Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Consequences of anemia on language development of children from a public day care center

Abstracts

OBJECTIVE: To compare language development in anemic and non-anemic children from a public day care center in Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil. METHODS: Cross-sectional study with evaluation of language development of anemic (cases) and non-anemic children (controls) between two and six years old. All children had a digital puncture to detect anemia (hemoglobin <11.3g/dL). Cases were 22 anemic children and controls, 44 children selected by randomized paired sampling. The language development of each participant was observed and classified according to two main fields: communicative aspects (reception and emission) and cognitive aspects, based on the Child Behavior Observation Guide for children from zero to six years old. Performance rates were created in order to qualify children's answers. RESULTS: The hemoglobin values observed in case and control groups were 10.6 and 12.5g/dL, respectively. The groups did not differ regarding age, gender, breastfeeding and mother's schooling. Significant differences were observed in the language evaluation in all examined fields: levels of reception (p=0,02) and emission (p<0.001) and cognitive aspects (p <0.001), with worse performance of anemic children. CONCLUSIONS: Anemic children presented worse language development when compared to non-anemic ones. In the public health context, childhood anemia should be considered as a relevant problem due to language development alterations with possible consequences on learning abilities and future social and professional performance.

language; anemia; child day care centers; child development


OBJETIVO: Comparar o desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças anêmicas e não-anêmicas de uma creche pública de Belo Horizonte. MÉTODOS: Estudo transversal com avaliação do desenvolvimento de linguagem de crianças anêmicas (casos) e não-anêmicas (controles) entre dois e seis anos de idade. Todas as crianças realizaram punção digital para detecção da anemia (hemoglobina <11,3g/dL). O Grupo Caso foi constituído por 22 crianças anêmicas e o Controle, por 44 crianças, selecionadas por amostragem aleatória pareada. O desenvolvimento de linguagem de cada um dos participantes foi observado e classificado em duas grandes áreas: aspectos comunicativos (recepção e emissão) e aspectos cognitivos da linguagem, com utilização do Roteiro de Observação de Comportamentos de crianças de zero a seis anos. Índices de desempenho foram aplicado para qualificar as respostas das crianças. RESULTADOS: Os valores médios de hemoglobina dos Grupos Caso e Controle foram 10,6 e 12,5g/dL, respectivamente. Os grupos não diferiram quanto às seguintes variáveis: idade, gênero, aleitamento materno e escolaridade materna. Na avaliação de linguagem, observou-se uma diferença estatisticamente significante nos índices de recepção (p=0,02), emissão (p<0,001) e aspectos cognitivos da linguagem (p<0,001), com pior desempenho das crianças anêmicas. CONCLUSÕES: Crianças anêmicas apresentaram pior desenvolvimento da linguagem em comparação às não-anêmicas. A anemia deve ser considerada um problema relevante de Saúde Pública também pelas possíveis alterações que pode provocar no desenvolvimento da linguagem e, consequentemente, na aprendizagem e futuro desempenho social e profissional da criança.

linguagem; anemia; creches; desenvolvimento infantil


ORIGINAL ARTICLE

IProfessora do Curso de Fonoaudiologia da Fundação Educacional Antônio Dadalto, Vitória, ES, Brasil, Mestre em Saúde da Criança e do Adolescente pela FM-UFMG, Belo Horizonte, MG, Brasil

IIMédico especialista em Pediatra, professor de Pediatria da Faculdade de Saúde e Ecologia Humana, mestrando no Programa de Saúde da Criança e do Adolescente pela FM-UFMG, Belo Horizonte, MG, Brasil

IIIDoutora em Distúrbios da Comunicação Humana, professora adjunta do curso de graduação em Fonoaudiologia da UFMG, Belo Horizonte, MG, Brasil

IVProfessor titular do Departamento de Pediatria e Coordenador do Programa de Pós-graduação em Saúde da Criança e do Adolescente da FM-UFMG, Belo Horizonte, MG, Brasil

Corresponding author

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE:To compare language development in anemic and non-anemic children from a public day care center in Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil.

METHODS:Cross-sectional study with evaluation of language development of anemic (cases) and non-anemic children (controls) between two and six years old. All children had a digital puncture to detect anemia (hemoglobin <11.3g/dL). Cases were 22 anemic children and controls, 44 children selected by randomized paired sampling. The language development of each participant was observed and classified according to two main fields: communicative aspects (reception and emission) and cognitive aspects, based on the Child Behavior Observation Guide for children from zero to six years old. Performance rates were created in order to qualify children's answers.

RESULTS:The hemoglobin values observed in case and control groups were 10.6 and 12.5g/dL, respectively. The groups did not differ regarding age, gender, breastfeeding and mother's schooling. Significant differences were observed in the language evaluation in all examined fields: levels of reception (p=0,02) and emission (p<0.001) and cognitive aspects (p <0.001), with worse performance of anemic children.

CONCLUSIONS:Anemic children presented worse language development when compared to non-anemic ones. In the public health context, childhood anemia should be considered as a relevant problem due to language development alterations with possible consequences on learning abilities and future social and professional performance.

Key-words:language; anemia; child day care centers; child development.

Introduction

Iron deficiency anemia affects approximately 2 billion people all over the world(1). In developing countries, nutritional anemia due to iron deprivation affects more than 50% of children aged from six months to five years, and is considered to be one of the four worldwide risk factors for abnormal child development and a serious public health problem that must be combated urgently(2). In Brazil, the prevalence of anemia among preschool children varies from 30.2(3) to 68.8%(4,5). Prevalence rates among children who attend public daycare centers have been found to be considerable(4-6). A 37.5% prevalence of anemia has been observed among children from the eastern part of the city of Belo Horizonte(7).

In developing countries, iron deficiency anemia is associated with low birth weight, malnutrition, unfavorable socioeconomic conditions and high morbidity(2,8-10). Although it is infants who are at greatest risk of iron deficiency anemia resulting from rapid growth combined with exhaustion of the body's stocks of the mineral, this risk also extends to preschool and school children whose adverse living conditions and inadequate diets mean that iron deficiency is a common problem in this age group(3,10).

Anemia leads to significant damage, with repercussions primarily affecting acquisition of motor and cognitive skills, language development, and also the learning process(11-14). The manifestations of iron deprivation affect several of the body's systems. Clinical diagnosis is based on observation of signs such as cutaneous-mucosal pallor, heart murmurs, tachycardia, reduced resistance to cold, reduction in certain immune functions and delayed growth, among others, and will only be established once the child is already in an advanced stage of iron deficiency. Nevertheless, among the long-term consequences of anemia, those that affect cognitive, language, and behavior development, as well as motor coordination, have been receiving increased attention, not only because of the lower rate of suspicion that these conditions provoke, but also because of the diagnostic difficulties, the severity and the late presentation of the signs(11,15). Furthermore, the most intense period of cerebral growth and the formation of neuronal connections coincides with the period of greatest prevalence of iron deficiency anemia(16).

Several studies have identified a relationship between iron deficiency and delays in cognitive and psychomotor development in early childhood(11,13-18). A large number of studies carried out during the last decade have shown the negative effects of iron deficiency on cognitive acquisition in schoolchildren and adolescents(19,20). Anemic children over two years of age also demonstrate reduced cognitive acquisition when compared with non-anemic children, although there is an evident improvement once treatment is started(21). Research that has specifically studied auditory development in anemic patients suggests that myelination of the auditory nervous system is delayed in these patients(12).

Some studies have indicated that neurocognitive abnormalities may occur as a result of the cerebral repercussions of iron deficiency, even in the absence of anemia(19,22), which is similar to results that have been obtained with animal models. In these models, iron deficiency can lead to a reduction in the activity of iron-dependent enzymes that are necessary for the synthesis, function and breakdown of neurotransmitters, directly affecting the metabolism of neuroransmission and also leading to central nervous system myelination abnormalities(23-25).

No studies have been carried out with the Brazilian population evaluating the repercussions of anemia on the language development of children. Therefore, the objective of this investigation was to compare language development of anemic children with that of non-anemic children, all aged from two to six years of a public daycare center in the city of Belo Horizonte.

Methods

The present study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee at Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (UFMG). This was a cross-sectional study that evaluated the language development of anemic and non-anemic children aged from two to six years and enrolled at a public daycare center in the city of Belo Horizonte. The daycare center assessed is affiliated with the Belo Horizonte municipal government, is located in the eastern part of the city and is responsible for the full-time care of 139 children with low socioeconomic status.

All children at the daycare center were eligible to take part in the study. The children's parents were notified about the voluntary principle of this study, its objectives and repercussions, and also signed the consent memorandum. The parents also answered a questionnaire which requested identification data and information on the children's clinical history and development and their mothers' level of education.

Children were excluded if there was a history of prematurity, asphyxia, infection or other perinatal conditions, cognitive abnormalities, motor-based speech impediments, significant emotional disorders or neurological symptomology or if they had not undergone otoacoustic emission tests(26). Additionally, children were excluded if, on the day of the clinical examination, they showed signs of any acute disease (fever, diarrhea, vomiting, etc.), since acute and/or chronic inflammatory processes alter hemoglobin levels. The study included 132 children enrolled at the center; the guardians of all selected children signed the aforementioned consent form.

Initially, finger prick samples were taken from the children in order to determine hemoglobin levels (Hg), using a HemoCue© high-precision spectrophotometer. A microcuvette was used to obtain the precise volume of blood in contact with the exact quantity of dry reagent. The microcuvette was then inserted into the HemoCue©, which determines hemoglobin levels in 15-45 seconds(27). Children were defined as anemic if their hemoglobin levels were below 11.3g/dL (11.0g/dL + 0.3 for the variation of the HemoCue© measurement), following the recommendation of the World Health Organization(28).

Later, all children underwent auditory evaluation, consisting of the following tests: transient otoacoustic emissions (TOAE) and acoustic immittance measurements. The apparatus used for these hearing tests were as follows: a Heidji otoscope, an AUDIX I 580-AX2191 TOAE machine by Biologic and a CATZA42 middle-ear analyzer by Dicton. The TOAE machine and the middle-ear analyzer were calibrated in advance. The children underwent meatoscopy and then the TOAE test, where results were classed as pass or fail. These criteria were based on a signal/noise ratio >3 dB and reproducibility >50%(29). Immittance tests were carried out to obtain tympanometry and static compliance measurements(30). Tympanometric curves were interpreted with reference to published literature, and the range of tympanometric peak pressure values considered as normal ranged from -100 to +50 Pa(31). All procedures were conducted at the daycare center, in a silent room, in which sound pressure levels did not exceed 45 dBNPS, measured with a decibel meter, thereby avoiding the creation of artifacts or noise contamination.

After being tested for hemoglobin levels, the children were classified into two groups based on the presence (cases) or absence (controls) of anemia. The children were then examined by the principal investigator, who was blind to hemoglobin status. Language evaluation was carried out at the daycare center, in a room that was appropriate for observation, in individual sessions lasting approximately 40 minutes, and in the play area, when necessary, according to the criteria defined by Chiari et al(32), i.e., using the Behavior Observation Guide for children aged zero to six years. Language development was observed and classified according to two large areas: communicative aspects (reception and emission) and language cognitive aspects. Individual forms were used to record the behavior expected for each age range, marking yes or no depending on whether or not each item was present.

Although the instrument used is not a standardized test, but rather a protocol for observing behavior, performance indexes (PI) were created, expressed in percentages, in order to classify the results. We considered as cognitive performance index (CPI) those aspects rated by the author within the language cognitive aspects, and as reception performance index (RPI), and emission performance index (EPI) those aspects rated for language reception and emission, respectively. The PIs were analyzed with relation to the presence or absence of anemia for each age group.

Pi: performance indexes

Nba: number of behaviors analyzed

Nbno: number of behaviors analyzed

Epi-Info version 6.04 was used for data entry, processing and analysis. The descriptive analysis consisted of frequency distribution for categorical variables involved in the language analysis and measures of central tendency and dispersion for continuous variables. The following tests were used for statistical analysis: the chi-square test and Fisher's exact test were used to assess differences between proportions; analysis of variance (ANOVA for parametric variables) and the Kruskal-Wallis test (non-parametric) were used to compare continuous variables between the anemic and non-anemic groups.

Results

Twenty-seven (20.4%) of the 132 children initially enrolled in the study had hemoglobin levels compatible with anemia. Five of these were excluded because they had had abnormalities at birth (prematurity and perinatal complications) and/or abnormal TOAE findings. Therefore, the case group included 22 anemic children and the control group contained 44 children without anemia, selected by randomized sampling and paired for age and sex.

The distribution of the variables sex, age and hemoglobin level and those collected using the questionnaires (mothers' educational level, duration of breastfeeding and mother's perception of the child's learning) are shown in Table 1.

Otoacoustic emissions were present bilaterally in all children in both the case and control groups; 87% had type A tympanometric curves, indicating that the integrity of the peripheral auditory canal was not compromised. Table 2 and Figure 1 illustrate the results of the learning evaluation tests for children in the case and control groups.


Discussion

This study focused on language development from the perspective of speech and hearing. One difficulty encountered was the lack of a standardized and validated assessment instrument in Brazil appropriate for this study population. Therefore, the authors decided to use the behavior observation guide for children aged zero to six years proposed by Chiari(32) and to create three performance indexes based on the results. The CPI illustrates the acquisition of cognitive features that impact on language development; the RPI illustrates how children receive information from the external environment and their ability to understand spoken language; and the EPI provided more information on how they articulate and connect together different structural components (syntax, phonology, and semantics) and their relationship with communicative intentions - and all three indicators are of fundamental importance to their development. The choice of this instrument was also justified by the fact that behavior observation is an evaluation technique that studies language in natural situations, prompting greater richness and variety of communicative structures, whereas formal tests demand that children perform activities that are not part of their routine(33). It is also worth pointing out that the instrument most often cited in the international literature for registering the negative impact of iron deficiency anemia during childhood is the Bayley scale of child development, which is an instrument used by psychologists(11,14,21).

The children assessed in the present investigation have very similar constitutional and environmental histories. The daycare center only cares for deprived children whose parents have been means-tested. All mothers are residents of the same sociodemographic region, have a similar educational level and, from the point of view of maternal perception, their children are exposed to the same environmental factors (Table 1). All of these children showed peripheral auditory integrity and no differences in terms of sex, age, presence or duration of breastfeeding, abnormalities at birth and/or current health status. There is evidence in the literature that anemia is linked with a large number of social and economic factors, which may well affect child development in their own right(21); among these, low maternal educational level is considered a risk factor for anemia(3,9,17), as well as absent and/or reduced breastfeeding(11). Thus, the findings of this study emphasize the fact that, with relation to language problems, children with anemia may be considered vulnerable as a result of the neuromaturational physiological compromise they suffer.

Our language evaluations showed worse scores on the index of cognitive features of language in anemic children when compared with children who were not anemic. The worst performance in these tasks was observed at the ages of three, four and five years, and this difference was only not confirmed statistically in the group of children up to two years old (Table 2), which might be attributable to the small number of children tested in this age group (n=6). These findings are compatible with results found in the international literature, from studies undertaken in Israel(17), France(18), Chile(14), and Costa Rica(11,20), where cognitive development and neuropsychomotor test results were worse among anemic subjects.

The index of language reception, expressed by the RPI, when analyzed in conjunction, also differed statistically between anemic and non-anemic individuals (Table 2). Mothers of iron-deficient children, investigated using the Icelandic Parent Development Inventory, reported that their children had worse comprehension abilities(22). In anemic children, research using electrophysiological hearing evaluation showed slower conduction of the electrical stimulus(12). In our study, although all children showed integrity of the peripheral auditory canal, their hearing abilities were not investigated, which means that the possibility of abnormalities in the processing of auditory information was not ruled out.

The anemic children assessed in our study had worse language emission indexes (Table 2, p<0.05). According to different studies, anemic children from Costa Rica showed worse verbalization rates during interaction with their mothers(34), anemic Chilean children showed worse performance with communicative language and vocalization(16), and Indian children with anemia presented less social interaction than non-anemic children from the same place(35). Although no studies were found in the literature specifically investigating speech and hearing, other areas demonstrate the effects of anemia on the higher cognitive processes that make language learning possible. Studies indicate that anemia acts reducing oxygenation in the brain, altering the processes of neurotransmission and myelination(15,23-25). Furthermore, there is increased vulnerability during development in the presence of iron deficiency, which is demonstrated by abnormalities in the structure of hippocampus pyramidal cell dendrites, important components of the limbic system, which is also the seat of memory(16,25).

The prevalence of anemia in this study was 20.4%, which is lower than the figure found in other daycare centers in the Belo Horizonte area(7) and in other places in Brazil, such as Pelotas(3), Cuiabá(4), São Paulo(5), and Recife(6). This fact may be explained by the fact that this daycare center receives extra financial support for dietary supplementation, which is not the case at the majority of Brazilian daycare centers. Despite the lower prevalence of anemia and the center's suitable infrastructure, the effects of anemia on language development in the children attending this daycare center were still evident, emphasizing once more the vulnerability of these children to language disorders.

It is worth pointing out that this is a comparative cross-sectional study; therefore, a causal relationship between anemia and worse language development cannot be established. Nevertheless, statistically significant differences were observed between anemic and non-anemic children in terms of language development. It is necessary to confirm these observations with prospective studies using adequate sample sizes, with control for other variables that interfere with language development. If the hypothesis is confirmed, it will further reinforce the need for developing anemia screening programs as well as interventions aimed at fortifying food with iron for children attending daycare centers.

References

  • 1. Stoltzfus RJ. Defining iron-deficiency anemia in public health terms: a time for reflection. J Nutr 2001;131:S565-7.
  • 2. Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, Lozoff B, Wasserman GA, Pollitt E et al Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet 2007;369:145-57.
  • 3. Assunção MC, Santos Ida S, Barros AJ, Gigante DP, Victora CG. Anemia in children under six: population-based study in Pelotas, Southern Brazil. Rev Saude Publica 2007;41:328-35
  • 4. Brunken GS, Muniz PT, Silva SM. Weekly iron supplementation reduces anemia prevalence by 1/3 in preschool children. Rev Bras Epidemiol 2004;7:210-9.
  • 5. Bueno MB, Selem SS, Arêas JA, Fisberg RM. Prevalence and associated factors for anemia among children of public day-care centers in the city São Paulo. Rev Bras Epidemiol 2006;9:462-70.
  • 6. Vieira AC, Diniz AS, Cabral PC, Oliveira RS, Lóla MM, Silva SM et al Nutritional assessment of iron status and anemia in children under 5 years old at public daycare centers. J Pediatr (Rio J) 2007;83:370-6.
  • 7. Capanema FD. Anemia em crianças de 0 a 6 anos em creches conveniadas da prefeitura de Belo Horizonte - MG: aspectos clínicos e laboratoriais [tese de mestrado]. Belo Horizonte (MG): UFMG; 2002.
  • 8. Stoltzfus RJ. Iron-deficiency anemia: reexamining the nature and magnitude of the public health problem. Summary: implications for research and programs. J Nutr 2001;131(2S-2):697S-701.
  • 9. Oliveira MA, Osório MM, Raposo MC. Socioeconomic and dietary risk factors for anemia in children aged 6 to 59 months. J Pediatr (Rio J) 2007;83:39-46.
  • 10. Assunção MC, Santos IS. Effect of food fortiûcation with iron on childhood anemia: a review study. Cad Saude Publica 2007;23:269-81.
  • 11. Lozoff B, Wolf AW, Jimenez E. Iron-deficiency anemia and infant development: effects of extended oral iron therapy. J Pediatr 1996;129:382-9.
  • 12. Algarín C, Peirano P, Garrido M, Pizarro F, Lozoff B. Iron deficiency anemia in infancy: long-lasting effects on auditory and visual system functioning. Pediatr Res 2003;53:217-23.
  • 13. McCann JC, Ames BN. An overview of evidence for a causal relation between iron deficiency during development and deficits in cognitive or behavioral function. Am J Clin Nutr 2007;85:931-45.
  • 14. Walter T, De Andraca I, Chadud P, Perales CG. Iron deficiency anemia: adverse effects on infant psychomotor development. Pediatrics 1989;84:7-17.
  • 15. Lozoff B, Beard J, Connor J, Felt B, Georgieff M, Schallert T. Long-Lasting neural and behavioral effects of iron deficiency in infancy. Nutr Rev 2006;64:S34-91.
  • 16. Lozoff B, De Andraca I, Castillo M, Smith JB, Walter T, Pino P. Behavioral and developmental effects of preventing iron-deficiency anemia in healthy full-term infants. Pediatrics 2003;112:846-54.
  • 17. Palti H, Meijer A, Adler B. Learning achievement and behavior at school of anemic and non-anemic infants. Early Hum Dev 1985;10:217-23.
  • 18. Dommergues JP, Archambeaud MP, Ducot B, Gerval Y, Hiard C, Rossignol C et al Iron deficiency and psychomotor development tests. Longitudinal study between 10 months and 4 years of age. Arch Fr Pediatr 1989;46:487-90.
  • 19. Halterman JS, Kaczorowski JM, Aligne CA, Auinger P, Szilagyi PG. Iron deficiency and cognitive achievement among school-aged children and adolescents in the United States. Pediatrics 2001;107:1381-6.
  • 20. Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Smith JB. Double burden of iron deficiency in infancy and low socioeconomic status: a longitudinal analysis of cognitive test scores to age 19 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2006;160:1108-13.
  • 21. Grantham-McGregor S, Ani C. A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children. J Nutr 2001;131:649S-68.
  • 22. Gunnarsson BS, Thorsdottir I, Palsson G, Gretarsson SJ. Iron status at 1 and 6 years versus developmental scores at 6 years in a well-nourished affluent population. Acta Paediatr 2007;96:391-5.
  • 23. Felt BT, Beard JL, Schallert T, Shao J, Aldridge JW, Connor JR et al Persistent neurochemical and behavioral abnormalities in adulthood despite early iron supplementation for perinatal iron deficiency anemia in rats. Behav Brain Res 2006;171:261-70.
  • 24. Unger EL, Paul T, Murray-Kolb LE, Felt B, Jones BC, Beard JL. Early iron deficiency alters sensorimotor development and brain monoamines in rats. J Nutr 2007;137:118-24.
  • 25. Lozoff B, Georgieff MK. Iron deficiency and brain development. Semin Pediatr Neurol 2006;13:158-65.
  • 26. Tomblin JB, Smith E, Zhang X. Epidemiology of specific language impairment: prenatal and perinatal risk factors. J Commun Disord 1997;30:325-44.
  • 27. Von Schenck H, Falkensson M, Lundberg B. Evaluation of "HemoCue," a new device for determining hemoglobin. Clin Chem 1986;32:526-9.
  • 28. DeMaeyer EM, Dallman P, Gurney JM, Hallberg L, Sood SK, Srikantia SG. Preventing and controlling iron deficiency anaemia through primary health care. A guide for health administrators and programme managers. Genebra: World Health Organization; 1989. p. 8-10.
  • 29. Martin GK, Probst RM, Lonsbury-Martin BL. Otoacoustic emissions in human ears: normative findings. Ear and Hearing 1990;11:106-20.
  • 30. Chaves AD, Gatto CI, Tomazzetti CT, Rossi DB, Malheiros MA, Aita FS et al Estudo das medidas de imitância acústica em pré-escolares. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol 1998;4:29-33.
  • 31. Jerger J. Clinical experience with impedance audiometry. Arch Otolaryngol 1970;92:311-24.
  • 32. Chiari BM, Basílio CS, Nakagwa EA, Cormedi MA, Silva NS, Cardoso RM et al Proposta de sistematização de dados da avaliação fonoaudiológica através da observação de comportamentos de criança de 0-6 anos. Pró-Fono 1991;3:29-36.
  • 33. Hewitt LE, Hammer CS, Yont KM, Tomblin JB. Language sampling for kindergarten children with and without SLI: mean length of utterance, IPSYN, and NDW. J Commun Disord 2005;38:197-213.
  • 34. Corapci F, Radan AE, Lozoff B. Iron deficiency in infancy and mother-child interaction at 5 years. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2006;27:371-8.
  • 35. Lozoff B, Corapci F, Burden MJ, Kaciroti N, Angulo-Barroso R, Sazawal S et al Preschool-aged children with iron deficiency anemia show altered affect and behavior. J Nutr 2007;137:683-9.
  • Consequences of anemia on language development of children from a public day care center

    Juliana Nunes SantosI; Silmar Paulo M. RatesII; Stela Maris A. LemosIII; Joel Alves LamounierIV
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      27 Mar 2009
    • Date of issue
      Mar 2009

    History

    • Received
      07 May 2008
    • Accepted
      01 Oct 2008
    Sociedade de Pediatria de São Paulo R. Maria Figueiredo, 595 - 10o andar, 04002-003 São Paulo - SP - Brasil, Tel./Fax: (11 55) 3284-0308; 3289-9809; 3284-0051 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: rpp@spsp.org.br