Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Insights into the development of a juvenile harpy eagle’s hunting skills

Considerações sobre o desenvolvimento da habilidade de caça de um gavião-real juvenil

ABSTRACT

The post-fledging period is of paramount importance for raptors, since this is when a juvenile develops its hunting skills and gains the abilities required in adulthood and independence through dispersal. Little is known however, about this stage in the lives of raptors such as harpy eagles, Harpia harpyja. Between March 2016 and July 2017, we recorded three predation attempts on groups of primates by a wild juvenile harpy eagle in southwestern Brazilian Amazonia, including the first predation of an adult squirrel monkey, Saimiri ustus. These sequential records give insights into the gradual development of hunting skills during the post-fledging period, similar to what has been reported for other birds of prey. We hypothesize that a link between developing flight skills, decreasing parental food provisioning and increasingly successful captures triggers adulthood and independence in harpy eagles.

KEYWORDS:
hunting behavior; juvenile raptor; predator-prey interaction; foraging; Harpia harpyja

RESUMO

O período pós-emplumagem é de suma importância para os rapinantes, pois é quando um jovem desenvolve a habilidade de caça necessária na idade adulta e a independência por meio da dispersão. Entretanto, pouco se sabe sobre esse estágio na vida de rapinantes como o gavião-real, Harpia harpyja. Entre março 2016 e julho 2017, três tentativas de predação sobre grupos de primatas por um gavião-real juvenil foram registradas no sudoeste da Amazônia brasileira, incluindo a primeira predação de um macaco-de-cheiro adulto, Saimiri ustus. Estes registros sequenciais indicam um desenvolvimento gradual da habilidade de caça ao longo do período de pós-emplumagem, de forma similar ao relatado para outros rapinantes. Hipotetizamos que a associação entre o desenvolvimento da habilidade de voo, diminuição do provisionamento pelos pais e aumento da taxa de capturas bem-sucedidas promove a maturidade e independência no gavião-real.

PALAVRAS-CHAVE:
comportamento de caça; rapinantes juvenis; interações predador-presa; forrageio; Harpia harpyja

The post-fledging period, between the first flights and recruitment, is of paramount importance for raptors because it is when juveniles develop their hunting skills and acquire the skills required in adulthood and the independence necessary to disperse (Penteriani and Delgado 2009Penteriani, V.; Delgado, M.M. 2009. Thoughts on natal dispersal. Journal of Raptor Research, 43: 90-98.). Despite its importance (Varland and Klaas 1991Varland, D.E.; Klaas, E.E. 1991. Development of foraging behavior in the american kestrel. Journal of Raptor Research, 25: 9-17.; McCann and Kemp 1994McCann, K.I.; Kemp, A.C. 1994. Hunting behaviour of a fledgling greater kestrel, Falco rupicoloides and its mother during the post-fledging period. Ostrich, 65: 1-6.; Kitowski 2004Kitowski, I. 2004. Play behaviour and active training of Montagu’s harrier (Circus pygargus) offspring in the post-fledging period. Journal of Ethology, 23: 3-8., 2009Kitowski, I. 2009. Social learning of hunting skills in juvenile marsh harriers Circus aeruginosus. Journal of Ethology, 27: 327-332.), the development of hunting skills during the post-fledging is still poorly known for many species of raptors.

The harpy eagle, Harpia harpyja is the largest and most powerful Neotropical raptor, hunting from small birds to sloths (Miranda 2018Miranda, E.B. 2018. Prey composition of harpy eagles (Harpia harpyja) in Raleighvallen, Suriname. Tropical Conservation Science, 11: 1-8. ). Items eaten by harpy eagles are known from inspection of nest remains and monitoring of prey brought to nests (Rettig 1978Rettig, N.L. 1978. Breeding behavior of the harpy eagle (Harpya harpyja). The Auk, 95: 629-643.; Aguiar-Silva et al. 2014Aguiar-Silva, F.H.; Sanaiotti, T.M.; Luz, B.B. 2014. Food habits of the harpy eagle, a top predator from the Amazonian rainforest canopy. Journal of Raptor Research, 48: 24-35.; Miranda 2018). Direct observations of predation attempts have allowed to identify new prey species of harpy eagles, and to obtain information on behavioral interactions between raptors and their prey (e.g., Barnett et al. 2011Barnett, A.A.; Schiel, V.; Deveny, A.; Valsko, J.; Spironello, W.R.; Ross, C. 2011. Predation on Cacajao ouakary and Cebus albifrons (Primates: Platyrrhini) by harpy eagles. Mammalia, 75: 169-172.; Lenz and dos Reis 2011Lenz, B.B.; dos Reis, A.M. 2011. Harpy eagle-primate interactions in the central Amazon. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology, 123: 404-408.). The available knowledge about sequential predation events involving juveniles is limited to a study focusing on captive-bred individuals (Touchton et al. 2002Touchton, J.M.; Hsu, Y.; Palleroni, A. 2002. Foraging ecology of reintroduced captive-bred subadult harpy eagles (Harpia harpyja) on Barro Colorado Island, Panama. Ornitología Neotropical, 13: 365-379.). Here, we provide data on the development of hunting skills in a wild juvenile harpy eagle.

We observed an active nest built at a height of 20 m into a 30-m Ficus tree (366 cm DBH) located in a pasture, 15 m away from the edge of a 383-ha forest fragment at Fazenda Coqueiral (11°38’27.0”S, 61°39’08.9”W; 260 m above sea level), in Rolim de Moura, Rondônia State, Brazil. The climate is tropical humid (Koppën AW) with mean temperature of 27 °C, and annual precipitation of 1600 - 2400 mm (rainy season: October - May) (SEDAM 2012SEDAM. 2012. Secretaria de Estado do Desenvolvimento Ambiental, Rondônia, Brasil. Boletim climatológico de Rondônia - 2010. 12: 1-34.). The vegetation is Amazonian open ombrophilous forest. Observations were carried out from March 2016 to July 2017. The nest was monitored during the breeding period once a week, up to the 40th week. Subsequent visits occurred biweekly, resulting in a total of 47 observation sessions, and 108 monitoring hours (mean = 2 h 30 min per session).

The study subject was born between April 9th and 16th, 2016. The chick began wing exercising at around the 12th week (first record on July 7th, 2016). The first fledge took place at around the 22nd week (first record on September 10th, 2016) and the juvenile was perfectly capable of flying around two weeks later. Seven provisioning events were recorded (last record in the 55th week, on April 29th, 2017), all of which took place in the nest. Three predation attempts, all of them involving primates, were recorded.

At 1700 h on March 12th, 2017 the juvenile was perched 18 m high in a standing-dead tree (ca. 20 m) in the pasture, about 200 m from the nest, and 30 m from the forest fragment edge. It was staring at an unidentified primate group at least 50 m away within the forest. After observing the monkeys for approximately 10-15 min, it took flight towards the group. The dense forest canopy prevented further direct observation, but from the sudden, short (<10 seconds) loud screams emitted by several individuals and the branch agitation it was possible to infer that at least one predation attempt had occurred there. The harpy eagle landed with empty claws on a 25 m high perch in a tree ~80 m away from the departure point.

The second attempt occurred at 1630 h on April 1st, 2017. The juvenile was on a 25 m high perch and about 150 m away from the nest. There was a small group of 3-4 adult black-faced black spider monkeys (Ateles chamek) feeding in a tree close to the fragment edge which the juvenile stared at for about 6 min. The nest tree was approached to search for remains and the juvenile fled to a ~20 m high perch in a tree about 50 m from the nest. From there, it stared alternately at the spider monkeys and the researchers for another 5 min before flying towards the monkeys. Within 4 m from the monkeys, the raptor abruptly changed its direction and flew away. Immediately afterwards, the spider monkeys slowly and silently left the area.

A successful predation attempt occurred at 1730 h on July 1st, 2017. The juvenile flew from the forest and over the pasture matrix near the fragment edge carrying an adult bare-eared squirrel monkey (Saimiri ustus). It perched ~5 m high in an approximately 10 m high Cecropia tree. Then, the juvenile flew for about 50 m landing on two relatively low perches (ca. 8-10 m high) close to the forest edge, where it stayed for a few minutes before moving out of sight. This event lasted ~26 min.

Unsuccessful predation is relatively common even among experienced adult harpy eagles (Boinski et al. 2003Boinski, S.; Kauffman, L.; Westoll, A.; Stickler, C.M.; Cropp, S.; Ehmke, E. 2003. Are vigilance, risk from avian predators and group size consequences of habitat structure? A comparison of three species of squirrel monkey (Saimiri oerstedii, S. boliviensis, and S. sciureus). Behaviour, 140: 1421-1467.; de Luna et al. 2010de Luna, A.G.; Sanmiguel, R.; Di Fiore, A.; Fernandez-Duque, E. 2010. Predation and predation attempts on red titi monkeys (Callicebus discolor) and equatorial sakis (Pithecia aequatorialis) in Amazonian Ecuador. Folia Primatologica, 81: 86-95.; Lenz and dos Reis 2011Lenz, B.B.; dos Reis, A.M. 2011. Harpy eagle-primate interactions in the central Amazon. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology, 123: 404-408.). A possible cause for the unsuccessful outcome during the first attempt was the last-minute vocalization by monkeys during the attack. This kind of startle vocalization can be interpreted as a key instantaneous anti-predator response that creates a moment of hesitation at the very last moment, thus interfering with attack completion (Sargent 1990Sargent, T.D. 1990. Startle as an anti-predator mechanism, with special reference to the underwing moths, (Catocala). In: Evans, D.L.; Schmidt, J.O. (Eds.). Insect defenses: adaptive mechanisms and strategies of prey and predators. State University of New York Press, Albany, p.229-249. ; Lenz and dos Reis 2011).

The second attempt was one of the first documented interactions between harpy eagles and Ateles chamek. A similar behavior was described when a large unidentified eagle tried to attack an equatorial saki (Pithecia aequatorialis; de Luna et al. 2010de Luna, A.G.; Sanmiguel, R.; Di Fiore, A.; Fernandez-Duque, E. 2010. Predation and predation attempts on red titi monkeys (Callicebus discolor) and equatorial sakis (Pithecia aequatorialis) in Amazonian Ecuador. Folia Primatologica, 81: 86-95.). Even adult raptors make several flights before a successful attack (Boinski et al. 2003Boinski, S.; Kauffman, L.; Westoll, A.; Stickler, C.M.; Cropp, S.; Ehmke, E. 2003. Are vigilance, risk from avian predators and group size consequences of habitat structure? A comparison of three species of squirrel monkey (Saimiri oerstedii, S. boliviensis, and S. sciureus). Behaviour, 140: 1421-1467.; de Luna et al. 2010; Lenz and dos Reis 2011Lenz, B.B.; dos Reis, A.M. 2011. Harpy eagle-primate interactions in the central Amazon. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology, 123: 404-408.), probably due to visual and anatomical constraints in birds of prey that limit fine-adjustments during flights (Land 1999Land, M. 1999. The roles of head movements in the search and capture strategy of a tern (Aves, Laridae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 184: 265-272.; Shifferman and Eilam 2004Shifferman, E.; Eilam, D. 2004. Movement and direction of movement of a simulated prey affect the success rate in barn owl Tyto alba attack. Journal of Avian Biology, 35: 111-116.). Juvenile raptors may also feel confident to attack adult spider monkeys when they are far away, but quit as they get closer. Harpy eagles usually attack prey averaging 2.6 kg in central Amazonia (Aguiar-Silva et al. 2014Aguiar-Silva, F.H.; Sanaiotti, T.M.; Luz, B.B. 2014. Food habits of the harpy eagle, a top predator from the Amazonian rainforest canopy. Journal of Raptor Research, 48: 24-35.), but adult spider monkeys can reach up to 9.8 kg (Peres 1994Peres, C.A. 1994. Which are the largest New World monkeys? Journal of Human Evolution, 26: 245-249.). Therefore, this failure can also be attributed to the junenile’s lack of experience. Spider monkeys apparently do not show observable responses to raptors and other flying pseudo-predators (van Roosmalen 1985van Roosmalen, M.G.M. 1985. Habitat preferences, diet, feeding strategy and social organization of the black spider monkey (Ateles paniscus Linnaeus, 1758) in Surinam. Acta Amazonica, 15: 1-238.; Mourthé and Barnett 2014Mourthé, I.; Barnett, A.A. 2014. Crying tapir: the functionality of errors and accuracy in predator recognition in two Neotropical high-canopy primates. Folia Primatologica, 85: 379-398.). In fact, there are only a few reported cases of spider monkey predation by eagles (Julliot 1994Julliot, C. 1994. Predation of a young spider monkey (Ateles paniscus) by a crested eagle (Morphnus guianensis). Folia Primatologica, 63: 75-77.; Miranda 2018Miranda, E.B. 2018. Prey composition of harpy eagles (Harpia harpyja) in Raleighvallen, Suriname. Tropical Conservation Science, 11: 1-8. ). Although it appeared that spider monkeys did not react here, they did immediately retreat from the tree in a manner that corresponds to a known anti-predatory response (Matsuda and Izawa 2008Matsuda, I.; Izawa, K. 2008. Predation of wild spider monkeys at La Macarena, Colombia. Primates, 49: 65-68.; Mourthé 2011Mourthé, I. 2011. Reactions of white-bellied spider monkeys to a predation attempt by a cougar. Neotropical Primates, 18: 28-29.).

In the third attempt, it was not possible to ascertain that the juvenile captured the monkey by itself as juvenile raptors are frequently provisioned by their parents, but there is evidence in favour of the former. Firstly, the prey was practically intact, with the exception of injuries on the head and where the claws had penetrated the body (Figure 1); secondly, the last recorded parental provisioning was on April 29th, 2017; and thirdly, the parents had not been observed near the nest since May 13th, 2017, strongly indicating that the juvenile hunted the monkey by itself.

Figure 1
Juvenile harpy eagle preying upon a recently captured adult Saimiri ustus in a forest fragment in southwestern Brazilian Amazonia. Credit: Thiago Cavalcante. This figure is in color in the electronic version.

The deadly wound to the front of the captured squirrel monkey’s head is similar to those reported by Martins et al. (2005Martins, S.S.; Lima, E.M.; Silva Jr, J.S. 2005. Predation of a bearded saki (Chiropotes utahicki) by a harpy eagle (Harpia harpyja). Neotropical Primates, 13: 7-10.) and de Luna et al. (2010de Luna, A.G.; Sanmiguel, R.; Di Fiore, A.; Fernandez-Duque, E. 2010. Predation and predation attempts on red titi monkeys (Callicebus discolor) and equatorial sakis (Pithecia aequatorialis) in Amazonian Ecuador. Folia Primatologica, 81: 86-95.). A necropsy report suggested that cerebral wounds were the cause of death of a bearded saki (Chiropotes utahicki) attacked by a harpy eagle in eastern Amazonia (Martins et al. 2005). This fatal injury may be important if the prey does not die at the first strike, thus avoiding counterattacks that may result in damage to the raptor (Martins et al. 2005). In extreme situations, failed attacks can be fatal to large raptors (Jones et al. 2006Jones, T.; Laurent, S.; Mselewa, F.; Mtui, A. 2006. Sanje mangabey Cercocebus sanjei kills an African crowned eagle Stephanoaetus coronatus. Folia Primatologica, 77: 359-363.). Therefore, it is important to kill the prey as fast as possible after it has been caught.

Capturing primates is not an easy task for an immature harpy eagle due to their anti-predatory behaviors (Ferrari 2009Ferrari, S.F. 2009. Predation risk and antipredator strategies. In: Garber, P.; Estrada, A.; Bicca-Marques, J.C.; Heymann, E.W.; Strier, K.B. (Eds.). South American Primates: Comparative perspectives in the study of behaviour, ecology and conservation. Springer, New York, p.251-278. ; de Luna et al. 2010de Luna, A.G.; Sanmiguel, R.; Di Fiore, A.; Fernandez-Duque, E. 2010. Predation and predation attempts on red titi monkeys (Callicebus discolor) and equatorial sakis (Pithecia aequatorialis) in Amazonian Ecuador. Folia Primatologica, 81: 86-95.; Barnett et al. 2011Barnett, A.A.; Schiel, V.; Deveny, A.; Valsko, J.; Spironello, W.R.; Ross, C. 2011. Predation on Cacajao ouakary and Cebus albifrons (Primates: Platyrrhini) by harpy eagles. Mammalia, 75: 169-172.; Mourthé and Barnett 2014Mourthé, I.; Barnett, A.A. 2014. Crying tapir: the functionality of errors and accuracy in predator recognition in two Neotropical high-canopy primates. Folia Primatologica, 85: 379-398.). Squirrel monkeys exhibit a wide anti-predatory repertoire, including vigilance, alarm-calls, changes in group size and habitat use, and the formation of mixed-species groups (Terborgh 1983Terborgh, J. 1983. Five new world primates: a study in comparative ecology. University Press, Princeton, 260p.; Boinski et al. 2003Boinski, S.; Kauffman, L.; Westoll, A.; Stickler, C.M.; Cropp, S.; Ehmke, E. 2003. Are vigilance, risk from avian predators and group size consequences of habitat structure? A comparison of three species of squirrel monkey (Saimiri oerstedii, S. boliviensis, and S. sciureus). Behaviour, 140: 1421-1467.). Despite all of these complex anti-predatory strategies, our study subject was able to hunt a monkey before reaching the dispersion age (Muñiz-López et al. 2012Muñiz-López, R.; Limiñana, R.; Cortés, G.D.; Urios, V. 2012. Movements of harpy eagles Harpia harpyja during their first two years after hatching. Bird Study, 59: 509-514.). The juvenile was seen for the last time approximately six months (January 13th, 2018) after its first assumed successful attack. It possibly dispersed since there were no further sightings on further 15 visits to the nest.

Our study indicates a gradual development of hunting skills by a post-fledging harpy eagle. Various raptors have similar chronological patterns (Shrubb 1982Shrubb, M. 1982. The hunting behaviour of some farmland kestrels. Bird Study, 29: 121-128.; Sherrod 1983Sherrod, S.K. 1983. Behavior of fledgling peregrines. Peregrine Fund, Ithaca, New York.; Johnson 1986Johnson, S. 1986. Development of hunting and self-sufficiency in juvenile red-tailed hawks (Buteo jamaicensis). Journal of Raptor Research, 20: 29-34.; Konrad and Gilmer 1986Konrad, P.M.; Gilmer, D.S. 1986. Post fledging behavior of ferruginous hawks in North Dakota. Journal of Raptor Research, 20: 35-39.). Hunting skills and confidence seem to increase with time and increasing experience as soon as flight skills are developed and the rates of parental provisioning decrease. Our observations raise the question of what triggers the dispersal of juvenile harpy eagles, since hunting ability, at least in this case, seems to have developed before dispersal. Thus the hypothesis of adulthood and independence in harpy eagles and other raptors being triggered by concomitant development of flight skills, decreasing rates of provisioning, and increasing rates of successful predation (e.g., McCann and Kemp 1994McCann, K.I.; Kemp, A.C. 1994. Hunting behaviour of a fledgling greater kestrel, Falco rupicoloides and its mother during the post-fledging period. Ostrich, 65: 1-6.) remains to be tested.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We are grateful to Valdir Moura from Fazenda Coqueiral, who kindly allowed us to collect data in his property, and Maria Cristina de Andrade Tuyama for her help during the fieldwork. We thank Claudia Keller and two anonymous reviewers for their criticisms, and Tim Vincent for the English review. IM thanks the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior−CAPES (Brazil) for a research fellowship (PNPD/CAPES).

  • Aguiar-Silva, F.H.; Sanaiotti, T.M.; Luz, B.B. 2014. Food habits of the harpy eagle, a top predator from the Amazonian rainforest canopy. Journal of Raptor Research, 48: 24-35.
  • Barnett, A.A.; Schiel, V.; Deveny, A.; Valsko, J.; Spironello, W.R.; Ross, C. 2011. Predation on Cacajao ouakary and Cebus albifrons (Primates: Platyrrhini) by harpy eagles. Mammalia, 75: 169-172.
  • Boinski, S.; Kauffman, L.; Westoll, A.; Stickler, C.M.; Cropp, S.; Ehmke, E. 2003. Are vigilance, risk from avian predators and group size consequences of habitat structure? A comparison of three species of squirrel monkey (Saimiri oerstedii, S. boliviensis, and S. sciureus). Behaviour, 140: 1421-1467.
  • de Luna, A.G.; Sanmiguel, R.; Di Fiore, A.; Fernandez-Duque, E. 2010. Predation and predation attempts on red titi monkeys (Callicebus discolor) and equatorial sakis (Pithecia aequatorialis) in Amazonian Ecuador. Folia Primatologica, 81: 86-95.
  • Ferrari, S.F. 2009. Predation risk and antipredator strategies. In: Garber, P.; Estrada, A.; Bicca-Marques, J.C.; Heymann, E.W.; Strier, K.B. (Eds.). South American Primates: Comparative perspectives in the study of behaviour, ecology and conservation Springer, New York, p.251-278.
  • Johnson, S. 1986. Development of hunting and self-sufficiency in juvenile red-tailed hawks (Buteo jamaicensis). Journal of Raptor Research, 20: 29-34.
  • Jones, T.; Laurent, S.; Mselewa, F.; Mtui, A. 2006. Sanje mangabey Cercocebus sanjei kills an African crowned eagle Stephanoaetus coronatus Folia Primatologica, 77: 359-363.
  • Julliot, C. 1994. Predation of a young spider monkey (Ateles paniscus) by a crested eagle (Morphnus guianensis). Folia Primatologica, 63: 75-77.
  • Kitowski, I. 2004. Play behaviour and active training of Montagu’s harrier (Circus pygargus) offspring in the post-fledging period. Journal of Ethology, 23: 3-8.
  • Kitowski, I. 2009. Social learning of hunting skills in juvenile marsh harriers Circus aeruginosus Journal of Ethology, 27: 327-332.
  • Konrad, P.M.; Gilmer, D.S. 1986. Post fledging behavior of ferruginous hawks in North Dakota. Journal of Raptor Research, 20: 35-39.
  • Land, M. 1999. The roles of head movements in the search and capture strategy of a tern (Aves, Laridae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 184: 265-272.
  • Lenz, B.B.; dos Reis, A.M. 2011. Harpy eagle-primate interactions in the central Amazon. The Wilson Journal of Ornithology, 123: 404-408.
  • Martins, S.S.; Lima, E.M.; Silva Jr, J.S. 2005. Predation of a bearded saki (Chiropotes utahicki) by a harpy eagle (Harpia harpyja). Neotropical Primates, 13: 7-10.
  • Matsuda, I.; Izawa, K. 2008. Predation of wild spider monkeys at La Macarena, Colombia. Primates, 49: 65-68.
  • McCann, K.I.; Kemp, A.C. 1994. Hunting behaviour of a fledgling greater kestrel, Falco rupicoloides and its mother during the post-fledging period. Ostrich, 65: 1-6.
  • Miranda, E.B. 2018. Prey composition of harpy eagles (Harpia harpyja) in Raleighvallen, Suriname. Tropical Conservation Science, 11: 1-8.
  • Mourthé, I. 2011. Reactions of white-bellied spider monkeys to a predation attempt by a cougar. Neotropical Primates, 18: 28-29.
  • Mourthé, I.; Barnett, A.A. 2014. Crying tapir: the functionality of errors and accuracy in predator recognition in two Neotropical high-canopy primates. Folia Primatologica, 85: 379-398.
  • Muñiz-López, R.; Limiñana, R.; Cortés, G.D.; Urios, V. 2012. Movements of harpy eagles Harpia harpyja during their first two years after hatching. Bird Study, 59: 509-514.
  • Penteriani, V.; Delgado, M.M. 2009. Thoughts on natal dispersal. Journal of Raptor Research, 43: 90-98.
  • Peres, C.A. 1994. Which are the largest New World monkeys? Journal of Human Evolution, 26: 245-249.
  • Rettig, N.L. 1978. Breeding behavior of the harpy eagle (Harpya harpyja). The Auk, 95: 629-643.
  • Sargent, T.D. 1990. Startle as an anti-predator mechanism, with special reference to the underwing moths, (Catocala). In: Evans, D.L.; Schmidt, J.O. (Eds.). Insect defenses: adaptive mechanisms and strategies of prey and predators State University of New York Press, Albany, p.229-249.
  • SEDAM. 2012. Secretaria de Estado do Desenvolvimento Ambiental, Rondônia, Brasil. Boletim climatológico de Rondônia - 2010 12: 1-34.
  • Sherrod, S.K. 1983. Behavior of fledgling peregrines Peregrine Fund, Ithaca, New York.
  • Shifferman, E.; Eilam, D. 2004. Movement and direction of movement of a simulated prey affect the success rate in barn owl Tyto alba attack. Journal of Avian Biology, 35: 111-116.
  • Shrubb, M. 1982. The hunting behaviour of some farmland kestrels. Bird Study, 29: 121-128.
  • Terborgh, J. 1983. Five new world primates: a study in comparative ecology University Press, Princeton, 260p.
  • Touchton, J.M.; Hsu, Y.; Palleroni, A. 2002. Foraging ecology of reintroduced captive-bred subadult harpy eagles (Harpia harpyja) on Barro Colorado Island, Panama. Ornitología Neotropical, 13: 365-379.
  • van Roosmalen, M.G.M. 1985. Habitat preferences, diet, feeding strategy and social organization of the black spider monkey (Ateles paniscus Linnaeus, 1758) in Surinam. Acta Amazonica, 15: 1-238.
  • Varland, D.E.; Klaas, E.E. 1991. Development of foraging behavior in the american kestrel. Journal of Raptor Research, 25: 9-17.
  • CITE AS:

    Cavalcante, T.; Tuyama, C.A.; Mourthe, I. 2019. Insights into the development of a juvenile harpy eagle’s hunting skills. Acta Amazonica 49: 114-117.

Edited by

Associate Editor:

Sergio Henrique Borges

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    06 May 2019
  • Date of issue
    Apr-Jun 2019

History

  • Received
    28 Sept 2018
  • Accepted
    12 Dec 2018
Instituto Nacional de Pesquisas da Amazônia Av. André Araujo, 2936 Aleixo, 69060-001 Manaus AM Brasil, Tel.: +55 92 3643-3030, Fax: +55 92 643-3223 - Manaus - AM - Brazil
E-mail: acta@inpa.gov.br