Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The mediational role of distracting stimuli in emotional word recognition

Abstract

Emotions are considered distractions that often prompt subsequent actions. In this way, the aim of this work was to examine the role of distracting stimuli on the relationship of RT and accuracy. In order to do that, a word recognition task was carried out in which emotional valence was manipulated. More precisely, a mediational model, testing how changes in distracting stimuli mediate RT predicting accuracy across emotional conditions, was carried out. The results suggest that changes in task demands should distract from the secondary task to the extent that these task demands implicate and affect accuracy. Moreover, the distracting task seems to mediate between accuracy and the target task under emotional stimuli, showing the negative distracting condition to be the most remarkable effect. Furthermore, neutral distracting latencies did not affect accuracy. Understanding the mechanisms by which emotion impairs cognitive functions has important implications in several fields, such as affective disorders. However, the effects of emotion on goal-directed cognitive processing remain unclear.

Keywords:
Word recognition; Emotional valence; Mediation; Response times

Background

How emotion affects memory is a subject of interest for several areas such as clinical or even the forensic fields. In this way, the effect of emotional valence on word recognition has been described in both theoretical and empirical research. This is an effect that has been reported as a robust one on several aspects of cognition, as well as on behavior (Van Tol, Demenescu, Van der Wee, Kortekaas, Marjan, et al. 2012Van Tol, M. J., Demenescu, L. R., Van der Wee, N. J., Kortekaas, R., Marjan, N., Den Boer, J. A., … Veltman, D. J. (2012). Functional magnetic resonance imaging correlates of emotional word encoding and recognition in depression and anxiety disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 71(7), 593–602.). Several studies claimed that emotional words might capture more attention than neutral ones (Bowen, Kark, & Kensinger 2017Bowen, H. J., Kark, S. M., & Kensinger, E. A. (2017). NEVER forget: negative emotional valence enhances recapitulation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–22.; Sereno, Scott, Yao, Thaden, & O'Donnell 2015Sereno, S. C., Scott, G. G., Yao, B., Thaden, E. J., & O'Donnell, P. J. (2015). Emotion word processing: does mood make a difference? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1191.). More precisely, and focusing on the particular effect of negative emotional content on word recognition, it has been suggested that negative stimuli elicit slower latencies under this condition. According to Moret-Tatay, Moreno-Cid, Argimon, Quarti Irigaray, Szczerbinski, et al. (2014)Moret-Tatay, C., Moreno-Cid, A., Argimon, I. I. D. L., Quarti Irigaray, T., Szczerbinski, M., Murphy, M., … Fernández de Córdoba Castellá, P. (2014). The effects of age and emotional valence on recognition memory: an ex-Gaussian components analysis. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 55(5), 420–426., this is a plausible result suggesting that an automatic vigilance process might operate to engage attention longer. In particular, the literature (León Gordillo, Martínez, Hernández, Cruz, Meilán, et al. 2010León Gordillo, F., Martínez, J. M. A., Hernández, L. M., Cruz, J. S., Meilán, J. J. G., Ramos, J. C., & Sáez, E. P. (2010). Emoción y memoria de reconocimiento: la discriminación de la información negativa como un proceso adaptativo. Psicothema, 22(4), 765–771.; Meng, Zhang, Liu, Ding, Li, et al. 2017Meng, X., Zhang, L., Liu, W., Ding, X., Li, H., Yang, J., & Yuan, J. (2017). The impact of emotion intensity on recognition memory: valence polarity matters. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 116, 16–25.) has suggested that the “normal population” might be predisposed to direct their attention to negative stimuli, supporting the idea that the negative emotional charge could have an essential role in our evolution.

Another remarkable issue regarding this topic is that emotional words seem to be remembered better than neutral ones (Ferré, García, Fraga, Sánchez-Casas, & Molero 2010Ferré, P., García, T., Fraga, I., Sánchez-Casas, R., & Molero, M. (2010). Memory for emotional words in bilinguals: do words have the same emotional intensity in the first and in the second language? Cognition and Emotion, 24, 760–785.; Herbert & Kissler, 2010Herbert, C., & Kissler, J. (2010). Motivational priming and processing interrupt: startle reflex modulation during shallow and deep processing of emotional words. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 76(2), 64–71.). Not surprisingly, it has been hypothesized that this rise of arousal might not only have an effect on response time (RT), slowing it down, but also in improving encoding (Ferré, Fraga, Comesaña, & Sánchez-Casas 2015Ferré, P., Fraga, I., Comesaña, M., & Sánchez-Casas, R. (2015). Memory for emotional words: the role of semantic relatedness, encoding task and affective valence. Cognition and Emotion, 29(8), 1401–1410.). In other words, to deal with emotional valence could be also considered a cognitive cost. Other authors have stipulated that accuracy and speed processing might be accommodated through several parameters such as decision components and variability (Mueller & Kuchinke 2016Mueller, C. J., & Kuchinke, L. (2016). Individual differences in emotion word processing: a diffusion model analysis. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(3), 489–501.; Ratcliff, Smith, Brown, & McKoon 2016Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D., & McKoon, G. (2016). Diffusion decision model: current issues and history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(4), 260–281.). In terms of memory processing, recognition might include some retrievalbased processing (Racsmány, Szőllősi, & Bencze 2017Racsmány, M., Szőllősi, Á., & Bencze, D. (2017). Retrieval practice makes procedure from remembering: an automatization account of the testing effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition.). Specifically, RT has been negatively associated with accuracy (Robinson & Johnson 1996Robinson, M. D., & Johnson, J. T. (1996). Recall memory, recognition memory, and the eyewitness confidence–accuracy correlation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81(5), 587.).

As is popularly known, states such as depression and posttraumatic stress disorder are often characterized by increased susceptibility to emotional distraction. It involves conscious and unconscious processes that draw attention away from a task. Moreover, Foerde, Knowlton, and Poldrack (2006)Foerde, K., Knowlton, B. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(31), 11778–11783. found evidence that memory performance could be modulated by distraction by using functional MRI (fMRI). On a theoretical level, distraction has been described as an important key in theories of emotion regulation (Zhang, Gross, & Hayne 2017Zhang, W., Gross, J., & Hayne, H. (2017). The effect of mood on false memory for emotional DRM word lists. Cognition and Emotion, 31(3), 526–537.).

According to the literature (Dolcos & McCarthy 2006Dolcos, F., & McCarthy, G. (2006). Brain systems mediating cognitive interference by emotional distraction. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(7), 2072–2079.; Schwager & Rothermund 2013Schwager, S., & Rothermund, K. (2013). Counter-regulation triggered by emotions: positive/negative affective states elicit opposite valence biases in affective processing. Cognition & Emotion, 27(5), 839–855.), the neural systems try to mediate the detrimental effects of emotional distractors. These might be defined as emotional information that impairs cognitive functions due to a high attentional cost or a detrimental effect on inhibition. In this way, several authors have proven that emotional irrelevant information can capture attention from the task in question, in terms of latency components (Gupta, Hur, & Lavie 2016Gupta, R., Hur, Y. J., & Lavie, N. (2016). Distracted by pleasure: effects of positive versus negative valence on emotional capture under load. Emotion, 16(3), 328.; Hodsoll, Viding, & Lavie 2011Hodsoll, S., Viding, E., & Lavie, N. (2011). Attentional capture by irrelevant emotional distractor faces. Emotion, 11(2), 346.). Although many efforts have been made, it remains unclear whether there would be an effect of emotional valence on accuracy for the irrelevant distractors in conditions of low load. That is to say how emotional valence can interfere with the efficiency of the underlying basic cognitive process related to attention. For this reason, a mediation model among the variables described before was chosen. In this way, the type of analysis is related to a path analysis, which is of interest to examine the relationships between variables. In particular, this is a useful tool for the testing of a model and both direct and indirect effects on a given result (such as mediation and moderation among other relationships), under the basis of multiple regression. Moreover, it can popularly be understood as a particular case of structural equation modeling (SEM). For the purpose of the present study, this method is of interest, as it might allow us to examine the mediational role of distracting stimuli in latencies to emotional stimuli. Due to the above considerations, several results were expected. First of all, we expected a stronger correlation between accuracy and reaction time under emotional conditions, indicating a cognitive cost. Finally, we expected that latencies under distracting stimuli would mediate the recognition in terms of target latencies and accuracy variables, depicting working memory (WM) and inhibition processes.

Method

Participants

A sample of 95 university students took part in the experiment (62 women and 33 men, with an average age of 25.12 years and SD = 3.10). In terms of inclusion criteria, all the participants had normal or corrected to normal vision, were native Spanish speakers and did not report cognitive or neurological disorders after a personal interview.

Materials

The stimuli employed were a selection of words from the Busca Palabras database (Davis & Perea 2005Davis, C. J., & Perea, M. (2005). BuscaPalabras: a program for deriving orthographic and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, 37(4), 665–671.). A total of 90 words were divided into three sets of 30 stimuli, based on their scores on emotional valence (positive, negative, or neutral); see Table 1. Employing the same stimuli as Moreno-Cid, Moret-Tatay, Irigaray, Argimon, Murphy, et al. (2015)Moreno-Cid, A., Moret-Tatay, C., Irigaray, T. Q., Argimon, I. I., Murphy, M., Szczerbinski, M., … Fernández, P. (2015). The role of age and emotional valence in word recognition: an ex-gaussian analysis. Studia Psychologica, 57(2), 83., where stimuli rated 4 or lower were considered to be negative, rated 4 to 6 to be neutral, and rated above 6 to be positive (consistent with previous literature, see Moret-Tatay et al. 2014Moret-Tatay, C., Moreno-Cid, A., Argimon, I. I. D. L., Quarti Irigaray, T., Szczerbinski, M., Murphy, M., … Fernández de Córdoba Castellá, P. (2014). The effects of age and emotional valence on recognition memory: an ex-Gaussian components analysis. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 55(5), 420–426.). From the 90 words selected, 45 words were designated as target (and appeared in the first and second block) and the other 45 as distractors (and appeared only in the second block).

Table 1
Average valence for the selected words in the different sets (standard deviation in parenthesis)

Procedure

Participants were tested in a quiet room, in groups of three or four. The presentation of stimuli and recording of response times were controlled by a Windows operating system through the DMDX software (Forster & Forster 2003Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 35, 116–124.). The experiment consisted of two phases. In the first phase, 45 target stimuli were randomly presented (divided into 15 stimuli for each of the three valence categories) with short exposures of 2 s each. In the second phase (15 min after the participants were distracted by performing Stroop tasks), 45 target stimuli plus the 45 distracting stimuli were randomly presented. Each word was presented until the participant gave a response or 2000 ms had passed. The participants were instructed to press a button (labeled “Yes”) if the stimulus was a target stimulus and press another button (labeled “No”) if the stimulus was a distractor stimulus. The participants were also instructed to respond as quickly as possible while maintaining a reasonable level of accuracy. The session lasted approximately 30 min.

Design and data analysis

A repeated measure design was employed where a classical analysis of variance (ANOVA) explored the impact of stimulus identity (target or distractor) and emotional valence on response latency and accuracy. The statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 20. We conducted a mediational analysis using process macro for SPSS (Hayes 2015Hayes, A. F. (2015). An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 50(1), 1–22.) to test the hypothesis that latency changes in distracting stimuli mediate the effect of RT in predicting accuracy. In this way, regression-based mediation procedures were executed employing bootstrapping procedures (MacKinnon & Fairchild 2009MacKinnon, D. P., & Fairchild, A. J. (2009). Current directions in mediation analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(1), 16–20.; Hayes 2009Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: satistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408–420.). This method allows the measuring of the indirect effect that represents the impact of the mediator variable on the stipulated relation by a method of bootstrapping (10000) with confidence intervals. More precisely, a regression coefficient (and associated t test) was first calculated on the mediational M variable (and its inherent a and b paths), the X independent variable on the dependent variable without the inclusion of moderator (c’ path) and the X independent variable on the dependent variable after the mediator was included (c path). Figure 1 depicts this analysis in terms of variables and paths.

Fig. 1
Mediational model under study to test and its paths

Results

The RTs were longer for negative stimuli than positive and neutral ones. Table 2 presents the average reaction times (ms), error rates, and standard deviation for each group of words. In the ANOVA for latency analyses, RTs less than 250 ms and over 1800 ms were excluded (less than 2% of the data set). The 1800 ms cutoff point was adopted for consistency with earlier studies in the field (Moret-Tatay et al. 2014Moret-Tatay, C., Moreno-Cid, A., Argimon, I. I. D. L., Quarti Irigaray, T., Szczerbinski, M., Murphy, M., … Fernández de Córdoba Castellá, P. (2014). The effects of age and emotional valence on recognition memory: an ex-Gaussian components analysis. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 55(5), 420–426.; Moret-Tatay, Leth-Steensen, Irigaray, Argimon, Gamermann, et al. 2016Moret-Tatay, C., Leth-Steensen, C., Irigaray, T. Q., Argimon, I., Gamermann, D., Abad-Tortosa, D., … de Córdoba Castellá, P. F. (2016). The effect of corrective feedback on performance in basic cognitive tasks: an analysis of RT components. Psychologica Belgica, 56(4).; Moret-Tatay et al. 2016Moret-Tatay, C., Leth-Steensen, C., Irigaray, T. Q., Argimon, I., Gamermann, D., Abad-Tortosa, D., … de Córdoba Castellá, P. F. (2016). The effect of corrective feedback on performance in basic cognitive tasks: an analysis of RT components. Psychologica Belgica, 56(4).; Perea, Panadero, Moret-Tatay, & Gómez 2012Perea, M., Panadero, V., Moret-Tatay, C., & Gómez, P. (2012). The effects of interletter spacing in visual-word recognition: evidence with younger normal readers and developmental dyslexics. Learning and Instruction, 22(6), 420–430.). A 2 × 3 ANOVA was conducted.

Table 2
Response time averages (ms), error rate, and standard deviation (SD) for different experimental conditions

The ANOVA of RT showed that differences in emotional valence (where negative stimuli was recognized slower) were close to but did not reach statistical significant: F(2,188) = 2.94; MSE = 16,329.33; η2 =0.03; p =0.055. Bonferroni's pairwise comparison approached but did not reached the statistically significant for negative versus positive stimuli (p = 0.06). In the case of distracting and target conditions, target stimuli were processed faster than distractors, and this difference reached statistical significance: F(1,94) = 61.25; MSE = 1,131,718.40; η2 =0.39; p< 0.01. Any interaction or error differences did not reach statistical significance (F< 1).

On the other hand, the ANOVA of on accuracy showed that differences in emotional valence (where positive stimuli was recognized slower) reached the statistical significant, F(2,188) = 8.33; MSE = 0.015; η2 = 0.081; p < 0.01. In the case of distracting and target conditions, target stimuli were processed faster than distractors, and this difference reached the statistical significance: F(1,94) = 5.38; MSE = 0.434; η2 = 0.054; p < 0.05. All Bonferroni's pairwise comparison between negative versus positive/neutral stimuli were statistically significant for emotion (p < 0.05), but neutral versus positive differences did not reach the statistical level. An interaction (emotion*presentation) was found across variables: F(2,188) = 6.99; MSE = 0.016; η2 = 0.069; p < 0.01.

Finally, a mediational model was tested. Previously, any relationship between variables was examined through Pearson correlation coefficient (Fig. 2). As depicted in Table 3, changes in latencies to distracting stimuli significantly mediated errors on target stimuli for the negative condition (F(1,93) = 53.07; MSE = 0.6435; R2 = .36; p< 0.001), the neutral (F(1,93) = 47.83; MSE = 0.6675; R2 =.34; p< 0.001) and the positive one (F(1,93) = 40.85; MSE = 0.7022; R2 =.31; p< 0.001). Figure 1 depicts each of the paths for each model. All of them reached the statistical significance, except for the b path in the neutral condition. Any other combination across emotional condition did not reach the statistical significance.

Table 3
Pearson coefficients across RT, accuracy, emotional valence, and condition (target and distracting)
Fig. 2
Mediational model testing how changes in distracting stimuli mediate RT predicting accuracy across emotional conditions

Conclusions and discussion

Emotions are considered distractions that often prompt subsequent actions. The aim of this work was to examine the role of distracting stimuli on the relationship of RT and accuracy. Understanding the mechanisms by which emotion impairs cognitive functions has important implications in several fields, such as affective disorders. Moreover, and according to Dolcos and McCarthy (2006)Dolcos, F., & McCarthy, G. (2006). Brain systems mediating cognitive interference by emotional distraction. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(7), 2072–2079., the neural systems that mediate the effects of emotion on goal-directed cognitive processing remain unclear.

The results could be described as follows: (i) First of all, changes in task demands should distract from the secondary task to the extent that these task demands implicate and affect accuracy, (ii) the distracting task mediates between the target task and accuracy under emotional stimuli, (iii) negative distracting stimuli seem to have a higher mediational effect than neutral or positive distracting stimuli, and (iv) positive distracting stimuli seem to have a higher mediational effect than neutral distracting stimuli.

As expected from the literature, latencies to distracting words presented higher RT. This indicates a cognitive cost in rejecting the second demand in a task. Moreover, and as it was hypothesized, RTs were inversely correlated to accuracy in most of the cases, in particular, under emotional conditions (as expected from previous literature (León Gordillo et al. 2010León Gordillo, F., Martínez, J. M. A., Hernández, L. M., Cruz, J. S., Meilán, J. J. G., Ramos, J. C., & Sáez, E. P. (2010). Emoción y memoria de reconocimiento: la discriminación de la información negativa como un proceso adaptativo. Psicothema, 22(4), 765–771.; Moreno-Cid et al. 2015Moreno-Cid, A., Moret-Tatay, C., Irigaray, T. Q., Argimon, I. I., Murphy, M., Szczerbinski, M., … Fernández, P. (2015). The role of age and emotional valence in word recognition: an ex-gaussian analysis. Studia Psychologica, 57(2), 83.). In this way, a mediational effect was found from distracting stimuli, except for the neutral ones. This might emphasize the role of emotion on memory, as a bias described before. Moreover, this type of approach could be considered as an extra value of the present work, as traditional analyses of variance, such as ANOVA, might be implemented by taking advantage of other more sophisticated analyses (Moret-Tatay et al. 2017Moret-Tatay, C., Lemus-Zúñiga, L. G., Tortosa, D. A., Gamermann, D., Vázquezmartínez, A., Navarro-Pardo, E., & Conejero, J. A. (2017). Age slowing down in detection and visual discrimination under varying presentation times. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 58(4), 304–311.).

In sum, our results showed that distraction should be intermediate under emotional conditions. Moreover, a stronger prediction from the negative stimuli was found. This hypothesis has received strong empirical support that negative stimuli generally hold more attention than neutral or positive stimuli, even for mood-congruent cognitions (Van Dillen & Koole 2007Van Dillen, L. F., & Koole, S. L. (2007). Clearing the mind: a working memory model of distraction from negative mood. Emotion, 7(4), 715.). This work also presents potential limitations. There are remarkably high error rates, which indicate that the task might be difficult for participants indicating a floor effect. For future research, it would be interesting to develop an easier series of experiments that might also examine the role attentional demands as indicated by several authors (Moret-Tatay et al. 2014Moret-Tatay, C., Moreno-Cid, A., Argimon, I. I. D. L., Quarti Irigaray, T., Szczerbinski, M., Murphy, M., … Fernández de Córdoba Castellá, P. (2014). The effects of age and emotional valence on recognition memory: an ex-Gaussian components analysis. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 55(5), 420–426.; Moret-Tatay & Perea 2011Moret-Tatay, C., & Perea, M. (2011). Is the go/no-go lexical decision task preferable to the yes/no task with developing readers? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(1), 125–132.; Navarro-Pardo, Navarro-Prados, Gamermann, & Moret-Tatay 2013Navarro-Pardo, E., Navarro-Prados, A. B., Gamermann, D., & Moret-Tatay, C. (2013). Differences between young and old university students on a lexical decision task: evidence through an ex-gaussian approach. The Journal of General Psychology, 140(4), 251–268.) and its possible interactions with emotional valence.

  • Publisher's Note
    Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
  • Published online: 25 January 2018

Acknowledgements

This research was sponsored by The Generalitat Valenciana (Ajudes per a la realització de projectes de I + D per a equips d'investigació emergents, GV/2016/188) and the Universidad Catolica de Valencia San Vicente Mártir (ayudas internas, 2016-198-001/PRUCV/2015/630).

References

  • Bowen, H. J., Kark, S. M., & Kensinger, E. A. (2017). NEVER forget: negative emotional valence enhances recapitulation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1–22.
  • Davis, C. J., & Perea, M. (2005). BuscaPalabras: a program for deriving orthographic and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, 37(4), 665–671.
  • Dolcos, F., & McCarthy, G. (2006). Brain systems mediating cognitive interference by emotional distraction. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(7), 2072–2079.
  • Ferré, P., Fraga, I., Comesaña, M., & Sánchez-Casas, R. (2015). Memory for emotional words: the role of semantic relatedness, encoding task and affective valence. Cognition and Emotion, 29(8), 1401–1410.
  • Ferré, P., García, T., Fraga, I., Sánchez-Casas, R., & Molero, M. (2010). Memory for emotional words in bilinguals: do words have the same emotional intensity in the first and in the second language? Cognition and Emotion, 24, 760–785.
  • Foerde, K., Knowlton, B. J., & Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(31), 11778–11783.
  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 35, 116–124.
  • Gupta, R., Hur, Y. J., & Lavie, N. (2016). Distracted by pleasure: effects of positive versus negative valence on emotional capture under load. Emotion, 16(3), 328.
  • Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: satistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408–420.
  • Hayes, A. F. (2015). An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 50(1), 1–22.
  • Herbert, C., & Kissler, J. (2010). Motivational priming and processing interrupt: startle reflex modulation during shallow and deep processing of emotional words. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 76(2), 64–71.
  • Hodsoll, S., Viding, E., & Lavie, N. (2011). Attentional capture by irrelevant emotional distractor faces. Emotion, 11(2), 346.
  • León Gordillo, F., Martínez, J. M. A., Hernández, L. M., Cruz, J. S., Meilán, J. J. G., Ramos, J. C., & Sáez, E. P. (2010). Emoción y memoria de reconocimiento: la discriminación de la información negativa como un proceso adaptativo. Psicothema, 22(4), 765–771.
  • MacKinnon, D. P., & Fairchild, A. J. (2009). Current directions in mediation analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(1), 16–20.
  • Meng, X., Zhang, L., Liu, W., Ding, X., Li, H., Yang, J., & Yuan, J. (2017). The impact of emotion intensity on recognition memory: valence polarity matters. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 116, 16–25.
  • Moreno-Cid, A., Moret-Tatay, C., Irigaray, T. Q., Argimon, I. I., Murphy, M., Szczerbinski, M., … Fernández, P. (2015). The role of age and emotional valence in word recognition: an ex-gaussian analysis. Studia Psychologica, 57(2), 83.
  • Moret-Tatay, C., Lemus-Zúñiga, L. G., Tortosa, D. A., Gamermann, D., Vázquezmartínez, A., Navarro-Pardo, E., & Conejero, J. A. (2017). Age slowing down in detection and visual discrimination under varying presentation times. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 58(4), 304–311.
  • Moret-Tatay, C., Leth-Steensen, C., Irigaray, T. Q., Argimon, I., Gamermann, D., Abad-Tortosa, D., … de Córdoba Castellá, P. F. (2016). The effect of corrective feedback on performance in basic cognitive tasks: an analysis of RT components. Psychologica Belgica, 56(4).
  • Moret-Tatay, C., Moreno-Cid, A., Argimon, I. I. D. L., Quarti Irigaray, T., Szczerbinski, M., Murphy, M., … Fernández de Córdoba Castellá, P. (2014). The effects of age and emotional valence on recognition memory: an ex-Gaussian components analysis. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 55(5), 420–426.
  • Moret-Tatay, C., & Perea, M. (2011). Is the go/no-go lexical decision task preferable to the yes/no task with developing readers? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(1), 125–132.
  • Mueller, C. J., & Kuchinke, L. (2016). Individual differences in emotion word processing: a diffusion model analysis. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(3), 489–501.
  • Navarro-Pardo, E., Navarro-Prados, A. B., Gamermann, D., & Moret-Tatay, C. (2013). Differences between young and old university students on a lexical decision task: evidence through an ex-gaussian approach. The Journal of General Psychology, 140(4), 251–268.
  • Perea, M., Panadero, V., Moret-Tatay, C., & Gómez, P. (2012). The effects of interletter spacing in visual-word recognition: evidence with younger normal readers and developmental dyslexics. Learning and Instruction, 22(6), 420–430.
  • Racsmány, M., Szőllősi, Á., & Bencze, D. (2017). Retrieval practice makes procedure from remembering: an automatization account of the testing effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition
  • Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D., & McKoon, G. (2016). Diffusion decision model: current issues and history. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(4), 260–281.
  • Robinson, M. D., & Johnson, J. T. (1996). Recall memory, recognition memory, and the eyewitness confidence–accuracy correlation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81(5), 587.
  • Schwager, S., & Rothermund, K. (2013). Counter-regulation triggered by emotions: positive/negative affective states elicit opposite valence biases in affective processing. Cognition & Emotion, 27(5), 839–855.
  • Sereno, S. C., Scott, G. G., Yao, B., Thaden, E. J., & O'Donnell, P. J. (2015). Emotion word processing: does mood make a difference? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1191.
  • Van Dillen, L. F., & Koole, S. L. (2007). Clearing the mind: a working memory model of distraction from negative mood. Emotion, 7(4), 715.
  • Van Tol, M. J., Demenescu, L. R., Van der Wee, N. J., Kortekaas, R., Marjan, N., Den Boer, J. A., … Veltman, D. J. (2012). Functional magnetic resonance imaging correlates of emotional word encoding and recognition in depression and anxiety disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 71(7), 593–602.
  • Zhang, W., Gross, J., & Hayne, H. (2017). The effect of mood on false memory for emotional DRM word lists. Cognition and Emotion, 31(3), 526–537.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    2018

History

  • Received
    11 Aug 2017
  • Accepted
    18 Dec 2017
Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia da Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul Rua Ramiro Barcelos, 2600 - sala 110, 90035-003 Porto Alegre RS - Brazil, Tel.: +55 51 3308-5691 - Porto Alegre - RS - Brazil
E-mail: prc@springeropen.com