alcohol or illegal drug abuse were excluded. Control participants scoring below the cut-off on the MMSE for educational level99. Brucki SM, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bertolucci PH, Okamoto IH. [Suggestions for utilization of the mini-mental state examination in Brazil]. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2003 Sep;61(3B):777-81. Portuguese. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2003000500014
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X200300...
and above the cut-off on the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (8 for symptoms of anxiety and 9 for depression) were excluded1010. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983 Jun;67(6):361-70. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983...
.

Instruments and procedures

The patients underwent a comprehensive cognitive assessment, which included specific tests of VEM such as the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test1111. Brandt JB, Benedict RH. Hopkins verbal learning test-revised: professional manual. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resource; 2001. and the Logical Memory subtest of the Memory Wechsler Scale1212. Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale. (WMS-III). 3rd ed. New York: The Psychological Corporation; 1999. (Table 1). The same protocol was used for the assessments at the study baseline and endpoint.

Table 1
Neuropsychological instruments used and cognitive domains assessed.

The interval between the two neuropsychological assessments applied to the patient group ranged from 3.1 to 5.7 years with a mean and standard deviation of 4.5 (0.7) years.

The mood assessment of the patients was carried out at both baseline and follow-up using the Beck Depression Inventory1313. Gorenstein C, Wang YP, Argimon IL, Werlang BS. Manual do inventário de depressão de Beck - BDI-II. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2011. and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale1010. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983 Jun;67(6):361-70. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983...
.

Patients, specifically, underwent the neurologic examination at two time points to identify the degree of physical disability by completing the Expanded Disability Status Scale1414. Kurtzke JF. Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: an expanded disability status scale (EDSS). Neurology. 1983 Nov;33(11):1444-52. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.33.11.1444
https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.33.11.1444...
applied by the neurologist.

The healthy participants from the control group were given a single neuropsychological assessment using the same instrument that was applied to the patient group (Table 1).

Additional clinical data for the patients were drawn directly from medical records held at the neurology clinic of the Clínicas Hospital (Hospital das Clínicas) of the São Paulo University School of Medicine. All patients were using disease modifying therapies, although there were some changes and interruptions during the follow-up period (non-adherence to treatment) for different reasons such as the presence of collateral effects, pregnancy and due to patients seeking alternative treatment.

All study participants signed the free and informed consent form when asked to undergo the examinations.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were carried out using the SPSS V20 for Windows 8.1 software package. Cognitive data extracted from the sample were expressed as mean, standard deviation, median and measures of spread. Raw scores were converted into Z-scores to allow comparison of the data. Data on the use of medication by patients were expressed as absolute medians.

Sociodemographic data for the control and patient groups were compared using the Student's t-test.

The normality of cognitive data extracted from the neuropsychological tests of patients was analyzed by the Shapiro-Wilk test. Given that most of the variables exhibited a non-normal distribution, non-parametric statistical tests were applied.

Cognitive data collected from patients at the first and second neuropsychological assessments were analyzed using the Wilcoxon paired statistical test. The Mann Whitney statistical test was used to compare the cognitive data from the baseline neuropsychological assessments of patients versus controls.

Spearman's correlation was used to better characterize patient deficits in VEM at the baseline assessment and to determine a possible relationship or interdependence among the clinical, sociodemographic and physical disability variables of patients for VEM.

The following clinical variables were investigated: disease duration, time since last relapse, number of relapses, use of disease modifying therapies, and use of antidepressants. Sociodemographic variables were: age, education and gender. Lastly, the variable for physical disability was assessed by the EDSS (Expanded Disability Status Scale).

Because executive functioning and attention play a role in the memorization and learning process, instruments assessing these cognitive domains were also correlated with the VEM tests using Spearman's correlation. Variables showing a level of significance of p ≤ 0.05 on this statistical test were included in the covariance analysis. Thus, it was possible to check whether differences between patient and control performances on the VEM test persisted even after controlling for the effects of the attention and executive functioning tests.

RESULTS

Sociodemographic data for the control and patient groups are given in Table 2. There was no statistically significant difference between the groups for age, educational level or gender. Clinical and physical disability data for the patient group at the baseline assessment are given in Table 3. At the follow-up, mean disease duration was 7.25 years (SD = 2.33), time since diagnosis was 5.57 years (SD = 1.85) and the mean number of relapses was 4.10 (SD = 2.63). Twenty-five patients were on a disease modifying drug and 13 out of the 29 patients discontinued this. Ten patients were using an antidepressant at the follow-up.

Table 2
Sociodemographic data of sample of patients and controls.
Table 3
Clinical data of patients at baseline and follow-up.

The results of the longitudinal analysis, shown in Table 4, indicate patient stability in VEM, with statistically significant improvement on the immediate recall of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (p = 0.019) and delayed recall of the Logical Memory test (p = 0.042). In addition, there were statistically significant improvements by patients on the tests assessing sustained attention (Trail Making Test A) and working memory (WAIS-III Digit span), as well as on the semantic verbal fluency (animals) and naming tasks (Boston Naming Test). At the follow-up, patient scores on the MMSE ranged from 27 to 30 points, with a mean and standard deviation of 29.17 (0.84).

Table 4
Data in Z-scores obtained by patients on cognitive tests – baseline vs. follow-up.

The results of the baseline assessment of control participants were statistically better than those of the patients. The patients had worse performance than the controls on the VEM tests for the immediate recall task of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (p = 0.001; p = 0.006) and for the delayed recall of the Logical Memory test (p = 0.013; p = 0.003). The statistically significant poorer performances of patients also occurred in the attention, semantic verbal fluency and naming processes. Comparison of quantitative results for the sample of patients and controls on the neuropsychological test battery is given in Table 5.

Table 5
Cognitive data at baseline for patient vs.control groups.

Also, regarding the VEM, no statistically significant difference in the recognition phase of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test was evident on comparison of the patient and control groups, or comparison of the longitudinal performance of patients.

Spearman's analysis revealed a strong positive correlation between attention and executive tasks with VEM tasks, suggesting that mnemonic impairments were accompanied by attentional and executive deficits (Table 6). The executive function tests exhibiting this correlation were the Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (assessing strategy and mental flexibility) and the Letter-Number Sequencing and Digit Span subtests of the WAIS-III (both assessing verbal working memory). One attention test (Symbol Digit Modalities Test) correlated with the VEM tests. The Symbol Digit Modalities Test provides a more accurate assessment of information processing speed.

Table 6
Result of analysis of covariance between patient groups baseline vc controls corrected by neuropsychological tests Digits, SNL, SDMT and MWCST.

On the analysis of covariance using the attention and executive function tests cited above (Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Letter-Number Sequencing, Digit Span, and Symbol Digit Modalities Test), the patient group at baseline differed from the control group on the VEM tests even when including the attention and executive functioning measures (Table 7).

Table 7
Impact of the functioning of the other cognitive areas on the verbal episodic memory of the patients in the baseline

No correlation was found between VEM and sociodemographic status or clinical and physical disability data for patients at baseline or follow-up.

With regard to mood, patients in the sample had mean scores below the cut-off point on the scales assessing this parameter at both baseline and follow-up.

At the baseline, only three patients (3%) had mild depression while the remainder were classified as having minimal or nonsignificant depression with a mean and standard deviation of 6.68 (5.22), where a score of up to 10 points defines minimal depression/nonsignificant.

At the follow-up, the mean score for the sample was below the cut-off point for mood, i.e. no significant symptoms of anxiety (mean = 7.14, SD = 3.20) or depression (mean = 5.15, SD = 3.04).

DISCUSSION

In the present study, the group of RRMS patients showed VEM deficits at the baseline assessment compared to the group of healthy controls. These changes were characterized by poor patient performance on encoding (immediate recall) and retrieval (delayed recall) of information on VEM tasks. The groups did not differ statistically on the recognition stage assessing storage. According to a systematic review on the subject11. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 2008 Dec;7(12):1139-51. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70...
, this result can be expected for the neuropsychological profile of MS, but the nature of decline in episodic memory, however, is controversial.

Chiaravalloti e DeLuca11. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 2008 Dec;7(12):1139-51. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70...
discussed whether the nature of VEM impairment is characterized by ineffective performance at the stages of information encoding, retrieval or both these stages55. Brissart H, Morele E, Baumann C, Debouverie M. Verbal episodic memory in 426 multiple sclerosis patients: impairment in encoding, retrieval or both? Neurol Sci. 2012 Oct;33(5):1117-23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-...
. There is evidence in the literature showing that impairments in working memory1515. Fuso SF, Callegaro D, Pompéia S, Bueno OF. Working memory impairment in multiple sclerosis relapsing-remitting patients with episodic memory deficits. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2010 Apr;68(2):205-11. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2010000200010
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X201000...
, processing speed11. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 2008 Dec;7(12):1139-51. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70...
, strategies1616. Faglioni P, Bertolani L, Botti C, Merelli E. Verbal learning strategies in patients with multiple sclerosis. Cortex. 2000 Apr;36(2):243-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70527-6
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70...
and resistance to distractibility55. Brissart H, Morele E, Baumann C, Debouverie M. Verbal episodic memory in 426 multiple sclerosis patients: impairment in encoding, retrieval or both? Neurol Sci. 2012 Oct;33(5):1117-23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-...
can negatively impact episodic memory functioning. Other studies have shown deficits in delayed recall, even in patients receiving sufficient help to assimilate the information at the encoding stage, suggesting accelerated forgetting in MS55. Brissart H, Morele E, Baumann C, Debouverie M. Verbal episodic memory in 426 multiple sclerosis patients: impairment in encoding, retrieval or both? Neurol Sci. 2012 Oct;33(5):1117-23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-...
. Based on the current findings of deficits at both the encoding and delayed recall stages, the nature of the episodic memory impairment might be explained in both phases by the brain areas affected55. Brissart H, Morele E, Baumann C, Debouverie M. Verbal episodic memory in 426 multiple sclerosis patients: impairment in encoding, retrieval or both? Neurol Sci. 2012 Oct;33(5):1117-23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-...
.

In the present study, the results on tests assessing processing speed, strategy building and working memory correlated with those of VEM tests at the baseline assessment, thereby corroborating the findings in the literature outlined in the previous paragraph. However, the covariance analysis, even when including the effects of the executive function and attention tests on the VEM tests, revealed that the patient and control groups differed at baseline. This finding may be explained by other attentional and executive processes that impact episodic memory but have yet to be correlated. Another hypothesis is that mnemonic impairments occur independently of attentional and executive processes.

The scores obtained by patients on VEM tasks showed no correlation with sociodemographic status or with clinical and physical disability data of patients at baseline or follow-up. These variables have been the focus of studies to ascertain whether they impact cognitive functioning or otherwise, although a review has shown conflicting results in the literature22. Jongen PJ, Ter Horst AT, Brands AM. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Minerva Med. 2012 Apr;103(2):73-96..

In the longitudinal analysis of the present study, the patients showed stabilization or improvement in VEM performance, corroborating the findings of some longitudinal studies1717. Bergendal G, Fredrikson S, Almkvist O. Selective decline in information processing in subgroups of multiple sclerosis: an 8-year longitudinal study. Eur Neurol. 2007;57(4):193-202. https://doi.org/10.1159/000099158
https://doi.org/10.1159/000099158...
,1818. Jønsson A, Andresen J, Storr L, Tscherning T, Soelberg Sørensen P, Ravnborg M. Cognitive impairment in newly diagnosed multiple sclerosis patients: a 4-year follow-up study. J Neurol Sci. 2006 Jun;245(1-2):77-85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.09.016
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.09.01...
,1919. Schwid SR, Goodman AD, Weinstein A, McDermott MP, Johnson KP; Copaxone Study Group. Cognitive function in relapsing multiple sclerosis: minimal changes in a 10-year clinical trial. J Neurol Sci. 2007 Apr;255(1-2):57-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2007.01.070
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2007.01.07...
,2020. Glanz BI, Healy BC, Hviid LE, Chitnis T, Weiner HL. Cognitive deterioration in patients with early multiple sclerosis: a 5-year study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2012 Jan;83(1):38-43. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2010.237834
https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2010.237834...
,2121. Nygaard GO, Celius EG, Benavent SAR, Sowa P, Gustavsen MW, Fjell AM et al. A longitudinal study of disability, cognition and gray matter atrophy in early multiple sclerosis patients according to evidence of disease activity. PLoS One. 2015 Aug;10(8):e0135974. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0135974
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.013...
,2222. Nourbakhsh B, Nunan-Saah J, Maghzi AH, Julian LJ, Spain R, Jin C et al. Longitudinal associations between MRI and cognitive changes in very early MS. Mult Scler Relat Disord. 2016 Jan;5:47-52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2015.10.010
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2015.10....
,2323. Hankomäki E, Multanen J, Kinnunen E, Hämäläinen P. The progress of cognitive decline in newly diagnosed MS patients. Acta Neurol Scand. 2014 Mar;129(3):184-91. https://doi.org/10.1111/ane.12161
https://doi.org/10.1111/ane.12161...
,2424. Zivadinov R, Sepcic J, Nasuelli D, De Masi R, Bragadin LM, Tommasi MA et al. A longitudinal study of brain atrophy and cognitive disturbances in the early phase of relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001 Jun;70(6):773-80. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.70.6.773
https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.70.6.773...
, yet contrasting with others showing decline44. Moccia M, Lanzillo R, Palladino R, Chang KC, Costabile T, Russo C et al. Cognitive impairment at diagnosis predicts 10-year multiple sclerosis progression. Mult Scler. 2016 Apr;22(5):659-67. https://doi.org/10.1177/1352458515599075
https://doi.org/10.1177/1352458515599075...
,66. Strober LB, Rao SM, Lee JC, Fischer E, Rudick R. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis: An 18 year follow-up study. Mult Scler Relat Disord. 2014 Jul;3(4):473-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2014.03.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2014.03....
,2525. Piras MR, Magnano I, Canu ED, Paulus KS, Satta WM, Soddu A et al. Longitudinal study of cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: neuropsychological, neuroradiological, and neurophysiological findings. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2003 Jul;74(7):878-85. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.74.7.878
https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.74.7.878...
,2626. Pereira AG. Evolução das funções cognitivas psíquicas e motoras dos pacientes portadores de esclerose múltipla [tese]. Porto Alegre: Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul; 2013. in this domain. These incongruent findings have been reported in recently-published systematic reviews on the subject11. Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 2008 Dec;7(12):1139-51. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70...
,22. Jongen PJ, Ter Horst AT, Brands AM. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Minerva Med. 2012 Apr;103(2):73-96.,33. Amato MP, Zipoli V, Portaccio E. Multiple sclerosis-related cognitive changes: a review of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. J Neurol Sci. 2006 Jun;245(1-2):41-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.08.019
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.08.01...
.

In the longitudinal studies cited in the preceding paragraph, part of the sample that showed a worsening of VEM over time comprised participants who evolved or were diagnosed with more progressive clinical forms of MS. The present sample was homogenous for the clinical form of the disease. This might explain the cognitive stability of the patients studied, given that cognitive impairment tends to be milder in the relapsing-remitting form of MS.

Another aspect that supports the stability and cognitive improvement of the present sample over time is the mechanism of brain neuroplasticity. A recent systematic review on functional magnetic resonance imaging related to the execution of neuropsychological tasks2727. Rocca MA, Amato MP, De Stefano N, Enzinger C, Geurts JJ, Penner IK et al.; MAGNIMS Study Group. Clinical and imaging assessment of cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 2015 Mar;14(3):302-17. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70250-9
https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70...
has shown that MS patients without cognitive dysfunction had different brain dynamics from control participants. The patients had greater brain activation, widely-distributed cortical recruitment and changes in functional connectivity in cognition-related regions. These findings suggest that increased recruitment of important cortical networks can attenuate the negative effect of MS on cognitive function.

On the other hand, episodic memory deficits, and likewise for other cognitive dysfunctions, are heterogeneous in MS where their degree of severity varies significantly between patients55. Brissart H, Morele E, Baumann C, Debouverie M. Verbal episodic memory in 426 multiple sclerosis patients: impairment in encoding, retrieval or both? Neurol Sci. 2012 Oct;33(5):1117-23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-...
. Based on this variability, in the present sample specifically, the patients did not show a relevant decline in VEM.

One point to consider that may influence the detection of cognitive impairments is the time interval between neuropsychological assessments. The short follow-up of the present study, coupled with the low rate of VEM impairment in the patients, may explain the longitudinal stability observed.

However, studies in the literature have shown that variation in follow-up time is an incongruent factor in terms of the impact of evolution of cognitive impairment. Some studies have detected cognitive impairments within follow-up periods of two years1616. Faglioni P, Bertolani L, Botti C, Merelli E. Verbal learning strategies in patients with multiple sclerosis. Cortex. 2000 Apr;36(2):243-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70527-6
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70...
,2424. Zivadinov R, Sepcic J, Nasuelli D, De Masi R, Bragadin LM, Tommasi MA et al. A longitudinal study of brain atrophy and cognitive disturbances in the early phase of relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001 Jun;70(6):773-80. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.70.6.773
https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.70.6.773...
,2626. Pereira AG. Evolução das funções cognitivas psíquicas e motoras dos pacientes portadores de esclerose múltipla [tese]. Porto Alegre: Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul; 2013.,2828. López-Góngora M, Querol L, Escartín A. A one-year follow-up study of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) and the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis: an appraisal of comparative longitudinal sensitivity. BMC Neurol. 2015 Mar;15:40. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-015-0296-2
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-015-0296-...
,2929. Amato MP, Portaccio E, Goretti B, Zipoli V, Iudice A, Della Pina D et al.; TuSCIMS Study Group. Relevance of cognitive deterioration in early relapsing-remitting MS: a 3-year follow-up study. Mult Scler. 2010 Dec;16(12):1474-82. https://doi.org/10.1177/1352458510380089
https://doi.org/10.1177/1352458510380089...
, reporting impairments in VEM and, particularly, attentional processes. Conversely, a systematic review33. Amato MP, Zipoli V, Portaccio E. Multiple sclerosis-related cognitive changes: a review of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. J Neurol Sci. 2006 Jun;245(1-2):41-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.08.019
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.08.01...
found that studies with a follow-up of three to five years showed inconsistent and slow cognitive changes, whereas a longer follow-up period (10-18 years) was needed to detect deficits.

In summary, the incongruence of the cognitive findings in patients with MS is due to the many different limitations inherent in longitudinal studies. These limitations include different criteria for determining cognitive impairment parameters, possible practice effects on the neuropsychological tests given the number of assessments administered per time interval, the choice of tests involving different levels of difficulty, and heterogeneous samples in terms of the clinical forms of the disease.

As outlined above, the healthy controls were not reassessed to better define the parameters of VEM impairments in the patients over time. In addition, the high cognitive variability among individuals with MS requires a larger sample size for greater representativeness of the data.

In conclusion, the results of this study revealed that the patients showed ineffective VEM relative to controls at the baseline assessment and attained improvement and stability in this cognitive domain over time. The clinical, sociodemographic and physical disability variables showed no correlation with patient performance on the VEM tests. An impact of the attention and executive functioning tests on VEM in the information encoding and retrieval stages was evident. The present sample was homogenous for the clinical form of the disease, a factor that may have enhanced the reliability of the results.

References

  • 1
    Chiaravalloti ND, DeLuca J. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 2008 Dec;7(12):1139-51. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70259-X
  • 2
    Jongen PJ, Ter Horst AT, Brands AM. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis. Minerva Med. 2012 Apr;103(2):73-96.
  • 3
    Amato MP, Zipoli V, Portaccio E. Multiple sclerosis-related cognitive changes: a review of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. J Neurol Sci. 2006 Jun;245(1-2):41-6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.08.019
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.08.019
  • 4
    Moccia M, Lanzillo R, Palladino R, Chang KC, Costabile T, Russo C et al. Cognitive impairment at diagnosis predicts 10-year multiple sclerosis progression. Mult Scler. 2016 Apr;22(5):659-67. https://doi.org/10.1177/1352458515599075
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1352458515599075
  • 5
    Brissart H, Morele E, Baumann C, Debouverie M. Verbal episodic memory in 426 multiple sclerosis patients: impairment in encoding, retrieval or both? Neurol Sci. 2012 Oct;33(5):1117-23. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-7
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-011-0915-7
  • 6
    Strober LB, Rao SM, Lee JC, Fischer E, Rudick R. Cognitive impairment in multiple sclerosis: An 18 year follow-up study. Mult Scler Relat Disord. 2014 Jul;3(4):473-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2014.03.004
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2014.03.004
  • 7
    Polman CH, Reingold SC, Banwell B, Clanet M, Cohen JA, Filippi M et al. Diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: 2010 revisions to the McDonald criteria. Ann Neurol. 2011 Feb;69(2):292-302. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.22366
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.22366
  • 8
    Ringe WK, Saine KC, Lacritz LH, Hynan LS, Cullum CM. Dyadic short forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III. Assessment. 2002 Sep;9(3):254-60. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191102009003004
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191102009003004
  • 9
    Brucki SM, Nitrini R, Caramelli P, Bertolucci PH, Okamoto IH. [Suggestions for utilization of the mini-mental state examination in Brazil]. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2003 Sep;61(3B):777-81. Portuguese. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2003000500014
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2003000500014
  • 10
    Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983 Jun;67(6):361-70. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x
  • 11
    Brandt JB, Benedict RH. Hopkins verbal learning test-revised: professional manual. Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resource; 2001.
  • 12
    Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale. (WMS-III). 3rd ed. New York: The Psychological Corporation; 1999.
  • 13
    Gorenstein C, Wang YP, Argimon IL, Werlang BS. Manual do inventário de depressão de Beck - BDI-II. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2011.
  • 14
    Kurtzke JF. Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: an expanded disability status scale (EDSS). Neurology. 1983 Nov;33(11):1444-52. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.33.11.1444
    » https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.33.11.1444
  • 15
    Fuso SF, Callegaro D, Pompéia S, Bueno OF. Working memory impairment in multiple sclerosis relapsing-remitting patients with episodic memory deficits. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2010 Apr;68(2):205-11. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2010000200010
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2010000200010
  • 16
    Faglioni P, Bertolani L, Botti C, Merelli E. Verbal learning strategies in patients with multiple sclerosis. Cortex. 2000 Apr;36(2):243-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70527-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70527-6
  • 17
    Bergendal G, Fredrikson S, Almkvist O. Selective decline in information processing in subgroups of multiple sclerosis: an 8-year longitudinal study. Eur Neurol. 2007;57(4):193-202. https://doi.org/10.1159/000099158
    » https://doi.org/10.1159/000099158
  • 18
    Jønsson A, Andresen J, Storr L, Tscherning T, Soelberg Sørensen P, Ravnborg M. Cognitive impairment in newly diagnosed multiple sclerosis patients: a 4-year follow-up study. J Neurol Sci. 2006 Jun;245(1-2):77-85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.09.016
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2005.09.016
  • 19
    Schwid SR, Goodman AD, Weinstein A, McDermott MP, Johnson KP; Copaxone Study Group. Cognitive function in relapsing multiple sclerosis: minimal changes in a 10-year clinical trial. J Neurol Sci. 2007 Apr;255(1-2):57-63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2007.01.070
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2007.01.070
  • 20
    Glanz BI, Healy BC, Hviid LE, Chitnis T, Weiner HL. Cognitive deterioration in patients with early multiple sclerosis: a 5-year study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2012 Jan;83(1):38-43. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2010.237834
    » https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2010.237834
  • 21
    Nygaard GO, Celius EG, Benavent SAR, Sowa P, Gustavsen MW, Fjell AM et al. A longitudinal study of disability, cognition and gray matter atrophy in early multiple sclerosis patients according to evidence of disease activity. PLoS One. 2015 Aug;10(8):e0135974. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0135974
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0135974
  • 22
    Nourbakhsh B, Nunan-Saah J, Maghzi AH, Julian LJ, Spain R, Jin C et al. Longitudinal associations between MRI and cognitive changes in very early MS. Mult Scler Relat Disord. 2016 Jan;5:47-52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2015.10.010
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2015.10.010
  • 23
    Hankomäki E, Multanen J, Kinnunen E, Hämäläinen P. The progress of cognitive decline in newly diagnosed MS patients. Acta Neurol Scand. 2014 Mar;129(3):184-91. https://doi.org/10.1111/ane.12161
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/ane.12161
  • 24
    Zivadinov R, Sepcic J, Nasuelli D, De Masi R, Bragadin LM, Tommasi MA et al. A longitudinal study of brain atrophy and cognitive disturbances in the early phase of relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2001 Jun;70(6):773-80. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.70.6.773
    » https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.70.6.773
  • 25
    Piras MR, Magnano I, Canu ED, Paulus KS, Satta WM, Soddu A et al. Longitudinal study of cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis: neuropsychological, neuroradiological, and neurophysiological findings. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2003 Jul;74(7):878-85. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.74.7.878
    » https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.74.7.878
  • 26
    Pereira AG. Evolução das funções cognitivas psíquicas e motoras dos pacientes portadores de esclerose múltipla [tese]. Porto Alegre: Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul; 2013.
  • 27
    Rocca MA, Amato MP, De Stefano N, Enzinger C, Geurts JJ, Penner IK et al.; MAGNIMS Study Group. Clinical and imaging assessment of cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 2015 Mar;14(3):302-17. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70250-9
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70250-9
  • 28
    López-Góngora M, Querol L, Escartín A. A one-year follow-up study of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) and the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis: an appraisal of comparative longitudinal sensitivity. BMC Neurol. 2015 Mar;15:40. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-015-0296-2
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/s12883-015-0296-2
  • 29
    Amato MP, Portaccio E, Goretti B, Zipoli V, Iudice A, Della Pina D et al.; TuSCIMS Study Group. Relevance of cognitive deterioration in early relapsing-remitting MS: a 3-year follow-up study. Mult Scler. 2010 Dec;16(12):1474-82. https://doi.org/10.1177/1352458510380089
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1352458510380089

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    May 2018

History

  • Received
    02 Aug 2017
  • Reviewed
    12 Jan 2018
  • Accepted
    22 Feb 2018
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia - ABNEURO R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices Torre Norte, 04101-000 São Paulo SP Brazil, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista.arquivos@abneuro.org