Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Factors affecting executive function performance of Brazilian elderly in the Stroop test

Abstract

Aging is related to a decrease in physiological abilities, especially cognitive functions. To unravel further evidence of age-related cognitive decline, we analyzed which physical and functional variables are predictors of cognitive performance in a sample of 498 Brazilian elderly (67.26% women). To do so, we used the Stroop test as a tool to evaluate executive functions and the General functional fitness index (GFFI) to evaluate the functional fitness of the participants. A linear regression analysis revealed that female sex (β=-0.097; t=-2.286; P=0.023), younger age (β=0.205; t=4.606; P<0.0001), more years of education (β=-0.280; t=-6.358; P<0.0001), and higher GFFI (β=-0.101; t=-2.347; P<0.02) were predictors of better cognitive performance. Body mass index (kg/m2) and nutritional status (underweight, eutrophic, overweight, or obese) were not predictors of cognitive performance. Interestingly, among the GFFI tasks, muscle strength influenced the test execution time, both in upper and lower limbs (elbow flexion: β=-0.201; t=-4.672; P<0.0001; sit-to-stand: β=-0.125; t=-2.580; P<0.01). Our findings showed that: 1) women performed the Stroop test faster than men; 2) the older the person, the lower was the cognitive performance; 3) the higher the education, the better the test execution time; and 4) higher scores in the GFFI were associated with a better performance in the Stroop test. Therefore, gender, age, education, and functional fitness and capacity were predictors of cognitive performance in the elderly.

Aging; Stroop test; Physical fitness; Muscle strength


Introduction

The world is facing a significant increase in the elderly population. The World Health Organization (WHO) estimates a population of 2 billion elderly people in the world by 2050, which will represent 22% of the global population, 80% of which will live in developing countries (11. Dey AB. World report on ageing and health. Indian J Med Res 2017; 145: 150-151, doi: 10.4103/0971-5916.207249.
https://doi.org/10.4103/0971-5916.207249...
). In Brazil, the elderly population tripled between the 1960s and 2010, and data from WHO state that by 2025 the number of elderly will increase to 32 million, making the country the sixth largest elderly population in the world (22. Campolina AG, Adami F, Santos JLF, Lebrão ML. A transição de saúde e as mudanças na expectativa de vida saudável da população idosa: possíveis impactos da prevenção de doenças crônicas [in Portuguese]. Cad Saude Publica 2013; 29: 1217-1229, doi: 10.1590/S0102-311X2013000600018.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-311X201300...
).

One of the major concerns of aging is the decline in functional capacity such as muscle strength, gait, balance, cognition, among others. In the elderly, loss of cognitive ability is associated with neurodegenerative diseases, which directly affect quality of life (33. Keith NR, Stump TE, Clark DO. Developing a self-reported physical fitness survey. Med Sci Sport Exerc 2012; 44: 1388-1394, doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31824bdc35.
https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e31824b...
). Both loss of cognition and decreased quality of life are evidenced in the study by Lebrão et al. (44. Lebrão ML, Laurenti R. Saúde, bem-estar e envelhecimento: o estudo SABE no Município de São Paulo [in Portuguese]. Rev Bras Epidemiol 2005, doi: 10.1590/S1415-790X2005000200005.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1415-790X200500...
). Using the Mini Mental State Examination in elderlies, a prevalence of cognitive impairment of 6.9% was found, affecting 4.2% of those aged 60-74 years and 17.7% of those aged 75 years and older. In this study, it was found that the greater the cognitive loss, the greater the dependence on third parties, directly impacting quality of life. In addition, other studies show a decline in cognitive ability, as aging is associated with neuronal death, decreased plasticity, and an increase in neurodegenerative diseases (55. Telegina DV, Suvorov GK, Kozhevnikova OS, Kolosova NG. Mechanisms of neuronal death in the cerebral cortex during aging and development of Alzheimer's disease-like pathology in rats. Int J Mol Sci 2019; 20: 5632, doi: 10.3390/ijms20225632.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms20225632...
).

Studies have highlighted the importance of preserving cognitive function during aging, especially executive function (66. Rivera D, Perrin PB, Stevens LF, Garza MT, Weil C, Saracho CP, et al. Stroop color-word interference test: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 2015; 37: 591-624, doi: 10.3233/NRE-151281.
https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-151281...
,77. Fjell AM, Sneve MH, Grydeland H, Storsve AB, Walhovd KB. The disconnected brain and executive function decline in aging. Cereb Cortex 2016; 27: 2303-2317, doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw082.
https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw082...
). Executive function is a set of integrated skills that enable people to direct their behaviors towards goals and carry out voluntary actions.

Several studies have shown that the older the age, the worse the score on cognitive tasks (66. Rivera D, Perrin PB, Stevens LF, Garza MT, Weil C, Saracho CP, et al. Stroop color-word interference test: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 2015; 37: 591-624, doi: 10.3233/NRE-151281.
https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-151281...
,88. da Silva AT, Duarte YAO, Santos JLF, Wong R, Lebrão ML. Sarcopenia according to the European working group on Sarcopenia in older people (Ewgsop) vs Dynapenia as a risk factor for mortality in the elderly. J Nutr Heal Aging 2014; 18: 751-756, doi: 10.1007/s12603-014-0540-2.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12603-014-0540-...
,99. Maruya K, Fujita H, Arai T, Hosoi T, Ogiwara K, Moriyama S, et al. Identifying elderly people at risk for cognitive decline by using the 2-step test. J Phys Ther Sci 2018; 30: 145-149, doi: 10.1589/jpts.30.145.
https://doi.org/10.1589/jpts.30.145...
). Among the neuropsychological tools used to access executive functions, the Stroop test (1010. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935; 18: 643-662, doi: 10.1037/h0054651.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651...
) is usually used. This test evaluates the ability to inhibit cognitive interference, that is, it generates a stimulus incongruity effect (1111. Scarpina F, Tagini S. The Stroop color and word test. Front Psychol 2017; 8: 1-8, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00557.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00557...
). For instance, an investigation that used the Stroop test found that there is a linear decline in executive functions with aging, regardless of processing speed, sex, and education (1212. Adólfsdóttir S, Wollschlaeger D, Wehling E, Lundervold AJ. Inhibition and switching in healthy aging: a longitudinal study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2017; 23: 90-97, doi: 10.1017/S1355617716000898.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561771600089...
). Tremblay et al. (1313. Tremblay MP, Potvin O, Belleville S, Bier N, Gagnon L, Blanchet S, et al. The victoria Stroop test: normative data in Quebec-French adults and elderly. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2016; 31: 926-933, doi: 10.1093/arclin/acw029.
https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acw029...
), when analyzing the performance of the Stroop test in the elderly, found that age was associated with worse performance in all attempts. A study conducted by Rivera et al. (66. Rivera D, Perrin PB, Stevens LF, Garza MT, Weil C, Saracho CP, et al. Stroop color-word interference test: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 2015; 37: 591-624, doi: 10.3233/NRE-151281.
https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-151281...
) reported that only two out of eleven countries presented differences between genders in the Stroop test. In addition to age and gender, other sociodemographic factors may be associated with performance on the Stroop test, such as schooling (1414. Burger L, Fay S, Angel L, Borella E, Noiret N, Plusquellec P, et al. Benefit of practice of the Stroop test in young and older adults: pattern of gain and impact of educational level. Exp Aging Res 2020; 46: 52-67, doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2019.1693013.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2019.16...
) and level of physical activity (1515. Himmelmeier RM, Nouchi R, Saito T, Burin D, Wiltfang J, Kawashima R. Study protocol: does an acute intervention of high-intensity physical exercise followed by a brain training video game have immediate effects on brain activity of older people during Stroop task in fMRI?-A randomized controlled trial with crossover design. Front Aging Neurosci 2019; 11, doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00260.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00260...
). Knowing that aging is associated with decreased cognitive ability and that this change can be modulated by factors such as age, gender, education, and physical fitness, the aim of the present study was to analyze physical and functional variables that can be predictive factors of cognitive performance in the elderly in Brazil. We used the General functional fitness index (GFFI) to evaluate functional fitness and the Stroop test to evaluate the executive functions of the participants.

Material and Methods

We conducted a cross-sectional study with a convenience and non-probabilistic sampling that was approved by the Ethics committee of the Federal University of São Paulo (CEP/UNIFESP - CAAE 57307016.9.0000.5505). To be included in the study, participants had to sign the informed consent form (466/2012-CNS/CONEP) and have a medical consent (issued up to 1 year) to perform physical tests (requirement of elderly centers). Those who presented musculoskeletal and/or neurological diseases that made it impossible to perform the functional capacity tests or were being treated for cognitive, psychological, or psychiatric illnesses and alcoholism were excluded. A total of 756 elderly people of both sexes were recruited from elderly centers in the Alto Tiete region of São Paulo state. Of these, 154 did not meet the inclusion criterion and 104 met the exclusion criterion, leaving a final sample of 498 participants (Figure 1).

Figure 1
Participant selection flow chart.

With the selected participants, the power of the study was calculated using the software G*Power version 3.1.7 (post hoc analysis; http://g-power.apponic.com), adopting an effect size of 25%, a 5% probability of error (P<0.05), a sample size of 498 volunteers, and 9 predictors, resulting in a power of 100%.

All evaluators and those involved in this study received prior training and performed the tests blindly.

Education and anthropometric data

Data were collected to determine years of education. Body mass index (BMI) was determined by the following equation: BMI = Weight / (Height)2.

General functional fitness index

The GFFI was initially adapted as proposed in the Senior Fit Test (1616. Rikli R, Jones C. Senior fitness test manual. Champaign: Human Kinetics 2001; ISBN: 0736033564 9780736033565.,1717. Rikli RE, Jones CJ. Development and validation of criterion-referenced clinically relevant fitness standards for maintaining physical independence in later years. Gerontologist 2013; 53: 255-267, doi: 10.1093/geront/gns071.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gns071...
). The GFFI model used in this study was composed of the following tests: elbow flexion and sit-to-stand (to analyze muscular strength), time to up and go (TUG) (for analysis of sitting balance, transfer from sitting to standing, walking stability, and changing gait course), and 6-min walk test (for aerobic capacity). Tests were conducted in the following order: 1) sit-to-stand; 2) elbow flexion; 3) TUG; and 4) 6-min walk. Each functional capacity test was individually scored as very weak, weak, regular, good, and very good, stratified for every five years of life (60-64; 65-69; etc.) and gender, as proposed by Rikli and Jones (1616. Rikli R, Jones C. Senior fitness test manual. Champaign: Human Kinetics 2001; ISBN: 0736033564 9780736033565.,1717. Rikli RE, Jones CJ. Development and validation of criterion-referenced clinically relevant fitness standards for maintaining physical independence in later years. Gerontologist 2013; 53: 255-267, doi: 10.1093/geront/gns071.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gns071...
). The GFFI was calculated by adding the scores of the functional capacity tests.

Stroop test

The Stroop test was used to evaluate cognitive function. Because the studied sample contained illiterate participants, we used the validated model by Kulaif and Valle (1818. Kulaif T, Valle LER. Alternative to the Stroop Color-Word Test for illiterate individuals. Clin Neuropsychol 2008; 22: 73-83, doi: 10.1080/13854040601186964.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1385404060118696...
) with numbers and colors, adapted from the Victoria version (1919. Strauss E, Sherman E, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. Neurology 2006; 41: 4-6, doi: 10.1212/WNL.41.11.1856-a.
https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.41.11.1856-a...
). The chosen model consists of 4 cards. The first card (Card 1 - Colors) contains red, green, blue, and black rectangles that need to be quickly named. The second card (Card 2 - colors and numbers) has the numbers 4, 5, 8, and 9 in the colored rectangles as in Card 1. In this step, the participant must name the number or the color of the rectangles. The third card (Card 3 - Stroop Effect) consists of colored numbers in the same colors as card 1, and the participant must name the color of the non-black numbers, while black numbers must be named by their respective number. The participant was informed that a certain degree of interference and consequently frustration was unavoidable. A 4th card (Card 4) with numbers 4, 5, 8, and 9 colored in black was added to this model to minimize frustration. The evaluation process is based on the time the participant takes in each of the cards and the number of errors. Spontaneous self-corrected errors were considered correct answers. In order to validate the results, it was assured that the participant would recognize and name the colors without hesitation.

Statistical analysis

The data of the Stroop test, educational level, nutritional status, functional capacities, and respective GFFI were analyzed with the general linear model (GLM) or chi-squared test. Results of GLM are reported as mean and standard deviation followed by the Fisher (F) value. The chi-squared test was conditioned to the interpretation of residuals (R=observed value minus expected value) and of the adjusted residuals (AR). The residual analysis is necessary to show which category has a significant value (P value) and the adjusted residual determines the significance level for the excess of occurrences. P values were considered significant when higher than 1.96. Added to this interpretation is the value of X2.

Correspondence analysis was also used. This analysis consists of a graphical representation in a flat projection of multidimensional relations of X2 distances between the categories of the studied variables. The symmetric projection was used, which allows the simultaneous examination of the relationships between rows and columns in the contingency table, that is, the relationships between all categories of both variables. Categories close to the flat projection have a stronger relationship than categories separated by greater distances. Any category, represented as a point in the projection plane, can be analyzed separately and characterized according to the proximity of the projections of all other categories, on a line that links its characteristic point to the origin of the axes of the projection plane. When categories of the same variable are found in close positions on the map of the correspondence analysis, this suggests that, regardless of their semantic content, they can be considered equal in terms of the mass distribution of the total observations made. When categories of contingent variables are projected close together, an association between the events they represent is suggested, although nothing is considered statistically significant. For statistical analysis, it is necessary to interpret adjusted residuals, as already described.

To analyze the predictive factors of the Stroop test (dependent variable - time of card 3), linear regression (stepwise model - forward) was used with the following independent variables: gender, age, nutritional status, education, and GFFI. For the analysis of which GFFI tests are predictors of the Stroop test time, linear regression (stepwise - forward model; dependent variable - time of card 3) was performed with the following independent variables: gender, elbow flexion, sit-to-stand, TUG, and 6-min walk tests. Thus, the analysis of the models adopted was done by the interpretation of the standardized and adjusted coefficient (β), followed by the analysis of the t and significance values from the Student's t-test. In addition, the interpretation of collinearity of variables is offered. If an independent variable presented more than one category, one of them was fixed to be used as reference. For example, for gender, the woman category was fixed and for the other independent variables, the last category was fixed. For all statistical tests, a level of significance lower than 5% was adopted.

Results

Table 1 shows a descriptive analysis of the sample (gender, age, BMI, nutritional status, and education). Women made up the majority of the sample (67.26 vs 32.73%; X2=59.406; P<0.0001). Non-significant differences between men and women were found for age (F=1.444; P=0.230), BMI (F=3.097; P=0.079), and nutritional status (X2=2.094; P=0.553).

Table 1
Descriptive characteristics of study participants.

The observed frequency in men was significantly higher than expected compared to women (X2=18.038; P<0.0001) in the following categories: 9 to 11 years of study (30.7 vs 18.8%) and >12 years of study (19.0 vs 11.9%), while in women, the observed frequency was higher than expected in the illiterate category (14.7 vs 25.1%; X2=18.564 P=0.0001).

Table 2 shows GFFI and functional capacities of participants as well as an association with gender. The observed value for the categories weak, regular, and good was higher than expected (26.1, 44.6, and 24.5% respectively; X2=319.811; P<0.0001). However, the observed frequency in men was higher than expected in the categories very weak compared to women (2.5 vs 0.3%; X2=11.094; P=0.026), while women participants had a higher than expected frequency in the weak classification compared to men (19.6 vs 29.3%; X2=11.094; P=0.026). When adding up the percentage of weak and regular classifications, 70.7% of the participants were in these categories.

Table 2
General linear model results for the General functional fitness index (GFFI) and functional capacity tests.

The observed value for the good and very good categories for strength of upper limbs assessed by the elbow flexion test was higher than expected (20.7 and 60.4%; X2=553.888; P<0.0001), indicating the maintenance of strength. Nonetheless, a significant difference between genders (X2=5.687; P=0.224) was found, with the observed value in men being higher than in women.

As for strength of lower limbs, measured by the sit-to-stand chair test, the observed frequencies of weak, regular, and good classifications were higher than expected (24.1, 22.5, and 21.5% respectively; X2=19.851; P<0.0001). However, in gender comparison, the observed frequency in men was higher than expected for the good classification (28.2 vs 18.2%. X2=14.636; P=0.006), while the observed frequency in women was higher in the very weak classification (7.4 vs 15.2%; X2=14.636; P=0.006). These findings showed that men presented ideal values for this functional capacity while women presented values below the recommended level.

In the TUG test, observed values were higher than expected in the categories very weak and weak (45.6 vs 25.1%; X2=63.380 and 282.201; P<0.0001, respectively). There was no significant difference between gender for this test (P=0.058). It is possible to conclude that this functional capacity was worse than the recommendation in our sample.

Finally, in the 6-min walk test, the observed values were higher than expected in the categories very weak, weak, and regular (28.7, 33.3, and 23.7% respectively; X2=160.173; P<0.0001). However, no significant difference was observed between genders (X2=5.149; P=0.272), indicating that the aerobic capacity in men and women was below the regular, presenting an unsatisfactory result.

Figure 2 is a graphical representation of the results of Table 2. In a first analysis, it was possible to observe a representativeness of 99.6% of the variations of the quadratic-chi distances. The removal of the classifications very weak and very good for the GFFI, very weak for the elbow flexion, and very good for the 6-min walk test showed that such classifications were not common for the studied population. Another analysis was the classifications of functional capacities. As they had small distances from each other, they were considered equivalent, which supports the grouping by GFFI. Finally, Figure 2 suggests that the distribution of the variables was similar between genders, as the formation of a cloud is evident in the upper right quadrant of the map. This information adds to the study of functional capacities, as it presents a new perspective by quadratic-chi analysis.

Figure 2
Map of the relationship between genders, GFFI classifications, and functional capacity test classifications. GFFI: General functional fitness index; TUG: time to up and go.

Table 3 shows the Stroop test values, time spent at each card, errors, interference, and time spent at card 3 compared to card 1. Elderly women performed the tasks of cards 1 and 2 faster than elderly men (18.89±6.02 vs 20.90±10.44 s; F=7.280; P=0.007; 21.32±7.42 vs 23.27±12.00 s; F=4.958; P=0.026). Women had fewer errors in card 2 than men (0.31±0.9 vs 0.53±1.27 errors; F=4.910; P=0.027). No significant difference between genders was found for card 3 (Stroop effect), card 4, and interference neither for time nor for error counting. Yet, a significant difference was found between genders for both average time spent in card 3 compared to card 1 and error counts (F=9.265 and F=8.132; P<0.0001 for time and errors). Thus, the Stroop effect was not significantly different between genders, but gender difference was evident between different cards.

Table 3
Time in seconds taken to complete the card tasks, errors, and interference of the Stroop test.

Table 4 shows the predictors of performance in the Stroop test. In this model, with the analysis made by the interpretation of the standardized and adjusted coefficient (β) of the independent variables gender, age, education, and GFFI, it was observed that elderly women performed the task 0.097 s faster than elderly men (t=-2.286; P=0.023). Regarding age, each year of life added 0.205 s in task execution (t=4.606; P<0.0001). In addition, the higher the education, the less the time required to finish the test, 0.28 s faster (t=-6.358; P<0.0001). Finally, the higher the GFFI, the less the time required to complete the task, 0.10 s faster (t=-2.347; P=0.019). Nutritional status was excluded from the model for not being a predictor for the Stroop test performance in the study population. In brief, these findings show that gender (women), age (younger), schooling (higher), and GFFI (higher) were predictors for better performance in the Stroop test.

Table 4
Predictive factors of Stroop test performance.

Since the GFFI is composed of four distinct tests, and these combined with gender contribute to the composition of the index, we enlightened the concept of functional capacities and gender being predictors of performance in the Stroop test (Table 5). Elbow flexion and the sit-to-stand tests were predictors of performance, in which the better the performance on these tests, the lower the time needed to perform the Stroop test: 0.201 s faster (t=-4.672; P<0.0001) for elbow flexion and 0.125 s faster (t=-2.580; P=0.01) for the sit-to-stand test. Gender and functional capacities tests (TUG and 6-min walk) were excluded from the model for not being predictors of Stroop test performance. Finally, our findings suggested that muscle strength (both in upper and lower limbs) was a predictor of Stroop test performance.

Table 5
Predictive factors of Stroop test performance in relation to the components of General functional fitness index.

Discussion

Aging is associated with a decrease in physiological capacities, especially functional and cognitive abilities. In the present study, we aimed to analyze predictive factors for cognitive performance in a Brazilian elderly sample using the Stroop test as a tool to evaluate selective attention and executive functions. Our results indicated that gender (women), age (younger), education (higher), and GFFI (higher) were predictive factors for the Stroop test performance. Moreover, among the tests of the GFFI, only muscle strength was a potential predictor for Stroop test performance.

In the social context, the current findings suggested that age and educational level were crucial factors when planning programs for cognitive rehabilitation. Furthermore, the more the years of education, the greater the gain in cognitive function. Our results were consistent with other studies (2020. Burger L, Fay S, Angel L, Borella E, Noiret N, Plusquellec P, et al. Benefit of practice of the Stroop Test in young and older adults: pattern of gain and impact of educational level. Exp Aging Res 2020; 46: 52-67, doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2019.1693013.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2019.16...
). In the study by Tian et al. (2121. Tian Q, Osawa Y, Resnick SM, Ferrucci L, Studenski SA. Rate of muscle contraction is associated with cognition in women, not in men. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2019; 74: 714-719, doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly115.
https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/gly115...
), female gender showed significantly better executive function, attention, and memory compared to male gender. However, there is still no consensus in the literature, as in the study by Rivera et al. (66. Rivera D, Perrin PB, Stevens LF, Garza MT, Weil C, Saracho CP, et al. Stroop color-word interference test: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 2015; 37: 591-624, doi: 10.3233/NRE-151281.
https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-151281...
), where only two out of eleven countries showed differences between genders in the Stroop test.

Another predictor of cognitive function was functional fitness. Other studies show similar results, where physically active elderly women showed better executive function compared to men (2222. Barha CK, Davis JC, Falck RS, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T. Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front Neuroendocrinol 2017; 46: 71-85, doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.002.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04....
,2323. Humphries KH, Izadnegahdar M, Sedlak T, Saw J, Johnston N, Schenck-Gustafsson K, et al. Sex differences in cardiovascular disease - Impact on care and outcomes. Front Neuroendocrinol 2017; 46: 46-70, doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04....
). In addition, factors such as lifestyle and physical and functional performance, especially at the end of adult life and the beginning of old age, are directly related to cognitive performance, especially executive functions (2424. Kamradt-Scott A. The International Health Regulations (2005). Int Organ Law Rev 2019; 16: 242-271, doi: 10.1163/15723747-01602002.
https://doi.org/10.1163/15723747-0160200...
). Thus, the data confirmed that the higher the functional fitness, the better the preservation of cognitive functions.

Among the functional capacities, upper limb strength was preserved in both genders in our sample. Data regarding this subject are controversial. For instance, our data differ from the study by dos Santos et al. (2525. dos Santos CCC, Pedrosa R, Costa FA da, de Mendonça KMPP, Holanda GM. Análise da função cognitiva e capacidade funcional em idosos hipertensos [in Portuguese]. Rev Bras Geriatr Gerontol 2011; 14: 241-250, doi: 10.1590/S1809-98232011000200006.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1809-9823201100...
) in which the healthy elderly group was classified in the very weak category. However, in a study by Souza et al. (2626. Souza LH, Brandão JCS, Fernandes AKC, Cardoso BLC. Queda em idosos e fatores de riscos associados [in Portuguese]. Rev Bras Ciências Saúde 2017; 15, doi: 10.13037/ras.vol15n54.4804.
https://doi.org/10.13037/ras.vol15n54.48...
), in which elderly subjects underwent an eight-week functional training program, the volunteers made an average of 33.90±4.09 movements, which is considered very good for the age.

As for lower limb strength, elderly men had better than expected values, while more women were included in the very weak category. The association between lower limb strength and the probability of hospitalization has been reported, showing that participants with weaker legs have a higher prevalence of hospital admission (2727. Batsis JA, Mackenzie TA, Barre LK, Lopez-Jimenez F, Bartels SJ. Sarcopenia, sarcopenic obesity and mortality in older adults: results from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey III. Eur J Clin Nutr 2014; 68: 1001-1007, doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2014.117.
https://doi.org/10.1038/ejcn.2014.117...
).

Participants of both sexes were classified as very weak and weak in the TUG test. The TUG test has been widely used in clinical practice and in research for the evaluation of sarcopenia, survival, functional mobility, and risk of falls in the elderly community (2828. Nascimento M, Appell IC, Coriolano HJA. Teste de equilíbrio corporal (TEC) para idosos independentes [in Portuguese]. Rev Port Ciências Desporto 2012; 12: 71-81, doi: 10.5628/rpcd.12.02.71.
https://doi.org/10.5628/rpcd.12.02.71...
). Elderlies with values below the recommended have greater levels of fragility and morbidity (2929. Diz JBM, Leopoldino AAO, Moreira BS, Nicholas H, Rosangela CD, Leani SMP, et al. Prevalence of sarcopenia in older Brazilians: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Geriatr Gerontol Int 2017; 17: 5-16, doi: 10.1111/ggi.12720.
https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12720...
).

The results of the 6-min walk test showed that the aerobic capacity of participants of both sexes was unsatisfactory. Similar to TUG, the 6-min walk test has been widely used as a scientific instrument because of its high correlation with morbidity/mortality (3030. Billinger SA, Arena R, Bernhardt J, Eng JJ, Franklin BA, Johnson CM, et al. Physical activity and exercise recommendations for stroke survivors: A statement for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke 2014; 45: 2532-2553, doi: 10.1161/STR.0000000000000022.
https://doi.org/10.1161/STR.000000000000...
). Cahalin et al. (3131. Cahalin LP, Mathier MA, Semigran MJ, Dec GW, DiSalvo TG. The six-minute walk test predicts peak oxygen uptake and survival in patients with advanced heart failure. Chest 1996; 110: 325-332, doi: 10.1378/chest.110.2.325.
https://doi.org/10.1378/chest.110.2.325...
) found that the probability of death/hospitalization in individuals who walked less than 300 m in 6 min was high. In addition, in a study with participants aged 68.9±5.4 years, women walked 521.7±71.5 m while men walked 584.2±82.6 m, which was significantly more. In addition, both these values would be considered weak in our protocol. Our data corroborated those findings, since women presented worse performance compared to men.

Since GFFI was a predictor of executive functions, we sought to verify which of the GFFI tests was contributing to the result. Muscle strength was the only factor associated with performance in the Stroop test. Unlike our results, the literature shows that both acute aerobic and strength exercises improve executive functions (3030. Billinger SA, Arena R, Bernhardt J, Eng JJ, Franklin BA, Johnson CM, et al. Physical activity and exercise recommendations for stroke survivors: A statement for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke 2014; 45: 2532-2553, doi: 10.1161/STR.0000000000000022.
https://doi.org/10.1161/STR.000000000000...
,3232. Dunsky A, Abu-Rukun M, Tsuk S, Dwolatzky T, Carasso R, Netz Y. The effects of a resistance vs. an aerobic single session on attention and executive functioning in adults. PLoS One 2017; 12: e0176092, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0176092.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.017...
). However, there is an increasing number of studies showing the relationship between higher muscle strength and improvement of cognitive function. In older women, quadriceps strength is associated with executive functions (i.e., attention/working memory), independent of aerobic fitness (3333. Verburgh L, Königs M, Scherder EJA, Oosterlaan J. Physical exercise and executive functions in preadolescent children, adolescents and young adults: a meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med 2014; 48: 973-979, doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091441.
https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2012-09...
). Elderly women who practiced resistance training and had increased muscle strength had a significantly better performance on the Stroop test (3434. Dao E, Davis JC, Sharma D, Chan A, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T. Change in body fat mass is independently associated with executive functions in older women: a secondary analysis of a 12-month randomized controlled trial. PLoS One 2013; 8: e52831, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052831.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.005...
). Elderly women with a greater rate of torque had faster simple tapping speed and better language performance. Moreover, a greater rate of velocity development in the upper extremity was associated with better executive functions, attention, and memory (1212. Adólfsdóttir S, Wollschlaeger D, Wehling E, Lundervold AJ. Inhibition and switching in healthy aging: a longitudinal study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2017; 23: 90-97, doi: 10.1017/S1355617716000898.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561771600089...
). In addition, higher levels of physical functioning (especially muscle strength) is associated with improved cognition in healthy older adults (3535. Desjardins-Crépeau L, Berryman N, Vu TTM, Villalpando JM, Kergoat MJ, Li KZ, et al. Physical functioning is associated with processing speed and executive functions in community-dwelling older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2014; 69: 837-844, doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu036.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbu036...
).

Several studies have found that sarcopenia is associated with decreased cognitive ability (44. Lebrão ML, Laurenti R. Saúde, bem-estar e envelhecimento: o estudo SABE no Município de São Paulo [in Portuguese]. Rev Bras Epidemiol 2005, doi: 10.1590/S1415-790X2005000200005.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1415-790X200500...
,3636. Cruz-Jentoft AJ, Landi F, Schneider SM, Clemente Z, Arai H, Boirei Y, et al. Prevalence of and interventions for sarcopenia in ageing adults: a systematic review. Report of the International Sarcopenia Initiative (EWGSOP and IWGS). Age Ageing 2014; 43: 748-759, doi: 10.1093/ageing/afu115.
https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afu115...
). In addition, elderly people who have lower muscle mass index (muscle mass-to-body surface ratio) have lower cognitive function (3737. Kim M, Won CW. Sarcopenia is associated with cognitive impairment mainly due to slow gait speed: results from the Korean Frailty and Aging Cohort Study (KFACS). Int J Environ Res Public Health 2019; 16: 1491, doi: 10.3390/ijerph16091491.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16091491...
). Furthermore, our results were similar to those of Smolarek et al. (3838. Smolarek AC, Boiko Ferreira LH, Gomes Mascarenhas LP, McAnulty SR, Varela KD, Dangui MC, et al. The effects of strength training on cognitive performance in elderly women. Clin Interv Aging 2016; 11: 749-754, doi: 10.2147/CIA.S102126.
https://doi.org/10.2147/CIA.S102126...
), who found that significant gains in muscle strength are associated with cognitive improvements in the elderly.

Finally, the majority of studies address levels of physical fitness through questionnaires (a subjective assessment) or isolated functional capacity tests, rather than through an index. In this study, we used an index and the classification proposed by Rikli and Jones (1616. Rikli R, Jones C. Senior fitness test manual. Champaign: Human Kinetics 2001; ISBN: 0736033564 9780736033565.,1717. Rikli RE, Jones CJ. Development and validation of criterion-referenced clinically relevant fitness standards for maintaining physical independence in later years. Gerontologist 2013; 53: 255-267, doi: 10.1093/geront/gns071.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gns071...
), as mentioned previously. In this way, the natural aging process and the gradual decline in functional capacity are taken into account when classifying individuals (e.g., the result of 17±6.3 movements in the elbow flexion test for 64-year-old men is classified as regular, while it is classified as good for 75-year-olds).

The limitations of our study should be mentioned to guide future investigations. First, there were no tools to equalize the initial cognitive status of participants or even factors that could interfere with their cognitive function. Second, there is a lack of instruments available for cognitive assessment, and it would be interesting to associate multiple tests to assess executive cognitive function or tasks for other cognitive domains (e.g., processing speed). However, due to the number of participants and research volunteers that worked in this study, we were unable to perform further cognitive analyses. The average time spent applying the tests was 60 min per person, with 30% of this time used for the Stroop test.

Nevertheless, our results showed that GFFI was a predictor of executive capacity as measured by the Stroop test. We also observed that muscle strength played an important role in executive function. This information adds to the existing literature and reinforces the attention that should be paid to the reduction of muscle mass and especially muscle strength in the elderly, as they are much more prone to sarcopenia. In this sense, the present study indicated an interesting association between muscle strength and cognitive capacity. These findings are also relevant for healthcare professionals, since they indicate the importance of maintaining functional capacities (especially muscle strength) for the maintenance of cognitive function in the elderly. Improving executive functions can be a valuable asset in the quality of life of older adults. Once predictors of cognitive performance are known, the scientific and medical communities will have more accurate subsidies for the treatment/prevention of cognitive decline.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Ivan de Oliveira Gonçalves, Yaraci Santos, Auclésio Ranieri, and students of Physical Education from Clube Náutico Mogiano College for their technical assistance and support. This work was supported by Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior - Brasil (CAPES-PRINT #88881.310490/2018-01), Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq #301732/2018-3; #408676/2018-3), Fundação de Amparo ao Ensino e Pesquisa (FAEP), and Fundação de Amparo è Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (FAPESP; #2014/00035-1; #2018/16565-0).

References

  • 1
    Dey AB. World report on ageing and health. Indian J Med Res 2017; 145: 150-151, doi: 10.4103/0971-5916.207249.
    » https://doi.org/10.4103/0971-5916.207249
  • 2
    Campolina AG, Adami F, Santos JLF, Lebrão ML. A transição de saúde e as mudanças na expectativa de vida saudável da população idosa: possíveis impactos da prevenção de doenças crônicas [in Portuguese]. Cad Saude Publica 2013; 29: 1217-1229, doi: 10.1590/S0102-311X2013000600018.
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-311X2013000600018
  • 3
    Keith NR, Stump TE, Clark DO. Developing a self-reported physical fitness survey. Med Sci Sport Exerc 2012; 44: 1388-1394, doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31824bdc35.
    » https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e31824bdc35
  • 4
    Lebrão ML, Laurenti R. Saúde, bem-estar e envelhecimento: o estudo SABE no Município de São Paulo [in Portuguese]. Rev Bras Epidemiol 2005, doi: 10.1590/S1415-790X2005000200005.
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1415-790X2005000200005
  • 5
    Telegina DV, Suvorov GK, Kozhevnikova OS, Kolosova NG. Mechanisms of neuronal death in the cerebral cortex during aging and development of Alzheimer's disease-like pathology in rats. Int J Mol Sci 2019; 20: 5632, doi: 10.3390/ijms20225632.
    » https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms20225632
  • 6
    Rivera D, Perrin PB, Stevens LF, Garza MT, Weil C, Saracho CP, et al. Stroop color-word interference test: normative data for the Latin American Spanish speaking adult population. NeuroRehabilitation 2015; 37: 591-624, doi: 10.3233/NRE-151281.
    » https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-151281
  • 7
    Fjell AM, Sneve MH, Grydeland H, Storsve AB, Walhovd KB. The disconnected brain and executive function decline in aging. Cereb Cortex 2016; 27: 2303-2317, doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw082.
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw082
  • 8
    da Silva AT, Duarte YAO, Santos JLF, Wong R, Lebrão ML. Sarcopenia according to the European working group on Sarcopenia in older people (Ewgsop) vs Dynapenia as a risk factor for mortality in the elderly. J Nutr Heal Aging 2014; 18: 751-756, doi: 10.1007/s12603-014-0540-2.
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12603-014-0540-2
  • 9
    Maruya K, Fujita H, Arai T, Hosoi T, Ogiwara K, Moriyama S, et al. Identifying elderly people at risk for cognitive decline by using the 2-step test. J Phys Ther Sci 2018; 30: 145-149, doi: 10.1589/jpts.30.145.
    » https://doi.org/10.1589/jpts.30.145
  • 10
    Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935; 18: 643-662, doi: 10.1037/h0054651.
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651
  • 11
    Scarpina F, Tagini S. The Stroop color and word test. Front Psychol 2017; 8: 1-8, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00557.
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00557
  • 12
    Adólfsdóttir S, Wollschlaeger D, Wehling E, Lundervold AJ. Inhibition and switching in healthy aging: a longitudinal study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2017; 23: 90-97, doi: 10.1017/S1355617716000898.
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617716000898
  • 13
    Tremblay MP, Potvin O, Belleville S, Bier N, Gagnon L, Blanchet S, et al. The victoria Stroop test: normative data in Quebec-French adults and elderly. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2016; 31: 926-933, doi: 10.1093/arclin/acw029.
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acw029
  • 14
    Burger L, Fay S, Angel L, Borella E, Noiret N, Plusquellec P, et al. Benefit of practice of the Stroop test in young and older adults: pattern of gain and impact of educational level. Exp Aging Res 2020; 46: 52-67, doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2019.1693013.
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2019.1693013
  • 15
    Himmelmeier RM, Nouchi R, Saito T, Burin D, Wiltfang J, Kawashima R. Study protocol: does an acute intervention of high-intensity physical exercise followed by a brain training video game have immediate effects on brain activity of older people during Stroop task in fMRI?-A randomized controlled trial with crossover design. Front Aging Neurosci 2019; 11, doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00260.
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00260
  • 16
    Rikli R, Jones C. Senior fitness test manual. Champaign: Human Kinetics 2001; ISBN: 0736033564 9780736033565.
  • 17
    Rikli RE, Jones CJ. Development and validation of criterion-referenced clinically relevant fitness standards for maintaining physical independence in later years. Gerontologist 2013; 53: 255-267, doi: 10.1093/geront/gns071.
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gns071
  • 18
    Kulaif T, Valle LER. Alternative to the Stroop Color-Word Test for illiterate individuals. Clin Neuropsychol 2008; 22: 73-83, doi: 10.1080/13854040601186964.
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/13854040601186964
  • 19
    Strauss E, Sherman E, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary. Neurology 2006; 41: 4-6, doi: 10.1212/WNL.41.11.1856-a.
    » https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.41.11.1856-a
  • 20
    Burger L, Fay S, Angel L, Borella E, Noiret N, Plusquellec P, et al. Benefit of practice of the Stroop Test in young and older adults: pattern of gain and impact of educational level. Exp Aging Res 2020; 46: 52-67, doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2019.1693013.
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2019.1693013
  • 21
    Tian Q, Osawa Y, Resnick SM, Ferrucci L, Studenski SA. Rate of muscle contraction is associated with cognition in women, not in men. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2019; 74: 714-719, doi: 10.1093/gerona/gly115.
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/gly115
  • 22
    Barha CK, Davis JC, Falck RS, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T. Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front Neuroendocrinol 2017; 46: 71-85, doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.002.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.002
  • 23
    Humphries KH, Izadnegahdar M, Sedlak T, Saw J, Johnston N, Schenck-Gustafsson K, et al. Sex differences in cardiovascular disease - Impact on care and outcomes. Front Neuroendocrinol 2017; 46: 46-70, doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.001.
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.001
  • 24
    Kamradt-Scott A. The International Health Regulations (2005). Int Organ Law Rev 2019; 16: 242-271, doi: 10.1163/15723747-01602002.
    » https://doi.org/10.1163/15723747-01602002
  • 25
    dos Santos CCC, Pedrosa R, Costa FA da, de Mendonça KMPP, Holanda GM. Análise da função cognitiva e capacidade funcional em idosos hipertensos [in Portuguese]. Rev Bras Geriatr Gerontol 2011; 14: 241-250, doi: 10.1590/S1809-98232011000200006.
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1809-98232011000200006
  • 26
    Souza LH, Brandão JCS, Fernandes AKC, Cardoso BLC. Queda em idosos e fatores de riscos associados [in Portuguese]. Rev Bras Ciências Saúde 2017; 15, doi: 10.13037/ras.vol15n54.4804.
    » https://doi.org/10.13037/ras.vol15n54.4804
  • 27
    Batsis JA, Mackenzie TA, Barre LK, Lopez-Jimenez F, Bartels SJ. Sarcopenia, sarcopenic obesity and mortality in older adults: results from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey III. Eur J Clin Nutr 2014; 68: 1001-1007, doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2014.117.
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/ejcn.2014.117
  • 28
    Nascimento M, Appell IC, Coriolano HJA. Teste de equilíbrio corporal (TEC) para idosos independentes [in Portuguese]. Rev Port Ciências Desporto 2012; 12: 71-81, doi: 10.5628/rpcd.12.02.71.
    » https://doi.org/10.5628/rpcd.12.02.71
  • 29
    Diz JBM, Leopoldino AAO, Moreira BS, Nicholas H, Rosangela CD, Leani SMP, et al. Prevalence of sarcopenia in older Brazilians: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Geriatr Gerontol Int 2017; 17: 5-16, doi: 10.1111/ggi.12720.
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.12720
  • 30
    Billinger SA, Arena R, Bernhardt J, Eng JJ, Franklin BA, Johnson CM, et al. Physical activity and exercise recommendations for stroke survivors: A statement for healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke 2014; 45: 2532-2553, doi: 10.1161/STR.0000000000000022.
    » https://doi.org/10.1161/STR.0000000000000022
  • 31
    Cahalin LP, Mathier MA, Semigran MJ, Dec GW, DiSalvo TG. The six-minute walk test predicts peak oxygen uptake and survival in patients with advanced heart failure. Chest 1996; 110: 325-332, doi: 10.1378/chest.110.2.325.
    » https://doi.org/10.1378/chest.110.2.325
  • 32
    Dunsky A, Abu-Rukun M, Tsuk S, Dwolatzky T, Carasso R, Netz Y. The effects of a resistance vs. an aerobic single session on attention and executive functioning in adults. PLoS One 2017; 12: e0176092, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0176092.
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0176092
  • 33
    Verburgh L, Königs M, Scherder EJA, Oosterlaan J. Physical exercise and executive functions in preadolescent children, adolescents and young adults: a meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med 2014; 48: 973-979, doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091441.
    » https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2012-091441
  • 34
    Dao E, Davis JC, Sharma D, Chan A, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T. Change in body fat mass is independently associated with executive functions in older women: a secondary analysis of a 12-month randomized controlled trial. PLoS One 2013; 8: e52831, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052831.
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052831
  • 35
    Desjardins-Crépeau L, Berryman N, Vu TTM, Villalpando JM, Kergoat MJ, Li KZ, et al. Physical functioning is associated with processing speed and executive functions in community-dwelling older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2014; 69: 837-844, doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu036.
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbu036
  • 36
    Cruz-Jentoft AJ, Landi F, Schneider SM, Clemente Z, Arai H, Boirei Y, et al. Prevalence of and interventions for sarcopenia in ageing adults: a systematic review. Report of the International Sarcopenia Initiative (EWGSOP and IWGS). Age Ageing 2014; 43: 748-759, doi: 10.1093/ageing/afu115.
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afu115
  • 37
    Kim M, Won CW. Sarcopenia is associated with cognitive impairment mainly due to slow gait speed: results from the Korean Frailty and Aging Cohort Study (KFACS). Int J Environ Res Public Health 2019; 16: 1491, doi: 10.3390/ijerph16091491.
    » https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16091491
  • 38
    Smolarek AC, Boiko Ferreira LH, Gomes Mascarenhas LP, McAnulty SR, Varela KD, Dangui MC, et al. The effects of strength training on cognitive performance in elderly women. Clin Interv Aging 2016; 11: 749-754, doi: 10.2147/CIA.S102126.
    » https://doi.org/10.2147/CIA.S102126

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    27 Apr 2022
  • Date of issue
    2022

History

  • Received
    2 Nov 2021
  • Accepted
    15 Feb 2022
Associação Brasileira de Divulgação Científica Av. Bandeirantes, 3900, 14049-900 Ribeirão Preto SP Brazil, Tel. / Fax: +55 16 3315-9120 - Ribeirão Preto - SP - Brazil
E-mail: bjournal@terra.com.br