Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Word final prolongations: acoustic characteristics and influence on speech fluency perception

Prolongamentos no final de palavras: características acústicas e influência na percepção da fluência de fala

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to characterize word final prolongations in individuals with and without stuttering as well as to investigate the influence of end of words prolongations on speech fluency perception. In Experiment 1, 14 subjects were submitted to speech fluency evaluation for analysis of duration and average frequency of extended phones at the end of words. In Experiment 2, twenty lay judges were asked to judge the fluency degree of utterances without disfluency, utterances containing prolongations and utterances containing filled pauses. In experiment 1 the groups differed only in the duration’s variation; in both groups the prolongations in monosyllabic words prevailed; 80% of the prolonged phones in both groups were vowels. In experiment 2 no significant difference was found in the comparison between the judgements of prolongations and filled pauses. The utterances without disfluency differentiated themselves significantly from the others. We suggest that characteristics such as position in the word, duration and physical concomitants should be considered before deeming prolongations as a stuttering-like disfluency.

Keywords:
stuttering; speech acoustics; speech; language and hearing sciences

RESUMO

O presente estudo tem como objetivo caracterizar os prolongamentos em final de palavras em indivíduos com e sem gagueira do desenvolvimento persistente, bem como investigar a influência de prolongamentos no final de palavras na percepção da fluência de fala. No Experimento 1, 14 sujeitos foram submetidos à avaliação da fluência de fala para análise da duração e frequência média dos fones prolongados no final das palavras. No Experimento 2, 20 juízes leigos julgaram o grau de fluência de enunciados sem disfluência, contendo prolongamentos e contendo pausas preenchidas. No Experimento 1, os grupos diferiram apenas na variação da duração; nos dois grupos prevaleceu o prolongamento das palavras monossilábicas; 80% dos fones prolongados nos dois grupos eram vogais. No experimento 2, não foi encontrada diferença significativa na comparação entre os julgamentos de prolongamentos e pausas preenchidas. Os enunciados sem disfluências se diferenciaram significativamente dos demais. Sugere-se que características como posição na palavra, duração e concomitantes físicos devem ser consideradas antes de caracterizar o prolongamento como disfluência típica da gagueira.

Palavras-chave:
gagueira; acústica da fala; fala; fonoaudiologia

1. Introduction

Speech fluency can be characterized as a liveliness of the speech flow stream, recourse to speed, rhythm, smoothness and effort for speech production (ASHA, 1993). In continuity, the influence of decisions on the linguistic planning and formulation of speech processes (Scarpa & Fernandez-Svatsman, 2012Scarpa, E. M., & Fernandez-Svartsman, F. (2012). A estrutura prosódica das disfluências em português brasileiro. Cadernos de Estudos Linguísticos, 54(1), 25-40. https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v54i1.8636969
https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v54i1.86369...
). Speech fluency manifests in a different manner on each speaker and can depend on factors such as previous knowledge of the subject (Felsenfeld et al., 2000Felsenfeld, S., Kirk, K. M., Zhu, G., Statham, D. J., Neale, M. C., & Martin, N. G. (2000). A study of the genetic and environmental etiology of stuttering in a selected twin sample. Behavior Genetics, 30(5), 359-366. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1002765620208
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1002765620208...
). According to the Psychiatry Association (Coleman, 2013Coleman, C. (2013). How can you tell if childhood stuttering is the real deal? In: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Leader Live: happening now in the speech-language-hearing world.LeaderLive. https://leader.pubs.asha.org/do/10.1044/how-can-you-tell-if-childhood-stuttering-is-the-real-deal/full/. Acesso em: 14 de abril de 2020.
https://leader.pubs.asha.org/do/10.1044/...
), stuttering is characterized as a disorder related to a disturbance on motor speech production that has an impact on fluency, it may cause disfluencies such as syllable and sound repetition, blocks and sound prolongation and those may occur in vowels and consonants.

Disfluency is the interruption of the speech continuous flow. At first, the simple presence of disfluency is not a speech disorder indicator considering that speech rarely occurs without it. Speech produced by people who stutter (PWS) and people who do not stutter (PWDS) frequently contains elements such as pauses, words repetition and prolongations (Macgregor, Corley, & Donaldson, 2009MacGregor, L. J., Corley, M., & Donaldson, D. I. (2009). Not all disfluencies are are equal: The effects of disfluent repetitions on language comprehension. Brain and language, 111(1), 36-45. https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/9048741.pdf (Accessed 10 March, 2023)
https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/9048741....
). Disfluencies do not have the same syntactic function (Tree, 1995Tree, J. E. F. (1995). The effects of false starts and repetitions on the processing of subsequent words in spontaneous speech. Journal of Memory and Language , 34(6), 709-738. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1032
https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1032...
) and the functional and structural connectivity of the brain when disfluency occurs is not the same in people who stutter and in people who do not stutter (Sitek et al. 2016Sitek, K. R., Cai, S., Beal, D. S., Perkell, J. S., Guenther, F. H., & Ghosh, S. S. (2016). Decreased cerebellar-orbitofrontal connectivity correlates with stuttering severity: whole-brain functional and structural connectivity associations with persistent developmental stuttering. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 190. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00190
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00190...
).

There are two elements related to disfluency that seems to differentiate people who stutter from people who do not stutter: frequency and disfluency type. Yairi (1997Yairi, E. (1997). Disfluency characteristics of early childhood stuttering. In R. F. Curlee & G. M. Siegel (Eds.), Nature and treatment of stuttering: New directions (2nd ed., pp. 49-78). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.) presented the term “stuttering-like disfluency” or disfluencies typically observed in developmental stuttering. As to frequency, it is necessary to have at least 3% of “stuttering-like disfluencies” for a speaker to be consider as a PWS. Though stuttering-like disfluencies are important to stuttering diagnosis, they also occur in PWDS (Juste & Andrade, 2006Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2006). Typology of speech disruptions and grammatical classes in stuttering and fluent children. Pro-fono: revista de atualizacao cientifica, 18(2), 129-140. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0104-56872006000200002
https://doi.org/10.1590/s0104-5687200600...
).

Prolongation is a stuttering-like disfluency (Costa, Ritto, Juste & Andrade, 2017Costa, J. B., Ritto, A. P., Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. D. (2017). Comparison between the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter. In CoDAS, 29(2). https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20172016136
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/201720...
) and it can be characterized as a sound (consonant or vowel) spoken with a larger duration than normally expected (Johnson, 1961Johnson, W. (1961). Measurements of oral reading and speaking rate and disfluency of adult male and female stutterers and nonstutterers. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders. Monograph Supplement, 7, 1-20.), causing an inappropriate elocution duration, and it may be accompanied by pitch elevation (Yaruss, 1998Yaruss, J. S. (1998). Real-time analysis of speech fluency: Procedures and reliability training. American journal of speech-language pathology , 7(2), 25-37. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360.0702.25
https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360.0702.2...
). In the literature related to linguistics, the prolongation is called elongation or filled pauses (Betz & Kosmala, 2019Betz, S., & Kosmala, L. (2019). Fill the silence! Basics for modeling hesitation. Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 12-14. hal-02360611f. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00027-5
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00...
; Gold, Ross, & Earnshaw, 2018Gold, E., Ross, S., & Earnshaw, K. (2018). Regional variation in West Yorkshire filled pauses: Implications for forensic speaker comparisons. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 144(3), 1903-1903. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.5068336
https://doi.org/10.1121/1.5068336...
; Defracq & Plevoets, 2018 ) and it is defined as a conversational marker (Betz & Kosmala, 2019Betz, S., & Kosmala, L. (2019). Fill the silence! Basics for modeling hesitation. Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 12-14. hal-02360611f. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00027-5
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00...
; Bellinghausen, Fangmeier, Schröder, & Keller, 2019Bellinghausen, C., Fangmeier, T., Schröder, B., Keller, J., Drechsel, S., Birkholz, P., & Riedel, A. (2019). On the role of disfluent speech for uncertainty in articulatory speech synthesis. In The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 39 (pp. 39-42). https://www.ida.liu.se/~robek28/conferences/diss2019/DiSS2019_Proceedings.pdf#page=49
https://www.ida.liu.se/~robek28/conferen...
) until the utterance is complete (Betz & Kosmala 2019Betz, S., & Kosmala, L. (2019). Fill the silence! Basics for modeling hesitation. Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 12-14. hal-02360611f. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00027-5
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00...
; Gósy, 2019Gósy, M. (2019). Filler words in children’s and adults’ spontaneous speech. The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 89-90. https://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1353206/FULLTEXT01.pdf#page=99
https://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/di...
), showing the end-of-shift indication, attention and confirmation of the listener (Gósy, 2019Gósy, M. (2019). Filler words in children’s and adults’ spontaneous speech. The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 89-90. https://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1353206/FULLTEXT01.pdf#page=99
https://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/di...
), related or not to difficulties that the speaker has in planning and formulating what is said.

Some studies point to the presence of prolongation in the speech of the fluent individuals, regardless of age (Pinto, et al. 2013Pinto, J. C. B. R., Schiefer, A. M., & Ávila, C. R. B. (2013). Disfluencies and speech rate in spontaneous production and in oral reading in people who stutter and who do not stutter. Audiol Commun Res, 18(2), 63-70. http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.915.1934&rep=rep1&type=pdf
http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/dow...
; Rose & Watanabe, 2019Rose, R., & Watanabe, M. (2019). A crosslinguistic corpus study of silent and filled pauses: when do speakers use filled pauses to fill pauses? In: Proceedings of the 2019 ICPhS (pp. 2615-2619). https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2019/papers/ICPhS_2664.pdf (accessed 25 March, 2021).
https://www.internationalphoneticassocia...
; Andrade, & Martins, 2007Andrade, C. R. F., & Martins, V.O. (2007). Fluency variation in adolescents. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 21(10), 771-782. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200701502161
https://doi.org/10.1080/0269920070150216...
; Götz, 2019Götz, S. (2019). Filled pauses across proficiency levels, L1s and learning context variables: A multivariate exploration of the Trinity Lancaster Corpus Sample. International Journal of Learner Corpus Research, 5(2), 159-180. https://doi.org/10.1075/ijlcr.17018.got
https://doi.org/10.1075/ijlcr.17018.got...
; Andrade & Martins-Reis, 2011Andrade, C. R. F., & Martins, V.O. (2011). Influencia del sexo y el nivel educativo en la fluidez del habla en personas adultas. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, 31(2), 74-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0214-4603(11)70175-9
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0214-4603(11)70...
; Castro, Martins-Reis, Baptista, & Celeste, 2014Castro, B. S. A., Martins-Reis, V.O., Baptista, A. C., & Celeste, L. C. (2014). Fluency profile: comparison between Brazilian and European Portuguese speakers. CoDAS, 26(6), 444-446. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20142014184
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/201420...
; Silva, Fabron, Picoloto, & Oliveira, 2016Silva, P. B., Fabron, E. M. G., Picoloto, L. A., & Oliveira, C. M. C. (2016). Prolongations in the speech of adults who do and do not stutter. Revista CEFAC , 18(5), 1141-1150. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620161856116
https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-02162016185...
; Nogueira, Oliveira, Giacheti, & Moretti-Ferreira, 2015Nogueira, P. R., de Oliveira, C. M. C., Giacheti, C. M., & Moretti-Ferreira, D. (2015). Familial persistent developmental stuttering: disfluencies and prevalence. Revista CEFAC , 17(5), 1441-1448. https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/1693/169342586008.pdf
https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/1693/1693425...
; Natke, Sandrieser, Pietrosky, & Kalveram, 2006Natke, U., Sandrieser, P., Pietrowsky, R., & Kalveram, K. T. (2006). Disfluency data of German preschool children who stutter and comparison children. Journal of fluency disorders , 31(3), 165-176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.04.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.0...
), reinforcing the prolongation being directed as a discursive marker (Betz & Kosmala, 2019Betz, S., & Kosmala, L. (2019). Fill the silence! Basics for modeling hesitation. Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 12-14. hal-02360611f. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00027-5
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00...
; Bellinghausen et al., 2019Bellinghausen, C., Fangmeier, T., Schröder, B., Keller, J., Drechsel, S., Birkholz, P., & Riedel, A. (2019). On the role of disfluent speech for uncertainty in articulatory speech synthesis. In The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 39 (pp. 39-42). https://www.ida.liu.se/~robek28/conferences/diss2019/DiSS2019_Proceedings.pdf#page=49
https://www.ida.liu.se/~robek28/conferen...
; Gósy, 2019Gósy, M. (2019). Filler words in children’s and adults’ spontaneous speech. The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 89-90. https://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1353206/FULLTEXT01.pdf#page=99
https://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/di...
) that can be used as a strategic tool of the interlocutor in communication (Celeste & Reis, 2013Celeste, L. C., & Reis, C. (2013). Expressão de certeza e dúvida na gagueira: estudo dos aspectos temporais da fala. Revista CEFAC , 15(6), 1609-1620. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462012005000060
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462012...
). Despite these previous findings, prolongation is considered as a typical stuttering disorder in the speech-language literature (Costa et al. 2017Costa, J. B., Ritto, A. P., Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. D. (2017). Comparison between the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter. In CoDAS, 29(2). https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20172016136
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/201720...
; Natke et al. 2006Natke, U., Sandrieser, P., Pietrowsky, R., & Kalveram, K. T. (2006). Disfluency data of German preschool children who stutter and comparison children. Journal of fluency disorders , 31(3), 165-176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.04.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.0...
; Campbell & Hill, 1998Campbell, J., & Hill, D. (1998). Systematic disfluency analysis. In J. Campbell & D. Hill (Eds.), Stuttering therapy (pp. 51-75). Memphis: Northwestern University and Stuttering Foundation of America.; Souza, Paschoalino, Cardoso & Oliveira, 2013Souza, J. B., Paschoalino, F. C., Cardoso, V. M., & de Oliveira, C. M. C. (2013). Frequency and Typology Of Disfluencies: Comparative Analysis Between Clutterers And Stutterers. Revista CEFAC , 15(4), 857-863. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/6b49/d9fc75199413a78434862e51b4297709f5bd.pdf
https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/6b49/d9...
; Tumanova, Zebrowsi, Throneburg & Kayikci, 2011 Tumanova, V., Zebrowski, P. M., Throneburg, R. N., & Kayikci, M. E. K. (2011). Articulation rate and its relationship to disfluency type, duration, and temperament in preschool children who stutter. Journal of communication disorders , 44(1), 116-129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.09.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.0...
).

There is evidence that disfluencies affect the understanding of listeners (Macgregor et al. 2009MacGregor, L. J., Corley, M., & Donaldson, D. I. (2009). Not all disfluencies are are equal: The effects of disfluent repetitions on language comprehension. Brain and language, 111(1), 36-45. https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/9048741.pdf (Accessed 10 March, 2023)
https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/9048741....
; Dejoy & Jordan, 1988DeJoy, D. A., & Jordan, W. J. (1988). Listener reactions to interjections in oral reading versus spontaneous speech. Journal of fluency disorders , 13(1), 11-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(88)90025-3
https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(88)900...
; Tree, 2001Tree, J. E. F. (2001). Listeners’ uses of um and uh in speech comprehension. Memory & cognition, 29(2), 320-326. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194926
https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194926...
; Corley, Macgregor & Donaldson, 2007Corley, M., MacGregor, L. J., & Donaldson, D. I. (2007). It’s the way that you, er, say it: Hesitations in speech affect language comprehension. Cognition, 105(3), 658-668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.010
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006...
; Macgregor, Corley & Donaldson, 2010MacGregor, L. J., Corley, M., & Donaldson, D. I. (2010). Listening to the sound of silence: Disfluent silent pauses in speech have consequences for listeners. Neuropsychologia, 48(14), 3982-3992. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.024
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsycholog...
). Among the disfluencies reported in the studies, filled pauses (along with silent pauses and prolongations) marked hesitations on of the speakers’ part. Like facial expressions and tone of voice, hesitant disfluencies provide information to the listener. This information can refer to problems in the production by the speaker or to the next content of the message itself (Corley & Stewart, 2008Corley, M., & Stewart, O. W. (2008). Hesitation disfluencies in spontaneous speech: The meaning of um. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2(4), 589-602. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00068.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008...
).

Prognostic tools for stuttering use, among other criteria, the presence of prolongations and blocks, as well as prolongations longer than one second, as risk factors for stuttering chronicity (Tumanova et al. 2011 Tumanova, V., Zebrowski, P. M., Throneburg, R. N., & Kayikci, M. E. K. (2011). Articulation rate and its relationship to disfluency type, duration, and temperament in preschool children who stutter. Journal of communication disorders , 44(1), 116-129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.09.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.0...
). Boey, Wuyts, Van de Heyning, De Bodt and Heylen (2007Boey, R. A., Wuyts, F. L., Van de Heyning, P. H., De Bodt, M. S., & Heylen, L. (2007). Characteristics of stuttering-like disfluencies in Dutch-speaking children. Journal of fluency disorders, 32(4), 310-329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.07.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.0...
) found that children with stuttering were significantly more likely to have prolongations and blocks. Juste and Andrade (2010Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. D. (2010). Influência da tonicidade e local da ruptura na palavra em adolescentes e adultos gagos e fluentes. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica , 22(3), 175-182. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010000300004
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-5687201000...
) investigated the occurrence of prolongations and their location within the word in individuals with and without stuttering, in which it was observed that individuals who stutter had higher occurrences within the words and the fluent ones in the phoneme of the final syllable of the words.

In a study on the influence of gender and level of schooling on fluent adults’ speech fluency (Andrade & Martins-Reis, 2011Andrade, C. R. F., & Martins, V.O. (2011). Influencia del sexo y el nivel educativo en la fluidez del habla en personas adultas. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, 31(2), 74-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0214-4603(11)70175-9
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0214-4603(11)70...
), there were more occurrences of sound prolongation in subjects of higher education. As of common disfluencies, interjections and revisions were found in adults schooled up to primary education. The authors concluded that this seems to be a flexible strategy of the linguistic processing component in replacing a filled pause by prolongations in higher education individuals. For the authors, the form that individuals with higher education use to resume speech and language processing is the prolongation, which only appeared at the end of the words and can be considered, in terms of the analysis of the conversation, as a marker of hesitation. They also consider that it can be an indication of syntactic or semantic-lexical errors as a strategy in the production of speech (Postma & Kolk, 1993Postma, A., & Kolk, H. (1993). The covert repair hypothesis: Prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered disfluencies. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36(3), 472-487. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3603.472
https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3603.472...
).

Roberts, Melter and Wilding (2009Roberts, P. M., Meltzer, A., & Wilding, J. (2009). Disfluencies in non-stuttering adults across sample lengths and topics. Journal of communication disorders, 42(6), 414-427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.06.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.0...
) evaluated the effect of speech sample lengthening and correlated levels of fluency in monologues of fluent adult males, in which prolongation was present in the speech of half the subjects participating in the research and many of them occurred at the end or at the beginning of the word, thus emphasizing prolongation as an exercised ability to emphasize something. The same authors related the site of rupture of the prolongation with the places in which an interjection would normally occur.

In a study by Juste and Andrade (2011Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2011). Speech disfluency types of fluent and stuttering individuals: age effects. IALP Student Award. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica: official organ of the International Association of Logopedics and Phoniatrics, 63(2), 57-64. https://doi.org/10.1159/000319913
https://doi.org/10.1159/000319913...
) word prolongation and rupture site were analysed in the speech of stuttering and fluent individuals, the results showed that individuals with stuttering have a greater occurrence of prolongation in the syllable nucleus position. In syllables of Brazilian Portuguese, the nucleus is always filled by a vowel (Câmara, 1976Câmara, J. M. (1976). Estrutura da língua portuguesa, 7 ed. Editora Vozes.; Castro & Wertzner, 2009Castro, M. M., & Wertzner, H. F. (2009). Vowels influence in the stimulability for liquid sounds. Revista CEFAC, 11, 169-174. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462009005000031
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-1846200900...
; Souza, 1998Souza, A. C. D. (1998). Estrutura silábica do português brasileiro e do inglês americano: estudo comparativo. [Dissertação de mestrado], Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina. https://repositorio.ufsc.br/xmlui/handle/123456789/77538
https://repositorio.ufsc.br/xmlui/handle...
).

In the quantitative and qualitative analysis of the speech prolongation of subjects with and without fluency disorder (Silva et al. 2016Silva, P. B., Fabron, E. M. G., Picoloto, L. A., & Oliveira, C. M. C. (2016). Prolongations in the speech of adults who do and do not stutter. Revista CEFAC , 18(5), 1141-1150. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620161856116
https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-02162016185...
), individuals with disorders had a higher occurrence of non-hesitant prolongations at the beginning of words and isolated words, modifying the lexical unit, corroborating the study by Juste and Andrade (2010Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. D. (2010). Influência da tonicidade e local da ruptura na palavra em adolescentes e adultos gagos e fluentes. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica , 22(3), 175-182. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010000300004
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-5687201000...
). In fluent individuals, there was a higher frequency of hesitant prolongations at the beginning and end of the word, which may be associated with a hesitative mark. The hesitant prolongations tended to occur in monosyllabic words or in unstressed final syllables.

From the perspective of perception studies of such phenomena, Corley et al. (2007Corley, M., MacGregor, L. J., & Donaldson, D. I. (2007). It’s the way that you, er, say it: Hesitations in speech affect language comprehension. Cognition, 105(3), 658-668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.010
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006...
) point out that there are several corpus analyses and behavioural studies that suggest that disfluencies can affect listeners. A long-term consequence of disfluency is that the speaker is classified as less likely to know answers to questions of general knowledge when their answers are preceded by filled pauses (Brennan & Williams, 1995Brennan, S. E., & Williams, M. (1995). The feeling of another′ s knowing: Prosody and filled pauses as cues to listeners about the metacognitive states of speakers. Journal of Memory and Language , 34(3), 383-398. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1017
https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1017...
). For MacGregor et al. (2009MacGregor, L. J., Corley, M., & Donaldson, D. I. (2009). Not all disfluencies are are equal: The effects of disfluent repetitions on language comprehension. Brain and language, 111(1), 36-45. https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/9048741.pdf (Accessed 10 March, 2023)
https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/9048741....
), filled pauses and hesitations, as well as repeated or prolonged words, normally occur when the speaker is uncertain about how to continue and are part of the linguistic input that a listener must interpret. Bailey and Ferreira, (2003Bailey, K. G., & Ferreira, F. (2003). Disfluencies affect the parsing of garden-path sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(2), 183-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00027-5
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00...
) have shown that filled pauses can affect the interpretation of syntactically ambiguous sentences by listeners. Hearing a phrase that has a filled pause increases the memorization of the subsequent word (Corley et al. 2007Corley, M., MacGregor, L. J., & Donaldson, D. I. (2007). It’s the way that you, er, say it: Hesitations in speech affect language comprehension. Cognition, 105(3), 658-668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.010
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006...
), possibly due to an increase in attention (Collard, Corley, MacGregor, & Donaldson, 2008Collard, P., Corley, M., MacGregor, L. J., & Donaldson, D. I. (2008). Attention orienting effects of hesitations in speech: Evidence from ERPs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 696-702. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.696
https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.6...
).

MacGregor et al. (2010MacGregor, L. J., Corley, M., & Donaldson, D. I. (2010). Listening to the sound of silence: Disfluent silent pauses in speech have consequences for listeners. Neuropsychologia, 48(14), 3982-3992. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.024
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsycholog...
), concluded that disruptive pauses may increase listeners’ expectations for a lexical item that is harder for the speaker to produce. The authors emphasized that the disfluency statements of their study included features of disfluency, such as prolongation, before the interruption itself, and therefore the effects cannot be attributed solely to the presence of the silent pause.

The role of the listener and his/her perception of the disfluencies presented by the speaker is considered as a criterion for the evaluation of speech in stuttering. This may occur, for example, through judges who have no relation to the speaker or the therapist, consisting of “naive listeners” (Huinck, & Rietveld, 2007Huinck, W., & Rietveld, T. (2007). The validity of a simple outcome measure to assess stuttering therapy. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 59(2), 91-99. https://doi.org/10.1159/000098342
https://doi.org/10.1159/000098342...
). Lay judges are apt and able to distinguish even the attitudes expressed by people who are fluent over those expressed by people who stutter (Celeste, 2010Celeste, L. C. (2010). A prosódia na expressão de atitudes na fala de indivíduos com e sem gagueira [Tese de doutorado]. Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais.).

The literature review presented here points to at least two types of prolongations, hesitant (which can be classified as a common disfluency) and non-hesitant (considered a stuttering-like disfluency). It is questioned whether word final prolongations would have hesitant characteristics and, in this case, would be similar between people who stutter and people who do not stutter. It is believed that the acoustic characterization of such prolongations can be an important analysis for such an investigation.

As the literature indicates that fluent individuals tend to present vowel prolongation at the end of words, the present study aimed to (1) characterize the end of words prolongations regarding the type of prolonged sound, variation of the duration of the prolonged sound and variation of the average of prolonged sound frequency in individuals with and without developmental stuttering. The present study also aims to (2) verify lay judges’ speech fluency perception as to the degree of fluency of prolongations in rhyme position at end of words and (2) compare the influence of the prolongations in rhyme position in end of words and filled pauses in speech fluency perception. The study may represent one more step toward establishing subtypes of prolongation which will contribute to the accuracy of the diagnoses of stuttering and other fluency disorders.

2. Methods

This Research paper was submitted and approved by The Research Ethics Committees under CAAE 0308.0.203.000-11 and opinion number 122/09. All participants in the survey were volunteers and signed a Consent Form.

Experiment 1: acoustic analysis

Participants: 14 adults were recruited: six of them with a speech-language pathologist’ diagnosis of stuttering (stutterers - PWS) and eight fluent speakers (people who do not stutter - PWDS). The inclusion criteria common to both groups was: to be between 18 and 29 years old and to be a native speaker of Brazilian Portuguese. The specific inclusion criteria for the PWS group were that they had to have a previous diagnosis of stuttering and be on the waiting list for consultation at a Disfluency Clinic of any Brazilian University Hospital. Exclusion criteria for both groups were: history of neurological and/or psychiatric illness; history of associated communication disorders; poor quality audio recordings and individuals that did not present sound prolongations at the end of words. Initially there was no distinction of sex or age, but only males met both the criteria of inclusion and exclusion of the research.

Speech Fluency Profile analysis: all participants were assessed using a Brazilian standard Fluency Assessment Protocol (Andrade, 2000Andrade, C. R. F. (2000). Protocolo para avaliação da fluência da fala. Pró-fono, Ver. Atual. Científica, 12(2), 131-134. https://pesquisa.bvsalud.org/portal/resource/pt/lil-280907. (accessed 30 March, 2021).
https://pesquisa.bvsalud.org/portal/reso...
) and Riley’s Stuttering Severity Instrument (SSI-3) (Riley, 1994Riley, G.D. (1994). Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults, 3rd ed. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed,) to establish if there was or was not evidence of developmental stuttering. All speech samples were recorded using a tripod-mounted digital camcorder. To enable subsequent acoustic analysis, the speech samples were also recorded using a digital voice recorder coupled to a unidirectional microphone. Spontaneous speech samples were transcribed (fluent and disfluent syllables) to: a) classify the disfluencies (common disfluency: hesitation, interjection, revision, unfinished word, word repetition, segment repetition, repetition of phrase, and stuttered disfluencies: syllable repetition, sound repetition, blocking, pauses, intrusion of sound or segment and prolongation); b) calculate speech rate (words and syllables per minute). To guarantee data reliability, transcripts and analysis of the Fluency Assessment Protocol (Andrade, 2000Andrade, C. R. F. (2000). Protocolo para avaliação da fluência da fala. Pró-fono, Ver. Atual. Científica, 12(2), 131-134. https://pesquisa.bvsalud.org/portal/resource/pt/lil-280907. (accessed 30 March, 2021).
https://pesquisa.bvsalud.org/portal/reso...
) were submitted for inter-agreement analysis. The transcriptions and analysis were performed by two team members, with at least a 90% agreement rate.

Prolongations’ acoustic analysis: Data was transferred from the recorder to a computer and then the acoustic analysis was performed using the software Praat version 5.3.83 (Boersma, & Weenick, 2017Boersma, P., & Weenick, D. (2017) Praat: doing phonetics by computer. Version 5.3.83 [computer software] Amsterdam: Institute of Phonetic Sciences. http://www.praat.org.
http://www.praat.org...
). To label the data, an interval tier was created to mark the beginning and the end of prolongations that appeared at the end of words. Syllable was considered according to Selkirk (1982Selkirk, E. (1982). The syllable. In H. Hulst & V. D. Smith (Eds.), The structure of phonological representations, part II, (pp. 337-383). Foris, Dordrecht.) that suppose a structure composed by onset and rhyme; the rhyme is composed by one nucleus and one coda. The nucleus is it the only component that cannot be empty. In Brazilian Portuguese the nucleus is always occupied by a vowel or a diphthong (Bisol, 1989Bisol, L. (1989). O ditongo na perspectiva da fonologia atual. Delta, 5(2), 185-224.). To obtain reference values of duration and F0 of the prolongations, after labelling, non-prolonged sounds were selected from words with the same phonetic context. The duration average and fundamental frequency measures of the prolonged and referenced sounds were calculated. Duration is the time between the beginning and the end of the prolonged sound and as it was measured in seconds. For fundamental frequency measures the F0 mean was extracted for each prolonged sound. That means that for each analysed sound we had a referee from the same person in a similar phonetic context. To analyse the data, the variation of each of these measures was considered, according to the formula below:

Duration variation = prolonged sound duration - reference sound duration

F0 mean variation = F0 mean of prolonged sound - F0 mean of reference sound

Statistical analysis: An Excel spreadsheet database was created containing the characteristics of all prolongations identified: number of syllables and sounds of the word with prolongation(s), type of prolonged sound, duration and F0 mean of the prolonged and reference sound. For the statistical analysis, measures of central tendency and dispersion were calculated for the continuous variables and frequency for the categories. Most of the variables did not present normal distribution; therefore, the comparisons were made by Pearson’s chi-square, Fisher’s exact and Mann-Whitney’s. The level of significance was 5%.

Experiment 2: perceptual test

For the perceptual test, we collected 25 utterances produced by 39 fluent adults which consisted of 20 males and 19 females with an average age of 22.2 years old. Ten utterances contained prolongations in rhyme position at end of words, ten utterances contained filled hesitation and five utterances had no disfluencies. Phrases containing other concomitant disfluencies were discarded. The classification of disfluencies followed a methodology established by Andrade (2000Andrade, C. R. F. (2000). Protocolo para avaliação da fluência da fala. Pró-fono, Ver. Atual. Científica, 12(2), 131-134. https://pesquisa.bvsalud.org/portal/resource/pt/lil-280907. (accessed 30 March, 2021).
https://pesquisa.bvsalud.org/portal/reso...
) and the speech samples were edited using Praat 5.3.83 software (Boersma & Weenick, 2017Boersma, P., & Weenick, D. (2017) Praat: doing phonetics by computer. Version 5.3.83 [computer software] Amsterdam: Institute of Phonetic Sciences. http://www.praat.org.
http://www.praat.org...
).

Judges: Twenty participants (10 males and 10 females with an average age of 24 years old) were selected for the perceptual test. The inclusion criteria for this group were that they did not have any speech fluency disorders and should be older than 18 years old. Speech-language pathologists or speech-language pathology students were excluded, as well as participants who did not met the other inclusion criteria. None of the judges reported any hearing problems.

Procedures: The perceptual test had to be answered on a test sheet which contained numbers from 1 to 25 in a vertical column, which corresponded to the speech sample, and a score rating line numbered from 0 to 5, which indicated the grading of speech fluency (where 0 represented excess disfluency and 5 represented a high level of fluency). To standardize the data for analytical purposes, the following degrees of fluency were considered: from 0-1, bad speech; of 2-3, moderate speech; and 4-5, good speech. The participants were asked to mark the column corresponding to the utterance line according to their judgments of fluency for each speech sample. The utterances were arranged randomly in the test. Each utterance was presented twice and could be repeated up to three times if there were any cases of doubt. The application of the perceptive test was carried out collectively in the presence of all twenty judges.

Data analysis: data was submitted to descriptive statistical analysis (frequency table) and inferential analysis (One and Two Sample Proportion Test), with a significance level of 5%.

3. Results

Regarding the acoustic analysis, in the PWS group there were 20 prolongations at end of words and in the PWDS group there were 29. There was no difference between groups regarding the number of syllables in the words with prolongations (Pearson’s chi-square test), with predominance of monosyllable words (Table 1).

Table 1
Distribution of prolongations according to the number of syllables in the word with prolongations

In Table 2 it was not possible to verify if there was a difference between the groups in the number of sounds of the word with prolongations, due to the number of cells with an expected value less than 1.0, but the Mann-Whitney test did not find statistically significant differences between the groups in relation to the average number of sounds of the words with prolongations (p = 0.107).

Table 2
Distribution of prolongations according to the number of sounds in a word with prolongations

Table 3 presents the characterization of the groups regarding the distribution of the prolonged sounds. It was not possible to calculate the chi-square due to the number of cells with a value lower than 5.0, but there was a greater frequency of prolongations in the phone ɪ_ in the PWS group and ɐ_ in the group PWDS.

Table 3
Distribution of prolongations according to the prolonged phone

In Tables 4 and 5 the frequency of prolongation can be observed taking into account the type of prolonged phone, and no difference was observed between the groups. In both groups more than 80% of prolonged sounds were vowels.

Table 4
Distribution of prolongations according to the classification of prolonged sound.

Table 5
Frequency of prolongations in vowels and consonants.

It was not possible to calculate the chi-square due to the number of cells with a value lower than 5.

Regarding the acoustic aspects of duration and F0 mean of prolongations, the groups differed only in terms of the variation of duration of the prolonged sounds, which was higher for the PWDS group, this showed that the fluent participants demonstrated a longer duration of the prolongations (Table 6).

Table 6
Comparison between the groups of the variation of prolonged sounds duration and of mean frequency.

For the perceptual test, the descriptive analysis of the data indicated that in the study sample the sentences with filled pauses/hesitations and prolongations were classified mainly as moderate speech, whereas sentences without disfluencies were predominantly classified as good speech (Table 7).

Table 7
Perceptual test responses

To analyse if there was a significant difference between the assessments of the different statements, a One Sample Proportion Test was carried out comparing the proportions found. The p-value> 0.05 indicated that there was no significant difference between the proportions evaluated. The results of the Proportion Test can be seen in Table 8. In the comparison between the proportions of responses of the statements containing prolongation and the statements containing filled pauses/hesitations, there was no significant difference. In the comparison between statements with prolongation and statements without disfluency (fluent speech), the Proportion Test showed statistical differences. The Proportion Test also showed different proportions in the comparison between statements containing filled pauses/hesitations and statements without disfluency.

Table 8
Comparison of the degrees of fluency within sentences and comparison of the statements within the degrees of fluency

4. Discussion

The study, through experiment 1, sought to characterize word final prolongations regarding the type of prolonged sound, variation of duration and mean frequency of the prolonged sound of subjects who stutter and who do not stutter. In general, there were no difference between the two groups regarding characteristics of the word final prolongations. Prolongations were most frequent in monosyllabic words and vowels (Tables 1, 4 and 5). The groups differed only in terms of the variation of duration of the prolonged sounds (Table 6). The fact that fluent participants demonstrated a longer duration of the prolongations may be explained by previous study that observed that people who stutter tend to be less accurate and more variable during the production of speech (Boutsen, Brutten & Watts, 2000Boutsen, F. R., Brutten, G. J., & Watts, C. R. (2000). Timing and intensity variability in the metronomic speech of stuttering and non stuttering speakers. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 513-520.).

We also investigated the influence of the presence of prolongations on a) general perception of fluency; b) degrees of fluency according to type of speech, and c) comparisons between hesitation and speech without disfluency. The results of this experiment did not indicate a difference between word final prolongations and hesitation regarding the degree of fluency (Tables 7 and 8). It must also be emphasized that the utterances containing prolongation and hesitation were judged to be worse than statements without disfluencies (Table 8). This result seems to indicate that the presence of disfluencies in speech, typical of stuttering or not, is enough to worsen the perceived degree of fluency of a statement.

The occurrence of word final prolongations in the speech of both individuals with and without stuttering corroborates with previous research, especially regarding studies with different language variants of Brazilian Portuguese (Pinto et al. 2013Pinto, J. C. B. R., Schiefer, A. M., & Ávila, C. R. B. (2013). Disfluencies and speech rate in spontaneous production and in oral reading in people who stutter and who do not stutter. Audiol Commun Res, 18(2), 63-70. http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.915.1934&rep=rep1&type=pdf
http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/dow...
; Costa et al. 2017Costa, J. B., Ritto, A. P., Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. D. (2017). Comparison between the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter. In CoDAS, 29(2). https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20172016136
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/201720...
; Natke et al. 2006Natke, U., Sandrieser, P., Pietrowsky, R., & Kalveram, K. T. (2006). Disfluency data of German preschool children who stutter and comparison children. Journal of fluency disorders , 31(3), 165-176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.04.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.0...
; Campbell, & Hill, 1998Campbell, J., & Hill, D. (1998). Systematic disfluency analysis. In J. Campbell & D. Hill (Eds.), Stuttering therapy (pp. 51-75). Memphis: Northwestern University and Stuttering Foundation of America.; Souza et al. 2013Souza, J. B., Paschoalino, F. C., Cardoso, V. M., & de Oliveira, C. M. C. (2013). Frequency and Typology Of Disfluencies: Comparative Analysis Between Clutterers And Stutterers. Revista CEFAC , 15(4), 857-863. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/6b49/d9fc75199413a78434862e51b4297709f5bd.pdf
https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/6b49/d9...
; Tumanova et al. 2011 Tumanova, V., Zebrowski, P. M., Throneburg, R. N., & Kayikci, M. E. K. (2011). Articulation rate and its relationship to disfluency type, duration, and temperament in preschool children who stutter. Journal of communication disorders , 44(1), 116-129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.09.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.0...
; Andrade, 2004Andrade, C. R. F. (2004). Fluência (Parte C). In C. R. F. Andrade, D. M. Befi-Lopes, F. D. M. Fernandes & H. F. Wertzner. ABFW: teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática (pp. 51-81). 2ª ed., vol. 1. Pró-Fono.) and fluent trait (Silva et al. 2016Silva, P. B., Fabron, E. M. G., Picoloto, L. A., & Oliveira, C. M. C. (2016). Prolongations in the speech of adults who do and do not stutter. Revista CEFAC , 18(5), 1141-1150. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620161856116
https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-02162016185...
; Nogueira et al. 2015Nogueira, P. R., de Oliveira, C. M. C., Giacheti, C. M., & Moretti-Ferreira, D. (2015). Familial persistent developmental stuttering: disfluencies and prevalence. Revista CEFAC , 17(5), 1441-1448. https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/1693/169342586008.pdf
https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/1693/1693425...
; Celeste, & Reis, 2013Celeste, L. C., & Reis, C. (2013). Expressão de certeza e dúvida na gagueira: estudo dos aspectos temporais da fala. Revista CEFAC , 15(6), 1609-1620. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462012005000060
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462012...
; Roberts et al. 2009Roberts, P. M., Meltzer, A., & Wilding, J. (2009). Disfluencies in non-stuttering adults across sample lengths and topics. Journal of communication disorders, 42(6), 414-427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.06.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.0...
; Oliveira, Bernardes, Broglio, & Capellini, 2010Oliveira, C. M., Bernardes, A. P., Broglio, G. A., & Capellini, S. A. (2010). Speech fluency profile in cluttering individuals. Pro-fono: revista de atualizacao cientifica , 22(4), 445-450. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0104-56872010000400014
https://doi.org/10.1590/s0104-5687201000...
). Juste and Andrade (2011Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2011). Influência da extensão da palavra e local da ruptura na sílaba na fala de adolescentes e adultos gagos e fluentes. Revista da Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia, 16(1), 19-24. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342011000100005
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-8034201100...
) analysed the influence of word size and break point of the syllables in the speech of adolescents and adults for both groups (stutterers and non-stutterers). The results showed no influence of word size on the number of disfluencies with predominance of prolongations in monosyllabic words for the two groups studied, as with the present research. These combined results show that prolongations of monosyllabic words do not appear to be a trait of stuttering, but a linguistic marker of hesitation (Betz & Kosmala, 2019Betz, S., & Kosmala, L. (2019). Fill the silence! Basics for modeling hesitation. Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 12-14. hal-02360611f. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00027-5
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00...
; Alvar, Lee, & Huber, 2019Alvar, A. M., Lee, J., & Huber, J. E. (2019). Filled Pauses as a Special Case of Automatic Speech Behaviour’s and the Effect of Parkinson’s Disease. American journal of speech-language pathology, 28(2S), 835-843. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-MSC18-18-0111
https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-MSC18...
; Gold et al. 2018Gold, E., Ross, S., & Earnshaw, K. (2018). Regional variation in West Yorkshire filled pauses: Implications for forensic speaker comparisons. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 144(3), 1903-1903. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.5068336
https://doi.org/10.1121/1.5068336...
; Defrancq & Plevoets, 2018Defrancq, B., & Plevoets, K. (2018). Over-uh-Load, Filled Pauses in Compounds as a Signal of Cognitive Load. In M. Russo, C. Bendazzoli & B. Defrancq (eds) Making Way in Corpus-based Interpreting Studies (pp. 43-64). New Frontiers in Translation Studies. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-6199-8_3
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-6199-...
; Bellinghausen et al. 2019Bellinghausen, C., Fangmeier, T., Schröder, B., Keller, J., Drechsel, S., Birkholz, P., & Riedel, A. (2019). On the role of disfluent speech for uncertainty in articulatory speech synthesis. In The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 39 (pp. 39-42). https://www.ida.liu.se/~robek28/conferences/diss2019/DiSS2019_Proceedings.pdf#page=49
https://www.ida.liu.se/~robek28/conferen...
; Castro et al. 2014Castro, B. S. A., Martins-Reis, V.O., Baptista, A. C., & Celeste, L. C. (2014). Fluency profile: comparison between Brazilian and European Portuguese speakers. CoDAS, 26(6), 444-446. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20142014184
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/201420...
).

In a study with German pre-school children with and without fluency disorders, prolongation occurred in both groups, but was more frequent in stutterers (Natke et al. 2006Natke, U., Sandrieser, P., Pietrowsky, R., & Kalveram, K. T. (2006). Disfluency data of German preschool children who stutter and comparison children. Journal of fluency disorders , 31(3), 165-176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.04.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.0...
). This was repeated in a study carried out with adults who spoke Brazilian Portuguese (Pinto et al. 2013Pinto, J. C. B. R., Schiefer, A. M., & Ávila, C. R. B. (2013). Disfluencies and speech rate in spontaneous production and in oral reading in people who stutter and who do not stutter. Audiol Commun Res, 18(2), 63-70. http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.915.1934&rep=rep1&type=pdf
http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/dow...
), but these studies did not indicate the place of rupture (the breaking point). However, another study with fluent adults who spoke Brazilian Portuguese showed that the prolongation was the most frequent stuttering-like disfluency and only occurred at the end of words (Andrade & Martins-Reis, 2011Andrade, C. R. F., & Martins, V.O. (2011). Influencia del sexo y el nivel educativo en la fluidez del habla en personas adultas. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, 31(2), 74-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0214-4603(11)70175-9
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0214-4603(11)70...
).

In a study carried out by Moniz (2006Moniz, H. (2006). Contributo para a caracterização dos mecanismos de (dis) fluência no Português Europeu [Master thesis]. Faculdade de Letras da Universidade de Lisboa. https://www.clul.ulisboa.pt/files/ana_mata/MA-diss_HMoniz.pdf
https://www.clul.ulisboa.pt/files/ana_ma...
), the acceptability of the end of word prolongation was as good as that of the filled pauses, and in some cases even greater acceptance. This seems to suggest that prolongation at the end of speech is a disfluency common to all speakers, fluent or stutterers. One should also consider the possibility that prolongation is not only a disfluency but also a component of discourse capable of exercising its own function in the statement (Macgregor et al. 2009MacGregor, L. J., Corley, M., & Donaldson, D. I. (2009). Not all disfluencies are are equal: The effects of disfluent repetitions on language comprehension. Brain and language, 111(1), 36-45. https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/9048741.pdf (Accessed 10 March, 2023)
https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/9048741....
; Eklund, 2001Eklund, R. (2001). Prolongations: A dark horse in the disfluency stable. DiSS’01 Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, University of Edinburgh, August 29-31, 5-8. https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_open/archive_papers/diss_01/dis1_005.pdf
https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_open...
; Howell, 2007Howell, P. (2007). Signs of developmental stuttering up to age eight and at 12 plus. Clinical psychology review, 27(3), 287-306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.08.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.08.00...
; Schnadt, & Corley, 2006Schnadt, M. J., & Corley, M. (2006). The influence of lexical, conceptual and planning based factors on disfluency production. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. 28(28), (pp. 8-13). https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9337x2hk.
https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9337x2h...
).

Regarding the prolongation position, the occurrence of this disfluency at the beginning of the word is related to lower naturalness of speech, while its positioning at the end of the word indicates a greater naturalness of speech (Tables 7 and 8). In a study by Roberts et al. (2009Roberts, P. M., Meltzer, A., & Wilding, J. (2009). Disfluencies in non-stuttering adults across sample lengths and topics. Journal of communication disorders, 42(6), 414-427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.06.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.0...
) the prolongations, often described as a stuttering symptom and not typical of “normal” speech, were produced by 11 of the 25 subjects. Many of the prolongations occurred in words at the end or beginning of a sentence in places where an interjection would normally occur - these prolongations were not accompanied by tension and were relatively brief. The authors recommend caution in classifying all prolongations in adults as part of their stuttering. Juste and Andrade (2010Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. D. (2010). Influência da tonicidade e local da ruptura na palavra em adolescentes e adultos gagos e fluentes. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica , 22(3), 175-182. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010000300004
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-5687201000...
) found that the prolongations observed in individuals without stuttering speech occurred exclusively in the last phoneme of the last syllable of the word. These prolongations, for the authors, seem to have the same purpose of hesitation - they are strategies used to facilitate the co-articulation between words.

Studies have shown that in the speech of individuals with fluency disorders (such as stuttering and Tourrete Syndrome), the prolongations are more frequent within the word (Silva et al. 2016Silva, P. B., Fabron, E. M. G., Picoloto, L. A., & Oliveira, C. M. C. (2016). Prolongations in the speech of adults who do and do not stutter. Revista CEFAC , 18(5), 1141-1150. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620161856116
https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-02162016185...
; Nogueira et al. 2015Nogueira, P. R., de Oliveira, C. M. C., Giacheti, C. M., & Moretti-Ferreira, D. (2015). Familial persistent developmental stuttering: disfluencies and prevalence. Revista CEFAC , 17(5), 1441-1448. https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/1693/169342586008.pdf
https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/1693/1693425...
; Van Borsel, Goethals, & Vanryckegham, 2004Van Borsel, J., Goethals, L., & Vanryckeghem, M. (2004). Disfluency in Tourette syndrome: Observational study in three cases. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica , 56(6), 358-366. https://doi.org/10.1159/000081083
https://doi.org/10.1159/000081083...
), thus highlighting the existence of a rupture (break) of the lexical unit (Silva et al. 2016Silva, P. B., Fabron, E. M. G., Picoloto, L. A., & Oliveira, C. M. C. (2016). Prolongations in the speech of adults who do and do not stutter. Revista CEFAC , 18(5), 1141-1150. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620161856116
https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-02162016185...
; Juste, & Andrade, 2006Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2006). Tipologia das rupturas de fala e classes gramaticais em crianças gagas e fluentes. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica, 18(2), 129-140. https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872006000200002
https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-5687200...
), which is a characteristic of non-hesitant prolongations. As the aim of the present study was to characterize prolongations at the end of words, the prolongations that appear to be typical of stuttering (within words) were not accounted for. In this sense, we cannot say that the groups did not differentiate as to the general occurrence of prolongations.

Juste and Andrade (2011Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2011). Influência da extensão da palavra e local da ruptura na sílaba na fala de adolescentes e adultos gagos e fluentes. Revista da Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia, 16(1), 19-24. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342011000100005
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-8034201100...
) found more prolongations in the speech of individuals with and without stuttering in the nucleus position, corroborating the present study in which a greater occurrence of prolongations in vowels than in consonants was observed. In Brazilian Portuguese the vowel is obligatorily the nucleus of the syllable (Câmara Junior, 1976Câmara, J. M. (1976). Estrutura da língua portuguesa, 7 ed. Editora Vozes.; Castro & Wertzner, 2009Castro, M. M., & Wertzner, H. F. (2009). Vowels influence in the stimulability for liquid sounds. Revista CEFAC, 11, 169-174. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462009005000031
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-1846200900...
). It can also be seen that in Brazilian Portuguese there is a predominance of words that end with a vowel, being used as a marker of hesitation: the speaker takes advantage of the longest word to correct lexical-semantic processing failures (Eu comi banana versus Eu comi éh banana).

Nogueira et al. (2015Nogueira, P. R., de Oliveira, C. M. C., Giacheti, C. M., & Moretti-Ferreira, D. (2015). Familial persistent developmental stuttering: disfluencies and prevalence. Revista CEFAC , 17(5), 1441-1448. https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/1693/169342586008.pdf
https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/1693/1693425...
) pointed out that the prolongations in fluent speakers appeared shorter when compared with individuals who stuttered but did not extract the measures for comparison purposes. In the present study, on the other hand, in which acoustic parameters were used for analysis, it was found that individuals without fluency disorder presented a greater variation in the duration of prolonged sounds than stutterers, and no difference was seen between the groups of the variation of the F0 mean.

Juste and Andrade (2010Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. D. (2010). Influência da tonicidade e local da ruptura na palavra em adolescentes e adultos gagos e fluentes. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica , 22(3), 175-182. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010000300004
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-5687201000...
) studied the influence of the tonicity and location of the rupture (break) and saw that in fluent individuals, prolongation is present in the final syllable of words. They also pointed out that the acoustic characteristics of prolonged sounds do not seem to be typical of stuttering. This statement seems to be confirmed by the present study, as prolongations at the end of words were more frequent in the PWS group than in the PWDS group (Tables 1-6) and were judged by lay listeners to be as fluent as the filled hesitations (Tables 7 and 8) thus supporting the hypothesis that prolongations at the end of words function as hesitations, therefore should not be counted as stuttering-like disfluencies.

5. Conclusions

It was seen that the occurrence of prolongation at the end of words was not influenced by the length of the word in terms of syllables and phones, with predominance of prolongations in monosyllabic words for the PWS and PWDS groups. There were more occurrences of vowel prolongations than consonants. Concerning the acoustic aspects, non-stutterers present a greater variation in the duration of prolonged sounds than stutterers. The groups did not differ in the F0 mean.

Prolongations ending in rhyming position at the end of words were judged as common disfluency. The findings of the present study, as well as other studies presented here, indicate that prolongations can be classified as both a stuttering-like and common disfluency, depending on the duration, position in the word and presence of physical concomitants. Consideration of such characteristics in the classification of prolongation as a disfluency may contribute to an increase in the diagnostic precision of fluency disorders.

Referências

  • Alvar, A. M., Lee, J., & Huber, J. E. (2019). Filled Pauses as a Special Case of Automatic Speech Behaviour’s and the Effect of Parkinson’s Disease. American journal of speech-language pathology, 28(2S), 835-843. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-MSC18-18-0111
    » https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-MSC18-18-0111
  • Andrade, C. R. F. (2000). Protocolo para avaliação da fluência da fala. Pró-fono, Ver. Atual. Científica, 12(2), 131-134. https://pesquisa.bvsalud.org/portal/resource/pt/lil-280907 (accessed 30 March, 2021).
    » https://pesquisa.bvsalud.org/portal/resource/pt/lil-280907
  • Andrade, C. R. F., & Martins, V.O. (2011). Influencia del sexo y el nivel educativo en la fluidez del habla en personas adultas. Revista de Logopedia, Foniatría y Audiología, 31(2), 74-81. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0214-4603(11)70175-9
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0214-4603(11)70175-9
  • Andrade, C. R. F., & Martins, V.O. (2007). Fluency variation in adolescents. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 21(10), 771-782. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200701502161
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200701502161
  • Andrade, C. R. F. (2004). Fluência (Parte C). In C. R. F. Andrade, D. M. Befi-Lopes, F. D. M. Fernandes & H. F. Wertzner. ABFW: teste de linguagem infantil nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência e pragmática (pp. 51-81). 2ª ed., vol. 1. Pró-Fono.
  • Bailey, K. G., & Ferreira, F. (2003). Disfluencies affect the parsing of garden-path sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(2), 183-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00027-5
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00027-5
  • Bellinghausen, C., Fangmeier, T., Schröder, B., Keller, J., Drechsel, S., Birkholz, P., & Riedel, A. (2019). On the role of disfluent speech for uncertainty in articulatory speech synthesis. In The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 39 (pp. 39-42). https://www.ida.liu.se/~robek28/conferences/diss2019/DiSS2019_Proceedings.pdf#page=49
    » https://www.ida.liu.se/~robek28/conferences/diss2019/DiSS2019_Proceedings.pdf#page=49
  • Betz, S., & Kosmala, L. (2019). Fill the silence! Basics for modeling hesitation. Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 12-14. hal-02360611f. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00027-5
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00027-5
  • Bisol, L. (1989). O ditongo na perspectiva da fonologia atual. Delta, 5(2), 185-224.
  • Boersma, P., & Weenick, D. (2017) Praat: doing phonetics by computer. Version 5.3.83 [computer software] Amsterdam: Institute of Phonetic Sciences. http://www.praat.org
    » http://www.praat.org
  • Boey, R. A., Wuyts, F. L., Van de Heyning, P. H., De Bodt, M. S., & Heylen, L. (2007). Characteristics of stuttering-like disfluencies in Dutch-speaking children. Journal of fluency disorders, 32(4), 310-329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.07.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2007.07.003
  • Boutsen, F. R., Brutten, G. J., & Watts, C. R. (2000). Timing and intensity variability in the metronomic speech of stuttering and non stuttering speakers. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 43, 513-520.
  • Brennan, S. E., & Williams, M. (1995). The feeling of another′ s knowing: Prosody and filled pauses as cues to listeners about the metacognitive states of speakers. Journal of Memory and Language , 34(3), 383-398. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1017
    » https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1017
  • Câmara, J. M. (1976). Estrutura da língua portuguesa, 7 ed. Editora Vozes.
  • Campbell, J., & Hill, D. (1998). Systematic disfluency analysis. In J. Campbell & D. Hill (Eds.), Stuttering therapy (pp. 51-75). Memphis: Northwestern University and Stuttering Foundation of America.
  • Castro, B. S. A., Martins-Reis, V.O., Baptista, A. C., & Celeste, L. C. (2014). Fluency profile: comparison between Brazilian and European Portuguese speakers. CoDAS, 26(6), 444-446. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20142014184
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20142014184
  • Castro, M. M., & Wertzner, H. F. (2009). Vowels influence in the stimulability for liquid sounds. Revista CEFAC, 11, 169-174. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462009005000031
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462009005000031
  • Celeste, L. C. (2010). A prosódia na expressão de atitudes na fala de indivíduos com e sem gagueira [Tese de doutorado]. Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais.
  • Celeste, L. C., & Reis, C. (2013). Expressão de certeza e dúvida na gagueira: estudo dos aspectos temporais da fala. Revista CEFAC , 15(6), 1609-1620. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462012005000060
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S1516-18462012005000060
  • Coleman, C. (2013). How can you tell if childhood stuttering is the real deal? In: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Leader Live: happening now in the speech-language-hearing world.LeaderLive. https://leader.pubs.asha.org/do/10.1044/how-can-you-tell-if-childhood-stuttering-is-the-real-deal/full/ Acesso em: 14 de abril de 2020.
    » https://leader.pubs.asha.org/do/10.1044/how-can-you-tell-if-childhood-stuttering-is-the-real-deal/full/
  • Collard, P., Corley, M., MacGregor, L. J., & Donaldson, D. I. (2008). Attention orienting effects of hesitations in speech: Evidence from ERPs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 696-702. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.696
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.696
  • Corley, M., & Stewart, O. W. (2008). Hesitation disfluencies in spontaneous speech: The meaning of um. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2(4), 589-602. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00068.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00068.x
  • Corley, M., MacGregor, L. J., & Donaldson, D. I. (2007). It’s the way that you, er, say it: Hesitations in speech affect language comprehension. Cognition, 105(3), 658-668. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.010
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.010
  • Costa, J. B., Ritto, A. P., Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. D. (2017). Comparison between the speech performance of fluent speakers and individuals who stutter. In CoDAS, 29(2). https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20172016136
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20172016136
  • Defrancq, B., & Plevoets, K. (2018). Over-uh-Load, Filled Pauses in Compounds as a Signal of Cognitive Load. In M. Russo, C. Bendazzoli & B. Defrancq (eds) Making Way in Corpus-based Interpreting Studies (pp. 43-64). New Frontiers in Translation Studies. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-6199-8_3
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-6199-8_3
  • DeJoy, D. A., & Jordan, W. J. (1988). Listener reactions to interjections in oral reading versus spontaneous speech. Journal of fluency disorders , 13(1), 11-25. https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(88)90025-3
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(88)90025-3
  • Eklund, R. (2001). Prolongations: A dark horse in the disfluency stable. DiSS’01 Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, University of Edinburgh, August 29-31, 5-8. https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_open/archive_papers/diss_01/dis1_005.pdf
    » https://www.isca-speech.org/archive_open/archive_papers/diss_01/dis1_005.pdf
  • Felsenfeld, S., Kirk, K. M., Zhu, G., Statham, D. J., Neale, M. C., & Martin, N. G. (2000). A study of the genetic and environmental etiology of stuttering in a selected twin sample. Behavior Genetics, 30(5), 359-366. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1002765620208
    » https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1002765620208
  • Gold, E., Ross, S., & Earnshaw, K. (2018). Regional variation in West Yorkshire filled pauses: Implications for forensic speaker comparisons. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 144(3), 1903-1903. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.5068336
    » https://doi.org/10.1121/1.5068336
  • Gósy, M. (2019). Filler words in children’s and adults’ spontaneous speech. The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech, 89-90. https://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1353206/FULLTEXT01.pdf#page=99
    » https://www.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:1353206/FULLTEXT01.pdf#page=99
  • Götz, S. (2019). Filled pauses across proficiency levels, L1s and learning context variables: A multivariate exploration of the Trinity Lancaster Corpus Sample. International Journal of Learner Corpus Research, 5(2), 159-180. https://doi.org/10.1075/ijlcr.17018.got
    » https://doi.org/10.1075/ijlcr.17018.got
  • Howell, P. (2007). Signs of developmental stuttering up to age eight and at 12 plus. Clinical psychology review, 27(3), 287-306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.08.005
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.08.005
  • Huinck, W., & Rietveld, T. (2007). The validity of a simple outcome measure to assess stuttering therapy. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 59(2), 91-99. https://doi.org/10.1159/000098342
    » https://doi.org/10.1159/000098342
  • Johnson, W. (1961). Measurements of oral reading and speaking rate and disfluency of adult male and female stutterers and nonstutterers. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders. Monograph Supplement, 7, 1-20.
  • Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2006). Typology of speech disruptions and grammatical classes in stuttering and fluent children. Pro-fono: revista de atualizacao cientifica, 18(2), 129-140. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0104-56872006000200002
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/s0104-56872006000200002
  • Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2006). Tipologia das rupturas de fala e classes gramaticais em crianças gagas e fluentes. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica, 18(2), 129-140. https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872006000200002
    » https://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872006000200002
  • Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. D. (2010). Influência da tonicidade e local da ruptura na palavra em adolescentes e adultos gagos e fluentes. Pró-Fono Revista de Atualização Científica , 22(3), 175-182. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010000300004
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0104-56872010000300004
  • Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2011). Speech disfluency types of fluent and stuttering individuals: age effects. IALP Student Award. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica: official organ of the International Association of Logopedics and Phoniatrics, 63(2), 57-64. https://doi.org/10.1159/000319913
    » https://doi.org/10.1159/000319913
  • Juste, F. S., & Andrade, C. R. F. (2011). Influência da extensão da palavra e local da ruptura na sílaba na fala de adolescentes e adultos gagos e fluentes. Revista da Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia, 16(1), 19-24. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342011000100005
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-80342011000100005
  • MacGregor, L. J., Corley, M., & Donaldson, D. I. (2009). Not all disfluencies are are equal: The effects of disfluent repetitions on language comprehension. Brain and language, 111(1), 36-45. https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/9048741.pdf (Accessed 10 March, 2023)
    » https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/9048741.pdf
  • MacGregor, L. J., Corley, M., & Donaldson, D. I. (2010). Listening to the sound of silence: Disfluent silent pauses in speech have consequences for listeners. Neuropsychologia, 48(14), 3982-3992. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.024
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.024
  • Moniz, H. (2006). Contributo para a caracterização dos mecanismos de (dis) fluência no Português Europeu [Master thesis]. Faculdade de Letras da Universidade de Lisboa. https://www.clul.ulisboa.pt/files/ana_mata/MA-diss_HMoniz.pdf
    » https://www.clul.ulisboa.pt/files/ana_mata/MA-diss_HMoniz.pdf
  • Natke, U., Sandrieser, P., Pietrowsky, R., & Kalveram, K. T. (2006). Disfluency data of German preschool children who stutter and comparison children. Journal of fluency disorders , 31(3), 165-176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.04.002
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2006.04.002
  • Nogueira, P. R., de Oliveira, C. M. C., Giacheti, C. M., & Moretti-Ferreira, D. (2015). Familial persistent developmental stuttering: disfluencies and prevalence. Revista CEFAC , 17(5), 1441-1448. https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/1693/169342586008.pdf
    » https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/1693/169342586008.pdf
  • Oliveira, C. M., Bernardes, A. P., Broglio, G. A., & Capellini, S. A. (2010). Speech fluency profile in cluttering individuals. Pro-fono: revista de atualizacao cientifica , 22(4), 445-450. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0104-56872010000400014
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/s0104-56872010000400014
  • Pinto, J. C. B. R., Schiefer, A. M., & Ávila, C. R. B. (2013). Disfluencies and speech rate in spontaneous production and in oral reading in people who stutter and who do not stutter. Audiol Commun Res, 18(2), 63-70. http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.915.1934&rep=rep1&type=pdf
    » http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.915.1934&rep=rep1&type=pdf
  • Postma, A., & Kolk, H. (1993). The covert repair hypothesis: Prearticulatory repair processes in normal and stuttered disfluencies. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36(3), 472-487. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3603.472
    » https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3603.472
  • Ribeiro, J. C. W. (2003). Estudo comparativo da estrutura silábica em espanhol e português. https://repositorio.ufsc.br/xmlui/handle/123456789/84698
    » https://repositorio.ufsc.br/xmlui/handle/123456789/84698
  • Riley, G.D. (1994). Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults, 3rd ed. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed,
  • Roberts, P. M., Meltzer, A., & Wilding, J. (2009). Disfluencies in non-stuttering adults across sample lengths and topics. Journal of communication disorders, 42(6), 414-427. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.06.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.06.001
  • Rose, R., & Watanabe, M. (2019). A crosslinguistic corpus study of silent and filled pauses: when do speakers use filled pauses to fill pauses? In: Proceedings of the 2019 ICPhS (pp. 2615-2619). https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2019/papers/ICPhS_2664.pdf (accessed 25 March, 2021).
    » https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2019/papers/ICPhS_2664.pdf
  • Scarpa, E. M., & Fernandez-Svartsman, F. (2012). A estrutura prosódica das disfluências em português brasileiro. Cadernos de Estudos Linguísticos, 54(1), 25-40. https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v54i1.8636969
    » https://doi.org/10.20396/cel.v54i1.8636969
  • Schnadt, M. J., & Corley, M. (2006). The influence of lexical, conceptual and planning based factors on disfluency production. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. 28(28), (pp. 8-13). https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9337x2hk
    » https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9337x2hk
  • Selkirk, E. (1982). The syllable. In H. Hulst & V. D. Smith (Eds.), The structure of phonological representations, part II, (pp. 337-383). Foris, Dordrecht.
  • Silva, P. B., Fabron, E. M. G., Picoloto, L. A., & Oliveira, C. M. C. (2016). Prolongations in the speech of adults who do and do not stutter. Revista CEFAC , 18(5), 1141-1150. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620161856116
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620161856116
  • Sitek, K. R., Cai, S., Beal, D. S., Perkell, J. S., Guenther, F. H., & Ghosh, S. S. (2016). Decreased cerebellar-orbitofrontal connectivity correlates with stuttering severity: whole-brain functional and structural connectivity associations with persistent developmental stuttering. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 190. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00190
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00190
  • Souza, A. C. D. (1998). Estrutura silábica do português brasileiro e do inglês americano: estudo comparativo. [Dissertação de mestrado], Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina. https://repositorio.ufsc.br/xmlui/handle/123456789/77538
    » https://repositorio.ufsc.br/xmlui/handle/123456789/77538
  • Souza, J. B., Paschoalino, F. C., Cardoso, V. M., & de Oliveira, C. M. C. (2013). Frequency and Typology Of Disfluencies: Comparative Analysis Between Clutterers And Stutterers. Revista CEFAC , 15(4), 857-863. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/6b49/d9fc75199413a78434862e51b4297709f5bd.pdf
    » https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/6b49/d9fc75199413a78434862e51b4297709f5bd.pdf
  • Tree, J. E. F. (1995). The effects of false starts and repetitions on the processing of subsequent words in spontaneous speech. Journal of Memory and Language , 34(6), 709-738. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1032
    » https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1032
  • Tree, J. E. F. (2001). Listeners’ uses of um and uh in speech comprehension. Memory & cognition, 29(2), 320-326. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194926
    » https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194926
  • Tumanova, V., Zebrowski, P. M., Throneburg, R. N., & Kayikci, M. E. K. (2011). Articulation rate and its relationship to disfluency type, duration, and temperament in preschool children who stutter. Journal of communication disorders , 44(1), 116-129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.09.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.09.001
  • Van Borsel, J., Goethals, L., & Vanryckeghem, M. (2004). Disfluency in Tourette syndrome: Observational study in three cases. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica , 56(6), 358-366. https://doi.org/10.1159/000081083
    » https://doi.org/10.1159/000081083
  • Yairi, E. (1997). Disfluency characteristics of early childhood stuttering. In R. F. Curlee & G. M. Siegel (Eds.), Nature and treatment of stuttering: New directions (2nd ed., pp. 49-78). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
  • Yaruss, J. S. (1998). Real-time analysis of speech fluency: Procedures and reliability training. American journal of speech-language pathology , 7(2), 25-37. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360.0702.25
    » https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360.0702.25
  • Credit Author Statement

    We, Lívia Maria Santos de Souza, Vanessa de Oliveira Martins-Reis, Pâmela Jesus dos Santos, Dianete Ângela do Valle Gomes, Aveliny Mantovan Lima and Letícia Correa Celeste, hereby declare that we do not have any potential conflict of interest in this study.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    15 Sept 2023
  • Date of issue
    2023

History

  • Received
    26 June 2020
  • Accepted
    06 Dec 2022
Pontifícia Universidade Católica de São Paulo - PUC-SP PUC-SP - LAEL, Rua Monte Alegre 984, 4B-02, São Paulo, SP 05014-001, Brasil, Tel.: +55 11 3670-8374 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: delta@pucsp.br