Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Major Depressive Disorder: A Comparative Study on Social-Emotional Cognition and Executive Functions

Abstract

The present study aimed to assess socioemotional cognition and executive functions in patients with unipolar Major Depressive Disorder. The sample included 22 patients between 36 and 93 years of age (M = 59.32; SD = 12.89) and 23 patients between 30 and 81 years of age (M = 63.00; SD = 13.56) controls. In addition to demographic data, symptoms of anxiety and depression, empathy, theory of mind, recognition of emotions, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility and phonemic verbal fluency were obtained. There was no statistical difference between the groups regarding age and education. Patients had significantly more anxiety, depression and personal distress than controls. Individuals with more severe depressive symptoms had a lower processing speed than the others.

Keywords:
Major Depressive Disorder; executive functions; socio-emotional cognition; empathy

Resumo

O objetivo deste estudo foi avaliar a cognição socioemocional e as funções executivas em pacientes com Transtorno Depressivo Maior unipolar. A amostra incluiu 22 pacientes entre 36 e 93 anos de idade (M = 59,32; DP = 12,89) e 23 indivíduos controles entre 30 e 81 anos de idade (M = 63,00; DP = 13,56). Além de dados demográficos, foram avaliados sintomas de ansiedade e de depressão, empatia, teoria da mente, reconhecimento de emoções, controle inibitório, flexibilidade cognitiva e fluência verbal. Não houve diferença estatística significativa entre os grupos quanto à idade e à escolaridade. Os pacientes apresentaram significativamente mais ansiedade, depressão e angústia pessoal do que os controles. Indivíduos com sintomas depressivos mais graves apresentaram menor velocidade de processamento.

Palavras-chave:
Transtorno Depressivo Maior; funções executivas; cognição socioemocional; empatia

Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) is a condition that leads to intense suffering and considerable functional impairment. MDD is characterized by depressed mood and/or loss of interest or pleasure, along with other symptoms, such as alterations in psychomotricity, appetite, sleep, and fatigue, as well as recurrent thoughts about excessive guilt, worthlessness, death, and cognitive impairments related to attention and decision-making skills. Because these symptoms occur nearly every day, they lead to intense suffering that affects all contexts of people's lives (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2014American Psychiatric Association. (2014). Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais - DSM-5. Artmed.).

Cognitive symptoms are currently included among the central aspects of MDD (Millan et al., 2012Millan, M. J., Agid, Y., Brüne, M., Bullmore, E. T., Carter, C. S., Clayton, N. S., Connor, R., Davis, S., Deakin, B., DeRubeis, R.J., Dubois, B., Geyer, M. A., Goodwin, G. M., Gorwood, P., Jay, T. M., Joëls, M., Mansuy, I. M., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Murphy, D., ... Young, L. J. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: Characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature reviews Drug discovery, 11(2), 141. https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrd3628
https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrd3628...
; Roca, Vives, Lopez-Navarro, Garcia-Campayo, & Gili, 2015Roca, M., Vives, M., Lopez-Navarro, E., Garcia-Campayo, J., & Gili, M. (2015). Cognitive impairments and depression: A critical review. Actas Espanolas de Psiquiatria, 43(5), 187-193.; Zuckerman et al., 2018Zuckerman, H., Pan, Z., Park, C., Brietzke, E., Musial, N., Shariq, A. S., Iacobucci, M., Yim, S. J., Lui, L., Rong, C., & McIntyre, R. S. (2018). Recognition and treatment of cognitive dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder.Frontiers in psychiatry ,9, 655. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00655
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00655...
). According to a review (Millan et al., 2012Millan, M. J., Agid, Y., Brüne, M., Bullmore, E. T., Carter, C. S., Clayton, N. S., Connor, R., Davis, S., Deakin, B., DeRubeis, R.J., Dubois, B., Geyer, M. A., Goodwin, G. M., Gorwood, P., Jay, T. M., Joëls, M., Mansuy, I. M., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Murphy, D., ... Young, L. J. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: Characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature reviews Drug discovery, 11(2), 141. https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrd3628
https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrd3628...
), the most common cognitive alterations documented in MDD include deficits in working memory, episodic memory, processing speed, and executive functions. In particular, a meta-analysis showed that MDD patients were impaired in several measures of executive functions, with similar effect sizes for all subcomponents assessed: inhibition, shifting, updating, verbal and visuospatial working memory, planning, and verbal fluency. However, inhibition might be impaired to a greater extent than the others (Snyder, 2013Snyder, H. R. (2013). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: A meta-analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 81-132. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028727
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028727...
).

Even though several studies have investigated cognitive impairment in MDD, they do not reveal a homogeneous impairment profile. The diversity of symptoms present in MDD and of instruments used to measure those symptoms might be (partially) responsible for the conflicting results reported across studies (Roca et al., 2015Roca, M., Vives, M., Lopez-Navarro, E., Garcia-Campayo, J., & Gili, M. (2015). Cognitive impairments and depression: A critical review. Actas Espanolas de Psiquiatria, 43(5), 187-193.). Research on socioemotional cognition, on the other hand, is a much more recent but growing field. Studies on this topic have found that deficits in socioemotional cognition may explain, at least in part, the impairments in social functioning observed in MDD (Wolkenstein, Schönenberg, Schirm, & Hautzinger, 2011Wolkenstein, L., Schönenberg, M., Schirm, E., & Hautzinger, M. (2011). I can see what you feel, but I can't deal with it: Impaired theory of mind in depression. Journal of affective disorders, 132(1-2), 104-111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.010
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.01...
).

The term social-emotional cognition (or social cognition) describes the adequacy of an individual’s behavior to their environment and includes cognitive processes that allow them to perceive, understand, and think about themselves and others (Beer & Ochsner, 2006Beer, J. S., & Ochsner, K. N. (2006). Social cognition: A multi level analysis. Brain Research, 1079(1), 98-105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006....
) by creating and manipulating mental representations of social relationships (Adolphs, 2003Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(3), 165-178. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1056
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1056...
). Empirical studies and meta-analyses with MDD patients have reported alterations in skills such as theory of mind ([TOM] Bora & Berk, 2016Bora, E., & Berk, M. (2016). Theory of mind in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 191, 49-55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.023
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.02...
), recognition of emotional facial expressions (Cusi et al. 2013Cusi, A. M., Nazarov, A., MacQueen, G. M., & McKinnon, M. C. (2013). Theory of mind deficits in patients with mild symptoms of major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 210(2), 672-674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.018
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013....
; Dalili, Penton-Voak, Harmer, & Munafo, 2015Dalili, M. N., Penton-Voak, I. S., Harmer, C. J., & Munafò, M. R. (2015). Meta-analysis of emotion recognition deficits in major depressive disorder. Psychological medicine, 45(6), 1135-1144. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714002591
https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171400259...
), and empathy (Cusi et al., 2011Cusi, A. M., MacQueen, G. M., Spreng, R. N., & McKinnon, M. C. (2011). Altered empathic responding in major depressive disorder: Relation to symptom severity, illness burden, and psychosocial outcome. Psychiatry Research, 188(2), 231-236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011....
; Inoue, Tonooka, & Yamada, 2004Inoue, Y., Tonooka, Y., Yamada, K., & Kanba, S. (2004). Deficiency of theory of mind in patients with remitted mood disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82(3), 403-409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2004.04.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2004.04.00...
; Lee, Harkness, Sabbagh, & Jacobson, 2005Lee, L., Harkness, K. L., Sabbagh, M. A., & Jacobson, J. A. (2005). Mental state decoding abilities in clinical depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 86(2-3), 247-258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.02.007
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.02.00...
; Uekermann, Channon, Lehmkämper, Abdel-Hamid, Vollmoeller, & Daum, 2008Uekermann, J., Channon, S., Lehmkämper, C., Abdel-Hamid, M., Vollmoeller, W., & Daum, I. (2008). Executive function, mentalizing and humor in major depression. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(1), 55-62. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617708080016
https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561770808001...
; Wang, Wang, Chen, Zhu, & Wang, 2008Wang, Y. G., Wang, Y. Q., Chen, S. L., Zhu, C. Y., & Wang, K. (2008). Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: A componential view. Psychiatry Research, 161(2), 153-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.018
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007....
). However, there is considerable discrepancy among the results of those studies (Hoertnagl & Hofer, 2014Hoertnagl, C. M., & Hofer, A. (2014). Social cognition in serious mental illness. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 27(3), 197-202. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000055
https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.000000000000...
). An aspect that may underlie this discrepancy is the clinical and pathophysiological heterogeneity of MDD (Arnow et al., 2015Arnow, B. A., Blasey, C., Williams, L. M., Palmer, D. M., Rekshan, W., Schatzberg, A. F., Etkin, A., Kulkarni, J., Luther, J.F., & Rush, A. J. (2015). Depression subtypes in predicting antidepressant response: A report from the iSPOT-D trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(8), 743-750. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14020181
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14...
; Bora & Berk, 2016Bora, E., & Berk, M. (2016). Theory of mind in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 191, 49-55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.023
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.02...
).

It is also noteworthy that social-emotional impairments are a transdiagnostic issue that may constitute a clinical marker. This was suggested by Cotter et al. (2018Cotter, J., Granger, K., Backx, R., Hobbs, M., Looi, C. Y., & Barnett, J. H. (2018). Social cognitive dysfunction as a clinical marker: A systematic review of meta-analyses across 30 clinical conditions.Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews , 84, 92-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.014
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017...
) in a systematic review of 31 meta-analyses investigating TOM and recognition of emotional facial expressions in individuals with psychiatric, neurological, or developmental disorders. Consistent impairments in those skills were found in almost all of the 30 clinical conditions investigated, with magnitudes similar to those found in more frequently investigated skills, such as memory and processing speed.

Considering components of social-emotional cognition and executive functions are related (Devine & Hughes, 2014Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta‐analysis. Child development, 85(5), 1777-1794. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12237
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12237...
; Santamaría-García et al., 2020Santamaría-García, H., Baez, S., Gómez, C., Rodríguez-Villagra, O., Huepe, D., Portela, M., Reyes, P., Klahr, J., Matallana, D., & Ibanez, A. (2020). The role of social cognition skills and social determinants of health in predicting symptoms of mental illness.Translational psychiatry , 10(1), 165. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-0852-4
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-0852-...
; Shahaeian, Henry, Razmjoee, Teymoori, & Wang, 2014Shahaeian, A., Henry, J. D., Razmjoee, M., Teymoori, A., & Wang, C. (2014). Towards a better understanding of the relationship between executive control and theory of mind: An intra-cultural comparison of three diverse samples. Developmental Science, 18(5), 671-685. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12243
https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12243...
), it is important to investigate these constructs concomitantly in patients with MDD (Cusi et al., 2011Cusi, A. M., MacQueen, G. M., Spreng, R. N., & McKinnon, M. C. (2011). Altered empathic responding in major depressive disorder: Relation to symptom severity, illness burden, and psychosocial outcome. Psychiatry Research, 188(2), 231-236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011....
). The present study aims to (a) compare individuals with unipolar MDD and a control group in tasks of social-emotional cognition (recognition of emotional facial expressions, TOM, and empathy) and executive functions (inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and phonemic verbal fluency); (b) investigate associations between test performance and diagnosis-related variables (time since diagnosis, age at first episode, and duration of treatment); and, (c) create subgroups according to severity of depressive symptoms and compare the performance of both groups on tests of social-emotional cognition and executive functions.

Considering the literature reviewed above, we expected lower performance in the MDD group compared to the control group in measures of both social-emotional cognition and executive functions. We expected to a similar result in the comparison between groups defined by severity of depressive symptoms, with the highest severity group showing lowest performance.

Method

Because the goal of this study was to describe characteristics of a group of patients with MDD and compare them to a healthy control group, a descriptive-correlational design was adopted.

Participants

The sampling process was non-probabilistic. Participants from the Brazilian states of Minas Gerais and Rio Grande do Sul were invited to participate in the study through the Municipal Health Department of Osório (in Rio Grande do Sul), seniors’ groups, health institutions, and researchers’ acquaintances. Figure 1 shows participants invited at each institution and the absolute frequency of invitations, inclusions, and exclusions of participants.

Figure 1.
Flowchart of participant selection procedure.

Figure 2.
Flowchart of data collection procedures and instruments. MDD = Major Depressive Disorder; MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination; FAB = Frontal Assessment Battery; IRI = Interpersonal Reactivity Index; MINI-SEA = Social Cognition and Emotional Assessment, abbreviated version.

As shown in Figure 1, a total of 131 individuals were invited to participate in the study and 45 comprised the final sample. All participants met the inclusion criteria (Figure 2).

As shown in Figure 2, the Mini-Mental State Examination ([MMSE] Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & Mc Hugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)900...
; Kochhann, Varela, Lisboa, & Chaves, 2010Kochhann, R., Varela, J. S., Lisboa, C. S. L., & Chaves, M. L. F. (2010). The Mini Mental State Examination: Review of cutoff points adjusted for schooling in a large Southern Brazilian sample. Dementia Neuropsychologia, 4(1), 35-41. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642010DN40100006
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642010DN...
) and the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview ([MINI-PLUS] Amorim, 2000Amorim, P. (2000). Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): Validação de entrevista breve para diagnóstico de transtornos mentais. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 22(3), 106-115. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462000000300003
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-4446200000...
; Sheehan et al., 1998Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Hergueta, T., Baker, R., & Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of clinical psychiatry, 59(20), 22-33.) were employed as screening instruments, the latter being administered only to the control group. The MINI-PLUS diagnostic modules were used whenever screening questions were answered affirmatively, following the instructions of the instrument. Individuals who met criteria for current or past Major Depressive Episode, Manic Episode, Psychotic Disorders, or Anxiety Disorders were excluded from the sample.

The final sample consisted of male and female individuals between 30-93 years of age, with 4-29 years of formal education. Participants lived in the metropolitan region of Porto Alegre, in the city of Taquari, coastal region of the state of Rio Grande do Sul, and in the city of Belo Horizonte, state of Minas Gerais. They were split into two groups according to presence or absence of MDD. The control group comprised 23 participants between 30 and 81 years of age (M = 63.00; SD = 13.56) and 4-29 years of formal education (M = 12.74; SD = 8.18). The clinical group included 22 individuals between 36 and 93 years of age (M = 59.32; SD = 12.89) 4-27 years of formal education (M = 13.73; SD = 6.24). Table 1 presents descriptive data for the sample.

Table 1
Sample characteristics and analysis of diferences between groups

Table 1 shows that some participants in the control group took medication, but none had been diagnosed with any psychiatric disorder; this was confirmed by the MINI-PLUS. No participants took antipsychotics. The line “other medication” in Table 1 includes diuretics and drugs for hypertension and diabetes.

Instruments

The following instruments were employed: Demographic data and health conditions questionnaires (Fonseca et al., 2012Fonseca, R. P., Zimmermann, N., Pawlowski, J., Oliveira, C. R., Gindri, G., Scherer, L. C., … Parente, M. A. M. P. (2012). Métodos em avaliação neuropsicológica. In J. Landeira-Fernandez, & S. S. Fukusima. (Orgs.), Métodos em neurociência (pp. 266-296). Manole.), the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale ([HADS]; Botega, Bio, Zomignani, Garcia, & Pereira, 1995Botega, N. J., Bio, M. R., Zomignani, M. A., Garcia Jr, C., & Pereira, W. A. B. (1995). Transtornos do humor em enfermaria de clínica médica e validação de escala de medida (HAD) de ansiedade e depressão. Revista de Saúde Pública, 29(5), 359-363. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0034-89101995000500004
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0034-8910199500...
; Zigmond & Snaith, 1983Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta psychiatrica scandinavica, 67(6), 361-370.), the Hayling Test (Burgess & Shallice, 1997Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1997). The Hayling and Brixton tests. Thames Valley Test Company.; Zimmerman, Cardoso, Kristensen, & Fonseca, 2017Zimmermann, N., Cardoso, C. O., Kristensen, C. H., & Fonseca, R. P. (2017). Brazilian norms and effects of age and education on the Hayling and Trail Making Tests. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 39(3), 188-195. https://doi.org/10.1590/2237-6089-2016-0082
https://doi.org/10.1590/2237-6089-2016-0...
), a Phonemic Verbal Fluency Task (f/a/s) (Steiner, Mansur, Brucki, & Nitrini, 2008Steiner, V. A. G., Mansur, L. L., Brucki, S. M. D., & Nitrini, R. (2008). Phonemic verbal fluency and age: A preliminary study. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 2(4), 328-332.), the Frontal Assessment Battery ([FAB]; Beato et al., 2012Beato, R., Amaral-Carvalho, V., Guimarães, H. C., Tumas, V., Souza, C. P., Oliveira, G. N. De, & Caramelli, P. (2012). Frontal assessment battery in a Brazilian sample of healthy controls: Normative data. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 70(4), 278-280. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2012005000009
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X201200...
; Beato, Nitrini, Formigoni, & Caramelli, 2007Beato, R. G., Nitrini, R., Formigoni, A. P., & Caramelli, P. (2007). Brazilian version of the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB): Preliminary data on administration to healthy elderly. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 1(1), 59-65. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642008DN10100010
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642008DN...
; Dubois, Slachevsky, Litvan, & Pillon, 2000Dubois, B., Slachevsky, A., Litvan, I., & Pillon, B. (2000). The FAB: A frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology, 55(11), 1621-1626. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.57.3.565
https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.57.3.565...
), the Interpersonal Reactivity Index ([IRI]; Davis, 1980Davis, M. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.; 1983Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of personality and social psychology, 44(1), 113-136. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.1...
; Sampaio et al., 2011Sampaio, L. R., Guimarães, P. R. B., Camino, C. P. S., Formiga, N. S., & Menezes, I. G. (2011). Estudos sobre a dimensionalidade da empatia: Tradução e adaptação do Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI). Psico, 42(1), 67-76.), and the Social Cognition and Emotional Assessment, abbreviated version ([MINI-SEA] Bertoux et al., 2012Bertoux, M., Delavest, M., de Souza, L. C., Funkiewiez, A., Lepine, J.-P., Fossati, P., Dubois, B., & Sarazin, M. (2012). Social cognition and emotional assessment differentiates frontotemporal dementia from depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 83(4), 411-416. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2011-301849
https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2011-301849...
; Funkiewiez, Bertoux, de Souza, Lévy, & Dubois, 2012Funkiewiez, A., Bertoux, M., de Souza, L. C., Lévy, R., & Dubois, B. (2012). The SEA (Social Cognition and Emotional Assessment): A clinical neuropsychological tool for early diagnosis of frontal variant of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neuropsychology, 26(1), 81-90. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025318
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025318...
).

Procedures

A pilot study was conducted with six individuals to estimate the time required for data collection and to evaluate the understandability of instruction-items (or instructions-stimuli) sets, as well as the adequacy of the instruments. After this step, some changes were made to the order and structure of the protocols. Participants in the pilot study did not enter the final sample. Instruments were administered at the participant's own home, in educational institutions or in hospitals, between the years of 2015 and 2016.

Ethical considerations

This project was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Psychology Institute at UFRGS (Approval number: 1.099.551; CAAE 34753614.1.3002.5327) and by the Hospital de Clinicas de Porto Alegre (Approval number: 1.117.465; CAAE 34753614.1.0000.5334) and was financially supported by CNPq (Process number: 471755/2014-1). The study was carried out in collaboration with [information omitted to avoid identification of authors]. The project, entitled “Social-emotional cognition: clinical, neuroimaging, and biomarker study” was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of UFMG (Approval number: 17850513.2.0000.5149) and received financial support from CNPq (Process number: 402853/2012-1). An Informed Consent Form was read and signed by all individuals prior to participating in the study.

Data analysis

Initially, descriptive statistics of central tendency and dispersion were computed to characterize the sample. Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests were performed to assess the distribution of continuous variables; these tests indicated that the variables were normally distributed. ANCOVA and t-tests were used to compare scores between groups. To assess whether controlling for the effects of years of formal education and age was necessary, partial correlation analyzes were carried out: (a) between age and other variables, for each group; (b) between years of formal education and other variables, for each group. Whenever significant partial correlations were obtained for a variable in any of the groups, an ANCOVA was performed with years of formal education and/or age as control variables. Other variables were analyzed using t-tests.

The variables analyzed using ANCOVA, controlling for years of formal education, were: overall MMSE score; overall verbal fluency with letters F, A, and S; total hits on expression of anger; scores on the faux pas task (with gaffes, without gaffes, control questions, and overall score); total hits on recognition and emotional expression; overall score on the MINI-SEA; and the following scores on the Hayling Test: time in part A, time in part B, accuracy in part A, accuracy in part B, qualitative score in part B, and overall score. The variable age was controlled for only when analyzing our anxiety measure. Finally, t-tests were used to analyze the following variables: age, years of formal education, personal distress, empathic concern, fantasy, perspective taking, depression, and overall FAB score. Additionally, coefficients of variation for each group were calculated.

Pearson's correlation analyzes were performed to investigate associations between diagnostic variables (symptoms, time since diagnosis, age at first episode, and duration of treatment) and test performance. Finally, for the third objective, the sample was split into groups according to severity of depressive symptoms, as measured by the HADS. Kruskal-Wallis tests with Bonferroni's post hoc test were performed to compare performance on tasks of social-emotional cognition and executive functions. A 95% confidence level was adopted.

Results

Results did not show any statistically significant differences between groups for age (t = -0.83; p = .41) or education (t = 0.22; p = .83). Regarding depressive and anxiety symptoms at the time of assessment, as measured by the HADS, the clinical group had a significantly higher mean score than the control group. Table 2 displays the results of this analysis.

Table 2
Comparison of HADS mean scores between groups

Table 2 also shows some heterogeneity between participants' responses, as well as a small effect size for anxiety, and a large effect size for depression. To interpret effect sizes, the following cut-off values were adopted: insignificant (≤ 0.19), small (0.20-0.49), medium (0.50-0.79), and large (≥ 0.80; Cohen, 1988Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2.ª ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.).

The HADS also provides a categorization of symptomatology into severity levels: normal (0-7), mild (8-10), moderate (11-14), and severe (15-21). Regarding anxiety in the clinical group, 18.20% of the participants were categorized as normal, 27.30% as mild, 36.40% as moderate and 18.20% as severe. In the control group, 95.20% were categorized as normal and 4.80% as mild. For depression in the clinical group, 22.70% of participants were categorized as normal; 27.30% as mild, 22.70% as moderate, and 27.30% as severe. In the control group, 85.70% were categorized as normal and 14.30% as mild.

In the clinical group, 15 of the 22 patients were able to report the time since diagnosis, which ranged from 11 to 396 months, with a mean of 85.20 months (SD = 102.88, median = 36). Eighteen patients reported a duration for their current treatment between one and 144 months (M = 31.39, median = 20, SD = 35.42) and 14 reported that the first episode occurred between 17 and 71 years of age (M = 41.57, median = 41.50, SD = 18.55). The majority found it considerably difficult to report the number of depressive episodes they experienced throughout their lives, stating that there had been several of them. For the six patients who were able to answer, the mean was 1.83 episodes (median = 1, SD = 1.33).

Comparison between groups in tasks of social-emotional cognition and executive functions

Participants in the MDD and control groups performed similarly on all measures of social-emotional cognition, except for the personal distress dimension of the IRI. Group differences between mean scores in the IRI and in the MINI-SEA are shown in Table 3.

Table 3
Comparison between mean scores on social-emotional cognition tests using t-tests or ANCOVA and coefficients of variation

Table 3 shows results obtained by t-tests and ANCOVAs, controlling for the effect of either age or years of formal education, as explained above. We see that coefficients of variation are higher for emotion recognition measures and that, despite a lack of statistical significance, there is a small effect size for some of them. Likewise, comparisons of performance on executive functions tests did not reveal any significant differences between the groups. Results for the Hayling Test and the verbal fluency task are presented in Table 4.

Table 4
Comparison between mean scores in the Hayling test and verbal fluency using ANCOVA, controlling for years of study

Table 4 shows higher coefficients of variation for most measures and small effect sizes for some scores in the Hayling Test. Finally, there were no differences in mean scores obtained in the FAB (t = -0.28; p = .78) between groups (MDD: M = 15.27; SD = 2.19; CV = 0.14. CG: M = 15.43; SD = 1.40; CV = 0.09).

Relationship between MDD characteristics and performance in tasks of social-emotional cognition and executive functions

Our second goal was to further investigate the performance of the clinical group using Pearson correlation analysis between diagnosis-related variables - time since diagnosis, age at first episode, and duration of treatment - and other variables of social-emotional cognition and executive functions. Results were not statistically significant.

Comparison between groups defined by severity of depressive symptoms in tasks of social-emotional cognition and executive functions

For our third goal, we split the sample into groups according to severity of depressive symptoms, as measured by the HADS. A little over half of the sample (53.50%) was categorized as normal, 20.90% as mild, 11.60% as moderate, and 14% as severe. Then, we tested for differences between these groups in performance on tasks of social-emotional cognition and executive functions. A Kruskal-Wallis test employing Bonferroni's post-hoc test showed that the group with severe symptoms took significantly longer to perform part A of the Hayling test, compared to the group categorized as normal (χ2(3) = 8.56, p < .05). Other comparisons were not statistically significant.

Discussion

The goal of this study was to compare the performance of individuals with and without MDD on measures of social-emotional cognition and executive functions. We expected the clinical group to show significantly lower scores than the control group. In addition, we investigated whether social-emotional cognition and executive functions were related to clinical variables of MDD and whether there was a difference in performance between individuals with different levels of severity of symptoms.

Patients in our sample were diagnosed by psychiatrists with adequate training and experience, whereas the control group was recruited through careful screening. As expected, individuals with MDD reported more depressive and anxiety symptoms than controls. An examination of symptom severity levels revealed that a few patients did not report substantial depressive symptoms in HADS. Since all patients were undergoing pharmacological treatment at the time of evaluation, they were assumed to be in remission. In the control group, a few participants reported mild symptoms, but did not meet criteria for any mood or anxiety disorder as assessed by the MINI-PLUS.

Comparison between groups in tasks of social-emotional cognition and executive functions

We hypothesized that the MDD group would exhibit lower scores, compared to the control group, on measures of social-emotional cognition and executive functions. The results obtained partially corroborated this hypothesis. Patients scored higher on the personal distress dimension of the IRI, a result that has been reported in previous studies (Derntl et al., 2012Derntl, B., Seidel, E. M., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2012). How specific are emotional deficits? A comparison of empathic abilities in schizophrenia, bipolar and depressed patients. Schizophrenia research, 142(1-3), 58-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.09.020
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.09...
; Domes, Spenthof, Radtke, Isaksson, Normann, & Heinrichs, 2016Domes, G., Spenthof, I., Radtke, M., Isaksson, A., Normann, C., & Heinrichs, M. (2016). Autistic traits and empathy in chronic vs. episodic depression. Journal of affective disorders, 195, 144-147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.00...
; Schreiter et al., 2013Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G. H. M., & Aan Het Rot, M. (2013). Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.009
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.00...
; O'Connor et al., 2002O'Connor, L. E., Berry, J. W., Weiss, J., & Gilbert, P. (2002). Guilt, fear, submission, and empathy in depression. Journal of affective disorders, 71(1-3), 19-27. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0327(01)00408-6
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0327(01)00...
; Schneider et al., 2012Schneider, D., Regenbogen, C., Kellermann, T., Finkelmeyer, A., Kohn, N., Derntl, B., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2012). Empathic behavioral and physiological responses to dynamic stimuli in depression. Psychiatry research, 200(2-3), 294-305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.03.054
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012....
; Thoma et al., 2011Thoma, P., Zalewski, I., von Reventlow, H. G., Norra, C., Juckel, G., & Daum, I. (2011). Cognitive and affective empathy in depression linked to executive control. Psychiatry Research, 189(3), 373-378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.030
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011....
; Wilbertz et al., 2010Wilbertz, G., Brakemeier, E. L., Zobel, I., Härter, M., & Schramm, E. (2010). Exploring preoperational features in chronic depression. Journal of affective disorders, 124(3), 262-269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.11.021
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.11.02...
). Additionally, a meta-analysis (Schreiter et al., 2013Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G. H. M., & Aan Het Rot, M. (2013). Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.009
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.00...
) showed a difference between groups with an effect size of 0.86, which can be considered large. In our study, we found a similarly large value, 0.79.

Personal distress consists in an analysis and search for relief in another person's misfortune from a self-centered perspective (Davis, 1980Davis, M. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.). Patients with MDD exhibit a cognitive attentional bias towards their own negative aspects (Mor & Winquist, 2002Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 638-662. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.638
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.4....
), which may increase personal distress. Thus, when witnessing or listening to another person’s suffering, individuals with MDD may tend to imagine themselves experiencing the situation, resulting in an affective response that is related to their own reactions/emotions to a greater degree than to the other person’s emotional state (Schreiter et al., 2013Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G. H. M., & Aan Het Rot, M. (2013). Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.009
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.00...
). Other aspects of empathy did not differ between the groups investigated here, although other studies have reported low cognitive empathy (Schreiter et al., 2013Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G. H. M., & Aan Het Rot, M. (2013). Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.009
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.00...
) and deficits in empathic concern and perspective taking (Cusi et al., 2011Cusi, A. M., MacQueen, G. M., Spreng, R. N., & McKinnon, M. C. (2011). Altered empathic responding in major depressive disorder: Relation to symptom severity, illness burden, and psychosocial outcome. Psychiatry Research, 188(2), 231-236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.013
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011....
).

Cognitive empathy is related to the concept of TOM, since the ability to infer and understand feelings and thoughts of others is essential for empathic processing (Schreiter et al., 2013Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G. H. M., & Aan Het Rot, M. (2013). Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.009
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.00...
). The present study also investigated TOM skills, specifically, perception of gaffes (faux pas) and recognition of emotional facial expressions, using the MINI-SEA. This battery has the benefit of providing an overall score that includes both tasks.

The MINI-SEA was developed to assist in differential diagnosis of the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia (Bertoux et al., 2012Bertoux, M., Delavest, M., de Souza, L. C., Funkiewiez, A., Lepine, J.-P., Fossati, P., Dubois, B., & Sarazin, M. (2012). Social cognition and emotional assessment differentiates frontotemporal dementia from depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 83(4), 411-416. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2011-301849
https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2011-301849...
). Therefore, we did not expect MDD patients to exhibit low performance in the battery, or at least not as low as patients with frontotemporal dementia. However, studies on recognition of facial emotional expressions and TOM in MDD patients report divergent results. Some studies have shown significant differences between patients and healthy controls (Leppänen, Milders, Bell, Terriere, & Hietanen, 2004Leppänen, J. M., Milders, M., Bell, J. S., Terriere, E., & Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Depression biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces. Psychiatry Research, 128(2), 123-133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.020
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2004....
; Weightman, Air, & Baune, 2014Weightman, M. J., Air, T. M., & Baune, B. T. (2014). A review of the role of social cognition in major depressive disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 179. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00179
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00179...
), whereas others have not (Bediou et al., 2012Bediou, B., Brunelin, J., d’Amato, T., Fecteau, S., Saoud, M., Hénaff, M. A., & Krolak-Salmon, P. (2012). A comparison of facial emotion processing in neurological and psychiatric conditions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(APR), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00098
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00098...
; Gollan, McCloskey, Hoxha, & Coccaro, 2010Gollan, J. K., McCloskey, M., Hoxha, D., & Coccaro, E. F. (2010). How do depressed and healthy adults interpret nuanced facial expressions? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(4), 804-810. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020234
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020234...
; Gollan, Pane, McCloskey, & Coccaro, 2008Gollan, J. K., Pane, H. T., McCloskey, M. S., & Coccaro, E. F. (2008). Identifying differences in biased affective information processing in major depression. Psychiatry Research, 159(1-2), 18-24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.06.011
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007....
; Matthews, Strigo, Simmons, Yang, & Paulus, 2008Matthews, S. C., Strigo, I. A., Simmons, A. N., Yang, T. T., & Paulus, M. P. (2008). Decreased functional coupling of the amygdala and supragenual cingulate is related to increased depression in unmedicated individuals with current major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 111(1), 13-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.022
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.02...
; Suslow et al., 2010Suslow, T., Konrad, C., Kugel, H., Rumstadt, D., Zwitserlood, P., Schöning, S., Ohrmann, P, Bauer, J., Pyka, M., Kersting, A., Arolt, V., Heindel, W., & Dannlowski, U. (2010). Automatic mood-congruent amygdala responses to masked facial expressions in Major Depression. Biological Psychiatry, 67(2), 155-160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.023
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009....
). In our study, patients and controls showed similar performance on all MINI-SEA tasks, in line with Bertoux et al. (2012Bertoux, M., Delavest, M., de Souza, L. C., Funkiewiez, A., Lepine, J.-P., Fossati, P., Dubois, B., & Sarazin, M. (2012). Social cognition and emotional assessment differentiates frontotemporal dementia from depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 83(4), 411-416. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2011-301849
https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2011-301849...
), who also employed this battery to evaluate MDD patients

For the Faux Pas task, some studies have reported differences between MDD patients and controls (Cusi et al., 2013Cusi, A. M., Nazarov, A., MacQueen, G. M., & McKinnon, M. C. (2013). Theory of mind deficits in patients with mild symptoms of major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 210(2), 672-674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.018
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013....
; Wang et al., 2008Wang, Y. G., Wang, Y. Q., Chen, S. L., Zhu, C. Y., & Wang, K. (2008). Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: A componential view. Psychiatry Research, 161(2), 153-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.018
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007....
), whereas here we observed that patients tended to perform worse in recognition of gaffes. It is possible that our small sample size influenced the results, but it should be mentioned that most studies employed similarly sizes samples.

A comparison between clinical groups evaluated in other studies reveals differences between them, such as disease duration, type of medication being used, presence of psychotic symptoms, age of onset of symptoms, among others. There is no homogeneous profile of MDD patient groups that shows poor performance on TOM tasks. Each study includes distinct control variables. Wang et al. (2008Wang, Y. G., Wang, Y. Q., Chen, S. L., Zhu, C. Y., & Wang, K. (2008). Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: A componential view. Psychiatry Research, 161(2), 153-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.018
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007....
), for example, evaluated patients who experienced their first severe depressive episode, with and without psychotic symptoms, and without medication. Cusi et al. (2013Cusi, A. M., Nazarov, A., MacQueen, G. M., & McKinnon, M. C. (2013). Theory of mind deficits in patients with mild symptoms of major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 210(2), 672-674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.018
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013....
), on the other hand, included only patients with mild MDD. Yamada, Inoue, and Kanba (2015Yamada, K., Inoue, Y., & Kanba, S. (2015). Theory of mind ability predicts prognosis of outpatients with major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 230(2), 604-608. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.10.011
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015....
) assessed individuals in remission. Our study investigated patients with multiple depressive episodes and who underwent pharmacological treatment. Considering that difficulties in TOM have multiple causes, including social isolation, history of trauma and abuse, low socioeconomic status, and problems related to attachment (Kanba, Yamada, & Inoue, 2010; Liotti & Prunetti, 2010Liotti, G., & Prunetti, E. (2010). Metacognitive deficits in trauma-related disorders: Contingent on interpersonal motivational contexts? In Dimaggio, G. & Lysaker, P.H. (Eds.), Metacognition and severe adult mental disorders: From research to treatment (pp. 196-214). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.), it is possible that these factors explain, at least partially, the conflicting results regarding patients with MDD.

Some stories used in the faux-pas task may also have contributed to bias the results reported here, even in the control group, as in the case of the story in which a customer mistakes another customer for the waiter. Some participants mentioned this happening quite often to them, and did not find such situations embarassing, or found them more embarrassing for the person mistaken for the waiter than for the person who made the mistake; others explained that some people might get angry in this situation, but that they did not. In another story, in which a man comforts his girlfriend for not getting the part she wanted, some individuals responded that he should have cheered her up, rather than remarking that she must be disappointed. Different views on the same stories may have contributed to a higher coefficient of variation, particularly for the stories used in recognition of faux pas. It is possible that adapting these stories to Brazilian cultural standards will improve the task.

Regarding recognition of emotional facial expressions, a meta-analysis concluded that patients with MDD have impairments in recognition of all basic emotions (anger, disgust, fear, joy, and surprise) except for sadness (Dalili, Penton-Voak, Harmer, & Munafo, 2015Dalili, M. N., Penton-Voak, I. S., Harmer, C. J., & Munafò, M. R. (2015). Meta-analysis of emotion recognition deficits in major depressive disorder. Psychological medicine, 45(6), 1135-1144. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714002591
https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171400259...
). Since the effect size found was small, the authors noted that a sample of approximately 615 cases and 615 controls would be needed to detect it, considering 80% statistical power at an alpha of .05. On average, the studies included in the meta-analysis used samples of 21 patients and 25 controls.

Social-emotional skills in general, and empathy in particular, appear to depend on preserved executive control, more specifically on cognitive flexibility and inhibition. (Thoma et al., 2011Thoma, P., Zalewski, I., von Reventlow, H. G., Norra, C., Juckel, G., & Daum, I. (2011). Cognitive and affective empathy in depression linked to executive control. Psychiatry Research, 189(3), 373-378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.030
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011....
), both of which were also assessed in this study. Although most studies indicated that cognitive deficits in MDD occur regardless of age, test difficulty, motivation, symptom severity, depression subtype, and response bias (Austin, Mitchell, & Goodwin, 2001Austin, M. P., Mitchell, P., & Goodwin, G. M. (2001). Cognitive deficits in depression: Possible implications for functional neuropathology. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 178(3), 200-206. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.178.3.200
https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.178.3.200...
), the hypothesis of a difference in performance on tasks of executive functions was not corroborated by our results.

There is a consensus that executive functions are a multidimensional construct. Thus, patients with MDD may have difficulties in some, but not all subcomponents of executive functions (Knight & Baune, 2018Knight, M. J., & Baune, B. T. (2018). Executive subdomains are differentially associated with psychosocial outcomes in Major Depressive Disorder. Frontiers in psychiatry, 9, 309. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00309
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00309...
). The instruments used in this study assessed inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, and phonemic verbal fluency. These subcomponents were also investigated in other studies that did not report significant differences between MDD patients and healthy controls. For example, Thoma et al. (2011Thoma, P., Zalewski, I., von Reventlow, H. G., Norra, C., Juckel, G., & Daum, I. (2011). Cognitive and affective empathy in depression linked to executive control. Psychiatry Research, 189(3), 373-378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.030
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011....
) observed similar performance for both groups on measures of cognitive flexibility, response inhibition, and working memory; Wagner et al. (2018Wagner, S., Helmreich, I., Wollschläger, D., Meyer, K., Kaaden, S., Reiff, J., Roll, S. C., Braus, D., Tüscher, O., Müller-Dahlhaus, F., Tadić, A., & Lieb, K. (2018). Early improvement of executive test performance during antidepressant treatment predicts treatment outcome in patients with Major Depressive Disorder. PloS one, 13(4), e0194574. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0194574
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.019...
) reported similar results using the Trail Making Test and tasks of phonemic and semantic verbal fluency, as did Aker et al. (2014Aker, M., Harmer, C., & Landro, N. I. (2014). More rumination and less effective emotion regulation in previously depressed women with preserved executive functions. BMC Psychiatry, 14(1), 334. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0334-4
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0334-...
) using tasks of hot and cold executive functions.

Aker et al. (2014Aker, M., Harmer, C., & Landro, N. I. (2014). More rumination and less effective emotion regulation in previously depressed women with preserved executive functions. BMC Psychiatry, 14(1), 334. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0334-4
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0334-...
) highlighted that most previous studies included samples with highly impaired patients, either with severe comorbidities, alcohol or drug abuse, somatic problems, or low levels of formal educational. The sample studied by Aker et al. (2014Aker, M., Harmer, C., & Landro, N. I. (2014). More rumination and less effective emotion regulation in previously depressed women with preserved executive functions. BMC Psychiatry, 14(1), 334. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0334-4
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0334-...
), on the other hand, had low comorbidity, no substance abuse, high levels of formal education or high intelligence quotient and no use of psychotropic medication (excluded from the analysis of executive functions). The points raised by Aker et al. may also be relevant for our study, since patients did not report issues related to use of alcohol or other drugs; their average level of formal education was 13.73 years; and none had severe comorbidities.

Another explanation for the lack of performance differences on tasks of executive functions and social-emotional cognition might be that the instruments used here were not sensitive enough to detect the deficits in the clinical group. Indeed, the conflicting results reported by other studies might be explained by: differences between the instruments employed, some of which may be more sensitive to impairments in MDD than others; differences in methods; or differences in scores or versions of the same test used. Furthermore, the clinical groups in those studies showed a wide range of clinical and demographic characteristics (Snyder, 2013Snyder, H. R. (2013). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: A meta-analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 81-132. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028727
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028727...
).

We argue, however, that this is less likely, since tasks such as the Hayling Test and the phonemic verbal fluency used here are able to detect performance differences in other samples of MDD patients. Similarly, recognition of emotional facial expression using images from Ekman and Friesen's Pictures of Facial Affect and faux pas tasks have also detected differences in some studies (Cusi et al., 2013Cusi, A. M., Nazarov, A., MacQueen, G. M., & McKinnon, M. C. (2013). Theory of mind deficits in patients with mild symptoms of major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 210(2), 672-674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.018
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013....
; Leppänen et al., 2004Leppänen, J. M., Milders, M., Bell, J. S., Terriere, E., & Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Depression biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces. Psychiatry Research, 128(2), 123-133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.020
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2004....
; Wang et al., 2008Wang, Y. G., Wang, Y. Q., Chen, S. L., Zhu, C. Y., & Wang, K. (2008). Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: A componential view. Psychiatry Research, 161(2), 153-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.018
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007....
; Weightman et al., 2014Weightman, M. J., Air, T. M., & Baune, B. T. (2014). A review of the role of social cognition in major depressive disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 179. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00179
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00179...
), though not in others (Bertoux et al., 2012Bertoux, M., Delavest, M., de Souza, L. C., Funkiewiez, A., Lepine, J.-P., Fossati, P., Dubois, B., & Sarazin, M. (2012). Social cognition and emotional assessment differentiates frontotemporal dementia from depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 83(4), 411-416. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2011-301849
https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2011-301849...
; Gollan et al., 2010Gollan, J. K., McCloskey, M., Hoxha, D., & Coccaro, E. F. (2010). How do depressed and healthy adults interpret nuanced facial expressions? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(4), 804-810. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020234
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020234...
, 2008; Matthews et al., 2008Matthews, S. C., Strigo, I. A., Simmons, A. N., Yang, T. T., & Paulus, M. P. (2008). Decreased functional coupling of the amygdala and supragenual cingulate is related to increased depression in unmedicated individuals with current major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 111(1), 13-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.022
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.02...
; Suslow et al., 2010Suslow, T., Konrad, C., Kugel, H., Rumstadt, D., Zwitserlood, P., Schöning, S., Ohrmann, P, Bauer, J., Pyka, M., Kersting, A., Arolt, V., Heindel, W., & Dannlowski, U. (2010). Automatic mood-congruent amygdala responses to masked facial expressions in Major Depression. Biological Psychiatry, 67(2), 155-160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.023
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009....
). Thus, it is plausible that other characteristics of the participants or their clinical condition, rather than the tests, are responsible for the discrepancy between the results of different studies. In this regard, it is worth noting how heterogeneous the performance of each group was on most tasks, as indicated by the coefficients of variation.

Relationships between MDD and performance in social-emotional cognition and executive functions tasks

The literature on MDD lists some variables that are related to more severe cognitive symptoms, such as recurrent episodes, late onset of the disorder, and presence of melancholic or psychotic symptoms (Bora, Harrison, Yücel, & Pantelis, 2013Bora, E., Harrison, B. J., Yücel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2013). Cognitive impairment in euthymic major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 43(10), 2017-2026. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712002085
https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171200208...
). However, our correlation analyses indicated that time since diagnosis, age at first episode and duration of treatment had no relationship with the performance of this clinical group in tests of social-emotional cognition and executive functions. We were interested in relating the number of depressive episodes to the tasks, but the vast majority of participants were unable to inform the number of episodes they experienced, instead stating that there had been several of them.

Comparison between groups defined by severity of depressive symptoms in social-emotional cognition and executive functions tasks

The hypothesis that individuals with more severe depressive symptoms would show worse performance was partially corroborated. Although no significant differences were found between MDD patients and controls in most measures, when the whole sample was split by levels of depressive symptom severity, a difference was detected. Patients with severe symptoms took longer to perform part A of the Hayling test, compared to symptom-free individuals (i.e., those in the “normal” category), which indicates reduced processing speed. This result is in line with a previous study showing that depressive patients had lower processing speed than healthy controls, while patients in remission did not (Zaremba et al., 2019Zaremba, D., Kalthoff, I. S., Förster, K., Redlich, R., Grotegerd, D., Leehr, E. J., Meinert, S., Dohm, K., Bürger, C., Enneking, V., Böhnlein, J., Repple, J., Opel, N., Jörgens, S., Yüksel, D., Schmitt, S., Stein, F., Kircher, T., Krug, A., Nenadić, I., Zwitserlood, P., Baune, B.T., Arolt, V., & Dannlowski, U. (2019). The effects of processing speed on memory impairment in patients with major depressive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 92, 494-500.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.02.015
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.02....
). It has also been reported that patients with greater severity of depressive symptoms had larger deficits in TOM tasks (Bora & Berk, 2016Bora, E., & Berk, M. (2016). Theory of mind in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 191, 49-55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.023
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.02...
), but such a difference was not found in our sample.

Even in sublinical MDD patients or those in remission, cognitive (Darcet, Gardier, Gaillard, David, & Guilloux, 2016Darcet, F., Gardier, A., Gaillard, R., David, D., & Guilloux, J.-P. (2016). Cognitive dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder: A translational review in animal models of the disease. Pharmaceuticals, 9(1), 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/ph9010009
https://doi.org/10.3390/ph9010009...
; Zuckerman et al., 2018Zuckerman, H., Pan, Z., Park, C., Brietzke, E., Musial, N., Shariq, A. S., Iacobucci, M., Yim, S. J., Lui, L., Rong, C., & McIntyre, R. S. (2018). Recognition and treatment of cognitive dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder.Frontiers in psychiatry ,9, 655. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00655
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00655...
) and social-cognitive (Kessler, Zhao, Blazer, & Swartz, 1997Kessler, R. C., Zhao, S., Blazer, D. G., & Swartz, M. (1997). Prevalence, correlates, and course of minor depression and major depression in the National Comorbidity Survey. Journal of affective disorders, 45(1-2), 19-30. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0327(97)00056-6
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0327(97)00...
) impairments occur. It is important to evaluate clinical samples with distinct levels of depressive symptomatology (Schreiter, Pijnenborg, & Aan Het Rot, 2013Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G. H. M., & Aan Het Rot, M. (2013). Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.009
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.00...
), event though the current study did not support such findings.

Conclusion

MDD is a diagnosis with heterogeneous manifestations (Darcet et al., 2016Darcet, F., Gardier, A., Gaillard, R., David, D., & Guilloux, J.-P. (2016). Cognitive dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder: A translational review in animal models of the disease. Pharmaceuticals, 9(1), 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/ph9010009
https://doi.org/10.3390/ph9010009...
) and this may partly explain the conflicting results reported in the literature (Bediou et al., 2012Bediou, B., Brunelin, J., d’Amato, T., Fecteau, S., Saoud, M., Hénaff, M. A., & Krolak-Salmon, P. (2012). A comparison of facial emotion processing in neurological and psychiatric conditions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(APR), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00098
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00098...
). Studies with this population generally report this heterogeneity as a limitation; however, this might actually be a characteristic instrinsic to the disorder and responsible for its complexity. In this study, we measured that heterogeneity using standard deviation measures, clinical information, and coefficients of variation. Although there is a growing tendency to carry out studies with more homogeneous groups, recruiting such samples is in practice rather complex. For this reason, most researchers try to report the characteristics of their samples in detail.

In the sample assessed here, MDD did not influence performance on tasks of recognition of emotional facial expressions, faux pas, phonemic verbal fluency, and the Hayling Test. The high score we found for the personal distress dimension seems to be a robust result, with an effect size of 0.79.

In specific cases, empathy is disrupted by impaired emotional self-regulation and may become an aversive experience in the absence of accurate cognitive processing of the perception of other persons’ situation (Decety & Jackson, 2004Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3(2), 71-100. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187
https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187...
). Studies on social-emotional cognition in MDD may wish to focus on aspects related to emotional regulation, self-focus, and rumination. Patients with MDD exhibit a maladaptive tendency to focus on negative aspects of a situation (Boyraz & Waits, 2015Boyraz, G., & Waits, J. B. (2015). Reciprocal associations among self-focused attention, self-acceptance, and empathy: A two-wave panel study. Personality and Individual Differences, 74, 84-89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.042
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.0...
), which increases rumination (APA, 2014American Psychiatric Association. (2014). Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais - DSM-5. Artmed.) and significantly interferes with patients’ interpersonal relationships and their interpretations of social situations.

Limitations of this study include sample size, which, although similar to that of similar studies, is nevertheless small; intragroup heterogeneity in terms of age range and years of formal education; and use of the MINI-SEA, which is still being validated in Brazil. Some individuals in the control group took medications, which can be considered a limitation. However, none did so to treat psychiatric disorders, as informed by participants and their results in the MINI-PLUS.

Additionally, measures of functionality may provide useful information to interpret results, since many patients do not achieve functional recovery even after mood improvement (Millan et al., 2012Millan, M. J., Agid, Y., Brüne, M., Bullmore, E. T., Carter, C. S., Clayton, N. S., Connor, R., Davis, S., Deakin, B., DeRubeis, R.J., Dubois, B., Geyer, M. A., Goodwin, G. M., Gorwood, P., Jay, T. M., Joëls, M., Mansuy, I. M., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Murphy, D., ... Young, L. J. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: Characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature reviews Drug discovery, 11(2), 141. https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrd3628
https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrd3628...
Fiorillo et al., 2018Fiorillo, A., Carpiniello, B., De Giorgi, S., La Pia, S., Maina, G., Sampogna, G., Spina, E., Tortorella, A., & Vita, A. (2018). Assessment and management of cognitive and psychosocial dysfunctions in patients with Major Depressive Disorder: A clinical review. Frontiers in psychiatry, 9, 493. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00493
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00493...
; Hammer-Helmich et al., 2018Hammer-Helmich, L., Haro, J. M., Jönsson, B., Tanguy Melac, A., Di Nicola, S., Chollet, J., Milea, D., Rive, B., & Saragoussi, D. (2018). Functional impairment in patients with major depressive disorder: The 2-year PERFORM study. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment, 14, 239-249. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.2147/NDT.S146098
https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.2147/NDT....
; Hammer-Helmich et al. al., 2018Hammer-Helmich, L., Haro, J. M., Jönsson, B., Tanguy Melac, A., Di Nicola, S., Chollet, J., Milea, D., Rive, B., & Saragoussi, D. (2018). Functional impairment in patients with major depressive disorder: The 2-year PERFORM study. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment, 14, 239-249. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.2147/NDT.S146098
https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.2147/NDT....
; Zuckerman, et al., 2018Zuckerman, H., Pan, Z., Park, C., Brietzke, E., Musial, N., Shariq, A. S., Iacobucci, M., Yim, S. J., Lui, L., Rong, C., & McIntyre, R. S. (2018). Recognition and treatment of cognitive dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder.Frontiers in psychiatry ,9, 655. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00655
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00655...
). Intelligence quotient (IQ) measures were not obtained either, but the cognitive screening tool employed here, MMSE, has been widely employed, including by the publications discussed in the current study.

References

  • Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(3), 165-178. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1056
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1056
  • Aker, M., Harmer, C., & Landro, N. I. (2014). More rumination and less effective emotion regulation in previously depressed women with preserved executive functions. BMC Psychiatry, 14(1), 334. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0334-4
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-014-0334-4
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2014). Manual Diagnóstico e Estatístico de Transtornos Mentais - DSM-5 Artmed.
  • Amorim, P. (2000). Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): Validação de entrevista breve para diagnóstico de transtornos mentais. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 22(3), 106-115. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462000000300003
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462000000300003
  • Arnow, B. A., Blasey, C., Williams, L. M., Palmer, D. M., Rekshan, W., Schatzberg, A. F., Etkin, A., Kulkarni, J., Luther, J.F., & Rush, A. J. (2015). Depression subtypes in predicting antidepressant response: A report from the iSPOT-D trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(8), 743-750. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14020181
    » https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14020181
  • Austin, M. P., Mitchell, P., & Goodwin, G. M. (2001). Cognitive deficits in depression: Possible implications for functional neuropathology. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 178(3), 200-206. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.178.3.200
    » https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.178.3.200
  • Beato, R., Amaral-Carvalho, V., Guimarães, H. C., Tumas, V., Souza, C. P., Oliveira, G. N. De, & Caramelli, P. (2012). Frontal assessment battery in a Brazilian sample of healthy controls: Normative data. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 70(4), 278-280. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2012005000009
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0004-282X2012005000009
  • Beato, R. G., Nitrini, R., Formigoni, A. P., & Caramelli, P. (2007). Brazilian version of the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB): Preliminary data on administration to healthy elderly. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 1(1), 59-65. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642008DN10100010
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642008DN10100010
  • Bediou, B., Brunelin, J., d’Amato, T., Fecteau, S., Saoud, M., Hénaff, M. A., & Krolak-Salmon, P. (2012). A comparison of facial emotion processing in neurological and psychiatric conditions. Frontiers in Psychology, 3(APR), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00098
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00098
  • Beer, J. S., & Ochsner, K. N. (2006). Social cognition: A multi level analysis. Brain Research, 1079(1), 98-105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.002
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.002
  • Bertoux, M., Delavest, M., de Souza, L. C., Funkiewiez, A., Lepine, J.-P., Fossati, P., Dubois, B., & Sarazin, M. (2012). Social cognition and emotional assessment differentiates frontotemporal dementia from depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 83(4), 411-416. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2011-301849
    » https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2011-301849
  • Bora, E., & Berk, M. (2016). Theory of mind in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 191, 49-55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.023
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.023
  • Bora, E., Harrison, B. J., Yücel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2013). Cognitive impairment in euthymic major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 43(10), 2017-2026. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712002085
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291712002085
  • Botega, N. J., Bio, M. R., Zomignani, M. A., Garcia Jr, C., & Pereira, W. A. B. (1995). Transtornos do humor em enfermaria de clínica médica e validação de escala de medida (HAD) de ansiedade e depressão. Revista de Saúde Pública, 29(5), 359-363. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0034-89101995000500004
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0034-89101995000500004
  • Boyraz, G., & Waits, J. B. (2015). Reciprocal associations among self-focused attention, self-acceptance, and empathy: A two-wave panel study. Personality and Individual Differences, 74, 84-89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.042
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.042
  • Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1997). The Hayling and Brixton tests Thames Valley Test Company.
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2.ª ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Cotter, J., Granger, K., Backx, R., Hobbs, M., Looi, C. Y., & Barnett, J. H. (2018). Social cognitive dysfunction as a clinical marker: A systematic review of meta-analyses across 30 clinical conditions.Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews , 84, 92-99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.014
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.014
  • Cusi, A. M., MacQueen, G. M., Spreng, R. N., & McKinnon, M. C. (2011). Altered empathic responding in major depressive disorder: Relation to symptom severity, illness burden, and psychosocial outcome. Psychiatry Research, 188(2), 231-236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.013
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.013
  • Cusi, A. M., Nazarov, A., MacQueen, G. M., & McKinnon, M. C. (2013). Theory of mind deficits in patients with mild symptoms of major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 210(2), 672-674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.018
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.018
  • Dalili, M. N., Penton-Voak, I. S., Harmer, C. J., & Munafò, M. R. (2015). Meta-analysis of emotion recognition deficits in major depressive disorder. Psychological medicine, 45(6), 1135-1144. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714002591
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714002591
  • Darcet, F., Gardier, A., Gaillard, R., David, D., & Guilloux, J.-P. (2016). Cognitive dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder: A translational review in animal models of the disease. Pharmaceuticals, 9(1), 9. https://doi.org/10.3390/ph9010009
    » https://doi.org/10.3390/ph9010009
  • Davis, M. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.
  • Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of personality and social psychology, 44(1), 113-136. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113
  • Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3(2), 71-100. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187
  • Derntl, B., Seidel, E. M., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2012). How specific are emotional deficits? A comparison of empathic abilities in schizophrenia, bipolar and depressed patients. Schizophrenia research, 142(1-3), 58-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.09.020
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.09.020
  • Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta‐analysis. Child development, 85(5), 1777-1794. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12237
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12237
  • Domes, G., Spenthof, I., Radtke, M., Isaksson, A., Normann, C., & Heinrichs, M. (2016). Autistic traits and empathy in chronic vs. episodic depression. Journal of affective disorders, 195, 144-147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.006
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.02.006
  • Dubois, B., Slachevsky, A., Litvan, I., & Pillon, B. (2000). The FAB: A frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology, 55(11), 1621-1626. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.57.3.565
    » https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.57.3.565
  • Fiorillo, A., Carpiniello, B., De Giorgi, S., La Pia, S., Maina, G., Sampogna, G., Spina, E., Tortorella, A., & Vita, A. (2018). Assessment and management of cognitive and psychosocial dysfunctions in patients with Major Depressive Disorder: A clinical review. Frontiers in psychiatry, 9, 493. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00493
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00493
  • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & Mc Hugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
  • Fonseca, R. P., Zimmermann, N., Pawlowski, J., Oliveira, C. R., Gindri, G., Scherer, L. C., … Parente, M. A. M. P. (2012). Métodos em avaliação neuropsicológica. In J. Landeira-Fernandez, & S. S. Fukusima. (Orgs.), Métodos em neurociência (pp. 266-296). Manole.
  • Funkiewiez, A., Bertoux, M., de Souza, L. C., Lévy, R., & Dubois, B. (2012). The SEA (Social Cognition and Emotional Assessment): A clinical neuropsychological tool for early diagnosis of frontal variant of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neuropsychology, 26(1), 81-90. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025318
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025318
  • Gollan, J. K., McCloskey, M., Hoxha, D., & Coccaro, E. F. (2010). How do depressed and healthy adults interpret nuanced facial expressions? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(4), 804-810. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020234
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020234
  • Gollan, J. K., Pane, H. T., McCloskey, M. S., & Coccaro, E. F. (2008). Identifying differences in biased affective information processing in major depression. Psychiatry Research, 159(1-2), 18-24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.06.011
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.06.011
  • Hammer-Helmich, L., Haro, J. M., Jönsson, B., Tanguy Melac, A., Di Nicola, S., Chollet, J., Milea, D., Rive, B., & Saragoussi, D. (2018). Functional impairment in patients with major depressive disorder: The 2-year PERFORM study. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment, 14, 239-249. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.2147/NDT.S146098
    » https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.2147/NDT.S146098
  • Hoertnagl, C. M., & Hofer, A. (2014). Social cognition in serious mental illness. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 27(3), 197-202. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000055
    » https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000055
  • Inoue, Y., Tonooka, Y., Yamada, K., & Kanba, S. (2004). Deficiency of theory of mind in patients with remitted mood disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82(3), 403-409. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2004.04.004
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2004.04.004
  • Kanba, S., Yamada, K., & Inoue, Y. (2010). Deficit of theory of mind in depression and its correlation with poor clinical outcomes. In Dimaggio, G. & Lysaker, P.H. (Eds.), Metacognition and severe adult mental disorders: From research to treatment (pp. 150-160). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
  • Kessler, R. C., Zhao, S., Blazer, D. G., & Swartz, M. (1997). Prevalence, correlates, and course of minor depression and major depression in the National Comorbidity Survey. Journal of affective disorders, 45(1-2), 19-30. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0327(97)00056-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0327(97)00056-6
  • Knight, M. J., & Baune, B. T. (2018). Executive subdomains are differentially associated with psychosocial outcomes in Major Depressive Disorder. Frontiers in psychiatry, 9, 309. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00309
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00309
  • Kochhann, R., Varela, J. S., Lisboa, C. S. L., & Chaves, M. L. F. (2010). The Mini Mental State Examination: Review of cutoff points adjusted for schooling in a large Southern Brazilian sample. Dementia Neuropsychologia, 4(1), 35-41. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642010DN40100006
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642010DN40100006
  • Lee, L., Harkness, K. L., Sabbagh, M. A., & Jacobson, J. A. (2005). Mental state decoding abilities in clinical depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 86(2-3), 247-258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.02.007
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.02.007
  • Leppänen, J. M., Milders, M., Bell, J. S., Terriere, E., & Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Depression biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces. Psychiatry Research, 128(2), 123-133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.020
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.020
  • Liotti, G., & Prunetti, E. (2010). Metacognitive deficits in trauma-related disorders: Contingent on interpersonal motivational contexts? In Dimaggio, G. & Lysaker, P.H. (Eds.), Metacognition and severe adult mental disorders: From research to treatment (pp. 196-214). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
  • Matthews, S. C., Strigo, I. A., Simmons, A. N., Yang, T. T., & Paulus, M. P. (2008). Decreased functional coupling of the amygdala and supragenual cingulate is related to increased depression in unmedicated individuals with current major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 111(1), 13-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.022
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.022
  • Millan, M. J., Agid, Y., Brüne, M., Bullmore, E. T., Carter, C. S., Clayton, N. S., Connor, R., Davis, S., Deakin, B., DeRubeis, R.J., Dubois, B., Geyer, M. A., Goodwin, G. M., Gorwood, P., Jay, T. M., Joëls, M., Mansuy, I. M., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Murphy, D., ... Young, L. J. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: Characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature reviews Drug discovery, 11(2), 141. https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrd3628
    » https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrd3628
  • Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 638-662. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.638
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.638
  • O'Connor, L. E., Berry, J. W., Weiss, J., & Gilbert, P. (2002). Guilt, fear, submission, and empathy in depression. Journal of affective disorders, 71(1-3), 19-27. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0327(01)00408-6
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/s0165-0327(01)00408-6
  • Roca, M., Vives, M., Lopez-Navarro, E., Garcia-Campayo, J., & Gili, M. (2015). Cognitive impairments and depression: A critical review. Actas Espanolas de Psiquiatria, 43(5), 187-193.
  • Sampaio, L. R., Guimarães, P. R. B., Camino, C. P. S., Formiga, N. S., & Menezes, I. G. (2011). Estudos sobre a dimensionalidade da empatia: Tradução e adaptação do Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI). Psico, 42(1), 67-76.
  • Santamaría-García, H., Baez, S., Gómez, C., Rodríguez-Villagra, O., Huepe, D., Portela, M., Reyes, P., Klahr, J., Matallana, D., & Ibanez, A. (2020). The role of social cognition skills and social determinants of health in predicting symptoms of mental illness.Translational psychiatry , 10(1), 165. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-0852-4
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-0852-4
  • Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Hergueta, T., Baker, R., & Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of clinical psychiatry, 59(20), 22-33.
  • Schneider, D., Regenbogen, C., Kellermann, T., Finkelmeyer, A., Kohn, N., Derntl, B., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2012). Empathic behavioral and physiological responses to dynamic stimuli in depression. Psychiatry research, 200(2-3), 294-305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.03.054
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.03.054
  • Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G. H. M., & Aan Het Rot, M. (2013). Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150(1), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.009
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.03.009
  • Shahaeian, A., Henry, J. D., Razmjoee, M., Teymoori, A., & Wang, C. (2014). Towards a better understanding of the relationship between executive control and theory of mind: An intra-cultural comparison of three diverse samples. Developmental Science, 18(5), 671-685. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12243
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12243
  • Snyder, H. R. (2013). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: A meta-analysis and review. Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 81-132. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028727
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028727
  • Steiner, V. A. G., Mansur, L. L., Brucki, S. M. D., & Nitrini, R. (2008). Phonemic verbal fluency and age: A preliminary study. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 2(4), 328-332.
  • Suslow, T., Konrad, C., Kugel, H., Rumstadt, D., Zwitserlood, P., Schöning, S., Ohrmann, P, Bauer, J., Pyka, M., Kersting, A., Arolt, V., Heindel, W., & Dannlowski, U. (2010). Automatic mood-congruent amygdala responses to masked facial expressions in Major Depression. Biological Psychiatry, 67(2), 155-160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.023
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.023
  • Thoma, P., Zalewski, I., von Reventlow, H. G., Norra, C., Juckel, G., & Daum, I. (2011). Cognitive and affective empathy in depression linked to executive control. Psychiatry Research, 189(3), 373-378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.030
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.030
  • Uekermann, J., Channon, S., Lehmkämper, C., Abdel-Hamid, M., Vollmoeller, W., & Daum, I. (2008). Executive function, mentalizing and humor in major depression. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(1), 55-62. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617708080016
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617708080016
  • Wagner, S., Helmreich, I., Wollschläger, D., Meyer, K., Kaaden, S., Reiff, J., Roll, S. C., Braus, D., Tüscher, O., Müller-Dahlhaus, F., Tadić, A., & Lieb, K. (2018). Early improvement of executive test performance during antidepressant treatment predicts treatment outcome in patients with Major Depressive Disorder. PloS one, 13(4), e0194574. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0194574
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0194574
  • Wang, Y. G., Wang, Y. Q., Chen, S. L., Zhu, C. Y., & Wang, K. (2008). Theory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: A componential view. Psychiatry Research, 161(2), 153-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.018
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.018
  • Weightman, M. J., Air, T. M., & Baune, B. T. (2014). A review of the role of social cognition in major depressive disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 179. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00179
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00179
  • Wilbertz, G., Brakemeier, E. L., Zobel, I., Härter, M., & Schramm, E. (2010). Exploring preoperational features in chronic depression. Journal of affective disorders, 124(3), 262-269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.11.021
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2009.11.021
  • Wolkenstein, L., Schönenberg, M., Schirm, E., & Hautzinger, M. (2011). I can see what you feel, but I can't deal with it: Impaired theory of mind in depression. Journal of affective disorders, 132(1-2), 104-111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.010
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.010
  • Yamada, K., Inoue, Y., & Kanba, S. (2015). Theory of mind ability predicts prognosis of outpatients with major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 230(2), 604-608. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.10.011
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.10.011
  • Zaremba, D., Kalthoff, I. S., Förster, K., Redlich, R., Grotegerd, D., Leehr, E. J., Meinert, S., Dohm, K., Bürger, C., Enneking, V., Böhnlein, J., Repple, J., Opel, N., Jörgens, S., Yüksel, D., Schmitt, S., Stein, F., Kircher, T., Krug, A., Nenadić, I., Zwitserlood, P., Baune, B.T., Arolt, V., & Dannlowski, U. (2019). The effects of processing speed on memory impairment in patients with major depressive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 92, 494-500.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.02.015
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.02.015
  • Zigmond, A. S., & Snaith, R. P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta psychiatrica scandinavica, 67(6), 361-370.
  • Zimmermann, N., Cardoso, C. O., Kristensen, C. H., & Fonseca, R. P. (2017). Brazilian norms and effects of age and education on the Hayling and Trail Making Tests. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 39(3), 188-195. https://doi.org/10.1590/2237-6089-2016-0082
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/2237-6089-2016-0082
  • Zuckerman, H., Pan, Z., Park, C., Brietzke, E., Musial, N., Shariq, A. S., Iacobucci, M., Yim, S. J., Lui, L., Rong, C., & McIntyre, R. S. (2018). Recognition and treatment of cognitive dysfunction in Major Depressive Disorder.Frontiers in psychiatry ,9, 655. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00655
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00655

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    07 Nov 2022
  • Date of issue
    2022

History

  • Received
    26 Feb 2019
  • Accepted
    31 May 2021
Instituto de Psicologia, Universidade de Brasília Instituto de Psicologia, Universidade de Brasília, 70910-900 - Brasília - DF - Brazil, Tel./Fax: (061) 274-6455 - Brasília - DF - Brazil
E-mail: revistaptp@gmail.com