Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Infant-directed speech in Brazilian Portuguese: The prosody of questions

Fala dirigida à criança em português brasileiro: A prosódia de perguntas

ABSTRACT

Infant-directed speech (IDS) is the register commonly used in infant-adult interactions in many linguistic communities, and presents a number of special lexical, syntactic and prosodic particularities when compared to adult speech, such as words with syllable reduplication, high question rate and exaggerated prosody. In this study, we focus on interrogatives and their prosodic properties as the object of investigation. Data analysis of interactions of ten Brazilian Portuguese-acquiring infants aged 4 to 12 months and their caregivers, revealed that questions corresponded to 33% of the total utterances produced and are emphasized mainly by marked pitch (raised pitch range and expanded pitch span), as well as increased duration. Moreover, the pitch contours of Wh and Yes/No questions followed their respective patterns observed in adult speech. These results are compatible with those found in the literature and indicate that, in general, the IDS used in Brazilian Portuguese has similar characteristics to this register in other languages.

Keywords:
infant-directed speech; prosody of questions; intonation; Brazilian Portuguese

RESUMO

A fala dirigida à criança (FDC) é o registro comumente usado nas interações bebê-adulto em muitas comunidades linguísticas, e apresenta uma série de particularidades lexicais, sintáticas e prosódicas quando comparada à fala entre adultos, tais como palavras com reduplicação de sílabas, alta taxa de perguntas e prosódia exagerada. Neste estudo, elegemos como objeto de investigação as interrogativas e suas propriedades prosódicas. A análise de dados de interações de dez bebês de 4 a 12 meses adquirindo o português brasileiro e seus cuidadores revelou que as perguntas corresponderam a 33% do total de enunciados produzidos e são realçadas principalmente pelo pitch marcado (maior pitch range e pitch span), e maior duração. Ainda, os contornos de pitch das perguntas Qu e Sim/Não (perguntas totais) seguem seus respectivos padrões observados na fala adulta. Esses resultados são compatíveis com os encontrados na literatura e apontam que, de modo geral, A FDC usada no português brasileiro apresenta características semelhantes a esse registro em outras línguas.

Palavras-chave:
fala dirigida à criança; prosódia de perguntas; entoação; português brasileiro

1. Introduction

It’s practically impossible to talk to a baby without using a high-pitched sing-songy voice. (...) Not only do babies prefer listening to IDS but it also helps support little ones’ comprehension. Plus, babytalk often uses exaggerated prosody. (…) Infant-directed speech is exaggerated and filled with different rhythms and melodies because this is what infants comprehend. Research shows that infants are much more likely to associate meanings of words when the adult is utilizing IDS versus adult-directed speech (ADS). (Stangl, as cited in Cinelli, 2021Cinelli, E. (2021, September 17). How prosody plays a role in language development. Verywell Family. https://www.verywellfamily.com/prosody-and-language-acquisition-5191264.
https://www.verywellfamily.com/prosody-a...
)

Infant-directed speech (IDS) is the register used by adults when interacting with infants and young children in many and diverse cultures (Fernald, 1992Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391-428). Oxford University Press. ; Saint-Georges et al., 2013Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., Laznik, M.-C., & Cohen, D. (2013). Motherese in interaction: At the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PLoS One, 8(10), Article e78103. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078103.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1371/...
). Compared to adult-directed speech (ADS), this register is distinguished by presenting shorter and structurally simpler sentences, high number of questions, repeated and modified words by reduplication, hyperarticulation, long pauses between utterances, slower tempo, higher pitch and greater pitch variations (Fernald, 1992Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391-428). Oxford University Press. ; Garnica, 1977Garnica, O. K. (1977) Some prosodic and paralinguistics features of speech to young children. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 63-88). Cambridge University Press. ; Snow, 1977Snow, E. C. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: from input to interaction. In C. Snow, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 31-49). Cambridge University Press.; 1995, among others).

Although still a controversial issue, the idea that IDS may facilitate the language acquisition process is advocated by many authors. Syntactically simple sentences, repetitions and hyperarticulation would facilitate the identification of the syntax and lexicon of the language, but above all exaggerated prosody is said to play an important role not only in language development, conveying affect, attracting attention and engaging the infant in social interactions, but also in highlighting words, and boundaries between constituents and different structures (declarative and interrogative) (Adriaans & Swingley, 2017Adriaans, F., & Swingley, D. (2017). Prosodic exaggeration within infant-directed speech: Consequences for vowel learnability. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 141(5), 3070-3078. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4982246.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1121/...
; Fernald, 1992Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391-428). Oxford University Press. ; Snow, 1977Snow, E. C. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: from input to interaction. In C. Snow, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 31-49). Cambridge University Press.; Thiessen et al., 2005Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy , 7(1), 53-71. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0701_5.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1207/...
).

In fact, specific adaptations of prosodic features have been observed in the IDS of speakers of tonal and non-tonal languages (Kitamura et al., 2001Kitamura, C., Thanavishuth, C., Burnham, D., & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2001). Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behavior and Development , 24(4), 372-392. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00086-3.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
), stress-timed, syllable-timed, and mora-timed languages (Fernald, 1992Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391-428). Oxford University Press. ; Fernald et al., 1989Fernald, A., Taescher, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., De Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 16(3), p. 477-501. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010679.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
; Saint-George et al., 2013Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., Laznik, M.-C., & Cohen, D. (2013). Motherese in interaction: At the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PLoS One, 8(10), Article e78103. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078103.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1371/...
; Wang et al., 2016Wang, Y., Seidl, A., & Cristia, A. (2016). Acoustic characteristics of infant-directed speech as a function of prosodic typology. In J. Heinz, R. Goedemans, & H. Van der Hulst (Eds.), Dimensions of phonological stress (pp. 311-326). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316212745.012.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
). Women and men who use IDS when speaking to infants show similar acoustical features such as higher pitch and exaggerated pitch excursion (Fernald et al., 1989Fernald, A., Taescher, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., De Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 16(3), p. 477-501. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010679.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
; Niwano & Sugai, 2003Niwano, K., & Sugai, K. (2003). Pitch characteristics of speech during mother-infant and father-infant vocal interactions. The Japanese Journal of Special Education, 40(6), 663-674. https://doi.org/10.6033/tokkyou.40.663.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.6033/...
; Papousek et al., 1987Papoušek, M., Papoušek, H., & Haekel, M. (1987). Didactic adjustments in fathers’ and mothers’ speech to their 3-month-old infants. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 16, 491-516. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01073274.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/...
; Soderstrom, 2007Soderstrom, M. (2007). Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Development Review, 27(4), 501-532. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.06.002.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
), but some studies have pointed out some differences. For instance, Benders et al. (2021Benders, T., StGeorge, J., & Fletcher, R. (2021). Infant-directed speech by Dutch fathers: Increased pitch variability within and across utterances. Language Learning and Development, 17(3), 292-325. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
) reported pitch differences between fathers and mothers using IDS when talking to their infants in Dutch.

As for Brazilian Portuguese (BP), there are few studies focusing on IDS, and particularly fewer on its prosodic features. Dadalto and Goldfeld (2006Dadalto, E. V., & Goldfeld, M. (2006). Características do maternalês em duas crianças de idades distintas. Distúrbios da Comunicação, 18(2), 201-208. https://revistas.pucsp.br/index.php/dic/article/view/11786/8520.
https://revistas.pucsp.br/index.php/dic/...
) observed high pitch and exaggerated intonation on language interactions between a mother and her 12-month-old son. Splendore et al. (2019Splendore, K. M., Constantini, A. C., & Da Silva, K. C. (2019). Investigação da prosódia e da linguagem na interação mãe-bebê. Working Papers em Linguística, 20(1), 172-188. https://doi.org/10.5007/1984-8420.2019v20n1p172.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.5007/...
) analysed the interaction between 10 mothers and their respective infants aged from 3 to 8 months. The authors found that 9 of 10 mothers used IDS, mostly characterized by reduced speech rate/slower rhythm and increased vocal range.

Focusing on questions, Name and Sosa (2020aName, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020a). The prosody of questions in Brazilian Portuguese infant-directed speech. In N. Minematsu, M. Kondo, T. Arai & R. Hayashi (Eds.), Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2020 (pp. 334-337). https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020-68.
https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2...
, 2020bName, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020b). Cadê o amor da mamãe? As interrogativas na fala dirigida à criança adquirindo o PB. Veredas - Revista de Estudos Linguísticos, 24(1), 72-93. https://doi.org/10.34019/1982-2243.2020.v24.30916.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.34019...
), analysed the IDS used by adults in interactions with four BP-acquiring infants between ages 4- and 12-months. As reported in the literature, adults ask many questions to infants, even though they do not expect an effective verbal or non-verbal response from them (Geffen & Mintz, 2017Geffen, S., & Mintz, T. (2017). Prosodic differences between declaratives and interrogatives in infant-directed speech. Journal of Child Language , 44(4), 968-994. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000349.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
; Newport et al., 1977Newport, E., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I’d rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In C. Snow, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp.109-149). Cambridge University Press.; Soderstrom et al., 2008Soderstrom, M., Blossom, M., Foygel, R., & Morgan, J. (2008). Acoustic cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 35(4), 869-902. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008763.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
; Thorson et al, 2014Thorson, J., Borras-Comes, J., Crespo-Sendra, V., Vanrell, M., & Prieto, P. (2014). The acquisition of melodic form and meaning in yes-no interrogatives by Catalan and Spanish children. Probus, 27(1), 73-99. https://doi.org/10.1515/probus-2013-0019.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1515/...
). According to Name and Sosa (2020bName, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020b). Cadê o amor da mamãe? As interrogativas na fala dirigida à criança adquirindo o PB. Veredas - Revista de Estudos Linguísticos, 24(1), 72-93. https://doi.org/10.34019/1982-2243.2020.v24.30916.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.34019...
), questions accounted for 36.7% of the total utterances analysed, and more than 70% of them were marked by one or more prosodic features, with a prevalence of high register, pronounced differences between peaks and valleys and slow rhythm. The pitch contours of wh- and yes-no questions (also known as polar questions) followed the patterns found in the ADS - respectively, falling and rising-falling - but mostly marked by the wide pitch movement. As far as we know, there are no studies contrasting prosodic features of IDS of women and men in BP.

With the aim of contributing to the precise characterization of the prosodic properties of IDS in BP, this paper presents the results of our analysis of corpora of interactions between ten children and their families. We chose to analyse questions due to their communicative role in adult-infant interactions, and because of the fact that in Portuguese declarative and interrogative utterances are only distinguished by their respective intonational contours, which may be a cue to a preliminary identification of these syntactic structures by infants. We sought to find out (i) the rate of occurrences of interrogatives in relation to other utterances; (ii) the most common IDS prosodic features in interrogatives; (iii) the intonational pattern of wh- and yes-no questions, whether they were compatible or not with those found in BP ADS; and (iv) prosodic similarities and differences between women and men IDS. For this purpose, we expanded the corpora presented in Name and Sosa (2020aName, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020a). The prosody of questions in Brazilian Portuguese infant-directed speech. In N. Minematsu, M. Kondo, T. Arai & R. Hayashi (Eds.), Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2020 (pp. 334-337). https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020-68.
https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2...
, 2020bName, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020b). Cadê o amor da mamãe? As interrogativas na fala dirigida à criança adquirindo o PB. Veredas - Revista de Estudos Linguísticos, 24(1), 72-93. https://doi.org/10.34019/1982-2243.2020.v24.30916.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.34019...
), including adult-infant interactions with six more children (a total of 10 children).

2. Infant-Directed Speech - IDS

In many cultures adults and older children frequently use IDS when they talk to babies and younger children. Cross-linguistic similarities such as simpler syntactic structures, great number of questions, word repetition, hyperarticulation and exaggerated intonation were observed when this register was compared to ADS (Fernald, 1992Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391-428). Oxford University Press. ; Garnica, 1974Garnica, O. K. (1974). Some prosodic characteristics of speech to young children. [ Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Stanford University. ; Snow, 1977Snow, E. C. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: from input to interaction. In C. Snow, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 31-49). Cambridge University Press., 1995). Although its use may not be universal (Cristia et al., 2019Cristia, A., Dupoux, E., Gurven, M., & Stieglitz, J. (2019). Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager-farmer population: A time allocation study. Child Development , 90(3), 759-773. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12974.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/...
; Weber et al., 2017Weber, A., Fernald, A., & Diop, Y. (2017). When cultural norms discourage talking to babies: Effectiveness of a parenting program in Rural Senegal. Child Development , 88(5), 1513-1526. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12882.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/...
), IDS seems to have a positive impact on the emotional, cognitive, and linguistic development of the baby. IDS promotes greater babies’ social engagement (Cavalcante & Barros, 2012Cavalcante, M. C. B. & Barros, A. T. M. C. (2012). Manhês: Qualidade vocal e deslocamentos na dialogia mãe-bebê. Veredas - Revista de Estudos Linguísticos, 16(spe), 25-39. https://periodicos.ufjf.br/index.php/veredas/article/view/25054.
https://periodicos.ufjf.br/index.php/ver...
; De Pablo et al., 2020De Pablo, I, Murillo, E., & Romero, A. (2020). The effect of infant-directed speech on early multimodal communicative production in Spanish and Basque. Journal of Child Language, 47(2), 457-471. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000412.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
; Fernald & Simon, 1984Fernald, A., & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology, 20(1), 104-113. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.1.104.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/...
), facilitates word segmentation (Thiessen et al., 2005Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy , 7(1), 53-71. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0701_5.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1207/...
), word learning (Gout et al., 2004Gout, A., Christophe, A., & Morgan, J. L. (2004). Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access: II Infant data. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(4), 548-567. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.07.002.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
; Matsuoka & Name, 2011Matsuoka, A., & Name, C. (2011). O uso de pistas prosódicas na identificação do adjetivo por crianças e adultos falantes do PB. In Anais do VII Congresso Internacional da ABRALIN (pp. 577-587).), and syntactic structure distinction (Chiang et al., 2018Chiang, C., Geffen, S., & Mintz, T. H. (2018). Distinguishing questions and statements using sentence-initial prosodic cues. In A. B. Bertolini & M. J. Kaplan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 42nd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 153-158). http://www.lingref.com/bucld/42/BUCLD42-12.pdf.
http://www.lingref.com/bucld/42/BUCLD42-...
; Floccia et al., 2016Floccia, C., Keren-Portnoy, T., DePaolis, R., Duffy, H., Delle Luche, C., Durrant, S., White, L., Goslin, J., & Vihman, M. (2016). British English infants segment words only with exaggerated infant-directed speech stimuli. Cognition, 148, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.004.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
; Frota et al., 2014Frota, S., Butler, J., & Vigário, M. (2014). Infant’s perception of intonation: Is it a statement or a question? Infancy, 19(2), 194-213. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12037.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/...
; Graf Estes & Hurley, 2013Graf Estes, K., & Hurley, K. (2013). Infant-directed prosody helps infants map sounds to meanings. Infancy , 18(5), 797-824. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12006.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/...
; Splendore et al., 2019Splendore, K. M., Constantini, A. C., & Da Silva, K. C. (2019). Investigação da prosódia e da linguagem na interação mãe-bebê. Working Papers em Linguística, 20(1), 172-188. https://doi.org/10.5007/1984-8420.2019v20n1p172.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.5007/...
). Newborns and infants prefer this type of speech to ADS (Cooper & Aslin, 1990Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61(5), 1584-1595. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130766.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2307/...
), and evidence from 9-month-old infants’ brain responses suggests that IDS stimuli are easier to discriminate than ADS ones (Peter et al., 2016Peter, V., Kalashnikova, M., Santos, A., & Burnham, D. (2016). Mature neural responses to infant-directed speech but not adult-directed speech in pre-verbal infants. Nature Scientfic Reports, 6, Article 34273. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep34273.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1038/...
). Also, 12- to 15-month-old babies have expectations about the register adults would/should use according to their interlocutor, rejecting the use of ADS when addressed to children (Soley & Sebastian-Galles, 2020Soley, G., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2020). Infants’ expectations about the recipients of infant-directed and adult-directed speech. Cognition, 198, 104214, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104214.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
).

In particular, the prosodic properties of IDS, and especially its exaggerated pitch contours, seem to attract their attention (Fernald, 1992Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391-428). Oxford University Press. ). Fernald and Kuhl (1987Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 10(3), 279-293. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(87)90017-8.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
) manipulated IDS utterances by extracting and presenting separately F0, amplitude and duration to 4-month-old infants. The infants preferred the utterances that maintained the F0 compared to the other utterances. For Räsänen et al. (2018Räsänen, O., Kakouros, S., & Soderstrom, M. (2018). Is infant-directed speech interesting because it is surprising? Linking properties of IDS to statistical learning and attention at the prosodic level. Cognition, 178, 193-206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.015.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
), the exaggerated pitch contour would make the utterances more stimulating, recruiting more of the infant’s attentional resources.

IDS prosodic characteristics

In the first systematic analysis of IDS, Garnica (1974Garnica, O. K. (1974). Some prosodic characteristics of speech to young children. [ Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Stanford University. ) compared utterances produced by adult speakers of American English in contexts of interaction with children between 1;10 and 2;6 years (mean age: 2;3 years), with children between 5;1 and 5;7 years (mean age: 5;4 years), and among adults. The author observed in the speech directed at 2-year-old children “higher mean F0, a greatly expanded frequency range, numerous instances of rising sentence pitch terminals in declarative and imperative sentences, a high incidence of whispering, increased durations of certain key content words in sentences, and the multiple assignment of primary stress within a sentence unit” (Garnica, 1974Garnica, O. K. (1974). Some prosodic characteristics of speech to young children. [ Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Stanford University. , p. 56). Such features did not occur significantly in interactions with 5-year-old children or among adults.

Higher pitch, expanded pitch range and slower tempo have also been documented in IDS in different languages, such as British English, German, French, Italian, Japanese (Fernald et al., 1989Fernald, A., Taescher, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., De Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 16(3), p. 477-501. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010679.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
), Latvian, Comanche, Sinhala (Fernald, 1992Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391-428). Oxford University Press. ), Xhosa (Fernald & O’Neill, 1993Fernald, A., & O’Neill, D. K. (1993). Peekaboo across cultures: How mothers and infants play with voices, faces, and expectations. In K. MacDonald (Ed.), Parent-child play: Descriptions and implications ( pp. 259-285) State University of New York Press.), and Mandarin Chinese (Grieser & Kuhl, 1988Grieser, D. L., & Kuhl, P. K. (1988). Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. Developmental Psychology , 24(1), 14-20. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.1.14.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/...
) (see Cristia, 2013Cristia, A. (2013). Input to language: The phonetics and perception of infant-directed speech. Language and Linguistics Compass, 7(3), 157-170. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12015.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/...
; Saint-George et al., 2013Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., Laznik, M.-C., & Cohen, D. (2013). Motherese in interaction: At the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PLoS One, 8(10), Article e78103. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078103.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1371/...
and Wang et al., 2016Wang, Y., Seidl, A., & Cristia, A. (2016). Acoustic characteristics of infant-directed speech as a function of prosodic typology. In J. Heinz, R. Goedemans, & H. Van der Hulst (Eds.), Dimensions of phonological stress (pp. 311-326). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316212745.012.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
for systematic reviews of IDS literature). In a cross-linguistic study with Lankan Tamil, Tagalog and Korean speaking mothers, Narayan and McDermott (2016Narayan, C. R., & McDermott, L. C. (2016). Speech rate and pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech: Longitudinal and cross-linguistic observations. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 139, 1272-1281. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4944634.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1121/...
) verified slower speech rate, raised pitch and wide pitch excursions when they interacted with their 4- to 16-month-old children, than in their adult interactions. Interestingly, speech rate changed and became faster as the babies grew up, but pitch characteristics remained constant over the infants’ development.

In a systematic review of the literature, Saint-Georges et al. (2013Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., Laznik, M.-C., & Cohen, D. (2013). Motherese in interaction: At the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PLoS One, 8(10), Article e78103. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078103.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1371/...
) report that

longer pauses, a slower tempo, more prosodic repetitions, and a higher mean F0 and wider F0-range were observed for fathers and mothers across various languages (Fernald et al. 1989Fernald, A., Taescher, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., De Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 16(3), p. 477-501. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010679.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
; Niwano & Sugai, 2003Niwano, K., & Sugai, K. (2003). Pitch characteristics of speech during mother-infant and father-infant vocal interactions. The Japanese Journal of Special Education, 40(6), 663-674. https://doi.org/10.6033/tokkyou.40.663.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.6033/...
; Shute & Wheldall, 1999), except regarding to the wider F0-range (…) and also for grandmothers interacting with their grandchildren (Shute & Wheldall (2001). (Saint-Georges et al., 2013Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., Laznik, M.-C., & Cohen, D. (2013). Motherese in interaction: At the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PLoS One, 8(10), Article e78103. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078103.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1371/...
, p. 3)

Papousek et al. (1987Papoušek, M., Papoušek, H., & Haekel, M. (1987). Didactic adjustments in fathers’ and mothers’ speech to their 3-month-old infants. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 16, 491-516. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01073274.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/...
) compared syntactic, lexical and “temporal-melodic” features of German mothers’ and fathers’ IDS produced during interactions with three-month-old infants. According to the authors, both parents adjusted their talks in similar ways in all linguistic levels. Fernald et al. (1989Fernald, A., Taescher, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., De Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 16(3), p. 477-501. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010679.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
) analysed prosodic features of mothers’ and fathers’ IDS register in American and British English, French, Italian, German and Japanese (mean F0, minimum- and maximum-F0, F0 range and variability, utterance and pause durations). They found that across languages, both mothers and fathers produced higher mean, minimum and maximum of F0, greater F0 variability, shorter utterances and longer pauses in IDS compared to ADS. Only mothers produced a wider F0 range and American English parents exhibited more exaggerated prosodic modifications than the other parents. According to the authors, these results suggest universal prosodic characteristics of IDS, but also possible cultural and gender speaker variations in the use of this register. Regarding more recent studies, the picture is less clear. On the one hand no effect of gender was found in IDS prosodic properties produced by German (Weirich & Simpson, 2019Weirich, M., & Simpson, A. (2019). Effects of gender, parental role, and time on infant- and adult-directed read and spontaneous speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(11), 4001-4014. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-19-0047.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1044/...
) nor Japanese parents (Niwano & Sugai, 2003Niwano, K., & Sugai, K. (2003). Pitch characteristics of speech during mother-infant and father-infant vocal interactions. The Japanese Journal of Special Education, 40(6), 663-674. https://doi.org/10.6033/tokkyou.40.663.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.6033/...
). On the other hand, Benders et al. (2021Benders, T., StGeorge, J., & Fletcher, R. (2021). Infant-directed speech by Dutch fathers: Increased pitch variability within and across utterances. Language Learning and Development, 17(3), 292-325. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
) observed that Dutch-speaking fathers increased their pitch variability within and across utterances more than mothers did. Kitamura et al. (2001Kitamura, C., Thanavishuth, C., Burnham, D., & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2001). Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behavior and Development , 24(4), 372-392. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00086-3.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
) found that pitch range addressing girls is consistently higher than addressing boys in Thai and Australian English IDS.

Regarding interrogatives, Geffen and Mintz (2017Geffen, S., & Mintz, T. (2017). Prosodic differences between declaratives and interrogatives in infant-directed speech. Journal of Child Language , 44(4), 968-994. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000349.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
) compared declaratives, wh- questions and yes-no questions produced by three native American English mothers in interactions with their infants. Overall, the pitch contours of the three types of structures were similar to those found in ADS, but more exaggerated. In the comparison between structures, declaratives and yes-no questions differed significantly in pitch excursion, mean F0, Max F0 and duration, which did not occur with wh- questions. Soderstrom et al. (2008Soderstrom, M., Blossom, M., Foygel, R., & Morgan, J. (2008). Acoustic cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 35(4), 869-902. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008763.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
) also examined the prosody (among other characteristics) of questions in American English IDS in the data of two mothers talking to their babies. They found between 25% and 49% of interrogatives, depending on the baby and the baby’s age (6- to 12-month-olds). Yes-no questions were mostly produced with the ADS rising contour (76%), while only 29% of wh- questions showed a falling or sinusoidal contour.

In BP, Name and Sosa (2020aName, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020a). The prosody of questions in Brazilian Portuguese infant-directed speech. In N. Minematsu, M. Kondo, T. Arai & R. Hayashi (Eds.), Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2020 (pp. 334-337). https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020-68.
https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2...
, 2020bName, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020b). Cadê o amor da mamãe? As interrogativas na fala dirigida à criança adquirindo o PB. Veredas - Revista de Estudos Linguísticos, 24(1), 72-93. https://doi.org/10.34019/1982-2243.2020.v24.30916.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.34019...
) analysed interrogatives produced in interactions of adults (parents and relatives) and four infants aged 4 to 12 months. They found over 70% of interrogatives marked by one or more prosodic features, with high register, pronounced differences between peaks and valleys and slow speech rate being the most commonly observed features. They conclude that the pitch contours of yes-no and wh- questions maintained the patterns of ADS, however more characterized by wide pitch movement.

Intonation of Questions in Brazilian Portuguese

Interrogative intonation in BP has configurations and uses that differ from other languages, i.e., yes/no questions tend to be rising/falling with the peak on the final stressed syllable (‘circumflex’ contour). The shape of the terminal contour or nuclear accent is the feature that distinguishes statements from yes/no questions, which rises through the final stressed syllable with a late alignment of the F0 peak before the final fall, as seen in Figure 1 (taken from Moraes, 2008Moraes, J. A. (2008). The pitch accents in Brazilian Portuguese: Analysis by synthesis. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2008 (pp. 389-397). https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs/speechprosody_2008/moraes08_speechprosody.pdf.
https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs...
). The nucleus of yes/no questions has an accentual rise on the last stressed syllable followed by a L% boundary; this accentual rise has been characterized as a L+H* pitch accent. For an overview on the intonation of yes-no questions in Brazilian Portuguese dialects and in other Portuguese varieties, see Braga et al. (this volumeBraga, G., Frota, S., & Fernandes-Svartman, F. (2022). Guinea-Bissau Portuguese: What the intonation of yes-no question shows about this variety. Special issue Prosody and interfaces (Ed. by C. Serra, F. Fernandes-Svartman, and M. Cruz). DELTA, 38(3), Article 202258942, 1-27. https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678-460X202258942.
https://doi.org/https://dx.doi.org/10.15...
).

Figure 1
Contour of neutral yes/no question “Renata jogava?” (Moraes, 2008Moraes, J. A. (2008). The pitch accents in Brazilian Portuguese: Analysis by synthesis. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2008 (pp. 389-397). https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs/speechprosody_2008/moraes08_speechprosody.pdf.
https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs...
, p. 393)

Wh- questions in BP tend to be, as reported for several languages (Hedberg et al., 2010Hedberg, N., Sosa, J. M., Görgülü E., & Mameni, M. (2010). Prosody and pragmatics of wh-interrogatives. In M. Heijl (Ed.), Proceedings of the 2010 Annual Conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association. http://homes.chass.utoronto.ca/~cla-acl/actes2010/CLA2010_Hedberg_et_al.pdf.
http://homes.chass.utoronto.ca/~cla-acl/...
; Sosa, 2003Sosa, J. M. (2003). Wh-questions in Spanish: Meanings and configuration variability. Catalan Journal of Linguistics, 2, 229-247. https://raco.cat/index.php/CatalanJournal/article/view/308981.
https://raco.cat/index.php/CatalanJourna...
), falling throughout the utterance; high pitch on the initial wh-word with gradual fall over the following syllables, until the last stressed syllable is reached with a H+L*L% nuclear contour, as seen in Figure 2 (also from Moraes, 2008Moraes, J. A. (2008). The pitch accents in Brazilian Portuguese: Analysis by synthesis. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2008 (pp. 389-397). https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs/speechprosody_2008/moraes08_speechprosody.pdf.
https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs...
). Rising wh- questions were also found, as well as rising-falling, which are used for specific contexts such as rising for echo-questions, or rising-falling for emphasis, focus or exclamation. For a comparison of acoustical properties between wh-questions and wh-exclamations from a perceptive perspective, see Miranda et al. (this volumeMiranda, L., Moraes, J., & Rilliard, A. (2022). Effects of F0 movements, intensity, and duration in the perceptual identification of Brazilian Portuguese wh-questions and wh-exclamations. Special issue Prosody and interfaces (Ed. By C. Serra , F. Fernandes-Svartman , and M. Cruz). DELTA , 38(3), Article 202258882, 1-29. https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678-460X202258882.
https://doi.org/https://dx.doi.org/10.15...
).

Figure 2
Contour of canonical wh- question “Como ela jogava?” (Moraes, 2008Moraes, J. A. (2008). The pitch accents in Brazilian Portuguese: Analysis by synthesis. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2008 (pp. 389-397). https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs/speechprosody_2008/moraes08_speechprosody.pdf.
https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs...
, p. 392)

In our data these two canonical contours were predominant, although we also encountered other kinds of contour shapes: i) Falling; ii) Rising; iii) Falling-Rising; or iv) Plateau (flat). All these nuclear contours occurred for both yes-no and wh- questions and have different interpretations according to pragmatic considerations. We found though that the design of the question types, whether displaying the canonical pitch trajectory or not, as well as the relative frequency of each one, were in agreement with what is found in adult speech: we thus conclude that contour shape of questions is not a feature in which IDS would differ from ADS.

3. Our study

Participants and Data Collection

Ten babies, 5 girls and 5 boys and their families participated in the research. The data we gathered were mostly produced by the babies’ mothers, but fathers and other family members also participated in the interactions. Recordings took place in Juiz de Fora, Minas Gerais, and Florianópolis, Santa Catarina. The collection was carried out by the babies’ caregivers themselves, who were instructed to make recordings of everyday situations of adult-baby interaction. In order to minimize possible disturbances in the routine of the baby and the family caused by the collection, the type (audio or video) and the amount of the recordings were at the discretion of those in charge. The researchers did not participate directly in data collection. This research was approved by the Ethics Committee on Human Research (CAAE: 28142820.5.0000.5147) and those responsible for the children as well as the adults involved signed an Informed Consent Form. The names of the babies were changed for privacy concerns. Below we present the details of the data, by baby. In Table 1 we show a summary of the data.

  • - Interactions between Bibo, a 4-month-old male infant and his mother and/or father. The video recordings took place in spontaneous conversation and/or play situations at home, in which one or two adults and the infant participated, totaling 30 minutes and 57 seconds.

  • - Interactions between Guto, male infant and his mother or grandmother. The video recordings took place at 4 months and 10 months of age, in spontaneous play situations at home, involving one adult and the baby. The recordings lasted 2 minutes and 41 seconds at 4 months and 5 minutes and 10 seconds at 10 months, for a total of 8 minutes and 51 seconds. Given the interval of 6 months between the two collections, the data were analysed separately.

  • - Interactions between Gigi, a 4- to 5-month-old female infant and her mother, father, grandmother and uncle. The audio recordings took place in spontaneous play situations at home, in which at least two adults participated, and totalled 28 minutes and 6 seconds.

  • - Interactions between Don, a 6.5-month-old male infant and his mother, his great-grandmother, his uncle, and his aunt. The audio recordings took place during spontaneous play situations at home, in which at least two adults participated, and totalled 9 minutes and 21 seconds.

  • - Interactions between Bel, a 6- to 10-month-old female infant and her mother. The video recordings occurred during spontaneous situations of conversation and/or play at home, on three occasions - at 6, 8 and 10 months - totalling 4 minutes and 45 seconds.

  • - Interactions between Lala, female infant between 6.5 and 8.5 months and her mother, father, and brother. The video recordings occurred in spontaneous play situations at home and totalled 3 minutes and 10 seconds.

  • - Interactions between Cadu, a 7- to 8-month-old male infant, his mother and/or father. The video recordings occurred in spontaneous situations of conversation and/or play, at home or in the family’s car, totalling 6 minutes and 2 seconds.

  • - Interactions between Juju, a 9-month-old female infant, and her grandmother, grandfather, and uncle. The video recordings occurred during spontaneous situations of conversation and/or play at home and totalled 5 minutes and 4 seconds.

  • - Interactions between Nino, a 9.5-month-old male infant and his mother. The audio recordings occurred during spontaneous play situations or during meals at home, totalling 8 minutes and 6 seconds.

  • - Interactions between Tata, female infant between 11 and 12 months and her godmother. The audio recordings occurred during conversation situations, games and/or during meals at home, and totalled 12 minutes.

Table 1
Summary of the data by baby

The Prosodic Variables

For this research we studied the prosodic variables that were the most salient, which were the ones that stood out the most in our IDS data and are repeatedly mentioned in the literature. They were pitch register and pitch span, speech rate and final syllable duration, and quality of voice.5 5 The prosodic feature perceived loudness, sometimes used as synonymous to intensity, did not appear to be a distinguishing factor in IDS, given the kinds of interaction between the caregivers and the babies, usually at very close proximity; the voices were usually softer, quieter, giving an endearing, relaxed feel to the interaction. A low loudness level of modal voice shifts the affective ratings toward the low activation, intimate end (Grichkovtsova et al., 2012), but loudness contributes relatively little to the perceived affective colouring of specific voice qualities in our IDS data. These prosodic features used in classifying the questions follow:

Extent of pitch movement or pitch range

Also called ‘pitch span’ (Gussenhoven, 2004Gussenhoven, C. (2004). The phonology of tone and intonation. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511616983.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
; Ladd, 2008Ladd, D. R. (2008). Intonational phonology (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511808814.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
) or ‘excursion size’ (Hart et al., 1990Hart, J., Collier, R., & Cohen, A. (1990). A perceptual study of intonation: An experimental-phonetic approach to speech melody. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511627743.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
), defined as the difference between high and low targets in the speaker’s range. Questions were classified as either normal range or wide pitch span. For this, the quantitative distance between valleys and peaks was measured in semitones and octaves comparing the minimum and the maximum value. Differences of an octave or more, either up or down, were considered as belonging to the wide pitch span category for this feature. We chose the octave as the threshold to distinguish between normal range or wide pitch span as it is double the reported range of human voices, which normally spans between half an octave below the median of the speakers’ fundamental frequency and half an octave above (De Looze & Hirst, 2014De Looze, C., & Hirst, D. (2014). The OMe (octave median) scale: A natural scale for speech prosody. In N. Campbell, D. Gibbon, & D. Hirst (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Speech Prosody (pp. 910-914). https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_v0/SpeechProsody_2014/pdfs/175.pdf.
https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_v0/S...
). As these authors assert, intervals of octaves and half-octaves play crucial roles in speech production as well as perception. Increased pitch movement, particularly in the upper part of the speaker’s register, is a common feature of expressive speech.

Pitch register

We divided the questions into two groups, depending on whether the average pitch of the whole utterance was produced in a normal or a high pitch range, i.e., whether the lower and upper limits of pitch movement (in Hz) were within the expected range for adults, or if on the other hand the whole utterance was produced in a raised fashion. Although rare, the raised mean pitch of the utterance sometimes was coupled with a raised tonal baseline (Rietveld & Vermillion, 2003Rietveld, T., & Vermillion, P. (2003). Cues for perceived pitch register. Phonetica, 60(4), 261-272. https://doi.org/10.1159/000076376.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1159/...
), but by and large a high pitch range was the effect of the expanded upper limits and in some cases very high local accentual peaks. The majority of those peaks occurred at the beginning of wh- questions, mostly on the wh- word, and on the final syllables on yes-no questions.

Previous research (Faria et al., 2012Faria, B. S., Oliveira, K. V., Godoy e Silva, J. P., Reis, C., Ghio, A., & Gama, A. C. C. (2012). Electroglottography of speakers of Brazilian Portuguese through objective multiparameter vocal assessment (EVA). Brazilian Journal of Otorhinolaryngology, 78(4), 29-34. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1808-86942012000400007.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
) found that mean value for normal adult male voices in BP was a F0 of 127.77 Hz; and the value for adult female voices was a F0 of 204.87 Hz. Other studies that analysed European Portuguese and Brazilian Portuguese (Guimarães & Abberton, 2005; Silva, 1999Silva, V. (1999). Análise eletroglotográfica de diferentes tipos de vozes [Trabalho de conclusão de curso]. Centro de Estudos da Voz.) reported practically identical values in mean F0 and standard deviation values.

Based on these studies, we considered a question to be high according to the following criteria: i) the mean pitch is above 300 Hz for women and 180 Hz for men; ii) the top pitch reaches extremely high values, and/or the bottom is raised. The items that fit those criteria were classified as high in our analysis. During the data analysis stage, the utterances were first identified auditorily as high and then checked for confirmation in their quantitative values in Hertz for their mean and top frequencies and in semitones for their tonal span. We used Praat (Boersma & Weenink, 2019Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2019). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.1.08) [Computer software]. http://www.praat.org.
http://www.praat.org...
) for these analyses, adapting the pitch settings to the actual pitch range of the speaker. Following Liberman (2018Liberman, M. (2018, November 26). A better way to calculate pitch range. Language Log. https://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=40788.
https://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p...
), we estimated each speaker’s range as running from their modal F0 value minus seven semitones to their modal voice value plus ten semitones, although for some speakers with extremely high-pitched voices we manually adjusted the settings.

Speech rate and increased final vowel duration

We measured whether the questions were produced with a slower speech rate as opposed to what is considered normal speed in BP. This was done manually by dividing the number of syllables in the utterance with its duration to get the syllable rate in syllables/second and comparing that with what was considered to be the standard BP adult speech rate, established by Castro et al. (2014Castro, B. S. A., Martins-Reis, V. O., Baptista, A. C., & Celeste, L. C. (2014). Fluency profile: Comparison between Brazilian and European Portuguese speakers. CoDAS, 26(6), 444-446. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20142014184.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
) to be a median of 214.3 syllables/minute, and by Martins and Andrade (2008Martins, V. O., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2008). Perfil evolutivo da fluência da fala de falantes do português brasileiro. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica, 20(1), 7-12. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872008000100002.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
) to be a mean of 210.7 syllables/minute.

We observed that most of the questions that were initially auditorily labelled as long were made distinctive by a substantial increase in the duration of vowels, in particular the final stressed ones, some sort of final lengthening. When these vowels were more than three times longer than in non-phrase-final syllables, the question was counted as long, and many were even longer than that. Questions with such increased durations, either of the whole utterance or more commonly of the final stressed syllable, or both, give the impression of deliberate delivery, careful enunciation, compatible with the often-mentioned exaggerated articulation feature of IDS. This extra final lengthening was more evident in IDS yes-no questions than in wh- questions (Geffen & Mintz, 2017Geffen, S., & Mintz, T. (2017). Prosodic differences between declaratives and interrogatives in infant-directed speech. Journal of Child Language , 44(4), 968-994. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000349.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
).

Both duration of utterance and longer final stressed syllable were previously observed in BP IDS. Splendore et al. (2019Splendore, K. M., Constantini, A. C., & Da Silva, K. C. (2019). Investigação da prosódia e da linguagem na interação mãe-bebê. Working Papers em Linguística, 20(1), 172-188. https://doi.org/10.5007/1984-8420.2019v20n1p172.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.5007/...
) analysed ten mother-baby dyads between 3 and 8 months. Nine of the ten mothers made use of IDS; wide pitch variation, high-pitched voice, and slow speech rate were the most observed acoustic characteristics. Silva and Name (2014Silva, I. O., & Name, C. (2014). A sensibilidade de bebês brasileiros a pistas prosódicas de fronteiras de sintagma entoacional na fala dirigida à criança. Letrônica, 7(1), 4-25. https://doi.org/10.15448/1984-4301.2014.1.16855.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.15448...
) found, in the interactions of a mother with her 11- to 13-month-old baby, high register, slow rhythm with many elongations of stressed syllables, and recurrent and prolonged pauses.

Voice quality

We distinguished whether a question or part of a question was perceptually breathy or not. Breathy voice has been associated with intimacy and affection (Laver, 1980Laver, J. (1980). The phonetic description of voice quality. Cambridge University Press.), involving minimal laryngeal tension resulting in audible frication noise. Breathiness is related to perceptions of emotional states such as tenderness as well as attractiveness (Campbell & Mokhtari, 2003Campbell, N. & Mokhtari, P. (2003). Voice quality: The 4th prosodic dimension. In M. J. Solé, D. Recasens, & J. Romero (Eds.), Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 2417-2420). https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2003/papers/p15_2417.pdf.
https://www.internationalphoneticassocia...
; Grichkovtsova et al. 2012Grichkovtsova, I., Morel, M., & Lacheret, A. (2012). The role of voice quality and prosodic contour in affective speech perception. Speech Communication , 54(3), 414-429. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2011.10.005.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
).6 6 Utterances totally or partially uttered with breathiness, as perceived by the coders, qualified as breathy in our analysis. We did not deem it necessary to acoustically quantify which percentage of laryngeal frication was present in the questions with this feature.

4. Results and Discussion

Excluding chants, non-verbal vocalizations, and conversations between adults, 1672 utterances were counted. Tag questions were also excluded, due to the very low number of occurrences (less than 0.5% of the total number of questions). We found 554 interrogatives (33.13% of the total), thus distributed among the corpora in Table 2. As we pointed out, Guto’s data were collected in two distinct periods with an interval of 6 months between them, at 4 and 10 months of age, and therefore they are displayed separately.

Table 2
Total and percentage of questions and utterances

These results are consistent with the findings of Soderstrom et al. (2008Soderstrom, M., Blossom, M., Foygel, R., & Morgan, J. (2008). Acoustic cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 35(4), 869-902. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008763.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
) and Name and Sosa (2020bName, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020b). Cadê o amor da mamãe? As interrogativas na fala dirigida à criança adquirindo o PB. Veredas - Revista de Estudos Linguísticos, 24(1), 72-93. https://doi.org/10.34019/1982-2243.2020.v24.30916.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.34019...
). The mean rate of interrogatives (33.13%) is in line with that reported in the literature, which is around 30% (Snow, 1977Snow, E. C. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: from input to interaction. In C. Snow, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 31-49). Cambridge University Press.). We observed wide variation in the percentage of questions addressed to the different infants, but only two had a rate far below 30%: Juju (9 months), who had a very small number of questions (4 out of 62 utterances, 6.4%), and Nino (9.5 months, 18.4%). On the other hand, interrogatives corresponded to 72.7% of all utterances addressed to Guto at 4 months, a rate well above the average. Excluding these three outliers, the mean rate of interrogatives in the corpora is 35.4%.

Increased pitch distribution

Regarding the pitch variables, we had analysed separately the variables extent of pitch excursion and pitch register (see section 3). However, for the tabulation of the results we ultimately binned these two variables into one category since they tended to be concomitant to a high degree. In most cases pitch movements spanning an octave or more implied high peaks therefore higher average pitches of utterances. They were thus classified as marked for increased pitch in our nomenclature.

The discourse of adult males to infant males did not have much increased pitch height and/or width (high and wide). In contrast, adult males to infant females had much more pitch variation, both in mean height of questions and in pitch span, in some cases reaching up to two octaves. Adult females addressing infant males showed even more pitch variation and height. However, adult females addressing infant females showed less pitch variation and span. We checked whether adult females addressing infant females would have more variation and even higher proportion of high and wide pitches but found this wasn’t the case. Why did this not happen? We think it was mainly because Gigi’s mother and father - that had the greatest number of questions in our data - used more other IDS strategies: both employed breathy voice abundantly, indeed they were virtually the only caregivers that had this feature, and almost all their questions were of increased duration. However, their use of increased pitch was not. It has been proposed in the literature that increased flow and breathiness lead to lower F0 as pitch parameters interact with voice quality. For example, wider pitch excursions are associated with more tenseness and vowel prolongation (Grichkovtsova et al., 2012Grichkovtsova, I., Morel, M., & Lacheret, A. (2012). The role of voice quality and prosodic contour in affective speech perception. Speech Communication , 54(3), 414-429. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2011.10.005.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
). Klatt and Klatt (1990Klatt, D. H., & Klatt, L.C. (1990). Analysis, synthesis, and perception of voice quality variations among female and male talkers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 87(2), 820-857. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.398894.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1121/...
) found that fundamental frequency tends to be lower in breathy voice and argued that breathy voice can result in lowered F0 as low F0 coincides with breathiness.

In Tata’s case, less use of increased pitch features could be because the girl is older, as it was noted. We think for these reasons increased pitch was less used, probably affecting the proportion of use of this variable in the speech of women to female infants.

Of the total 554 questions, 318 were marked for increased pitch (57.4%). Adult males produced 100 questions in total, of which 32 were in interactions with baby girls. Of these, 53.1% (17) were marked for increased pitch. Differently, of the 68 questions addressed to baby boys, only 19.1% (13) were marked for pitch increase. As for the questions produced by women, the picture is more balanced: there were 224 questions addressed to baby girls, 55.4% (124) pitch marked, and 230 to baby boys, 67.8% (156) pitch marked. The general picture of male and female adult production addressed to baby boys and girls is shown in Figures 3 (total values) and 4 (percent values). Figures 5 to 8 illustrate the intonation of questions marked for increased pitch produced by mothers and fathers to their baby boy or girl.

Figure 3
Number of questions by gender/sex of adult and infant, marked/not marked by pitch - Total values

Figure 4
Number of questions by gender/sex of adult and infant, marked/not marked by pitch - Percent values

Figure 5
An extra-high rising yes/no question by Lala’s mother, “Não filha, você não é não?” (No daughter, you are not?). The final rise goes up to 880 Hz an upward movement of 22 semitones (st), close to two octaves

Figure 6
A yes/no question by Cadu’s father, “Cê quer fazer bagunça?” (You wanna make a mess?) in a low register. The movement of the interrogative rise/fall goes up only 3.5 st, then down 5 st

Figure 7
High wh- question by Gigi’s father, “Que confusão que é essa?” (What is this confusion?) with a wide movement of 14 st. In this case the high initial pitch is on the first syllable of the focused word confusão

Figure 8
Extra-high wh-question by Guto’s mother, “Cadê bebêeee?” (Where’s baby?) with a very long final vowel, the question is 1.1 sec and the last vowel is 0.51 sec, more than half the duration of the whole utterance. The pitch is very high, beginning at 550 Hz and falling to 215 Hz, a pitch movement of 16 st

The pitch differences observed between men and women, between girl and boy babies and in an older baby may be related to family characteristics (Gigi’s case), baby’s age (Tata’s case) and/or cultural issues. As seen, prosodic similarities between maternal and paternal speech were reported (Fernald et al., 1989Fernald, A., Taescher, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., De Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 16(3), p. 477-501. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010679.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/...
; Niwano & Sugai, 2003Niwano, K., & Sugai, K. (2003). Pitch characteristics of speech during mother-infant and father-infant vocal interactions. The Japanese Journal of Special Education, 40(6), 663-674. https://doi.org/10.6033/tokkyou.40.663.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.6033/...
; Weirich & Simpson, 2019Weirich, M., & Simpson, A. (2019). Effects of gender, parental role, and time on infant- and adult-directed read and spontaneous speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(11), 4001-4014. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-19-0047.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1044/...
), but so were differences, as observed in the speech directed at baby boys or girls (Benders et al., 2021Benders, T., StGeorge, J., & Fletcher, R. (2021). Infant-directed speech by Dutch fathers: Increased pitch variability within and across utterances. Language Learning and Development, 17(3), 292-325. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/...
, Kitamura et al., 2001Kitamura, C., Thanavishuth, C., Burnham, D., & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2001). Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behavior and Development , 24(4), 372-392. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00086-3.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
). Further studies specifically focusing on parental IDS and/or infant sex are needed to understand to what extent these variables may impact IDS prosody.

Increased duration

Slower speech rate and longer duration of accented final vowels, which we also conflated into one analytical category, increased duration (long), was used systematically by some adults, but not by all of them; for instance, Don’s uncle did not have any long question. It is noteworthy that in Guto’s case, when he was younger at 4 months, 14 out of 16 questions were long, but at 10 months only 2 out of 34. In terms of speech rhythm, we found that questions with increased duration of vowels as those we encountered in BP IDS give an overall auditory impression of syllable-timed rhythm. Syllables in IDS tend to sound distinctively pronounced giving an impression of syllabic isochrony; it is known that syllabic timing and durations tend to vary at distinct speech rate (Barbosa, 2000Barbosa, P. A. (2000). “Syllable-timing in Brazilian Portuguese”: Uma crítica a Roy Major. DELTA, 16(2), 369-402. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-44502000000200006.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/...
). As Meireles et al. (2010Meireles, A. R., Tozetti, J. P., & Borges, R. R. (2010). Speech rate and rhythmic variation in Brazilian Portuguese. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2010. Paper 875. https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_v0/sp2010/papers/sp10_875.pdf.
https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_v0/s...
) have noted, a slower tempo may be associated with syllable-timing, as speech rate influences tendencies to syllabic or to stress-timed rhythm (Meireles & Barbosa, 2008Meireles, A. R., & Barbosa, P. A. (2008). Speech rate effects on speech rhythm. In Proceedings of the Speech Prosody 2008 (pp. 327-330). https://www.isca-peech.org/archive/pdfs/speechprosody_2008/meireles08_speechprosody.pdf.
https://www.isca-peech.org/archive/pdfs/...
).

Breathy Voice

Given the importance attributed to this feature in the literature describing the so-called affective speech, associating it with intimacy (Gobl & Ni Chasaide, 2003Gobl, C., & Chasaide, A. N. (2003). The role of voice quality in communicating emotion, mood and attitude. Speech Communication, 40(1-2), 189-212. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00082-1.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
) and tenderness (Guzman et al., 2013Guzman, M., Correa, S., Muñoz, D., & Mayerhoff, R. (2013). Influence on spectral energy distribution of emotional expression. Journal of Voice , 27(1), Article 129.e1-129.e10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2012.08.008.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
), we were expecting the variable breathy to be more of a ubiquitous feature in BP IDS than what it actually turned out to be in our data. Altogether only 11.4% of all questions were breathy (63 of 554). Of these 63 items, 40 were uttered by the parents of a single baby, Gigi; another 13 were produced by another caregiver, Bibo’s; Tatá’s had seven, Cadu’s two, Nino’s one. The rest of the caregivers did not employ it at all. We interpreted this breathiness as being an idiosyncratic feature of these parents, therefore concluding that it does not qualify as a general feature of BP IDS.

5. Conclusion

In this paper we present the results of our corpora analysis of interactions between ten infants and their families, in the IDS register. We endeavored to verify the rate of occurrence of questions, their main features and if the intonational patterns of wh- and yes/no questions are similar to their respective patterns in ADS. We also aimed at verifying if there were major prosodic differences between women and men’s use of IDS in our data. We observed that the rate of occurrence of questions is in line with what is reported in the literature, around 30%. Also, the intonational pattern of wh- and yes/no questions agreed with what is found in ADS, respectively, falling and rising-falling.

Our main result was that the most instrumental features in characterizing BP IDS are related to pitch range and pitch span, and to a lesser degree to tempo and duration. We conclude that although these prosodic features are prevalent, there are no homogeneous or uniform patterns of IDS usage for all speakers. When present, IDS is manifested differently for different subjects, idiosyncratically. We noticed some differences related to gender/sex of the adults and the infants. Some caregivers, especially male, did not use the feature long at all, while others did not substantially raise their pitch range, and breathy voice was only used by some parents. This also depends on the age of the baby: as he/she grows, there seems to be a shift in the use of some variables. However, regardless of which distinct combinations of prosodic patterns were used in BP IDS, the great majority of questions were characterized by one or more of the features we have described.

As far as we know, this is the largest IDS study in Brazilian Portuguese. This register seems to play an important role in the infant’s affective, social, and linguistic development, and prosodically enhanced questions can promote communicative skills and be used as a cue for syntax acquisition.

The contribution of each family to the total data is quite varied. For example, the questions addressed to Gigi correspond to 27% of the corpora, whereas in Lala’s case they were only 3%. We are thus aware of the limitations that the results of our analysis present for possible generalizations. Our main goal was to describe and characterize the prosodic properties more frequent in questions in BP IDS from a large volume of data, since the few published works present little data from a reduced number of infants. Therefore, we expected diversity. Our corpora were composed of naturalistic data from 10 infants aged 4 to 12 months interacting with various caregivers. More than 500 questions were produced by mothers, fathers, grandparents and relatives in various everyday situations. More research is currently underway in order to further contribute to a more precise characterization of IDS and particularly of interrogatives.

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank our students Júlia Fonseca Camilo and Fernando Andrade Guimarães for their help gathering and classifying the data, and Thales Buzan for his help with the figures. We are grateful to the parents and caregivers of the babies, who supplied us with the data, and to three anonymous reviewers for their comments and contributions. This research was supported by the CNPq.

References

  • Adriaans, F., & Swingley, D. (2017). Prosodic exaggeration within infant-directed speech: Consequences for vowel learnability. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 141(5), 3070-3078. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4982246.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4982246
  • Barbosa, P. A. (2000). “Syllable-timing in Brazilian Portuguese”: Uma crítica a Roy Major. DELTA, 16(2), 369-402. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-44502000000200006.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-44502000000200006
  • Benders, T., StGeorge, J., & Fletcher, R. (2021). Infant-directed speech by Dutch fathers: Increased pitch variability within and across utterances. Language Learning and Development, 17(3), 292-325. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698
  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2019). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.1.08) [Computer software]. http://www.praat.org
    » http://www.praat.org
  • Braga, G., Frota, S., & Fernandes-Svartman, F. (2022). Guinea-Bissau Portuguese: What the intonation of yes-no question shows about this variety. Special issue Prosody and interfaces (Ed. by C. Serra, F. Fernandes-Svartman, and M. Cruz). DELTA, 38(3), Article 202258942, 1-27. https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678-460X202258942.
    » https://doi.org/https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678-460X202258942
  • Campbell, N. & Mokhtari, P. (2003). Voice quality: The 4th prosodic dimension. In M. J. Solé, D. Recasens, & J. Romero (Eds.), Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 2417-2420). https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2003/papers/p15_2417.pdf
    » https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2003/papers/p15_2417.pdf
  • Castro, B. S. A., Martins-Reis, V. O., Baptista, A. C., & Celeste, L. C. (2014). Fluency profile: Comparison between Brazilian and European Portuguese speakers. CoDAS, 26(6), 444-446. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20142014184.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20142014184
  • Cavalcante, M. C. B. & Barros, A. T. M. C. (2012). Manhês: Qualidade vocal e deslocamentos na dialogia mãe-bebê. Veredas - Revista de Estudos Linguísticos, 16(spe), 25-39. https://periodicos.ufjf.br/index.php/veredas/article/view/25054
    » https://periodicos.ufjf.br/index.php/veredas/article/view/25054
  • Chiang, C., Geffen, S., & Mintz, T. H. (2018). Distinguishing questions and statements using sentence-initial prosodic cues. In A. B. Bertolini & M. J. Kaplan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 42nd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 153-158). http://www.lingref.com/bucld/42/BUCLD42-12.pdf
    » http://www.lingref.com/bucld/42/BUCLD42-12.pdf
  • Cinelli, E. (2021, September 17). How prosody plays a role in language development. Verywell Family. https://www.verywellfamily.com/prosody-and-language-acquisition-5191264
    » https://www.verywellfamily.com/prosody-and-language-acquisition-5191264
  • Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61(5), 1584-1595. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130766.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.2307/1130766
  • Cristia, A. (2013). Input to language: The phonetics and perception of infant-directed speech. Language and Linguistics Compass, 7(3), 157-170. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12015.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12015
  • Cristia, A., Dupoux, E., Gurven, M., & Stieglitz, J. (2019). Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager-farmer population: A time allocation study. Child Development , 90(3), 759-773. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12974.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12974
  • Dadalto, E. V., & Goldfeld, M. (2006). Características do maternalês em duas crianças de idades distintas. Distúrbios da Comunicação, 18(2), 201-208. https://revistas.pucsp.br/index.php/dic/article/view/11786/8520
    » https://revistas.pucsp.br/index.php/dic/article/view/11786/8520
  • De Looze, C., & Hirst, D. (2014). The OMe (octave median) scale: A natural scale for speech prosody. In N. Campbell, D. Gibbon, & D. Hirst (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Speech Prosody (pp. 910-914). https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_v0/SpeechProsody_2014/pdfs/175.pdf
    » https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_v0/SpeechProsody_2014/pdfs/175.pdf
  • De Pablo, I, Murillo, E., & Romero, A. (2020). The effect of infant-directed speech on early multimodal communicative production in Spanish and Basque. Journal of Child Language, 47(2), 457-471. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000412.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000412
  • Faria, B. S., Oliveira, K. V., Godoy e Silva, J. P., Reis, C., Ghio, A., & Gama, A. C. C. (2012). Electroglottography of speakers of Brazilian Portuguese through objective multiparameter vocal assessment (EVA). Brazilian Journal of Otorhinolaryngology, 78(4), 29-34. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1808-86942012000400007.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/S1808-86942012000400007
  • Fernald, A. (1992). Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 391-428). Oxford University Press.
  • Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 10(3), 279-293. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(87)90017-8.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(87)90017-8
  • Fernald, A., & O’Neill, D. K. (1993). Peekaboo across cultures: How mothers and infants play with voices, faces, and expectations. In K. MacDonald (Ed.), Parent-child play: Descriptions and implications ( pp. 259-285) State University of New York Press.
  • Fernald, A., & Simon, T. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns. Developmental Psychology, 20(1), 104-113. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.1.104.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.1.104
  • Fernald, A., Taescher, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., De Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I. (1989). A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 16(3), p. 477-501. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010679.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010679
  • Floccia, C., Keren-Portnoy, T., DePaolis, R., Duffy, H., Delle Luche, C., Durrant, S., White, L., Goslin, J., & Vihman, M. (2016). British English infants segment words only with exaggerated infant-directed speech stimuli. Cognition, 148, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.004.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.12.004
  • Frota, S., Butler, J., & Vigário, M. (2014). Infant’s perception of intonation: Is it a statement or a question? Infancy, 19(2), 194-213. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12037.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12037
  • Garnica, O. K. (1977) Some prosodic and paralinguistics features of speech to young children. In C. E. Snow & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 63-88). Cambridge University Press.
  • Garnica, O. K. (1974). Some prosodic characteristics of speech to young children. [ Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Stanford University.
  • Geffen, S., & Mintz, T. (2017). Prosodic differences between declaratives and interrogatives in infant-directed speech. Journal of Child Language , 44(4), 968-994. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000349.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000349
  • Gobl, C., & Chasaide, A. N. (2003). The role of voice quality in communicating emotion, mood and attitude. Speech Communication, 40(1-2), 189-212. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00082-1.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00082-1
  • Gout, A., Christophe, A., & Morgan, J. L. (2004). Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access: II Infant data. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(4), 548-567. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.07.002.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.07.002
  • Graf Estes, K., & Hurley, K. (2013). Infant-directed prosody helps infants map sounds to meanings. Infancy , 18(5), 797-824. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12006.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12006
  • Grichkovtsova, I., Morel, M., & Lacheret, A. (2012). The role of voice quality and prosodic contour in affective speech perception. Speech Communication , 54(3), 414-429. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2011.10.005.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2011.10.005
  • Grieser, D. L., & Kuhl, P. K. (1988). Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. Developmental Psychology , 24(1), 14-20. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.1.14.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.1.14
  • Guimarães, I., & Abberton E. (2005). Fundamental frequency in speakers of Portuguese for different voice samples. Journal of Voice, 19(4), 592-606. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2004.11.004.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2004.11.004
  • Gussenhoven, C. (2004). The phonology of tone and intonation. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511616983.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511616983
  • Guzman, M., Correa, S., Muñoz, D., & Mayerhoff, R. (2013). Influence on spectral energy distribution of emotional expression. Journal of Voice , 27(1), Article 129.e1-129.e10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2012.08.008.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2012.08.008
  • Hart, J., Collier, R., & Cohen, A. (1990). A perceptual study of intonation: An experimental-phonetic approach to speech melody. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511627743.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511627743
  • Hedberg, N., Sosa, J. M., Görgülü E., & Mameni, M. (2010). Prosody and pragmatics of wh-interrogatives. In M. Heijl (Ed.), Proceedings of the 2010 Annual Conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association. http://homes.chass.utoronto.ca/~cla-acl/actes2010/CLA2010_Hedberg_et_al.pdf
    » http://homes.chass.utoronto.ca/~cla-acl/actes2010/CLA2010_Hedberg_et_al.pdf
  • Kitamura, C., Thanavishuth, C., Burnham, D., & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2001). Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language. Infant Behavior and Development , 24(4), 372-392. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00086-3.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00086-3
  • Klatt, D. H., & Klatt, L.C. (1990). Analysis, synthesis, and perception of voice quality variations among female and male talkers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 87(2), 820-857. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.398894.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1121/1.398894
  • Ladd, D. R. (2008). Intonational phonology (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511808814.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511808814
  • Laver, J. (1980). The phonetic description of voice quality. Cambridge University Press.
  • Liberman, M. (2018, November 26). A better way to calculate pitch range. Language Log. https://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=40788
    » https://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=40788
  • Martins, V. O., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2008). Perfil evolutivo da fluência da fala de falantes do português brasileiro. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica, 20(1), 7-12. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872008000100002.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872008000100002
  • Matsuoka, A., & Name, C. (2011). O uso de pistas prosódicas na identificação do adjetivo por crianças e adultos falantes do PB. In Anais do VII Congresso Internacional da ABRALIN (pp. 577-587).
  • Meireles, A. R., & Barbosa, P. A. (2008). Speech rate effects on speech rhythm. In Proceedings of the Speech Prosody 2008 (pp. 327-330). https://www.isca-peech.org/archive/pdfs/speechprosody_2008/meireles08_speechprosody.pdf.
    » https://www.isca-peech.org/archive/pdfs/speechprosody_2008/meireles08_speechprosody.pdf.
  • Meireles, A. R., Tozetti, J. P., & Borges, R. R. (2010). Speech rate and rhythmic variation in Brazilian Portuguese. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2010. Paper 875. https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_v0/sp2010/papers/sp10_875.pdf
    » https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_v0/sp2010/papers/sp10_875.pdf
  • Miranda, L., Moraes, J., & Rilliard, A. (2022). Effects of F0 movements, intensity, and duration in the perceptual identification of Brazilian Portuguese wh-questions and wh-exclamations. Special issue Prosody and interfaces (Ed. By C. Serra , F. Fernandes-Svartman , and M. Cruz). DELTA , 38(3), Article 202258882, 1-29. https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678-460X202258882.
    » https://doi.org/https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1678-460X202258882
  • Moraes, J. A. (2008). The pitch accents in Brazilian Portuguese: Analysis by synthesis. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2008 (pp. 389-397). https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs/speechprosody_2008/moraes08_speechprosody.pdf
    » https://www.isca-speech.org/archive/pdfs/speechprosody_2008/moraes08_speechprosody.pdf
  • Name, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020a). The prosody of questions in Brazilian Portuguese infant-directed speech. In N. Minematsu, M. Kondo, T. Arai & R. Hayashi (Eds.), Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2020 (pp. 334-337). https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020-68
    » https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020-68
  • Name, C., & Sosa, J. M. (2020b). Cadê o amor da mamãe? As interrogativas na fala dirigida à criança adquirindo o PB. Veredas - Revista de Estudos Linguísticos, 24(1), 72-93. https://doi.org/10.34019/1982-2243.2020.v24.30916.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.34019/1982-2243.2020.v24.30916
  • Narayan, C. R., & McDermott, L. C. (2016). Speech rate and pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech: Longitudinal and cross-linguistic observations. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 139, 1272-1281. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4944634.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4944634
  • Newport, E., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I’d rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In C. Snow, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp.109-149). Cambridge University Press.
  • Niwano, K., & Sugai, K. (2003). Pitch characteristics of speech during mother-infant and father-infant vocal interactions. The Japanese Journal of Special Education, 40(6), 663-674. https://doi.org/10.6033/tokkyou.40.663.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.6033/tokkyou.40.663
  • Papoušek, M., Papoušek, H., & Haekel, M. (1987). Didactic adjustments in fathers’ and mothers’ speech to their 3-month-old infants. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 16, 491-516. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01073274.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01073274
  • Peter, V., Kalashnikova, M., Santos, A., & Burnham, D. (2016). Mature neural responses to infant-directed speech but not adult-directed speech in pre-verbal infants. Nature Scientfic Reports, 6, Article 34273. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep34273.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1038/srep34273
  • Räsänen, O., Kakouros, S., & Soderstrom, M. (2018). Is infant-directed speech interesting because it is surprising? Linking properties of IDS to statistical learning and attention at the prosodic level. Cognition, 178, 193-206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.015.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.05.015
  • Rietveld, T., & Vermillion, P. (2003). Cues for perceived pitch register. Phonetica, 60(4), 261-272. https://doi.org/10.1159/000076376.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1159/000076376
  • Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., Laznik, M.-C., & Cohen, D. (2013). Motherese in interaction: At the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PLoS One, 8(10), Article e78103. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078103.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078103
  • Silva, V. (1999). Análise eletroglotográfica de diferentes tipos de vozes [Trabalho de conclusão de curso]. Centro de Estudos da Voz.
  • Silva, I. O., & Name, C. (2014). A sensibilidade de bebês brasileiros a pistas prosódicas de fronteiras de sintagma entoacional na fala dirigida à criança. Letrônica, 7(1), 4-25. https://doi.org/10.15448/1984-4301.2014.1.16855.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.15448/1984-4301.2014.1.16855
  • Snow, E. C. (1977). Mothers’ speech research: from input to interaction. In C. Snow, & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 31-49). Cambridge University Press.
  • Snow, E. C. (1995). Issues in the study of input: Finetuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In P. Fletcher, & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 180-193). Blackwell.
  • Soderstrom, M. (2007). Beyond babytalk: Re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Development Review, 27(4), 501-532. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.06.002.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.06.002
  • Soderstrom, M., Blossom, M., Foygel, R., & Morgan, J. (2008). Acoustic cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants. Journal of Child Language , 35(4), 869-902. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008763.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008763
  • Soley, G., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2020). Infants’ expectations about the recipients of infant-directed and adult-directed speech. Cognition, 198, 104214, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104214.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104214
  • Sosa, J. M. (2003). Wh-questions in Spanish: Meanings and configuration variability. Catalan Journal of Linguistics, 2, 229-247. https://raco.cat/index.php/CatalanJournal/article/view/308981
    » https://raco.cat/index.php/CatalanJournal/article/view/308981
  • Splendore, K. M., Constantini, A. C., & Da Silva, K. C. (2019). Investigação da prosódia e da linguagem na interação mãe-bebê. Working Papers em Linguística, 20(1), 172-188. https://doi.org/10.5007/1984-8420.2019v20n1p172.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.5007/1984-8420.2019v20n1p172
  • Thiessen, E. D., Hill, E. A., & Saffran, J. R. (2005). Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy , 7(1), 53-71. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0701_5.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0701_5
  • Thorson, J., Borras-Comes, J., Crespo-Sendra, V., Vanrell, M., & Prieto, P. (2014). The acquisition of melodic form and meaning in yes-no interrogatives by Catalan and Spanish children. Probus, 27(1), 73-99. https://doi.org/10.1515/probus-2013-0019.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1515/probus-2013-0019
  • Wang, Y., Seidl, A., & Cristia, A. (2016). Acoustic characteristics of infant-directed speech as a function of prosodic typology. In J. Heinz, R. Goedemans, & H. Van der Hulst (Eds.), Dimensions of phonological stress (pp. 311-326). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316212745.012.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316212745.012
  • Weber, A., Fernald, A., & Diop, Y. (2017). When cultural norms discourage talking to babies: Effectiveness of a parenting program in Rural Senegal. Child Development , 88(5), 1513-1526. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12882.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12882
  • Weirich, M., & Simpson, A. (2019). Effects of gender, parental role, and time on infant- and adult-directed read and spontaneous speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62(11), 4001-4014. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-19-0047.
    » https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-19-0047
  • 5
    The prosodic feature perceived loudness, sometimes used as synonymous to intensity, did not appear to be a distinguishing factor in IDS, given the kinds of interaction between the caregivers and the babies, usually at very close proximity; the voices were usually softer, quieter, giving an endearing, relaxed feel to the interaction. A low loudness level of modal voice shifts the affective ratings toward the low activation, intimate end (Grichkovtsova et al., 2012Grichkovtsova, I., Morel, M., & Lacheret, A. (2012). The role of voice quality and prosodic contour in affective speech perception. Speech Communication , 54(3), 414-429. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2011.10.005.
    https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/...
    ), but loudness contributes relatively little to the perceived affective colouring of specific voice qualities in our IDS data.
  • 6
    Utterances totally or partially uttered with breathiness, as perceived by the coders, qualified as breathy in our analysis. We did not deem it necessary to acoustically quantify which percentage of laryngeal frication was present in the questions with this feature.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    28 Oct 2022
  • Date of issue
    2022

History

  • Received
    16 Dec 2021
  • Accepted
    02 Mar 2022
Pontifícia Universidade Católica de São Paulo - PUC-SP PUC-SP - LAEL, Rua Monte Alegre 984, 4B-02, São Paulo, SP 05014-001, Brasil, Tel.: +55 11 3670-8374 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: delta@pucsp.br