Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Decreased self-reported receiving of social touch and social support predict loneliness in healthy adults

ABSTRACT

Loneliness has emerged as a public health concern. Previous research has reported its deleterious effects on physical and mental health; however, some specific psychophysiological mechanisms in healthy adults remain to be elucidated. The aim of the current study is to investigate whether self-reported social support and social touch (giving and receiving social touch), as well as resting heart rate variability (HRV), are significant negative predictors of loneliness in healthy adults. The study sample consists of 120 healthy students (50% female) with a mean age of 21.85 years old (DP= 2.21). The students were asked to complete a psychiatric screening questionnaire utilizing loneliness, social support, and social touch scales. HRV was derived from an electrocardiographic signal recorded for 15 min, with the participant relaxed in a supine position. Linear regression analyses were conducted to evaluate loneliness as a function of social support, social touch (giving or receiving touch), and resting HRV. The results show that social support (p< 0.001) and social touch, specifically receiving touch (p< 0.002), accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in loneliness. However, neither giving touch nor resting HRV was a significant predictor of loneliness. The current study highlights specific psychosocial factors in healthy adults that should be considered as promising pathways in order to reduce or work toward preventing loneliness, thus promoting better health and well-being.

Keywords:
Loneliness; Social touch; Social support; Heart rate variability

Introduction

Many years of research have revealed the potential consequences of social isolation for human beings (see for review Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk : A meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine, 7(7), e1000316.; Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as a risk factors for mortality: A meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 227–237. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614568352.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614568352...
). One of the first and most important findings was that social isolation constitutes a risk factor for mortality equivalent to or greater than obesity or cigarette smoking (House et al., 1988House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships and health. Science, 241, 540–545. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3399889.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3399889...
). As well as in the actual absence of social interactions, the perception of this absence has a significant impact on health. Thus, perceived social isolation is the concept of loneliness (Weiss, 1973Weiss, R. (1973). Loneliness: The experience of emotional and social isolation. Cambridge: MIT Press.). More complex than being isolated, loneliness means feeling alone, and it depends on the quality of the social network rather than the quantity of friends (Cacioppo & Patrick, 2008Cacioppo, J. T., & Patrick, B. (2008). Loneliness: Human nature and the need for social connection. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.). Owing to the COVID-19 outbreak, in which social distancing and lockdowns were imposed, loneliness has become a more recent topic of interest. Nonetheless, even before the current pandemic, loneliness had already been reported as an emerging public health issue (Cacioppo & Cacioppo, 2018Cacioppo, J. T., & Cacioppo, S. (2018). The growing problem of loneliness. The Lancet, 391, 426. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30142-9.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30...
).

A broad body of research suggests that loneliness is a significant risk factor for the development of psychopathologies and other health impairments (Jung et al., 2019Jung, W., Jang, K., & Lee, S. (2019). Heart and brain interaction of psychiatric illness: A review focused on heart rate variability, cognitive function, and quantitative electroencephalography. Clinical Psychopharmacology and Neuroscience., 17(4), 459–474. https://doi.org/10.9758/cpn.2019.17.4.459.
https://doi.org/10.9758/cpn.2019.17.4.45...
; Wang et al., 2020Wang, J., Lloyd-evans, B., Marston, L., Mann, F., Ma, R., & Johnson, S. (2020). Loneliness as a predictor of outcomes in mental disorders among people who have experienced a mental health crisis: A 4- month prospective study. BMC Psychiatry, 20(1), 249. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02665-2.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02665...
). Regarding mental health, an important association was found between loneliness and depression (Ge et al., 2017Ge, L., Yap, C. W., Ong, R., & Heng, B. H. (2017). Social isolation, loneliness and their relatioships with depressive symptoms: A population-based study. PLoS One, 12(8), e0182145. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182145.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.018...
), chronic social stress (Hawkley et al., 2008Hawkley, L. C., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Masi, C. M., Thisted, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). From social structural factors to perceptions of relationship quality and loneliness: The Chicago health, aging, and social relations study. The Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 63(6), 375–384. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.6.s375.
https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.6.s375...
), anxiety (Muyan et al., 2016Muyan, M., Chang, E. C., Jilani, Z., Yu, T., Lin, J., & Hirsch, J. K. (2016). Loneliness and negative affective conditions in adults: Is there any room for hope in predicting anxiety and depressive sympoms? The Journal of Psychology, 150(3), 333–341. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2015.1039474.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2015.10...
), and psychosis (da Rocha et al., 2018da Rocha, B. M., Rhodes, S., Vasilopoulou, E., & Hutton, P. (2018). Loneliness in psychosis: A meta-analytical review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(1), 114–125. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx036.
https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx036...
). A causal relationship between loneliness and psychiatric disorders has also been suggested (Mushtaq et al., 2014Mushtaq, R., Shoib, S., Shah, T., & Mushtaq, S. (2014). Relationship between loneliness, psychiatric disorders and physical health? A review on the psychological aspects of loneliness. Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research, 8(9), 1–4. https://doi.org/10.7860/JCDR/2014/10077.4828.
https://doi.org/10.7860/JCDR/2014/10077....
).

It is not surprising that loneliness is an issue of increasing concern given that human beings are considered to be an ultra-social and hyperactive cooperative species (Tomasello, 2014Tomasello, M. (2014). The ultra-social animal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44(3), 187–194. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2015.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2015...
). For several species, including primates, social touch represents a fundamental aspect in communication and plays an important role in maintaining social bonds and the cohesion of groups (Dunbar, 2010Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral, 34(2), 260–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008...
; Jakubiak & Feeney, 2017Jakubiak, B. K., & Feeney, B. C. (2017). Affectionate touch to promote relational, psychological, and physical well-being in adulthood: A theoretical model and review of the research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(3), 228–252. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868316650307.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868316650307...
). Proximity and interpersonal social contact are prominent components for survival and well-being, from premature babies to the elderly (Charpak et al., 2017Charpak, N., Tessier, R., Ruiz, J. G., Hernandez, J. T., Uriza, F., Villegas, J., … Maldonado, D. (2017). Twenty-year follow-up of kangaroo mother care versus traditional care. Pediatrics, 139(1), e20162063. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-2063.
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-2063...
; Cruciani et al., 2021Cruciani, G., Zanini, L., Russo, V., Mirabella, M., Palamoutsi, E. M., & Spitoni, G. F. (2021). Strengths and weaknesses of affective touch studies over the lifetime: A systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 127, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.04.012.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021...
; Feldman & Eidelman, 2003Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2003). Skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo care) accelerates autonomic and neurobehavioural maturation in preterm infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45(4), 274–281. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0012162203000525.
https://doi.org/10.1017/s001216220300052...
). Indeed, recently, the C-tactile pleasant touch pathway, a specialized system underlying the processing of receiving social touch, was broadly described (Ackerley et al., 2014Ackerley, R., Backlund Wasling, H., Liljencrantz, J., Olausson, H., Johnson, R. D., & Wessberg, J. (2014). Human C-tactile afferents are tuned to the temperature of a skin-stroking caress. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 2879–2883. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2847-13.2014.
https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2847-1...
; Gazzola et al., 2012Gazzola, V., Spezio, M. L., Etzel, J. A., Castelli, F., Adolphs, R., & Keysers, C. (2012). Primary somatosensory cortex discriminates affective significance in social touch. Proceedings. National Academy of Sciences. United States of America, 109, 1657–1666. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113211109.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113211109...
; Lloyd et al., 2013Lloyd, D. M., Gillis, V., Lewis, E., Farrell, M. J., & Morrison, I. (2013). Pleasant touch moderates the subjective but not objective aspects of body perception. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 207. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00207.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00207...
; Löken et al., 2009Löken, L. S., Wessberg, J., Morrison, I., McGlone, F., & Olausson, H. (2009). Coding of pleasant touch by unmyelinated afferents in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 547–548. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2312.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2312...
; Morrison et al., 2010Morrison, I., Löken, L. S., & Olausson, H. (2010). The skin as a social organ. Experimental Brain Research, 204, 305–314. https://doi.org/101007/s00221-009-2007-y.
https://doi.org/101007/s00221-009-2007-y...
). Much less attention has been given to the investigation of the benefits for giving touch; thus, we have made this inclusion within the present study. Maturana and Verden-Zöller (2008)Maturana, H., & Verden-Zöller, G. (2008). The origin of humanness. In P. Bunnell (Ed.), The biology of love. Exeter: Imprint Academic. proposed that human hands are caressing organs. The tactile exploration of pleasantness of surfaces’ involves vibration-sensitive Pacinian Corpuscles and proprioceptive afferents in hand palms (Klöcker et al., 2013Klöcker, A., Wiertlewski, M., Théate, V., Hayward, V., & Thonnard, J. L. (2013). Physical factors influencing pleasant touch during tactile exploration. PLoS One, 8, e79085. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079085.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.007...
). Gentsch et al. (2015)Gentsch, A., Panagiotopoulou, E., & Fotopoulou, A. (2015). Active interpersonal touch gives rise to the social softness illusion. Current Biology, 25, 2392–2397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.07.049.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.07.04...
showed that touching others’ skin elicits sensory and haptic pleasure in the giver, possibly involving the same receptors as described by Klöcker et al. (2013)Klöcker, A., Wiertlewski, M., Théate, V., Hayward, V., & Thonnard, J. L. (2013). Physical factors influencing pleasant touch during tactile exploration. PLoS One, 8, e79085. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079085.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.007...
. It has been shown that chronic loneliness is associated with a greater preferred interpersonal distance (Saporta et al., 2021Saporta, N., Scheele, D., Lieberz, J., Stuhr-Wulff, F., Hurlemann, R., & Shamay- Tsoory, S. G. (2021). Opposing association of situational and chronic loneliness with interpersonal distance. Brain. Science., 11(9), 1135. https://doiorg/10.3390/brainsci11091135.
https://doiorg/10.3390/brainsci11091135...
) and that lonely individuals reported feeling social touch as less agreeable (Saporta et al., 2022Saporta, N., Peled-Avron, L., Scheele, D., Lieberz, J., Hurlemann, R., Shamay- Tsoory, S.G. (2022). Touched by loneliness - how loneliness impacts the response to observed human touch: a tDCS study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 17(1), 142–150. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab122.
https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab122...
, Cacioppo et al., 2010Cacioppo, J. T., Norris, C. J., Decety, J., & Monteleone, G. (2010). In the eye of the beholder: Individual differences in perceived social isolation predict regional brain activation to social stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 83–92. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21007.
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21007...
; Silva et al., 2017Silva, H. D., Campagnoli, R. R., Mota, B. E. F., Araújo, C. R. V., Mocaiber, I., Rego, V. R., … Souza, G. G. L. (2017). Bonding pictures: Affective ratings associated to empathy and loneliness. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1136. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01136.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01136...
). Thus, delineating specific links between self-reported social touch (giving and receiving) and loneliness is a gap in the literature that remains to be investigated.

Subjective perceptions of being inserted into a support network may have relevant implications for loneliness. As such, studies have demonstrated that social support is an important variable in lowering loneliness (Bernardon et al., 2011Bernardon, S., Babb, K. A., Hakim-larson, J., & Gragg, M. (2011). Loneliness, attachment, and the perception and use of social support in university students. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 43(1), 40–51. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021199.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021199...
; Deniz et al., 2005Deniz, M. E., Hamarta, E., & Ari, R. (2005). An investigation of social skills and loneliness levels of university students with respect to their attachment styles in a sample of Turkish students. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 33(1), 19–32. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2005.33.1.19.
https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2005.33.1.19...
). For example, perceived friendship support was found to be the best predictor of lower loneliness scores (Pierce et al., 1991Pierce, G. R., Sarason, I. G., & Sarason, B. R. (1991). General and relationship-based perceptions of social support: Are two constructs better than one? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(6), 1028–1039. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.6.1028.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.6.1...
). Furthermore, perceived social support from family and friends was found to buffer against loneliness in the study by Schmitt and Kurdek (1985)Schmitt, J. P., & Kurdek, L. A. (1985). Age and gender differences in and personality correlates of loneliness in different relationships. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(5), 485–496. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4905_5.
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4905...
. Thus, in this study, we would like to corroborate the literature about social support and loneliness and add to the discussion on self-reported social support and its ability predict loneliness in healthy adult participants.

During the last few decades, psychophysiological research has used heart rate variability (HRV) to study social engagement (Porges, 2007Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006...
; Shaffer & Ginsberg, 2017Shaffer, F., & Ginsberg, J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Frontiers in Public Health, 5, 258. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258...
). HRV is a standard noninvasive tool for assessing the action of the autonomic nervous system over the heart based on variations in the RR interval between consecutive heartbeats (Shaffer & Venner, 2013Shaffer, F., & Venner, J. (2013). Heart rate variability anatomy and physiology. Biofeedback, 41(1), 13–25. https://doi.org/10.5298/1081-5937-41.1.05.
https://doi.org/10.5298/1081-5937-41.1.0...
; Smith et al., 2020Smith, T. W., Deits-Lebehn, C., Williams, P. G., Baucom, B. R. W., & Uchino, B. N. (2020). Toward a social psychophysiology of vagally mediated heart rate variability: Concepts and methods in self-regulation, emotion, and interpersonal processes. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 14(3). https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12516.
https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12516...
). Importantly, HRV has been useful as a marker of pathological conditions (Beauchaine & Thayer, 2015Beauchaine, T. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2015). Heart rate variability as a transdiagnostic bio- marker of psychopathology. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 98, 338–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.08.004.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015....
). For example, it was demonstrated that a low HRV is associated with a higher risk of mortality (Kleiger et al., 2005Kleiger, R. E., Stein, P. K., & Bigger, T. (2005). Heart rate variability : Measurement and clinical utility. Annals of Noninvasive Electrocardiology, 10(1), 88–101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1542-474X.2005.10101.x.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1542-474X.2005...
), cardiovascular disease (Carnethon et al., 2002Carnethon, M. R., Liao, D., Evans, G. W., Cascio, W. E., Chambers, L. E., Rosamond, W. D., & Heiss, G. (2002). Does the cardiac auto- nomic response to postural change predict incident coronary heart disease and mortality? The atherosclerosis risk in communities study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 155(1), 48–56. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/155.1.48.
https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/155.1.48...
), obesity (Kageyama et al., 1997Kageyama, R., Nishikido, N., Honda, Y., Kurokawa, Y., Imai, H., & Kobayashi, T. (1997). Effects of obesity, current smoking status, and alcohol consumption on heart rate variability in male white-collar workers. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health, 9(6), 447–454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004200050173.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s004200050173...
), depression (Kemp et al., 2010Kemp, A. H., Quintana, D. S., Gray, M. A., Felmingham, K. L., Brown, K., & Gatt, J. M. (2010). Impact of depression and antidepressant treatment on heart rate variability: A review and meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 67(11), 1067–1074. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.012Epub.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009....
), anxiety (Servant et al., 2008Servant, D., Logier, R., Mouster, Y., & G. M. (2008). Heart rate variability. Applications in Psychiatry. Encephale., 35(5), 423–428. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.encep.2008.06.016.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.encep.2008.06....
), and chronic stress (Lampert et al., 2016Lampert, R., Tuit, K., Hong, K., Donovan, T., Forrester, L., & Sinha, R. (2016). Cumulative stress and autonomic dysregulation in a community sample. Stress, 19(3), 269–279. https://dorg/10.1080/10253890.2016.1174847Epub.
https://dorg/10.1080/10253890.2016.11748...
). Given that loneliness is a risk factor for several diseases, HRV could be a prominent tool for investigation. However, few studies have investigated the relationship between HRV and loneliness, and the results have not converged. Some studies did not find an association between resting HRV and loneliness in young (Cacioppo et al., 2002Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Crawford, L. E., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, J. M., Kowalewski, R. B., et al. (2002). Loneliness and health: Potential mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 407–417.; Muhtadie et al., 2015Muhtadie, L., Akinola, M., Koslov, K., & Mendes, W. B. (2015). Vagal flexibility: A physiological predictor of social sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 106–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000016.
https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000016...
), middleaged, or older adults (Hawkley et al., 2006Hawkley, L. C., Masi, C. M., Berry, J. D., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Loneliness is a unique predictor of age-related differences in systolic blood pressure. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 152–164. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.152.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.1...
; Muhtadie et al., 2015Muhtadie, L., Akinola, M., Koslov, K., & Mendes, W. B. (2015). Vagal flexibility: A physiological predictor of social sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 106–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000016.
https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000016...
). On the other hand, other studies with larger samples showed a significant (Roddick & Chen, 2020Roddick, C. M., & Chen, F. S. (2020). Effects of chronic and state loneliness on heart rate variability in women. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 55(5), 460–475.) and modest (Hawkley et al., 2003Hawkley, L. C., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Loneliness in everyday life: Cardiovascular activity, psychosocial context, and health behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 105–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.105.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.1...
) negative association between loneliness and resting HRV. Therefore, it is necessary to conduct more investigations using a healthy sample of both sexes to clarify these ambiguities.

Solving the question of loneliness is a major challenge, especially considering the occurrence of social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic and the possibility of future pandemics. Cacioppo and Cacioppo (2018)Cacioppo, J. T., & Cacioppo, S. (2018). The growing problem of loneliness. The Lancet, 391, 426. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30142-9.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30...
argue that a solution to this challenge is possible if a collective effort is met. In the current literature, there remains a lack of data regarding the psychophysiological mechanisms underlying loneliness. Thus, the present study aims to investigate whether psychosocial factors, specifically self-reported social support and social touch (receiving and giving touch), and HRV while resting, a physiological indicator of trait of health and social functioning, could be predictors of loneliness in healthy adults.

Methods

Participants

The sample comprised of 120 undergraduate students (60 females) with a mean age of 21.85 years old (DP = 2.21). Participants were recruited according to the following criteria: age ranging from 18 to 30 years, being an undergraduate or graduate student, not being under medication (except for contraceptives), not having a diagnosis of psychiatric or heart disease, being a non-smoker, and not using alcohol or drugs with daily or almost daily frequency. The study protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the local institution and all participants provided written informed consent. Data were collected prior to the COVID-19 outbreak.

Psychosocial assessment

Initially, participants completed a health and lifestyle questionnaire to evaluate data on age, sex, exercise practice, medication use, caffeine ingestion, and physical health. The mental health status of the participants was assessed using the Psychiatric Screening Questionnaire (PSQ) (Harding et al., 1980Harding, T. W., De Arango, M. V., Baltazar, J., Climent, C. E., Ibrahim, H. H., Ladrigo-Ignacio, L., … Wig, N. N. (1980). Mental disorders in primary health care: A study of their frequency and diagnosis in four developing countries. Psychological Medicine, 10(2), 231–241. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291700043993.
https://doi.org/10.1017/s003329170004399...
) translated and adapted to Portuguese (Mari & Williams, 1986Mari, J. J., & Williams, P. (1986). A validity study of a psychiatric screening questionnaire (SRQ-20) in primary care in the city of São Paulo. The British Journal of Psychiatry: the Journal of Mental Science, 148, 23–26.). The PSQ consists of a scale composed of 20 items with “yes” and “no” options for responses, used to diagnose suspicion of common mental disorders. To analyze this questionnaire, all affirmative answers were added. Scores equal to or greater than five (for males) or seven (for females) indicate the presence of some mental disorder, and in this case, the participants were not included in the analysis.

Loneliness scores were assessed using the revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (Russell et al., 1980Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA loneliness scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 472–480. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx036.
https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx036...
) translated and adapted to Portuguese (Neto, 1989Neto, F. (1989). Avaliação da Solidão. Revista de Psicologia Clínica, 2, 65–79.). The Portuguese version of the revised UCLA Loneliness Scale is an 18-item questionnaire that evaluates the loneliness experience of an individual during different periods in time and their satisfaction with their social relations. This scale score ranges from 18 to 72 with items randomly alternated (nine items scoring from 1 = “never” to 4 = “several times” and nine items scoring from 1 = “several times” to 4 = “never”). The Cronbach’s alpha of the scale for this study was 0.89.

Social support was measured using the Social Support Scale (Chor et al., 2001Chor, D., Griep, R. H., Lopes, C. S., & Faerstein, E. (2001). Medidas de rede e apoio social no Estudo Pró-Saúde: pré-testes e estudo piloto. Caderno de Saúde Pública, 17(4), 887–896. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-311X2001000400022.
https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-311X200100...
) translated and adapted to Portuguese (Griep et al., 2005Griep, R. H., Chor, D., Faerstein, E., Werneck, G. L., & Lopes, C. S. (2005). Validade de constructo de escala de apoio social do medical outcomes study adaptada Para o português no Estudo Pró-Saúde. Cadernos de Saúde Pública, 21(3), 703–714.). The Social Support Scale is a 19-item questionnaire evaluating different aspects of social support (i.e., affective support, material support, emotional support, and positive social interaction). In this study, social support was used as the total score, which was the sum of all subscales. This scale’s score ranges from 19 to 95 (all items scoring from 1 = “never” to 5 = “always”), with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.83.

Social touch was evaluated using the Mutual Grooming Scale (Nelson & Geher, 2007Nelson, H., & Geher, G. (2007). Mutual grooming in human dyadic relationships: An ethological perspective. Current Psychology, 26, 121–140. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-007-9009-3.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-007-9009-...
) translated to Portuguese (Campagnoli et al., 2015Campagnoli, R. R., Krutman, L., Vargas, C. D., Lobo, I., Oliveira, J. M., Oliveira, L., … Volchan, E. (2015). Preparing to caress: A neural signature of social bonding. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(16), 2015. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00016eCollection.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00016...
). This instrument consists of 12 items measuring the frequency of giving touch and 12 items measuring the frequency of receiving touch, both over the last 12 months. Participants rated each item on both subscales considering close individuals (i.e., family, intimate friends), therefore excluding strangers. Scores range from 14 to 98 (all items scoring from 1 = “never” to 7 = “one or more times/day”), both for the receiving and for the giving of touch. The Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was 0.81 for giving touch and 0.85 for receiving touch.

Heart rate variability

Heart rate variability was accessed through electrocardiographic (ECG) signal processing. One PC-compatible computer was used for ECG acquisition using the Acknowledge (BIOPAC Systems Inc, Goleta, USA) software program. The signal recording was performed using reusable 8 mm electrodes (Ag/AgCl) in the 1st cardiac lead through an electrocardiograph ECG100C module coupled to the MP100 system (BIOPAC Systems Inc, Goleta, USA) at a sampling frequency of 1000 Hz with the participant relaxed in the supine position. We decided to measure HRV in the supine position because our record lasted 15 min, and we believed that it would be more comfortable and relaxing for the participant.

Data processing followed the recommendations of the Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology, 1996Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology (1996). Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation, 93(5), 1043–1065.). We employed Kardia, a MATLAB (Math-Works Inc., MA) software toolbox, to analyze the cardiac parameters (Perakakis et al., 2010Perakakis, P., Joffily, M., Taylor, M., Guerra, P., & Vila, J. (2010). KARDIA: A Matlab software for the analysis of cardiac interbeat intervals. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 98(1), 83–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmpb.2009.10.002.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmpb.2009.10.0...
).

An offline peak detection algorithm (derivative plus threshold) was used to estimate the Rwave fiducial points, after which the series was screened manually and corrected for artifacts. Standard deviation of normal to normal of all intervals (SDNN) and root mean square of successive RR interval differences (RMSSD) were extracted using a time-domain analysis, whereas high-frequency (HF) and low-frequency (LF) components were extracted using a frequency-domain analysis, as recommended (Laborde et al., 2017Laborde, S., Mosley, E., & Thayer, J. F. (2017). Heart rate variability and cardiac vagal tone in psychophysiological research - recommendations for experiment planning, data analysis, and data reporting. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 213. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213eCollection2017.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213...
; Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology, 1996). These indices reflect different aspects of autonomic nervous system functioning; RMSSD and HF are measures of parasympathetic activity (Ernst, 2017Ernst, G. (2017). Hidden signals -the history and methods of heart rate variability. Frontiers in Public Health, 5, 265. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00265.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00265...
; Pentillä et al., 2001Pentillä, J., Helminen, A., Jarti, T., Kuusela, T., Huikuri, H. V., & Tulppo, M. P. (2001). Time domain, geometrical and frequency domain analysis of cardiac vagal outflow: Effects of various respiratory patterns. Clinical Physiology, 21(3), 365–376. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2281.2001.00337.x.
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2281.2001...
; Shaffer & Ginsberg, 2017Shaffer, F., & Ginsberg, J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Frontiers in Public Health, 5, 258. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258...
). SDNN represents a global estimate of HRV with both sympathetic and parasympathetic influences (Shaffer & Ginsberg, 2017Shaffer, F., & Ginsberg, J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Frontiers in Public Health, 5, 258. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258...
; Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology, 1996), and LF can be influenced by vagal, sympathetic, and baroreflex mechanisms (Del Paso et al., 2013Del Paso, R., Langewitz, W., Mulder, L. J., van Roon, A., & Duschek, S. (2013). The utility of low frequency heart rate variability as an index of sympathetic cardiac tone: A review with emphasis on a reanalysis of previous studies. Psychophysiology, 50(5), 477–487. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12027Epub2013.
https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12027Epub20...
; Goldstein et al., 2011Goldstein, D. S., Bentho, O., & Sharabi, Y. (2011). LF power of heart rate variability is not a measure of cardiac sympathetic tone but may be a measure of modulation of cardiac autonomic outflows by baroreflexes. Experimental Physiology, 96(12), 1255–1261. https://doi.org/10.1113/expphysiol.2010.056259.
https://doi.org/10.1113/expphysiol.2010....
; Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology, 1996).

Procedure

Upon arriving at the laboratory, the participant was asked to fill out a questionnaire on health and general habits, the Psychiatric Screening Questionnaire (Mari & Williams, 1986Mari, J. J., & Williams, P. (1986). A validity study of a psychiatric screening questionnaire (SRQ-20) in primary care in the city of São Paulo. The British Journal of Psychiatry: the Journal of Mental Science, 148, 23–26.), the revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (Neto, 1989Neto, F. (1989). Avaliação da Solidão. Revista de Psicologia Clínica, 2, 65–79.), the Social Support Scale (Griep et al., 2005Griep, R. H., Chor, D., Faerstein, E., Werneck, G. L., & Lopes, C. S. (2005). Validade de constructo de escala de apoio social do medical outcomes study adaptada Para o português no Estudo Pró-Saúde. Cadernos de Saúde Pública, 21(3), 703–714.), and the Mutual Grooming scale (Campagnoli et al., 2015Campagnoli, R. R., Krutman, L., Vargas, C. D., Lobo, I., Oliveira, J. M., Oliveira, L., … Volchan, E. (2015). Preparing to caress: A neural signature of social bonding. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(16), 2015. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00016eCollection.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00016...
). Soon after, the electrodes to obtain the electrocardiogram (ECG) were placed on the 1st cardiac lead. The participant was instructed to rest in a supine position to register the ECG. The total time for ECG recording was 15 min, where the initial 5 min comprised of adapting the participant to the electrodes and the last 10 min consisted of the baseline test session itself. The room temperature was controlled by reverse cycle air conditioning equipment, which was turned on whenever the days were very hot or cold. In addition, the room temperature was monitored using a digital thermometer. The total duration of each experiment was approximately 1 h. All experiments were carried out from 8 am to 4 pm to avoid major HRV fluctuations (Sammito & Böckelmann, 2016Sammito, S., & Böckelmann, I. (2016). Factors influencing heart rate variability. International Cardiovascular Forum Journal, 6, 242. https://doi.org/10.17987/icfj.v6i0.242.
https://doi.org/10.17987/icfj.v6i0.242...
).

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were made using Statistica version 7.0 (StatSoft Inc., OK). Normality of data was evaluated using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. The HRV components were logarithm-transformed to fit data on a parametric distribution. In order to test the associations between loneliness and social variables, linear regression analyses were conducted in separate models. In each model, loneliness was entered as the dependent variable and other measures as predictors (social support in model 1, receiving touch in model 2, and giving touch in model 3). For HRV analyses, 17 individuals were excluded because of technical problems in the recording or signal processing. Thus, the final HRV analyses were performed using the data from 103 participants (53 females and 50 males) where loneliness was entered as the dependent variable and other measures as predictors (log SDNN in model 4, log RMSSD in model 5, log HF in model 6, and log LF in model 7), all controlled by age and sex (1-β= 0.99, α=0.05). The alpha level for statistical significance was set at 0.05. All tests were corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni adjusted alpha levels of 0.007 per test.

Results

Descriptive statistics of the sample are reported in Table 1.

Table 1
Descriptive statistics for psychosocial measures and HRV

Linear regression analyses were performed to investigate whether psychosocial measures were significant predictors of loneliness. The results showed that social support explained 17% of the variance in loneliness (R2= 0.17, F(1,118)= 25.42, Β= -0.42, SE B= 0.08, t= -5.04, p< 0.001) (model 1) (Fig. 1A). Regarding self-reported mutual grooming, it was found that receiving touch accounted for 6.7% of the variance in loneliness (R2= 0.07, F(1,118)= 9.55, Β= - 0.27, SE B= 0.08, t= - 3.09, p= 0.002) (model 2) (Fig. 1B), whereas giving touch did not reach statistical significance (R2= 0.03, F(1,118)= 4.31, Β= -0.18, SE B= 0.09, t= -2.07, p=0.04) (model 3) (Fig. 1C).

Fig. 1
Graphic representation of regression analyses of loneliness and psychosocial assessments. A Regression analyses predicting loneliness as a function of social support (Β=- 0.42, p < 0.001). B Regression analyses predicting loneliness as a function of receiving touch (Β=- 0.27, p = 0.002). C Regression analyses predicting loneliness as a function of giving touch (Β= - 0.18, p = 0.04)

Multiple linear regression analyses were conducted to test HRV components as predictors of loneliness. The results revealed that none of the components accounted for a significant variance in loneliness (see details in Table 2 and Fig. 2).

Table 2
Regression analysis predicting loneliness as a function of HRV
Fig. 2
Graphic representation of regression analyses of loneliness and heart rate variability variables. A Regression analyses predicting loneliness as a function of SDNN (Β=- 0.12, p = 0.57). B Regression analyses predicting loneliness as a function of RMSSD (Β=- 0.11, p = 0.63). C Regression analyses predicting loneliness as a function of log HF (Β=- 0.05, p = 0.82). D Regression analyses predicting loneliness as a function of log LF (Β=- 0.10, p = 0.59)

Discussion

The current study aimed to investigate whether self-reported social support, social touch, and components of HRV while resting are significant predictors of loneliness in a healthy sample of undergraduate students. Our results showed that loneliness significantly accounted for social support and social touch, specifically receiving touch. In contrast, neither giving touch nor resting HRV was a significant predictor of loneliness.

First, in support of previous literature findings, a significant association was found between loneliness and social support. This finding was also reported in a previous study by Lee and Goldstein (2016)Lee, C.-Y. S., & Goldstein, S. E. (2016). Loneliness, stress, and social support in young adulthood: Does the source of support matter? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45(3), 568–580. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0395-9Epub.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0395-...
, which showed the effect of social support from friends on reducing levels of loneliness in a sample of undergraduate students. Early adulthood is characterized by several transitions (Goosbya et al., 2013Goosbya, B. J., Bellatorreb, A., Katrina, M., Walsemannc, K. M., & Cheadled, J. E. (2013). Adolescent loneliness and health in early adulthood. Sociological Inquiry, 83(4), 10.1111/soin.12018.
https://doi.org/10.1111/soin.12018...
). For example, many young people leave their families looking for opportunities to improve their careers, which was the case for the population investigated in the current study. This may be one reason as to why early adulthood is considered a period of greater vulnerability in experiencing loneliness. Such a fact raises the necessity of providing support and social inclusion to these vulnerable individuals. Additionally, lonelier people may feel less pleasure in social contexts (Silva et al., 2017Silva, H. D., Campagnoli, R. R., Mota, B. E. F., Araújo, C. R. V., Mocaiber, I., Rego, V. R., … Souza, G. G. L. (2017). Bonding pictures: Affective ratings associated to empathy and loneliness. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1136. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01136.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01136...
) and have increased attention to negative social stimuli (Cacioppo et al., 2010Cacioppo, J. T., Norris, C. J., Decety, J., & Monteleone, G. (2010). In the eye of the beholder: Individual differences in perceived social isolation predict regional brain activation to social stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 83–92. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21007.
https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21007...
). Thus, it could be argued that if the social environment seems less rewarding for lonely people, it may be an obstacle in searching for social interactions, reducing opportunities for creating and maintaining bonds. This may impact the possibility of receiving social support, which contributes to further increasing the feelings of loneliness.

In the present study, we provide new evidence in that self-reported receiving of decreased social touch during the last year (chronic effect) is a predictor of greater lone-liness. Several studies have documented the beneficial effects of social touch in humans in experimental designs different from ours. For example, Coan et al. (2006)Coan, J. A., Schaefer, H. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2006). Lending a hand: Social regulation of the neural response to threat. Psychological Science, 17, 12. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01832.x.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006...
showed a reduced activation in some brain regions associated with emotional and behavioral threat responses when women held their husband’s hand in comparison with not holding a hand. Ditzen et al. (2007)Ditzen, B., Neumann, I. D., Bodenmann, G., Von Dawans, B., Turner, R. A., Ehlert, U., & Heinrichs, M. (2007). Effects of different kinds of couple interaction on cortisol and heart rate responses to stress in women. Psychoneuroen-docrinology, 32(5), 565–574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.03.011.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007....
found that individuals who had physical contact during a stress task showed lower increases in cortisol levels and heart rate in comparison to the ones who had received verbal support or to the ones who had no support. There is also evidence that receiving a gentle touch reduces feelings of social exclusion (von Mohr et al., 2017Von Mohr, M., Kirsch, L. P., & Fotopoulou, A. (2017). The soothing function of touch: Affective touch reduces feelings of social exclusion. Scientific Reports, 7, 13516. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-13355-7.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-13355...
). Heatley Tejada et al. (2020)Heatley Tejada, A., Dunbar, R. I. M., & Montero, M. (2020). Physical contact and loneliness: Being touched reduces perceptions of loneliness. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 6(3), 292–306. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-020-00138-0.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-020-00138...
showed that even in low-contact, individualistic societies receiving touch plays an important role in decreasing loneliness scores. Our findings corroborate with the literature and add the important insight of self-reported receiving of touch in the last year (chronic effect), which is different from receiving touch in a laboratory experimental session (acute effect) as already had been showed. This finding could be beneficial when considering how to buffer the deleterious effects of loneliness.

From an ethological perspective, the conscious perception of receiving touch may have interesting outcomes. For example, in some primate species, where allo-grooming plays a crucial role in communication, engagement in social activities depends on the amount of touch received (Dunbar, 2010Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral, 34(2), 260–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008...
). In humans, there is evidence from an experimental study evaluating close partners’ interactions that although both providing and receiving gentle touch were perceived as pleasant, being touched was more pleasant and significantly decreased heart rate, producing a calming effect (Triscoli et al., 2017Triscoli, C., Croy, I., Olausson, H., & Sailer, U. (2017). Touch between romantic partners: Being stroked is more pleasant than stroking and decelerates heart rate. Physiology & Behavior, 177, 169–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.05.006.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.0...
). This could explain the significant result for receiving touch and the non-significant result for giving touch in the present study. Furthermore, there is a link between CT-fiber activation, which mediates gentle touch input, and oxytocin release during social interactions (Walker et al., 2017Walker, S. C., Trotter, P. D., Swaney, W. T., Marshall, A., & Mcglone, F. P. (2017). C-tactile: Cutaneous mediators of oxytocin release during affiliative tactile interactions afferents? Neuropeptides, 64, 27–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.npep.2017.01.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.npep.2017.01.0...
). Oxytocin also promotes an increase in vagal activity (Uvnäs-Moberg & Petersson, 2022Uvnäs-Moberg, K., & Petersson, M. (2022). Physiological effects induced by stimulation of cutaneous sensory nerves, with a focus on oxytocin. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 43, 159–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.10.001.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.10...
), which is related to social functioning (Porges, 2007Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006...
). Therefore, the link between the oxytocin system, vagal activity, and CT fibers may be another potential mechanism explaining the lack of social connection felt by lonely people. This may be a plausible mechanism underlying the decrease in loneliness upon receiving touch.

The present results indicate that touch is marginally significant as a predictor of loneliness. Of importance for the current discussion, a previous study showed that visual social stimuli promoted an accelerated reaction time in the flexion of the fingers, a motor task that bears resemblance with the social touch (Souza et al., 2012Souza, G. G., Pereira, M. G., Vila, J., Oliveira, L., & Volchan, E. (2012). Affiliative stimuli as primers to prosocial predispositions. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 15(1), 237–243. https://doi.org/10.5209/rev_sjop.2012.v15.n1.37315.
https://doi.org/10.5209/rev_sjop.2012.v1...
). Additionally, it was found that exposure to bonding pictures (with social touch cues) increased subjective feelings of sociability and activity of smile muscles (Mota et al., 2021Mota, B. E., Rodrigues, F., Lacerda, David, I. A., Volchan, E., Campagnoli, R. R., & Souza, G. G. L. (2021). Pictures of social interaction prompt a sustained increase of the smile expression and induce sociability. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 5518. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-84880-9.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-84880...
) as well as the activity of muscles involved in a caress-like movement (Campagnoli et al., 2015Campagnoli, R. R., Krutman, L., Vargas, C. D., Lobo, I., Oliveira, J. M., Oliveira, L., … Volchan, E. (2015). Preparing to caress: A neural signature of social bonding. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(16), 2015. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00016eCollection.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00016...
). Further-more, the authors provided evidence of decreased feelings of isolation after priming with bonding pictures and a reduction in the motor readiness potential amplitude preceding caress on a soft cloth (Campagnoli et al., 2015Campagnoli, R. R., Krutman, L., Vargas, C. D., Lobo, I., Oliveira, J. M., Oliveira, L., … Volchan, E. (2015). Preparing to caress: A neural signature of social bonding. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(16), 2015. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00016eCollection.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00016...
). Taken together, these findings are in line with the results of the present study, reinforcing the importance of social touch in promoting social bonding and thus decreasing loneliness, which is essential for human health and survival.

In this study, we did not find any evidence of resting HRV as a predictor of loneliness. Several studies have shown that a low HRV at rest is associated with a wide range of disorders and risk factors for several diseases (Beauchaine & Thayer, 2015Beauchaine, T. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2015). Heart rate variability as a transdiagnostic bio- marker of psychopathology. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 98, 338–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.08.004.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015....
; Carnethon et al., 2002Carnethon, M. R., Liao, D., Evans, G. W., Cascio, W. E., Chambers, L. E., Rosamond, W. D., & Heiss, G. (2002). Does the cardiac auto- nomic response to postural change predict incident coronary heart disease and mortality? The atherosclerosis risk in communities study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 155(1), 48–56. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/155.1.48.
https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/155.1.48...
; Kageyama et al., 1997Kageyama, R., Nishikido, N., Honda, Y., Kurokawa, Y., Imai, H., & Kobayashi, T. (1997). Effects of obesity, current smoking status, and alcohol consumption on heart rate variability in male white-collar workers. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health, 9(6), 447–454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004200050173.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s004200050173...
; Kemp et al., 2010Kemp, A. H., Quintana, D. S., Gray, M. A., Felmingham, K. L., Brown, K., & Gatt, J. M. (2010). Impact of depression and antidepressant treatment on heart rate variability: A review and meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 67(11), 1067–1074. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.012Epub.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009....
; Kleiger et al., 2005Kleiger, R. E., Stein, P. K., & Bigger, T. (2005). Heart rate variability : Measurement and clinical utility. Annals of Noninvasive Electrocardiology, 10(1), 88–101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1542-474X.2005.10101.x.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1542-474X.2005...
). In addition, a vast literature has outlined that some of these same pathological conditions are associated with loneliness (Cacioppo & Cacioppo, 2014Cacioppo, J. T., & Cacioppo, S. (2014). Social relationships and health: The toxic effects of perceived social isolation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8(2), 58–72. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12087.
https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12087...
; Mushtaq et al., 2014Mushtaq, R., Shoib, S., Shah, T., & Mushtaq, S. (2014). Relationship between loneliness, psychiatric disorders and physical health? A review on the psychological aspects of loneliness. Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research, 8(9), 1–4. https://doi.org/10.7860/JCDR/2014/10077.4828.
https://doi.org/10.7860/JCDR/2014/10077....
). As such, it would be expected an association between lower loneliness and higher resting HRV. In this study, we do not confirm this association. Roddick and Chen (2020)Roddick, C. M., & Chen, F. S. (2020). Effects of chronic and state loneliness on heart rate variability in women. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 55(5), 460–475., using a large healthy woman sample, showed a strong negative association between resting HRV and loneliness. The findings from Hawkley et al. (2003)Hawkley, L. C., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Loneliness in everyday life: Cardiovascular activity, psychosocial context, and health behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 105–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.105.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.1...
supported a modest negative association. On the other hand, other studies did not find this association (Cacioppo et al., 2002Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Crawford, L. E., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, J. M., Kowalewski, R. B., et al. (2002). Loneliness and health: Potential mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 407–417.; Hawkley et al., 2006Hawkley, L. C., Masi, C. M., Berry, J. D., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Loneliness is a unique predictor of age-related differences in systolic blood pressure. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 152–164. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.152.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.1...
; Muhtadie et al., 2015Muhtadie, L., Akinola, M., Koslov, K., & Mendes, W. B. (2015). Vagal flexibility: A physiological predictor of social sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 106–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000016.
https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000016...
), which is similar to our findings. Possible reasons for why we did not find a significant negative association between resting HRV and loneliness is that our sample size may have been underpowered to detect the effect previously reported in the literature. Our study also differs from previous research as we included participants from both sexes, unlike Roddick and Chen (2020)Roddick, C. M., & Chen, F. S. (2020). Effects of chronic and state loneliness on heart rate variability in women. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 55(5), 460–475., and we recorded HRV in supine position, whereas Roddick and Chen (2020)Roddick, C. M., & Chen, F. S. (2020). Effects of chronic and state loneliness on heart rate variability in women. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 55(5), 460–475. and Hawkley et al. (2003)Hawkley, L. C., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Loneliness in everyday life: Cardiovascular activity, psychosocial context, and health behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 105–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.105.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.1...
collected the HRV in seated position. Another point is that we used the Portuguese version of the revised UCLA Loneliness Scale that has 18-item, different from the English version that has 20-item. But as the questionnaire was translated and adapted to Portuguese (Neto, 1989Neto, F. (1989). Avaliação da Solidão. Revista de Psicologia Clínica, 2, 65–79.), we believe that it did not influence the results.

Loneliness is a relevant topic that has increased in interest over the last decade and to an even greater extent following the outbreak of the coronavirus pandemic. In fact, an emerging body of research has reported the impact of imposed social distancing and loneliness on well-being and overall health (Bao et al., 2021Bao, L., Li, W. T., & Zhong, B. L. (2021). Feelings of loneliness and mental health needs and services utilization among Chinese residents during the COVID-19 epidemic. Globalization and Health, 17, 51. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12992-021-00704-5.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12992-021-00704...
; Clair et al., 2021Clair, R., Gordon, M., Kroon, M., & Reilly, C. (2021). The effects of social isolation on well-being and life satisfaction during pandemic. Humanities and social sciences. Communications, 8(28). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00710-3.
https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00710...
; Cooper et al., 2021Cooper, K., Hards, E., Moltrecht, B., Reynolds, S., Shum, A., McElroy, E., & Loades, M. (2021). Loneliness, social relationships, and mental health in adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Affective Disorders, 289, 98–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.04.016.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.04.01...
; Szwarcwald et al., 2021Szwarcwald, C. L., Malta, D. C., Barros, M. B. A., Júnior, P. R. B. S., Romero, D., de Almeida, W. D. S., … de Pina, M. F. (2021). Associations of sociodemographic factors and health behaviors with the emotional well-being of adolescents during the covid-19 pandemic in Brazil. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(11), 6160. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18116160.
https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18116160...
). As such, it is possible that the COVID-19 pandemic might have worsened a scenario that was already underway. Thus, implications of the worldwide expansion of loneliness must be further explored taking into account the effects of the current pandemic as well as other factors previously known to affect this condition.

It is important to note that there were some limitations in this study. Firstly, we recorded the ECG in a supine position which could change the influence of sympathetic and parasympathetic control to the heart. Secondly, we recorded the ECG for a long period of time, in which some participants could fall asleep. Thirdly, we did not record respiration as a variable. In turn, these limitations may affect the interpretability of the findings.

Conclusion

The current study provides evidence that decreased self-reported social support and receiving of touch are important predictors of loneliness. These results highlight the effects of specific psychosocial factors that should be considered a promising pathway for reducing, or even preventing, loneliness, thus promoting better health and well-being.

  • Funding
    This work was supported by the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development, Brazil (Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico - CNPq); Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel, Brazil (Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior - CAPES); Research Foundation of the State of Minas Gerais (Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de Minas Gerais - FAPEMIG); and the Federal University of Ouro Preto - Brazil (Universidade Federal de Ouro Preto - UFOP).
  • Availability of data and materials
    Data and survey materials will be made available upon request.
  • Declarations
    Ethics approval and consent to participate
    The study protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the local institution (CAAE: 0431.4112.9.0000.5150) and all participants provided written informed consent.
  • Consent for publication
    All participants provided consent for the publication of their data.
  • Publisher's Note
    Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Alexandre Roberto Teixeira (UFOP) for technical assistance during the experimental sessions.

Abbreviations

  • ECG  Electrocardiographic
  • HF  High-frequency component
  • HRV  Heart rate variability
  • LF  Low-frequency component
  • PSQ  Psychiatric Screening Questionnaire
  • RMSSD  Root mean square of successive differences
  • SDNN  Standard deviation of the NN interval
  • UCLA  University of California, Los Angeles.

References

  • Ackerley, R., Backlund Wasling, H., Liljencrantz, J., Olausson, H., Johnson, R. D., & Wessberg, J. (2014). Human C-tactile afferents are tuned to the temperature of a skin-stroking caress. The Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 2879–2883. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2847-13.2014
    » https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2847-13.2014
  • Bao, L., Li, W. T., & Zhong, B. L. (2021). Feelings of loneliness and mental health needs and services utilization among Chinese residents during the COVID-19 epidemic. Globalization and Health, 17, 51. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12992-021-00704-5
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/s12992-021-00704-5
  • Beauchaine, T. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2015). Heart rate variability as a transdiagnostic bio- marker of psychopathology. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 98, 338–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.08.004
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.08.004
  • Bernardon, S., Babb, K. A., Hakim-larson, J., & Gragg, M. (2011). Loneliness, attachment, and the perception and use of social support in university students. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 43(1), 40–51. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021199
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021199
  • Cacioppo, J. T., & Cacioppo, S. (2014). Social relationships and health: The toxic effects of perceived social isolation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8(2), 58–72. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12087
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12087
  • Cacioppo, J. T., & Cacioppo, S. (2018). The growing problem of loneliness. The Lancet, 391, 426. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30142-9
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30142-9
  • Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., Crawford, L. E., Ernst, J. M., Burleson, J. M., Kowalewski, R. B., et al. (2002). Loneliness and health: Potential mechanisms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 407–417.
  • Cacioppo, J. T., Norris, C. J., Decety, J., & Monteleone, G. (2010). In the eye of the beholder: Individual differences in perceived social isolation predict regional brain activation to social stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 83–92. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21007
    » https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21007
  • Cacioppo, J. T., & Patrick, B. (2008). Loneliness: Human nature and the need for social connection New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
  • Campagnoli, R. R., Krutman, L., Vargas, C. D., Lobo, I., Oliveira, J. M., Oliveira, L., … Volchan, E. (2015). Preparing to caress: A neural signature of social bonding. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(16), 2015. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00016eCollection
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00016eCollection
  • Carnethon, M. R., Liao, D., Evans, G. W., Cascio, W. E., Chambers, L. E., Rosamond, W. D., & Heiss, G. (2002). Does the cardiac auto- nomic response to postural change predict incident coronary heart disease and mortality? The atherosclerosis risk in communities study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 155(1), 48–56. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/155.1.48
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/155.1.48
  • Charpak, N., Tessier, R., Ruiz, J. G., Hernandez, J. T., Uriza, F., Villegas, J., … Maldonado, D. (2017). Twenty-year follow-up of kangaroo mother care versus traditional care. Pediatrics, 139(1), e20162063. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-2063
    » https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-2063
  • Chor, D., Griep, R. H., Lopes, C. S., & Faerstein, E. (2001). Medidas de rede e apoio social no Estudo Pró-Saúde: pré-testes e estudo piloto. Caderno de Saúde Pública, 17(4), 887–896. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-311X2001000400022
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S0102-311X2001000400022
  • Clair, R., Gordon, M., Kroon, M., & Reilly, C. (2021). The effects of social isolation on well-being and life satisfaction during pandemic. Humanities and social sciences Communications, 8(28). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00710-3
    » https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00710-3
  • Coan, J. A., Schaefer, H. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2006). Lending a hand: Social regulation of the neural response to threat. Psychological Science, 17, 12. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01832.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01832.x
  • Cooper, K., Hards, E., Moltrecht, B., Reynolds, S., Shum, A., McElroy, E., & Loades, M. (2021). Loneliness, social relationships, and mental health in adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Affective Disorders, 289, 98–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.04.016
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.04.016
  • Cruciani, G., Zanini, L., Russo, V., Mirabella, M., Palamoutsi, E. M., & Spitoni, G. F. (2021). Strengths and weaknesses of affective touch studies over the lifetime: A systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 127, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.04.012
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.04.012
  • da Rocha, B. M., Rhodes, S., Vasilopoulou, E., & Hutton, P. (2018). Loneliness in psychosis: A meta-analytical review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 44(1), 114–125. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx036
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx036
  • Del Paso, R., Langewitz, W., Mulder, L. J., van Roon, A., & Duschek, S. (2013). The utility of low frequency heart rate variability as an index of sympathetic cardiac tone: A review with emphasis on a reanalysis of previous studies. Psychophysiology, 50(5), 477–487. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12027Epub2013
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12027Epub2013
  • Deniz, M. E., Hamarta, E., & Ari, R. (2005). An investigation of social skills and loneliness levels of university students with respect to their attachment styles in a sample of Turkish students. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 33(1), 19–32. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2005.33.1.19
    » https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2005.33.1.19
  • Ditzen, B., Neumann, I. D., Bodenmann, G., Von Dawans, B., Turner, R. A., Ehlert, U., & Heinrichs, M. (2007). Effects of different kinds of couple interaction on cortisol and heart rate responses to stress in women. Psychoneuroen-docrinology, 32(5), 565–574. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.03.011
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.03.011
  • Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral, 34(2), 260–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001
  • Ernst, G. (2017). Hidden signals -the history and methods of heart rate variability. Frontiers in Public Health, 5, 265. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00265
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00265
  • Faul, F., & Erdfelder, E. (2007). G* power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.
  • Feldman, R., & Eidelman, A. I. (2003). Skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo care) accelerates autonomic and neurobehavioural maturation in preterm infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45(4), 274–281. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0012162203000525
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/s0012162203000525
  • Gazzola, V., Spezio, M. L., Etzel, J. A., Castelli, F., Adolphs, R., & Keysers, C. (2012). Primary somatosensory cortex discriminates affective significance in social touch. Proceedings. National Academy of Sciences. United States of America, 109, 1657–1666. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113211109
    » https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113211109
  • Ge, L., Yap, C. W., Ong, R., & Heng, B. H. (2017). Social isolation, loneliness and their relatioships with depressive symptoms: A population-based study. PLoS One, 12(8), e0182145. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182145
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0182145
  • Gentsch, A., Panagiotopoulou, E., & Fotopoulou, A. (2015). Active interpersonal touch gives rise to the social softness illusion. Current Biology, 25, 2392–2397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.07.049
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.07.049
  • Goldstein, D. S., Bentho, O., & Sharabi, Y. (2011). LF power of heart rate variability is not a measure of cardiac sympathetic tone but may be a measure of modulation of cardiac autonomic outflows by baroreflexes. Experimental Physiology, 96(12), 1255–1261. https://doi.org/10.1113/expphysiol.2010.056259
    » https://doi.org/10.1113/expphysiol.2010.056259
  • Goosbya, B. J., Bellatorreb, A., Katrina, M., Walsemannc, K. M., & Cheadled, J. E. (2013). Adolescent loneliness and health in early adulthood. Sociological Inquiry, 83(4), 10.1111/soin.12018.
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/soin.12018
  • Griep, R. H., Chor, D., Faerstein, E., Werneck, G. L., & Lopes, C. S. (2005). Validade de constructo de escala de apoio social do medical outcomes study adaptada Para o português no Estudo Pró-Saúde. Cadernos de Saúde Pública, 21(3), 703–714.
  • Harding, T. W., De Arango, M. V., Baltazar, J., Climent, C. E., Ibrahim, H. H., Ladrigo-Ignacio, L., … Wig, N. N. (1980). Mental disorders in primary health care: A study of their frequency and diagnosis in four developing countries. Psychological Medicine, 10(2), 231–241. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291700043993
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291700043993
  • Hawkley, L. C., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Loneliness in everyday life: Cardiovascular activity, psychosocial context, and health behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 105–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.105
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.105
  • Hawkley, L. C., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Masi, C. M., Thisted, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). From social structural factors to perceptions of relationship quality and loneliness: The Chicago health, aging, and social relations study. The Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 63(6), 375–384. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.6.s375
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.6.s375
  • Hawkley, L. C., Masi, C. M., Berry, J. D., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Loneliness is a unique predictor of age-related differences in systolic blood pressure. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 152–164. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.152
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.152
  • Heatley Tejada, A., Dunbar, R. I. M., & Montero, M. (2020). Physical contact and loneliness: Being touched reduces perceptions of loneliness. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 6(3), 292–306. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-020-00138-0
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-020-00138-0
  • Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., & Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as a risk factors for mortality: A meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(2), 227–237. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614568352
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614568352
  • Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk : A meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine, 7(7), e1000316.
  • House, J. S., Landis, K. R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships and health. Science, 241, 540–545. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3399889
    » https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3399889
  • Jakubiak, B. K., & Feeney, B. C. (2017). Affectionate touch to promote relational, psychological, and physical well-being in adulthood: A theoretical model and review of the research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(3), 228–252. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868316650307
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868316650307
  • Jung, W., Jang, K., & Lee, S. (2019). Heart and brain interaction of psychiatric illness: A review focused on heart rate variability, cognitive function, and quantitative electroencephalography. Clinical Psychopharmacology and Neuroscience., 17(4), 459–474. https://doi.org/10.9758/cpn.2019.17.4.459
    » https://doi.org/10.9758/cpn.2019.17.4.459
  • Kageyama, R., Nishikido, N., Honda, Y., Kurokawa, Y., Imai, H., & Kobayashi, T. (1997). Effects of obesity, current smoking status, and alcohol consumption on heart rate variability in male white-collar workers. International Archives of Occupational and Environmental Health, 9(6), 447–454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004200050173
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s004200050173
  • Kemp, A. H., Quintana, D. S., Gray, M. A., Felmingham, K. L., Brown, K., & Gatt, J. M. (2010). Impact of depression and antidepressant treatment on heart rate variability: A review and meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 67(11), 1067–1074. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.012Epub
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.012Epub
  • Kleiger, R. E., Stein, P. K., & Bigger, T. (2005). Heart rate variability : Measurement and clinical utility. Annals of Noninvasive Electrocardiology, 10(1), 88–101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1542-474X.2005.10101.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1542-474X.2005.10101.x
  • Klöcker, A., Wiertlewski, M., Théate, V., Hayward, V., & Thonnard, J. L. (2013). Physical factors influencing pleasant touch during tactile exploration. PLoS One, 8, e79085. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079085
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0079085
  • Laborde, S., Mosley, E., & Thayer, J. F. (2017). Heart rate variability and cardiac vagal tone in psychophysiological research - recommendations for experiment planning, data analysis, and data reporting. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 213. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213eCollection2017
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213eCollection2017
  • Lampert, R., Tuit, K., Hong, K., Donovan, T., Forrester, L., & Sinha, R. (2016). Cumulative stress and autonomic dysregulation in a community sample. Stress, 19(3), 269–279. https://dorg/10.1080/10253890.2016.1174847Epub
    » https://dorg/10.1080/10253890.2016.1174847Epub
  • Lee, C.-Y. S., & Goldstein, S. E. (2016). Loneliness, stress, and social support in young adulthood: Does the source of support matter? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45(3), 568–580. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0395-9Epub
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0395-9Epub
  • Lloyd, D. M., Gillis, V., Lewis, E., Farrell, M. J., & Morrison, I. (2013). Pleasant touch moderates the subjective but not objective aspects of body perception. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 207. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00207
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00207
  • Löken, L. S., Wessberg, J., Morrison, I., McGlone, F., & Olausson, H. (2009). Coding of pleasant touch by unmyelinated afferents in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 547–548. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2312
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2312
  • Mari, J. J., & Williams, P. (1986). A validity study of a psychiatric screening questionnaire (SRQ-20) in primary care in the city of São Paulo. The British Journal of Psychiatry: the Journal of Mental Science, 148, 23–26.
  • Maturana, H., & Verden-Zöller, G. (2008). The origin of humanness. In P. Bunnell (Ed.), The biology of love Exeter: Imprint Academic.
  • Morrison, I., Löken, L. S., & Olausson, H. (2010). The skin as a social organ. Experimental Brain Research, 204, 305–314. https://doi.org/101007/s00221-009-2007-y
    » https://doi.org/101007/s00221-009-2007-y
  • Mota, B. E., Rodrigues, F., Lacerda, David, I. A., Volchan, E., Campagnoli, R. R., & Souza, G. G. L. (2021). Pictures of social interaction prompt a sustained increase of the smile expression and induce sociability. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 5518. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-84880-9
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-84880-9
  • Muhtadie, L., Akinola, M., Koslov, K., & Mendes, W. B. (2015). Vagal flexibility: A physiological predictor of social sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 106–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000016
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000016
  • Mushtaq, R., Shoib, S., Shah, T., & Mushtaq, S. (2014). Relationship between loneliness, psychiatric disorders and physical health? A review on the psychological aspects of loneliness. Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research, 8(9), 1–4. https://doi.org/10.7860/JCDR/2014/10077.4828
    » https://doi.org/10.7860/JCDR/2014/10077.4828
  • Muyan, M., Chang, E. C., Jilani, Z., Yu, T., Lin, J., & Hirsch, J. K. (2016). Loneliness and negative affective conditions in adults: Is there any room for hope in predicting anxiety and depressive sympoms? The Journal of Psychology, 150(3), 333–341. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2015.1039474
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2015.1039474
  • Nelson, H., & Geher, G. (2007). Mutual grooming in human dyadic relationships: An ethological perspective. Current Psychology, 26, 121–140. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-007-9009-3
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-007-9009-3
  • Neto, F. (1989). Avaliação da Solidão. Revista de Psicologia Clínica, 2, 65–79.
  • Pentillä, J., Helminen, A., Jarti, T., Kuusela, T., Huikuri, H. V., & Tulppo, M. P. (2001). Time domain, geometrical and frequency domain analysis of cardiac vagal outflow: Effects of various respiratory patterns. Clinical Physiology, 21(3), 365–376. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2281.2001.00337.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2281.2001.00337.x
  • Perakakis, P., Joffily, M., Taylor, M., Guerra, P., & Vila, J. (2010). KARDIA: A Matlab software for the analysis of cardiac interbeat intervals. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 98(1), 83–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmpb.2009.10.002
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmpb.2009.10.002
  • Pierce, G. R., Sarason, I. G., & Sarason, B. R. (1991). General and relationship-based perceptions of social support: Are two constructs better than one? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(6), 1028–1039. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.6.1028
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.6.1028
  • Porges, S. W. (2007). The polyvagal perspective. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 116–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009
  • Roddick, C. M., & Chen, F. S. (2020). Effects of chronic and state loneliness on heart rate variability in women. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 55(5), 460–475.
  • Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. E. (1980). The revised UCLA loneliness scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 472–480. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx036
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx036
  • Sammito, S., & Böckelmann, I. (2016). Factors influencing heart rate variability. International Cardiovascular Forum Journal, 6, 242. https://doi.org/10.17987/icfj.v6i0.242
    » https://doi.org/10.17987/icfj.v6i0.242
  • Saporta, N., Peled-Avron, L., Scheele, D., Lieberz, J., Hurlemann, R., Shamay- Tsoory, S.G. (2022). Touched by loneliness - how loneliness impacts the response to observed human touch: a tDCS study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 17(1), 142–150. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab122
    » https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsab122
  • Saporta, N., Scheele, D., Lieberz, J., Stuhr-Wulff, F., Hurlemann, R., & Shamay- Tsoory, S. G. (2021). Opposing association of situational and chronic loneliness with interpersonal distance. Brain. Science., 11(9), 1135. https://doiorg/10.3390/brainsci11091135
    » https://doiorg/10.3390/brainsci11091135
  • Schmitt, J. P., & Kurdek, L. A. (1985). Age and gender differences in and personality correlates of loneliness in different relationships. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(5), 485–496. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4905_5
    » https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4905_5
  • Servant, D., Logier, R., Mouster, Y., & G. M. (2008). Heart rate variability. Applications in Psychiatry. Encephale., 35(5), 423–428. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.encep.2008.06.016
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.encep.2008.06.016
  • Shaffer, F., & Ginsberg, J. P. (2017). An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Frontiers in Public Health, 5, 258. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258
  • Shaffer, F., & Venner, J. (2013). Heart rate variability anatomy and physiology. Biofeedback, 41(1), 13–25. https://doi.org/10.5298/1081-5937-41.1.05
    » https://doi.org/10.5298/1081-5937-41.1.05
  • Silva, H. D., Campagnoli, R. R., Mota, B. E. F., Araújo, C. R. V., Mocaiber, I., Rego, V. R., … Souza, G. G. L. (2017). Bonding pictures: Affective ratings associated to empathy and loneliness. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1136. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01136
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01136
  • Smith, T. W., Deits-Lebehn, C., Williams, P. G., Baucom, B. R. W., & Uchino, B. N. (2020). Toward a social psychophysiology of vagally mediated heart rate variability: Concepts and methods in self-regulation, emotion, and interpersonal processes. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 14(3). https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12516
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12516
  • Souza, G. G., Pereira, M. G., Vila, J., Oliveira, L., & Volchan, E. (2012). Affiliative stimuli as primers to prosocial predispositions. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 15(1), 237–243. https://doi.org/10.5209/rev_sjop.2012.v15.n1.37315
    » https://doi.org/10.5209/rev_sjop.2012.v15.n1.37315
  • Szwarcwald, C. L., Malta, D. C., Barros, M. B. A., Júnior, P. R. B. S., Romero, D., de Almeida, W. D. S., … de Pina, M. F. (2021). Associations of sociodemographic factors and health behaviors with the emotional well-being of adolescents during the covid-19 pandemic in Brazil. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(11), 6160. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18116160
    » https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18116160
  • Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing Electrophysiology (1996). Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation, 93(5), 1043–1065.
  • Tomasello, M. (2014). The ultra-social animal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44(3), 187–194. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2015
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2015
  • Triscoli, C., Croy, I., Olausson, H., & Sailer, U. (2017). Touch between romantic partners: Being stroked is more pleasant than stroking and decelerates heart rate. Physiology & Behavior, 177, 169–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.05.006
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.05.006
  • Uvnäs-Moberg, K., & Petersson, M. (2022). Physiological effects induced by stimulation of cutaneous sensory nerves, with a focus on oxytocin. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 43, 159–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.10.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.10.001
  • Von Mohr, M., Kirsch, L. P., & Fotopoulou, A. (2017). The soothing function of touch: Affective touch reduces feelings of social exclusion. Scientific Reports, 7, 13516. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-13355-7
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-13355-7
  • Walker, S. C., & McGlone, F. P. (2013). The social brain: Neurobiological basis of affiliative behaviours and psychological well-being. Neuropeptides, 47(6), 379–393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.npep.2013.10.008
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.npep.2013.10.008
  • Walker, S. C., Trotter, P. D., Swaney, W. T., Marshall, A., & Mcglone, F. P. (2017). C-tactile: Cutaneous mediators of oxytocin release during affiliative tactile interactions afferents? Neuropeptides, 64, 27–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.npep.2017.01.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.npep.2017.01.001
  • Wang, J., Lloyd-evans, B., Marston, L., Mann, F., Ma, R., & Johnson, S. (2020). Loneliness as a predictor of outcomes in mental disorders among people who have experienced a mental health crisis: A 4- month prospective study. BMC Psychiatry, 20(1), 249. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02665-2
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02665-2
  • Weiss, R. (1973). Loneliness: The experience of emotional and social isolation Cambridge: MIT Press.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    12 Sept 2022
  • Date of issue
    2022

History

  • Received
    16 Nov 2021
  • Accepted
    30 June 2022
  • Published
    01 Aug 2022
Curso de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia da Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul Rua Ramiro Barcelos, 2600 - sala 110, 90035-003 Porto Alegre RS - Brazil, Tel.: +55 51 3308-5691 - Porto Alegre - RS - Brazil
E-mail: prc@springeropen.com