Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Effects of cognitive interventions with video games on cognition in healthy elderly people: a systematic review

Efeitos de intervenções cognitivas com jogos eletrônicos na cognição de idosos saudáveis: revisão sistemática

Abstract

Background

Currently, studies using video games as an intervention to improve cognitive functions in the elderly are on the rise.

Objective

To investigate and evaluate the effects of cognitive interventions using video games on cognition in healthy elderly people published in the last ten years.

Methods

A systematic review involving a qualitative analysis carried out between July and September 2021 on the SciELO, LILACS and MEDLINE databases..

Results

A total of 262 articles were identified in the initial search. After exclusion of duplicates, analysis of titles/abstracts and of the full text, a final total of 9 studies were included in the review. The objectives of the studies included investigating the effects on cognition of cognitive training (CT) programs using video games compared to programs using entertainment games or to low-intensity CT games. Despite the growing number of studies, many of them were focused on cognitive rehabilitation in elderly people with some degree of cognitive impairment, and few involved training among healthy elderly people.

Keywords:
Elderly; Cognitive Aging; Video Games; Cognition

Resumo

Antecedentes

Atualmente, estão em alta estudos que utilizam video games para melhorar as funções cognitivas em idosos.

Objetivo

Investigar e avaliar os efeitos de intervenções cognitivas com video games na cognição de idosos saudáveis publicadas nos últimos dez anos.

Métodos

Estudo de revisão sistemática com análise qualitativa realizado entre julho e setembro de 2021, das bases de dados SciELO, LILACS e MEDLINE.

Resultados

Foram identificados 262 estudos na busca inicial. Após a exclusão de estudos duplicados, análise dos títulos e resumos, e análise integral dos estudos, nove estudos foram incluídos na revisão. Os objetivos de alguns estudos incluíam investigar os efeitos na cognição de programas de treino cognitivo (TC) com video games comparados aos de programas com jogos de entretenimento ou com jogos com baixa intensidade de TC. Apesar do crescente número de estudos, muitos centravam-se na reabilitação cognitiva em idosos com algum comprometimento cognitivo e poucos realizaram o treino em idosos saudáveis.

Conclusão

Pelos estudos analisados, as intervenções de TC com video games apresentaram melhorias significativas na velocidade de processamento (VP) e na memória operacional (MO); por outro lado, não foram observadas melhorias nas funções executivas.

Palavras-chave:
Idoso; Envelhecimento Cognitivo; Video Games; Cognição

INTRODUCTION

The number of studies investigating cognitive aging has grown in recent years, given that cognitive performance declines as part of the normal aging process, with slower processing speed (PS) and reduced performance of the working memory (WM). These impairments are often of particular concern among older adults and their family members because of the risk of progression to dementia, as in Alzheimer disease. The literature11 Rocha Fde S, Chariglione IPFS. Memória Episódica e Idoso: Principais Alterações a partir de Diferentes Intervenções Cognitivas. Psicol, Teor Pesqui 2021;36:1–10 shows that the quality of execution of complex activities of everyday living can change as part of the normal process of healthy aging.

Cognitive interventions, more specifically cognitive training (CT), can serve as a method of both prevention and rehabilitation of some cognitive deficits. Technological advances enable CT through the use of video games, an approach that renders tasks more engaging and fun, increasing participant motivation and reducing abandonment rates,22 Dassen FCM, Houben K, Van Breukelen GJP, Jansen A. Gamified working memory training in overweight individuals reduces food intake but not body weight. Appetite 2018; 124:89–98 with the added advantage of being more accessible to the general public.33 Contreras-Somoza LM, Irazoki E, Toribio-Guzmán JM, et al. Usability and User Experience of Cognitive Intervention Technologies for Elderly People With MCI or Dementia: A Systematic Review. Front Psychol 2021; 12:636116

In this context, cognitive decline is correlated with cognitive reserve. Greater cognitive reserve is associated with the prevention of dementia, but in order for the interventions to yield benefits, the activities must be performed over a relatively long period without loss of motivation. This interest can be achieved through the use of cognitive computer games, leisure-time activities, social interaction, intellectual stimulation etc. Social interactions and intellectual stimulation have been shown to reduce the risk of dementia.44 Iizuka A, Suzuki H, Ogawa S, et al. Does social interaction influence the effect of cognitive intervention program? A randomized controlled trial using Go game. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2019;34 (02):324–332

Video games can be divided into CT games and entertainment games.55 Cardoso Nde O, Landenberger T, Argimon Ide LJogos Eletrônicos como Instrumentos de Intervenção no Declínio Cognitivo - Uma Revisão Sistemática. Rev Psicol da IMED 2017;9:119–139 While CT games are designed to train cognitive functions and are developed by neuroscientists, games for entertainment are focused on individual leisure and fun, such as the game Super Mario Bros.

For the present review, we will use serious games that have the goal of not only entertaining, but also of teaching and training cognitive functions such as memory, attention etc., which differs from computerized CT, which focuses only on training cognition, regardless of the participant's enjoyment. This type of game has been used as a way of promoting cognitive stimulation, for example, in older adults during the period of social isolation caused by the Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic.66 Alves Fde O da S, Oliveira DCA, Borges Jdos SUm olhar para o uso de tecnologias no cuidado de idosos com deficiência cognitiva em tempos de pandemia. Rev Sci Hist 2020;1:6–6

The study by Belchior et al.77 Belchior P, Marsiske M, Leite WL, Yam A, Thomas K, Mann W. Older Adults' Engagement During an Intervention Involving Off-the-Shelf Videogame. Games Health J 2016;5(03):151–156 involved the use of video games and brain training. Video games are essentially designed to entertain the general public. By contrast, brain-training games are especially designed to train an ability in a specific population. A study88 Anguera JA, Boccanfuso J, Rintoul JL, et al. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 2013;501 (7465):97–101 based on a video game intervention in older adults reported improvements in WM and sustained attention relative to the control group. Similar results were found in the study by Toril et al.,99 Toril P, Reales JM, Mayas J, Ballesteros S. Video Game Training Enhances Visuospatial Working Memory and Episodic Memory in Older Adults. Front Hum Neurosci 2016;10:206. Doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2016.00206
https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2016.00206...
in which a video game intervention enhanced visuospatial working memory, episodic memory, and short-term memory in healthy older adults.

Ordonez et al.1010 Ordonez TN, Borges F, Kanashiro CS, Santos CCDN, Hora SS, Lima-Silva TB. Actively station: Effects on global cognition of mature adults and healthy elderly program using eletronic games. Dement Neuropsychol 2017;11(02):186–197 conducted a study using a cognitive stimulation program on a Japanese video game equipment adapted for use by Brazilian older adults. The results revealed improvements in cognitive performance, specifically on the memory and language domains, and there was a reduction in the level of anxiety and in the rate of memory complaints.

The study by Assis1111 Assis SAC das N. Neuro reabilitação com jogos eletrônicos controlados por movimento corporal em idosos portadores de comprometimento cognitivo leve. 2016. (Accessed October 6, 2021). http://tede2.pucrs.br:80/tede2/handle/tede/6621
http://tede2.pucrs.br:80/tede2/handle/te...
used video games controlled by body movements in the neurorehabilitation of older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) at three levels of schooling (primary, secondary, and higher education). The results showed improvements in the memory of participants with primary and secondary level of schooling, but no improvement in the group with higher education.

Also in this context, a systematic review and meta-analysis1212 Mansor NS, Chow CM, Halaki M. Cognitive effects of video games in older adults and their moderators: a systematic review with meta-analysis and meta-regression. Aging Ment Health 2020;24 (06):841–856 evaluated the effects of video games, in relation to the control condition (no game), on cognitive functions in the elderly. The authors1212 Mansor NS, Chow CM, Halaki M. Cognitive effects of video games in older adults and their moderators: a systematic review with meta-analysis and meta-regression. Aging Ment Health 2020;24 (06):841–856 obtained 27 studies that met the inclusion criteria, and they concluded that video game interventions improved the memory of the participants; however, they found no improvement in other cognitive functions.

Finally, another meta-analysis1313 Bonnechère B, Langley C, Sahakian BJ. The use of commercial computerised cognitive games in older adults: a meta-analysis. Sci Rep 2020;10(01):15276 aimed to summarize the current level of evidence for brain training using commercial computerized cognitive games (ccCGs) in healthy older adults. It has been documented33 Contreras-Somoza LM, Irazoki E, Toribio-Guzmán JM, et al. Usability and User Experience of Cognitive Intervention Technologies for Elderly People With MCI or Dementia: A Systematic Review. Front Psychol 2021; 12:636116, 55 Cardoso Nde O, Landenberger T, Argimon Ide LJogos Eletrônicos como Instrumentos de Intervenção no Declínio Cognitivo - Uma Revisão Sistemática. Rev Psicol da IMED 2017;9:119–139, 99 Toril P, Reales JM, Mayas J, Ballesteros S. Video Game Training Enhances Visuospatial Working Memory and Episodic Memory in Older Adults. Front Hum Neurosci 2016;10:206. Doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2016.00206
https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2016.00206...
that commercially available cognitive games are effective in improving PS, WM, executive functions, and verbal memory.

However, despite the existence of several studies1212 Mansor NS, Chow CM, Halaki M. Cognitive effects of video games in older adults and their moderators: a systematic review with meta-analysis and meta-regression. Aging Ment Health 2020;24 (06):841–856 that used electronic games as an intervention to improve cognitive functions in the elderly, specific games for healthy elderly people are hardly addressed to this contingent. With the rapid advancement of technologies and the recent COVID-19 pandemic, the population started to make more use of social networks, video games, and online news, for example. Thus, the aim of the present study was to investigate and evaluate the effects of cognitive interventions with video games on the cognition of healthy elderly people in studies published in the last ten years.

METHODS

A systematic review involving a qualitative analysis was conducted. All relevant scientific articles published in Portuguese, English or Spanish were retrieved from the SciELO, LILACS, and PubMed electronic databases. From the data obtained on PubMed, we noticed a large gradual and accelerated increase in studies with video games in the last 10 years; therefore, in the present review, we have searched for articles published between 2011 and 2021 (until the beginning of the article's completion).

Using the Population, Phenomena of Interest, and Context (PICo) method, the following research question was defined: what are the effects of interventions with video games, in studies published in the last ten years, on the cognition of healthy older people? The population is healthy older people, the phenomena of interest, effects on cognition, and the context, cognitive interventions with video games published in the last ten years. After this process, the eligible descriptors were selected in the Descriptors in Health Sciences (Descritores em Ciências da Saúde, DeCS, in Portuguese) system to serve as keywords for the search for articles published in Portuguese, English and Spanish on the SciELO, LILACS and PUBMED databases. The following search terms were used: idoso* OR elder OR older person* OR older people OR senior citizen* OR elderly OR aging people OR aging person* OR older adult* OR anciano* OR persona* mayor* and intervenção* cognitiva* OR cognitive intervention* OR treino cognitivo OR cognitive training OR treino de memória OR memory training OR intervención* cognitiva OR entrenamiento cognitivo OR entrenamiento de memoria and jogo* OR *juego* OR *game* OR online OR on-line OR computerized OR video game OR *juego* OR en línea OR computerizado.

The inclusion criteria were studies with interventions involving serious games; healthy participants aged ≥ 60 years; articles published in scientific journals since 2011; and use of cognitive and/or neuropsychological tests to determine the effects of the interventions. The exclusion criteria were: publications of Masters dissertations, book chapters, doctoral theses, letters to the editor, case studies, systematic reviews, meta-analyses, and research protocols; participants aged < 60 years or presenting cognitive impairment; studies performed in residential care homes, such as long-term care facilities (LTCFs); studies failing to report the effects of the intervention on the cognitive performance of the participants; and studies combining multimodal interventions (such as physical training, action games, and nutritional components).

The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA)1414 Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Group TPPRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med 2009;6(07): e1000097 statement was used for the identification, screening, and eligibility stages of the studies, and two reviewers acted independently throughout the stages. The initial identification of studies entailed searches of the aforementioned databases. During the screening stage, duplicate studies were removed, and titles and abstracts were analyzed by applying the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Differing opinions were resolved through a conference with all reviewers. For the eligibility stage, the selected studies were read in full and analyzed according to the same criteria. The studies remaining after this stage were included in the review. Only peer-reviewed articles were considered for the systematic review.

The scope of the systematic review was registered on the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews under number CRD42021267903 (Ishibashi et al., 2021). In addition, the included studies were rated for quality according to the Downs and Black Checklist.1515 Downs SH, Black N. The feasibility of creating a checklist for the assessment of the methodological quality both of randomised and non-randomised studies of health care interventions. J Epidemiol Community Health 1998;52(06):377–384 This evaluation tool devised by Downs and Black comprises 27 items distributed among 5 subscales: reporting or assessment (10 items), external validity (3 items), internal validity of the measurements described and outcome bias (7 items), confounding factors (6 items) and power (1 item). The checklist items were assigned scores of 0 or 1, except for the item assessing the reporting of confounding factors, whose scores ranged from 0 to 2 points, and the item assessing the reporting of the study power (item 27), modified as per other studies,1616 Ratcliffe E, Pickering S, McLean S, Lewis J. Is there a relationship between subacromial impingement syndrome and scapular orientation? A systematic review. Br J Sports Med 2014;48(16): 1251–1256, 1717 Benjamin DR, van de Water ATM, Peiris CL. Effects of exercise on diastasis of the rectus abdominis muscle in the antenatal and postnatal periods: a systematic review. Physiotherapy 2014; 100 (01):1–8, 1818 Engers PB, Rombaldi AJ, Portella EG, Silva MC. The effects of the Pilates method in the elderly: a systematic review. Rev Bras Reumatol 2016;56:352–365 whose original scores from 0 to 5 points were changed to scores of 0 or 1 point, with 1 meaning that the article reported calculation of the power and/or sample size and 0, that these calculations were not performed. Thus, the total scores on the checklist ranged from 0 to 28 points. In order to improve the reading of the data obtained, the scores were converted into percentages for each domain, and an overall mean total score for all domains was calculated. A system to classify the quality of the articles was defined: ≤ 0.39–poor; 0.40 to 0.69–regular; 0.70 to 0.79–good; and ≥ 0.80–excellent.

RESULTS

A total of 262 studies were retrieved in the initial search, 2 of which were subsequently excluded because they were duplicates. Titles and abstracts of the remaining 260 studies were read, and then, screened for selection according to the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Thus, 34 articles were read in full and, after an in-depth analysis, a total of 9 studies were included in the review. The screening and selection process is depicted in the PRISMA flow diagram (►Figure 1).

Figure 1
Flow diagram of the selection process of the present study.

Nine studies involved interventions with CT games.44 Iizuka A, Suzuki H, Ogawa S, et al. Does social interaction influence the effect of cognitive intervention program? A randomized controlled trial using Go game. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2019;34 (02):324–332, 1919 Bozoki A, Radovanovic M, Winn B, Heeter C, Anthony JC. Effects of a computer-based cognitive exercise program on age-related cognitive decline. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2013;57(01):1–7, 2020 Lee T-S, Goh SJA, Quek SY, et al. A brain-computer interface based cognitive training system for healthy elderly: a randomized control pilot study for usability and preliminary efficacy. PLoS One 2013;8(11):e79419, 2121 Simon SS, Tusch ES, Feng NC, Håkansson K, Mohammed AH, Daffner KR. Is computerized working memory training effective in healthy older adults? Evidence from a multi-site, randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis 2018;65(03):931–949, 2222 Yeo SN, Lee TS, Sng WT, et al. Effectiveness of a Personalized Brain-Computer Interface System for Cognitive Training in Healthy Elderly: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J Alzheimers Dis 2018; 66(01):127–138, 2323 Perrot A, Maillot P, Hartley A. Cognitive Training Game Versus Action Videogame: Effects on Cognitive Functions in Older Adults. Games Health J 2019;8(01):35-40 https://home.liebertpub.com/g4h
https://home.liebertpub.com/g4h...
, 2424 Faust ME, Multhaup KS, Ong MS, Demakis GJ, Balz KG. Exploring the Specificity, Synergy, and Durability of Auditory and Visual Computer Gameplay Transfer Effects in Healthy Older Adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020;75(06):1170–1180, 2525 Lee HK, Kent JD, Wendel C, et al. Home-Based, Adaptive Cognitive Training for Cognitively Normal Older adults: Initial Efficacy Trial. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020;75(06):1144–1154, 2626 West RK, Rabin LA, Silverman JM, Moshier E, Sano M, Beeri MS. Short-term computerized cognitive training does not improve cognition compared to an active control in non-demented adults aged 80 years and above. Int Psychogeriatr 2020;32 (01):65–73 The sample size ranged from 31 to 681 subjects, with ages ranging from 60 to 85 years. The length of the intervention ranged from 5 to 12 months, with a minimum of 8 and a maximum of 720 sessions lasting from 30 to 120 minutes each. Follow-up assessments took place within one year of the interventions (►Table 1).

Table 1
Studies involving interventions with CT games

Lastly, according to the scores for the categories of the methodological quality check list, none of the articles scored < 0.71 points, and 9 scored > 0.78, attaining good quality rating. The mean overall score of the articles across all categories was of 0.81 out of 1.0 in terms of fulfilling the Downs and Black methodological quality requirements. The scores for each domain were: reporting – 0.97; external validity – 0.20; bias/internal validity – 0.66; confounding – 0.98; and power – 0.90 (►Table 2).

Table 2
Results of the Downs and Black Checklist for the present systematic review

The games used in some studies are available on the internet: Cogmed (https://www.cogmed.com/) and CogniFit Personal Coach (https://www.cognifit.com/).

DISCUSSION

The objective of the present study was to carry out a systematic review of studies investigating and assessing the effects on the cognition of healthy older adults of cognitive interventions using video games. A total of nine eligible studies were retrieved with objectives such as investigating the effects on cognition of CT programs using video games compared with entertainment game programs2323 Perrot A, Maillot P, Hartley A. Cognitive Training Game Versus Action Videogame: Effects on Cognitive Functions in Older Adults. Games Health J 2019;8(01):35-40 https://home.liebertpub.com/g4h
https://home.liebertpub.com/g4h...
, 2525 Lee HK, Kent JD, Wendel C, et al. Home-Based, Adaptive Cognitive Training for Cognitively Normal Older adults: Initial Efficacy Trial. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020;75(06):1144–1154 or low-level CT game programs.1919 Bozoki A, Radovanovic M, Winn B, Heeter C, Anthony JC. Effects of a computer-based cognitive exercise program on age-related cognitive decline. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2013;57(01):1–7, 2121 Simon SS, Tusch ES, Feng NC, Håkansson K, Mohammed AH, Daffner KR. Is computerized working memory training effective in healthy older adults? Evidence from a multi-site, randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis 2018;65(03):931–949, 2626 West RK, Rabin LA, Silverman JM, Moshier E, Sano M, Beeri MS. Short-term computerized cognitive training does not improve cognition compared to an active control in non-demented adults aged 80 years and above. Int Psychogeriatr 2020;32 (01):65–73 A total of four studies44 Iizuka A, Suzuki H, Ogawa S, et al. Does social interaction influence the effect of cognitive intervention program? A randomized controlled trial using Go game. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2019;34 (02):324–332, 2020 Lee T-S, Goh SJA, Quek SY, et al. A brain-computer interface based cognitive training system for healthy elderly: a randomized control pilot study for usability and preliminary efficacy. PLoS One 2013;8(11):e79419, 2222 Yeo SN, Lee TS, Sng WT, et al. Effectiveness of a Personalized Brain-Computer Interface System for Cognitive Training in Healthy Elderly: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J Alzheimers Dis 2018; 66(01):127–138, 2424 Faust ME, Multhaup KS, Ong MS, Demakis GJ, Balz KG. Exploring the Specificity, Synergy, and Durability of Auditory and Visual Computer Gameplay Transfer Effects in Healthy Older Adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020;75(06):1170–1180 performed CT using video games in the training group, but not in the control group.

Of the nine studies involving CT interventions using video games, six reported significant improvements in the cognitive performance of the participants.44 Iizuka A, Suzuki H, Ogawa S, et al. Does social interaction influence the effect of cognitive intervention program? A randomized controlled trial using Go game. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2019;34 (02):324–332, 2020 Lee T-S, Goh SJA, Quek SY, et al. A brain-computer interface based cognitive training system for healthy elderly: a randomized control pilot study for usability and preliminary efficacy. PLoS One 2013;8(11):e79419, 2121 Simon SS, Tusch ES, Feng NC, Håkansson K, Mohammed AH, Daffner KR. Is computerized working memory training effective in healthy older adults? Evidence from a multi-site, randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis 2018;65(03):931–949, 2323 Perrot A, Maillot P, Hartley A. Cognitive Training Game Versus Action Videogame: Effects on Cognitive Functions in Older Adults. Games Health J 2019;8(01):35-40 https://home.liebertpub.com/g4h
https://home.liebertpub.com/g4h...
, 2424 Faust ME, Multhaup KS, Ong MS, Demakis GJ, Balz KG. Exploring the Specificity, Synergy, and Durability of Auditory and Visual Computer Gameplay Transfer Effects in Healthy Older Adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020;75(06):1170–1180, 2525 Lee HK, Kent JD, Wendel C, et al. Home-Based, Adaptive Cognitive Training for Cognitively Normal Older adults: Initial Efficacy Trial. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020;75(06):1144–1154 The study by Lee et al.2525 Lee HK, Kent JD, Wendel C, et al. Home-Based, Adaptive Cognitive Training for Cognitively Normal Older adults: Initial Efficacy Trial. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020;75(06):1144–1154 showed enhanced cognitive performance, with most significant improvements for WM and PS. The findings of the study by Lee et al.2020 Lee T-S, Goh SJA, Quek SY, et al. A brain-computer interface based cognitive training system for healthy elderly: a randomized control pilot study for usability and preliminary efficacy. PLoS One 2013;8(11):e79419 showed improvements in memory and attention, promoting a high participant adherence rate. The study by Iizuka et al.44 Iizuka A, Suzuki H, Ogawa S, et al. Does social interaction influence the effect of cognitive intervention program? A randomized controlled trial using Go game. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2019;34 (02):324–332 showed improvement in visual WM and also noted that the increased social interaction by playing board games face to face is more effective to improve cognitive function than playing alone. Perrot et al.2323 Perrot A, Maillot P, Hartley A. Cognitive Training Game Versus Action Videogame: Effects on Cognitive Functions in Older Adults. Games Health J 2019;8(01):35-40 https://home.liebertpub.com/g4h
https://home.liebertpub.com/g4h...
found improvements in inductive reasoning and that the CT game promoted less transfer to cognitive tasks than the action video game. The study by Simon et al.2121 Simon SS, Tusch ES, Feng NC, Håkansson K, Mohammed AH, Daffner KR. Is computerized working memory training effective in healthy older adults? Evidence from a multi-site, randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis 2018;65(03):931–949 showed evidence of performance improvement on trained tasks and on an untrained task dependent on WM and PS.

Corroborating these results, in the study by van Muijden et al.,2727 van Muijden J, Band GPH, Hommel B. Online games training aging brains: limited transfer to cognitive control functions. Front Hum Neurosci 2012;6:221. Doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2012.00221
https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2012.00221...
which involved a CT intervention using online games in healthy older adults, the authors showed that this type of intervention can, besides promoting gains in PS, promote the transfer of acquired skills to measures of inhibition and inductive reasoning. The results of the study by Simpson et al.,2828 Simpson T, Camfield D, Pipingas A, Macpherson H, Stough C. Improved Processing Speed: Online Computer-based Cognitive Training in Older Adults. Educ Gerontol 2012;38:445–458 in which the participants performed online computer-based CT, showed that 21 days of CT can yield improvements in PS for responses to simple and complex stimuli.

Finally, of the studies that promoted significant improvements in cognitive performance, Faust et al.2424 Faust ME, Multhaup KS, Ong MS, Demakis GJ, Balz KG. Exploring the Specificity, Synergy, and Durability of Auditory and Visual Computer Gameplay Transfer Effects in Healthy Older Adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020;75(06):1170–1180 showed that visual training yielded transfer effects to WM, but combined visual and auditory training failed to promote any significant transfer effects. Replicating the results of auditory training, Smith et al.2929 Smith GE, Housen P, Yaffe K, et al. A cognitive training program based on principles of brain plasticity: results from the Improvement in Memory with Plasticity-based Adaptive Cognitive Training (IMPACT) study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2009;57(04): 594–603 found no significant transfer effect.

The other three studies1919 Bozoki A, Radovanovic M, Winn B, Heeter C, Anthony JC. Effects of a computer-based cognitive exercise program on age-related cognitive decline. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2013;57(01):1–7, 2222 Yeo SN, Lee TS, Sng WT, et al. Effectiveness of a Personalized Brain-Computer Interface System for Cognitive Training in Healthy Elderly: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J Alzheimers Dis 2018; 66(01):127–138, 2626 West RK, Rabin LA, Silverman JM, Moshier E, Sano M, Beeri MS. Short-term computerized cognitive training does not improve cognition compared to an active control in non-demented adults aged 80 years and above. Int Psychogeriatr 2020;32 (01):65–73 included in the present review, which trained more than one cognitive ability, showed no significant improvements. For example, in the study by Bozoki et al.,1919 Bozoki A, Radovanovic M, Winn B, Heeter C, Anthony JC. Effects of a computer-based cognitive exercise program on age-related cognitive decline. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2013;57(01):1–7 learning effects for the suite of games were relatively small due to the great variation in the amount of time devoted to game play and reluctance to pursue more challenging levels, as participants had full independence in terms of choices during the intervention. Similarly, West et al.2626 West RK, Rabin LA, Silverman JM, Moshier E, Sano M, Beeri MS. Short-term computerized cognitive training does not improve cognition compared to an active control in non-demented adults aged 80 years and above. Int Psychogeriatr 2020;32 (01):65–73 using linear mixed models, found no significant differences found between the CT and control groups in terms of language functioning, attention, executive functioning, or memory. In the study of Yeo et al.,2222 Yeo SN, Lee TS, Sng WT, et al. Effectiveness of a Personalized Brain-Computer Interface System for Cognitive Training in Healthy Elderly: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J Alzheimers Dis 2018; 66(01):127–138 the game intervention promoted no general gains or improvements in cognitive performance or in specific cognitive domains, such as attention and delayed memory. Moreover, there were no improvements in memory performance in everyday tasks.

For studies that do not show improvements in cognition, one hypothesis would be the level of difficulty of the game and the freedom that some authors gave to the elderly to choose the difficulty and amount of time playing the game, unlike those that showed improvements. In the studies that presented superior performance, it was evidenced that the minimum duration of the intervention was o eight weeks and the sample consisted of younger elderly people.

In conclusion, the results of the present review revealed that half of the studies showed significant improvements in cognitive performance, whereas the other half failed to observe any improvement.

The feasibility issues reported in the studies reviewed included limited access to the internet at home, great variation in the amount of time devoted to game play, a reluctance to pursue more challenging levels, a sample of older adults with an active lifestyle participating in cognitive activities, and a lack of concern regarding digital inclusion and digital literacy. Participant engagement proved a barrier, and a strategy to improve engagement used by Bozoki1919 Bozoki A, Radovanovic M, Winn B, Heeter C, Anthony JC. Effects of a computer-based cognitive exercise program on age-related cognitive decline. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2013;57(01):1–7 was that participants knew that if they played the game 5 times for 30 min, they would be paid US$ 5 per week. Another potential strategy would be accompanying progress using monitors to motivate the participants.

Taken together, the results of the present review showed that most interventions using video games improved WM and PS. Finally, as a limitation of the present study, we identified the scarce number of articles on healthy elderly people in the literature. Few studies used serious games as an intervention and its effects on psychosocial variables and the quality of life of healthy elderly people. Therefore, for future research, it would be interesting to address this issue.

We can consider as a limitation of the present study the fact that we may not have found, during the search stage, articles that could be part of the present review, so we consider important the performance new systematic reviews that use other search terms.

References

  • 1
    Rocha Fde S, Chariglione IPFS. Memória Episódica e Idoso: Principais Alterações a partir de Diferentes Intervenções Cognitivas. Psicol, Teor Pesqui 2021;36:1–10
  • 2
    Dassen FCM, Houben K, Van Breukelen GJP, Jansen A. Gamified working memory training in overweight individuals reduces food intake but not body weight. Appetite 2018; 124:89–98
  • 3
    Contreras-Somoza LM, Irazoki E, Toribio-Guzmán JM, et al. Usability and User Experience of Cognitive Intervention Technologies for Elderly People With MCI or Dementia: A Systematic Review. Front Psychol 2021; 12:636116
  • 4
    Iizuka A, Suzuki H, Ogawa S, et al. Does social interaction influence the effect of cognitive intervention program? A randomized controlled trial using Go game. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2019;34 (02):324–332
  • 5
    Cardoso Nde O, Landenberger T, Argimon Ide LJogos Eletrônicos como Instrumentos de Intervenção no Declínio Cognitivo - Uma Revisão Sistemática. Rev Psicol da IMED 2017;9:119–139
  • 6
    Alves Fde O da S, Oliveira DCA, Borges Jdos SUm olhar para o uso de tecnologias no cuidado de idosos com deficiência cognitiva em tempos de pandemia. Rev Sci Hist 2020;1:6–6
  • 7
    Belchior P, Marsiske M, Leite WL, Yam A, Thomas K, Mann W. Older Adults' Engagement During an Intervention Involving Off-the-Shelf Videogame. Games Health J 2016;5(03):151–156
  • 8
    Anguera JA, Boccanfuso J, Rintoul JL, et al. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature 2013;501 (7465):97–101
  • 9
    Toril P, Reales JM, Mayas J, Ballesteros S. Video Game Training Enhances Visuospatial Working Memory and Episodic Memory in Older Adults. Front Hum Neurosci 2016;10:206. Doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2016.00206
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2016.00206
  • 10
    Ordonez TN, Borges F, Kanashiro CS, Santos CCDN, Hora SS, Lima-Silva TB. Actively station: Effects on global cognition of mature adults and healthy elderly program using eletronic games. Dement Neuropsychol 2017;11(02):186–197
  • 11
    Assis SAC das N. Neuro reabilitação com jogos eletrônicos controlados por movimento corporal em idosos portadores de comprometimento cognitivo leve. 2016. (Accessed October 6, 2021). http://tede2.pucrs.br:80/tede2/handle/tede/6621
    » http://tede2.pucrs.br:80/tede2/handle/tede/6621
  • 12
    Mansor NS, Chow CM, Halaki M. Cognitive effects of video games in older adults and their moderators: a systematic review with meta-analysis and meta-regression. Aging Ment Health 2020;24 (06):841–856
  • 13
    Bonnechère B, Langley C, Sahakian BJ. The use of commercial computerised cognitive games in older adults: a meta-analysis. Sci Rep 2020;10(01):15276
  • 14
    Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Group TPPRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med 2009;6(07): e1000097
  • 15
    Downs SH, Black N. The feasibility of creating a checklist for the assessment of the methodological quality both of randomised and non-randomised studies of health care interventions. J Epidemiol Community Health 1998;52(06):377–384
  • 16
    Ratcliffe E, Pickering S, McLean S, Lewis J. Is there a relationship between subacromial impingement syndrome and scapular orientation? A systematic review. Br J Sports Med 2014;48(16): 1251–1256
  • 17
    Benjamin DR, van de Water ATM, Peiris CL. Effects of exercise on diastasis of the rectus abdominis muscle in the antenatal and postnatal periods: a systematic review. Physiotherapy 2014; 100 (01):1–8
  • 18
    Engers PB, Rombaldi AJ, Portella EG, Silva MC. The effects of the Pilates method in the elderly: a systematic review. Rev Bras Reumatol 2016;56:352–365
  • 19
    Bozoki A, Radovanovic M, Winn B, Heeter C, Anthony JC. Effects of a computer-based cognitive exercise program on age-related cognitive decline. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2013;57(01):1–7
  • 20
    Lee T-S, Goh SJA, Quek SY, et al. A brain-computer interface based cognitive training system for healthy elderly: a randomized control pilot study for usability and preliminary efficacy. PLoS One 2013;8(11):e79419
  • 21
    Simon SS, Tusch ES, Feng NC, Håkansson K, Mohammed AH, Daffner KR. Is computerized working memory training effective in healthy older adults? Evidence from a multi-site, randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis 2018;65(03):931–949
  • 22
    Yeo SN, Lee TS, Sng WT, et al. Effectiveness of a Personalized Brain-Computer Interface System for Cognitive Training in Healthy Elderly: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J Alzheimers Dis 2018; 66(01):127–138
  • 23
    Perrot A, Maillot P, Hartley A. Cognitive Training Game Versus Action Videogame: Effects on Cognitive Functions in Older Adults. Games Health J 2019;8(01):35-40 https://home.liebertpub.com/g4h
    » https://home.liebertpub.com/g4h
  • 24
    Faust ME, Multhaup KS, Ong MS, Demakis GJ, Balz KG. Exploring the Specificity, Synergy, and Durability of Auditory and Visual Computer Gameplay Transfer Effects in Healthy Older Adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020;75(06):1170–1180
  • 25
    Lee HK, Kent JD, Wendel C, et al. Home-Based, Adaptive Cognitive Training for Cognitively Normal Older adults: Initial Efficacy Trial. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020;75(06):1144–1154
  • 26
    West RK, Rabin LA, Silverman JM, Moshier E, Sano M, Beeri MS. Short-term computerized cognitive training does not improve cognition compared to an active control in non-demented adults aged 80 years and above. Int Psychogeriatr 2020;32 (01):65–73
  • 27
    van Muijden J, Band GPH, Hommel B. Online games training aging brains: limited transfer to cognitive control functions. Front Hum Neurosci 2012;6:221. Doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2012.00221
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2012.00221
  • 28
    Simpson T, Camfield D, Pipingas A, Macpherson H, Stough C. Improved Processing Speed: Online Computer-based Cognitive Training in Older Adults. Educ Gerontol 2012;38:445–458
  • 29
    Smith GE, Housen P, Yaffe K, et al. A cognitive training program based on principles of brain plasticity: results from the Improvement in Memory with Plasticity-based Adaptive Cognitive Training (IMPACT) study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2009;57(04): 594–603

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    21 July 2023
  • Date of issue
    2023

History

  • Received
    07 Feb 2022
  • Reviewed
    02 Oct 2022
  • Accepted
    05 Oct 2022
Academia Brasileira de Neurologia - ABNEURO R. Vergueiro, 1353 sl.1404 - Ed. Top Towers Offices Torre Norte, 04101-000 São Paulo SP Brazil, Tel.: +55 11 5084-9463 | +55 11 5083-3876 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista.arquivos@abneuro.org