Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Cognitive assessment in preterms by Bayley-III: development in the first year and associated factors

Avaliação cognitiva em crianças pré-termo usando Bayley-III: desenvolvimento no primeiro ano de vida e fatores associados

ABSTRACT

Objective:

To analyze the cognitive development of preterm infants at six and 12 months of corrected age and the associations with perinatal and socioeconomic factors.

Methods:

Cognitive development of 40 infants (20 preterm and 20 full-term) at six and 12 months of age was evaluated using the Bayley-III scale. Correlations between cognitive outcome and associated factors were assessed using Spearman correlation. Stepwise multiple linear regression analysis with covariance was applied to identify changes on cognitive score between six and 12 months.

Results:

Bayley-III cognitive score in preterm group was significantly lower than in full-term group at both six and 12 months of age. Birth weight correlated with cognitive performance at six months and head circumference at birth at 12 months, in full-terms infants. The occurrence of necrotizing enterocolitis was inversely associated with cognitive score in preterms at 12 months. An increase in cognitive score was observed between six and 12 months in both groups, but the gain was more pronounced in preterms.

Conclusions:

These findings suggest some cognitive recovery capacity in the first year despite the restrictions imposed by premature birth and emphasize the importance of early interventions in this population.

Keywords:
Infant premature; Child development; Cognition; Cognition disorders

RESUMO

Objetivo:

Avaliar o desenvolvimento cognitivo de crianças pré-termo aos seis e 12 meses de idade corrigida e as associações com fatores perinatais e socioeconômicos.

Métodos:

O desenvolvimento cognitivo de 40 crianças (20 pré-termo e 20 a termo) foi avaliado aos seis e 12 meses de idade, utilizando a escala Bayley-III. Correlações entre resultados cognitivos e fatores associados foram avaliadas pelo teste de correlação de Spearman. A análise de regressão linear múltipla stepwise com covariância foi aplicada para identificar mudanças na pontuação cognitiva entre seis e 12 meses.

Resultados:

O escore cognitivo no grupo pré-termo foi significativamente menor que no grupo a termo aos seis e 12 meses. O peso ao nascer foi diretamente associado com o desempenho cognitivo aos seis meses e perímetro cefálico ao nascimento aos 12 meses, nas crianças a termo. A ocorrência de enterocolite necrosante foi inversamente associada ao desempenho cognitivo em pré-termos, aos 12 meses. Verificou-se aumento na pontuação cognitiva entre seis e 12 meses nos dois grupos, porém mais pronunciado no pré-termo.

Conclusões:

O estudo sugere que crianças pré-termo apresentam alguma capacidade de recuperação cognitiva no primeiro ano, apesar das restrições impostas pelo nascimento prematuro, e enfatizam a importância de acompanhamento dessa população desde os primeiros meses de vida.

Palavras-chave:
Prematuro; Desenvolvimento infantil; Cognição; Transtornos cognitivos

INTRODUCTION

Preterm-born children are a high-risk population for neurodevelopment delay and cognitive impairment, of which cognitive deficiencies are more frequent.11 Allotey J, Zamora J, Cheong-See F, Kalidindi M, Arroyo-Manzano D, Asztalos E, et al. Cognitive, motor, behavioural and academic performances of children born preterm: a meta-analysis and systematic review involving 64 061 children. BJOG. 2018;125:16-25. https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-0528.14832
https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-0528.14832...
,22 Stålnacke SR, Tessma M, Böhm B, Herlenius E. Cognitive development trajectories in preterm children with very low birth weight longitudinally followed until 11 years of age. Front Physiol. 2019;10:307. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2019.00307
https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2019.00307...
Developmental delays and differences in preterm, compared with full-term infants, are clearly seen within the first year of life.33 Valentini NC, Borba LS, Panceri C, Smith BA, Procianoy RS, Silveira RC. Early detection of cognitive, language, and motor delays for low-income preterm infants: a Brazilian cohort longitudinal study on infant neurodevelopment and maternal practice. Front Psychol. 2021;12:753551. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.753551
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.75355...

This setting becomes even more relevant if we consider that the number of preterm births and the survival rates for preterm infants have increased, however, without a reduction in the prevalence of morbidities, including neuropsychomotor alterations after hospital discharge.33 Valentini NC, Borba LS, Panceri C, Smith BA, Procianoy RS, Silveira RC. Early detection of cognitive, language, and motor delays for low-income preterm infants: a Brazilian cohort longitudinal study on infant neurodevelopment and maternal practice. Front Psychol. 2021;12:753551. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.753551
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.75355...
,44 Pinto F, Fernandes E, Virella D, Abrantes A, Neto MT. Born preterm: a public health issue. Port J Public Health. 2019;37:38-49. https://doi.org/10.1159/000497249
https://doi.org/10.1159/000497249...

It is recognized that development of cognitive skills is influenced by biological risk factors associated with prematurity, as well as by socioeconomic and environmental factors.55 Panceri C, Valentini NC, Silveira RC, Smith BA, Procianoy RS. Neonatal adverse outcomes, neonatal birth risks, and socioeconomic status: combined influence on preterm infants’ cognitive, language, and motor development in Brazil. J Child Neurol. 2020;35:989-98. https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073820946206
https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073820946206...
This highlights the importance of investigating the cognitive development of preterm children in different socioeconomic contexts and environments.

Although previous studies reinforce the interdependent and cumulative action of biological, socioeconomic, and environmental risk factors for unfavorable cognitive outcomes,55 Panceri C, Valentini NC, Silveira RC, Smith BA, Procianoy RS. Neonatal adverse outcomes, neonatal birth risks, and socioeconomic status: combined influence on preterm infants’ cognitive, language, and motor development in Brazil. J Child Neurol. 2020;35:989-98. https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073820946206
https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073820946206...
,66 Cheng ER, Poehlmann-Tynan J, Mullahy J, Witt WP. Cumulative social risk exposure, infant birth weight, and cognitive delay in infancy. Acad Pediatr. 2014;14:581-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2014.03.014
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2014.03.0...
knowledge concerning the cognitive development of preterm infants within the first year of life and living in developing countries, as well as associated factors, is still insufficient. This context becomes even more relevant if we consider that preterm birth rates have increased worldwide and that most preterm births occur in underdeveloped countries. According to this scenario, Brazil is among the ten countries in the world with the highest number of preterm births, with about 320,000 per year,77 Brazil. Ministério da Saúde. Portal da Saúde [homepage on the Internet]. Datasus: Estatísticas vitais [cited 2021 Nov 14]. Available from: https://datasus.saude.gov.br/estatisticas-vitais/
https://datasus.saude.gov.br/estatistica...
which makes Brazilian preterm children a population that should be investigated. Knowing the trajectory of early cognitive development and its associated factors in these children is important to devise better strategies for identifying changes and early intervention.

Therefore, the present study aimed to evaluate cognitive development of premature infants in two moments — at six and 12 months of corrected age — and to examine perinatal and socioeconomic factors associated with the best cognitive trajectories during the first year of life.

METHOD

This was a longitudinal study involving a cohort of infants born between September 2013 and August 2015. The cognitive development of preterm and full-term infants was assessed using the Bayley-III cognitive scale at the ages of six and 12 months. For preterm infants, we considered their corrected age (CA). The study was in accordance with the Helsinki declaration. All parents signed the informed consent form and the study was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Minas Gerais (UFMG), Brazil, under the protocol number 369.883 and Certificate of Presentation of Ethical Appreciation (Certificado de Apresentação de Apreciação Ética — CAAE: number 12213813.8.0000.5149).

Forty infants participated in this study, allocated into two groups: preterm and full-term. The children from both groups were born between 2013 and 2015 in the maternity unit at the Hospital das Clínicas of the Federal University of Minas Gerais (HC-UFMG). All participants resided in the metropolitan capital region of Belo Horizonte, Minas Gerais, Brazil. They came from low-income families and received free, universal healthcare services by the national public healthcare service. The preterm group included 20 infants recruited from the institution's program for high-risk children called ACRIAR. The ACRIAR is a follow-up service that assists infants born in the maternity unit of HC-UFMG with less than 34 weeks of gestational age or less than 1,500 g. At this service, children receive care from a multidisciplinary team comprised of pediatric doctors, nurses, physiotherapists, occupational therapists, speech therapists, and neurologists. The follow-up is carried out from hospital discharge up to seven years of age according to the following schedule: 1, 2, 4, 6, 8, 12, 18, and 24 months of age, and then annually until the age of seven. Preterm children were recruited from ACRIAR after hospital discharge. The medical records of all children admitted to the referred follow-up service during the study period were analyzed to comply with inclusion and exclusion criteria. Children who met the inclusion criteria were personally invited by one of the researchers who spoke with their parents or legal guardians about the first days of attendance during the follow-up service. Infants with severe sensory impairments (blindness, deafness), cerebral palsy, genetic syndromes, congenital abnormalities, congenital infections, symptomatic congenital heart defects, an Apgar score less than 7 at 5 minutes, muscle tone alterations, intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH) grades III and IV, and periventricular leukomalacia (PVL) were excluded. In order to define these criteria, in addition to the clinical information present in the medical records, the results of the following diagnostic tests conducted in the neonatal period were used: I) For hearing impairment, the result of the Neonatal Hearing Screening performed through the Evoked Otoacoustic Emissions Test (OAE) in neonates at low risk for hearing impairment, or the Brain Evoked Response Audiometry test (BERA) in cases of neonates who were considered at high risk for hearing impairment; II) For visual impairment, the result of the fundoscopic exam performed by an ophthalmologist trained in premature infants’ eye mapping examination; III) For IVH and PVL, the results of transfontanellar ultrasound performed at the corrected age of 40 weeks and conducted according to routine service; IV) For cerebral palsy and changes in muscle tone, results from the clinical records of pediatric neurology visits.

The full-term group included 20 infants born with more than 37 weeks of gestational age and without any known medical diagnosis or atypical development. These children were born in the same maternity hospital and during the same period than the preterm group. The parents were invited in person by one of the researchers during the hospitalization in the maternity ward. Families who consented to participate were asked to provide a telephone contact in addition to signing the consent form, so that the six- and 12-month evaluations could be scheduled. All infants in the control group also underwent transfontanellar ultrasound to exclude cases of brain morphological abnormalities. This examination was performed by the same pediatric neurologist before the children were discharged from the maternity ward.

Cognitive development was assessed using the Bayley-III scale in free-distraction environment. This scale consists of 91 items that assess the sensorimotor development, exploitation and manipulation, relationship between objects, concept formation, and memory. For analysis purposes, the raw scores were converted into composite scores. The Bayley-III was shown to be a valid tool both in research and in clinical practice with satisfactory reliability and validity.88 Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development. 3rd ed. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment; 2014. The Bayley-III test-retest reliability range from 0.67 to 0.94, internal consistency coefficients (using the split half method) of 0.87 to 0.93, with moderate to high correlations and measures of similar domains. Each composite score had a mean of 100 points and a standard deviation (SD) of 15 points.88 Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development. 3rd ed. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment; 2014. All evaluations were performed by the same researcher, who was previously trained. The assessment was done during a consultation scheduled for this purpose.

The definition of perinatal clinical data and socioeconomic characteristics included in this study was based on the results of a research about factors associated with the cognitive development until the age of five. Socioeconomic data included socioeconomic level and maternal paid employment. Perinatal factors included maternal age, cesarean section, antenatal corticosteroids, gestational age, birth weight, head circumference at birth, small for gestational age, male gender, Apgar score at 5 minutes, days in the neonatal unit, sepsis, retinopathy of prematurity (ROP), intraventricular hemorrhage (IVH) grade I or II, hyaline membrane disease (HMD), bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD), invasive mechanical ventilation time (IMVT) and necrotizing enterocolitis (NEC).

Perinatal clinical data were obtained from hospital records and entered on recorded forms. The socioeconomic data were obtained through interviews with the parents and through the Brazilian Criterion of Economic Classification (CCEB).99 Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa [homepage on the Internet]. Critério de classificação econômica Brasil 2015 [cited 2017 Feb 02]. Available from: https://www.abep.org/Servicos/Download.aspx?id=09
https://www.abep.org/Servicos/Download.a...
Briefly, the CCEB is an economic segmentation tool that uses household characteristics (presence and quantity of some household items of comfort and education degree of the household head) to classify population according to average monthly income (in US$) into six groups: A ($3,523 or higher), B1 ($1,917), B2 ($995), C1 ($595), C2 ($407) and D/E (until $285).

The sample size was estimated from a pilot study with 14 full-term and 14 preterm infants, with the same characteristics as the study participants. The statistical power was calculated using the software G*Power 3.1.9.2 to compare independent sample means. The total sample requirement was estimated at 40 infants (20 in each group). In order to reach this number, we assumed an alpha error of 0.05, a power study of 95%, and an effect size of 1.24 (using Cohen's d test) to detect differences in average cognitive score between groups.

We analyzed the results using IBM Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) 17.0, and the significance level was established at p<0.05. The descriptive statistical variables were presented as means±SD or percentages. The numerical variables were compared through the Mann-Whitney U test for non-parametric data and Student's t-test for independent samples for parametric data. Categorical variables were compared by the Pearson's chi-square test. The Bayley-III cognitive score at six and 12 months was compared using independent samples t-test.

To analyze the change in cognitive scores from six to 12 months, we used the t-test for repeated samples. The correlations between cognitive score and socioeconomic and perinatal characteristics were established from Spearman correlation. In trying to understand the predictors to this evolution in cognitive scores observed in infants at six and 12 months, we applied stepwise multiple linear regression. Cognitive scores at 12 months were set as the dependent variables, while the cognitive score at six months were set as independent variables (sociodemographic and clinical variables, if significantly correlated).

RESULTS

At six months of age, a cognitive assessment of 54 infants (25 preterm and 29 term) was performed. Among these, 40 (20 preterm and 20 term) returned for evaluation at 12 months. No differences were observed between infants who remained in the study and those who missed follow-up in terms of gestational age (p=0.593), weight at birth (p=0.521), sex (p=0.357); socioeconomic class (p=0.833), and Bayley-III cognitive score (p=0.334).

Descriptive statistics results are demonstrated in Table 1 and include infants who participated in all assessments at both ages. According to cognitive development results, the full-term group presented a significantly higher Bayley-III cognitive score than the preterm group at six and 12 months of age (p=0.002 and p=0.024, respectively).

Table 1
Comparison of perinatal, socioeconomic and cognitive development characteristics of preterm and full-term infants.

In addition to the characteristics described in Table 1, the children were also evaluated for clinical problems and hospitalizations in their first year. In the preterm group, there were four hospitalizations, with an average duration of 14.7 days (ranging from eight to 19 days), all due to bronchiolitis. In the term group, there were four hospitalizations, with an average duration of nine days (ranging from two to 18 days), for the following reasons: bronchiolitis (2), laryngitis (1), and skin infection (1).

Changes in cognitive scores from six to 12 months are presented in Table 2. We found that, for the total sample, infants showed a statistically significant increase in cognitive score between six and 12 months, and this gain is, on average, 6.1 points (p=0.008). Analyzing groups separately, we observed that the preterm group showed an even greater increase in cognitive scores, with a gain of 7.85 points (p=0.019). The control group had 4.85 points on cognitive score, despite of this gain, it was not considered statistically significant (0.190).

Table 2
Evolution of cognitive development from 6 to 12 months in preterm and full-term infants.

The association between cognitive development and socioeconomic and perinatal characteristics were also tested. The results are shown in Table 3.

Table 3
Correlation between cognitive development and socioeconomic and perinatal characteristics by group.

Table 4 presents the results of the multiple linear regression analysis. Based on the correlation analysis, we added birth weight, presence of necrotizing enterocolitis and head circumference at birth as independent variables along the cognitive development at six months of age. We observed a statistically significant model (p=0.002) where only the cognitive score at six months of age was a significant predictor of the cognitive score at 12 months of age, explaining 23% of model variance. For each unit added to the cognitive score at six months, there was an increase in the cognitive score of 0.445 units at 12 months of age.

Table 4
Stepwise multiple linear regression analysis for cognitive development covariate at 12 months for cognitive development at six months.

DISCUSSION

This study analyzed cognitive performance and associated factors in preterm and full-term infants at an early stage of neurodevelopment. Considering that this period is marked by the emergence of several cognitive and emotional competences,1010 Rose SA, Feldman JF, Jankowski JJ, Van Rossem R. A cognitive cascade in infancy: pathways from prematurity to later mental development. Intelligence. 2008;36:367-78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.07.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.07...
,1111 Carmo AL, Fredo FW, Bruck I, Lima JR, Janke RN, Fogaça TG, et al. Avaliação neurológica, cognitiva e de aprendizagem de escolares nascidos prematuros. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2022;40:e2020252. http://doi.org/10.1590/1984-0462/2022/40/2020252
http://doi.org/10.1590/1984-0462/2022/40...
it is important to improve our understanding of the cognitive development and the risk factors associated with prematurity in this period. Furthermore, previous studies showed that preterm infants exhibited poorer cognitive performance compared to full-terms.1212 Verreschi MQ, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Krebs VL, Carvalho WB, Befi-Lopes DM. Pré-escolares nascidos prematuros apresentam desempenho adequado em vocabulário expressivo e memória de curto prazo verbal? CoDAS. 2020;32:e20180107. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20192018107
https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/201920...
,1313 Breeman LD, Jaekel J, Baumann N, Bartmann P, Wolke D. Preterm cognitive function into adulthood. Pediatrics. 2015;136:415-23. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-0608
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-0608...

In this study, preterm group infants demonstrated a lower cognitive score compared to the full-term group at six and 12 months. However, the cognitive scores of preterm infants were within the normal range of the Bayley-III scale, even with a lower average compared to their controls. These results must be analyzed taking into account that the participants included in this study were infants that had a reduced biological risk, considering their mean gestational age and birth weight and that the several risk factors for delays in cognitive development were excluded. Therefore, we emphasize that, although this group of infants had a relatively low, biological vulnerability, differences in their cognitive scores were observed at a very early age. This becomes apparent, especially if we consider that during the first year of life, cognitive development mainly involves skills related to more autonomic functioning, such as sensory learning, perceptual-motor integration, and attention span.1414 Fenoglio A, Georgieff MK, Elison JT. Social brain circuitry and social cognition in infants born preterm. J Neurodev Disord. 2017;9:27. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-017-9206-9
https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-017-9206-...
It is only around two years of age (when the period of maturation of attention mechanisms begins and the development of more complex cognitive skills are observed) that cognitive alterations become more frequently identified.1515 Feldman R, Eidelman AI. Biological and environmental initial conditions shape the trajectories of cognitive and social-emotional development across the first years of life. Dev Sci. 2009;12:194-200. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00761.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008...

In addition, we evaluated a group of moderately premature infants, whose cognitive outcome is not as explored in research, since most studies investigate the development of infants with a history of extreme prematurity. However, evidence indicates that even these infants, considered to be at lower biological risk, seem to have a higher incidence of school problems, cognitive impairment, behavioral disorders and psychiatric disorders when compared to infants born at term.1616 Cheong JL, Doyle LW, Burnett AC, Lee KJ, Walsh JM, Potter CR, et al. Association between moderate and late preterm birth and neurodevelopment and social-emotional development at age 2 years. JAMA Pediatr. 2017;171:e164805. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.4805
https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2...
,1717 Jong M, Verhoeven M, van Baar AL. School outcome, cognitive functioning, and behaviour problems in moderate and late preterm children and adults: a review. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2012;17:163-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2012.02.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2012.02.0...
These deficits appear to remain throughout childhood and could contribute to altered neurodevelopmental trajectories in school age and into adolescence.1313 Breeman LD, Jaekel J, Baumann N, Bartmann P, Wolke D. Preterm cognitive function into adulthood. Pediatrics. 2015;136:415-23. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-0608
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-0608...
,1818 Cheong JL, Spittle AJ, Burnett AC, Anderson PJ, Doyle LW. Have outcomes following extremely preterm birth improved over time? Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2020;25:101114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2020.101114
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2020.1011...
,1919 Luu TM, Mian MO, Nuyt AM. Long-term impact of preterm birth: neurodevelopmental and physical health outcomes. Clin Perinatol. 2017;44:305-14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clp.2017.01.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clp.2017.01.00...
Thus, exploring the cognitive performance of preterm infants in early stages of child development can be useful to support the decision for inclusion in early monitoring and intervention programs.

Considering the clinical factors associated with cognitive development, we observed that head circumference at birth at six months and birth weight at 12 months were directly correlated with the cognitive development of the control group infants. Anthropometric characteristics such as birth weight and head circumference are mentioned in the literature as possible factors associated with neurological development, both in the first year of life2020 Eickmann SH, Malkes NF, Lima MC. Psychomotor development of preterm infants aged 6 to 12 months. São Paulo Med J. 2012;130:299-306. https://doi.org/10.1590/s1516-31802012000500006
https://doi.org/10.1590/s1516-3180201200...
and at school age.2121 Ream AM, Lehwald L. Neurologic consequences of preterm birth. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2018;18:48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-018-0862-2
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-018-0862-...

In the preterm group, NEC was inversely associated with cognitive development at 12 months. It should be noted that the history in the neonatal unit and potential complications during hospitalization are associated with adverse cognitive outcomes. Surviving preterm infants with a history of NEC stage II or higher are at risk for long-term neurodevelopmental impairment.2222 Imren C, Vlug LE, Koning BA, Diertens T, Snel HE, Suurland J, et al. Necrotizing enterocolitis in a dutch cohort of very preterm infants: prevalence, mortality, and long-term outcomes. Eur J Pediatr Surg. 2022;32:111-9. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0041-1741544
https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0041-1741544...

At six months, we observed that the control group was significantly older than the preterm group, with a difference of 11 days, on average. However, as the Bayley-III composite score was pre-adjusted for age and the infants were tested to their maximum level, we determined that this difference did not affect the interpretation of the results.2323 Evensen KA, Skranes J, Brubakk AM, Vik T. Predictive value of early motor evaluation in preterm very low birth weight and term small for gestational age children. Early Hum Dev. 2009;85:511-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2009.04.007
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.200...

We also investigated changes in cognitive performance between six and 12 months and found that both groups demonstrated an increase in cognitive scores. However, this increase was statistically significant in the total sample and in the preterm group. This suggests that the increase observed in the cognitive score between six and 12 months of age seems to be a phenomenon more related to the preterm group as a kind of “catch-up” during the first year of life. Cognitive development in the full-term group appears to be more stable during this period. It is important to note that, despite the preterm group showing a more significant gain in cognitive scores between six and 12 months, the average score in the preterm group at 12 months continued to be lower than that of full-term infants at the same age. These results are in agreement with previous studies that reported improvement in cognitive test scores in children with a history of prematurity during early childhood.2424 Romeo DM, Ricci M, Mirra F, Venezia I, Mallardi M, Pede E, et al. Longitudinal cognitive assessment in low-risk very preterm infants. Medicina (Kaunas). 2022;58:133. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58010133
https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58010133...
,2525 Sansavini A, Pentimonti J, Justice L, Guarini A, Savini S, Alessandroni R, et al. Language, motor and cognitive development of extremely preterm children: modeling individual growth trajectories over the first three years of life. J Commun Disord. 2014;49:55-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.02.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.0...

One possibility for this more pronounced cognitive gain in the preterm group may be a compensatory developmental trajectory.2525 Sansavini A, Pentimonti J, Justice L, Guarini A, Savini S, Alessandroni R, et al. Language, motor and cognitive development of extremely preterm children: modeling individual growth trajectories over the first three years of life. J Commun Disord. 2014;49:55-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.02.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.0...
In preterm infants, this process can be even more pronounced, since the head circumference “catch-up” seems to occur during the first year of life and this process has been associated with better developmental outcomes, as it is directly correlated with increased brain volume.2121 Ream AM, Lehwald L. Neurologic consequences of preterm birth. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2018;18:48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-018-0862-2
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-018-0862-...
It is important to consider that the rates of brain growth and development during the first year of life are very high, which cannot be comparable to any other time of postnatal development.2626 Hadders-Algra M. Early human brain development: starring the subplate. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;92:276-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.017
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018...
Post-mortem studies and neuroimaging studies have shown that at around 12 months of age, infants reach maximum synaptic density and the pattern of glucose utilization in an infant at this age resembles that of an adult2626 Hadders-Algra M. Early human brain development: starring the subplate. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;92:276-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.017
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018...
, which coincides with an improvement in children's cognitive and behavioral performance.2727 Chugani HT. A critical period of brain development: studies of cerebral glucose utilization with PET. Prev Med. 1998;27:184-8. https://doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1998.0274
https://doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1998.0274...
In this scenario, the more pronounced gain in cognitive performance observed in the preterm group may suggest the development of alternative compensatory neural networks in response to an injury to the preterm infant's brain, which arise during development and appear to preserve global function through plasticity.2828 DeMaster D, Bick J, Johnson U, Montroy JJ, Landry S, Duncan AF. Nurturing the preterm infant brain: leveraging neuroplasticity to improve neurobehavioral outcomes. Pediatr Res. 2019;85:166-75. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-018-0203-9
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-018-0203-...
In the same vein, recent long-term studies demonstrate progressive improvement over time in children with no history of IVH grade III or IV or significant white matter lesions, suggesting some capacity for recovery, which still needs to be further investigated.2929 Taylor HG. Multiple risks for long-term cognitive impairments following preterm birth. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104:218-20. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.12900
https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.12900...

Another condition that may be associated with this cognitive improvement, observed in the preterm group, was the fact that these infants regularly participated in a multidisciplinary project to monitor their growth and development and referred to the intervention program when necessary. Studies involving children born preterm, and living in urban regions of underdeveloped countries, have shown that cognitive development appears to be substantially benefited by interventions in the early stages of development, such as psychosocial stimulation and pre-preschool experiences.3030 Vasconcelos LT, Irineu ME, Santos JN, Modesto TS. Estimulação precoce multiprofissional em crianças com defasagem no desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor: revisão integrativa. Rev Pesqui Fisioter. 2019;9:284-92. https://doi.org/10.17267/2238-2704rpf.v9i2.2302
https://doi.org/10.17267/2238-2704rpf.v9...

We also tried to identify predictors for this evolution in the cognitive scores observed between six and 12 months. We found that a cognitive score at six months was the only variable correlated with cognitive performance at 12 months, explaining 23% of the variability in cognitive scores at 12 months. This finding also reinforces the importance of monitoring the cognitive development of preterm children from the early stages of development.

This study has some limitations. First, we studied a small sample size. Therefore, the number of subjects limited the number of analyzed variables associated with the target outcome when using multivariate regression. However, we reached the sample size calculated from a pilot study that included infants with the same characteristics of the studied sample and we obtained both a power study and high effect size. Therefore, we believe that the statistical analysis was not hindered by the small sample size and that the infants included in the present study were representative of the infants generally seen in public health services in our region. In addition, the data collection protocol was extensive, considering the age and attentional span of these children. Thus, the inclusion of a larger number of children than the calculated sample would imply additional costs and the submission of children to a research protocol that could be exhaustive, especially considering their young age. The study sample was, therefore, restricted to infants who suited the inclusion criteria and who attended this service during the study period. Furthermore, we evaluated only two age groups: six and 12 months, which limited our inferences and trajectory designs. Definitively, more research is needed, including more neurodevelopment assessments.

In conclusion, preterm infants demonstrated poorer cognitive performance than full-term infants at six and 12 months old. Anthropometric variables and perinatal complications seemed to be associated with cognitive development at these ages. Between six and 12 months, premature infants presented greater gains in cognitive scores, suggesting some recovery capacity despite the restrictions imposed by premature birth. The results reinforce the need of early assessment of the neurodevelopment and the importance of early intervention.

  • Funding
    This study was supported by the Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa de Minas Gerais — FAPEMIG (project number 21550) which was not involved in the elaboration of this study.

Declaration

The database that originated the article is available with the corresponding author.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Allotey J, Zamora J, Cheong-See F, Kalidindi M, Arroyo-Manzano D, Asztalos E, et al. Cognitive, motor, behavioural and academic performances of children born preterm: a meta-analysis and systematic review involving 64 061 children. BJOG. 2018;125:16-25. https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-0528.14832
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/1471-0528.14832
  • 2
    Stålnacke SR, Tessma M, Böhm B, Herlenius E. Cognitive development trajectories in preterm children with very low birth weight longitudinally followed until 11 years of age. Front Physiol. 2019;10:307. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2019.00307
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2019.00307
  • 3
    Valentini NC, Borba LS, Panceri C, Smith BA, Procianoy RS, Silveira RC. Early detection of cognitive, language, and motor delays for low-income preterm infants: a Brazilian cohort longitudinal study on infant neurodevelopment and maternal practice. Front Psychol. 2021;12:753551. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.753551
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.753551
  • 4
    Pinto F, Fernandes E, Virella D, Abrantes A, Neto MT. Born preterm: a public health issue. Port J Public Health. 2019;37:38-49. https://doi.org/10.1159/000497249
    » https://doi.org/10.1159/000497249
  • 5
    Panceri C, Valentini NC, Silveira RC, Smith BA, Procianoy RS. Neonatal adverse outcomes, neonatal birth risks, and socioeconomic status: combined influence on preterm infants’ cognitive, language, and motor development in Brazil. J Child Neurol. 2020;35:989-98. https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073820946206
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0883073820946206
  • 6
    Cheng ER, Poehlmann-Tynan J, Mullahy J, Witt WP. Cumulative social risk exposure, infant birth weight, and cognitive delay in infancy. Acad Pediatr. 2014;14:581-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2014.03.014
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2014.03.014
  • 7
    Brazil. Ministério da Saúde. Portal da Saúde [homepage on the Internet]. Datasus: Estatísticas vitais [cited 2021 Nov 14]. Available from: https://datasus.saude.gov.br/estatisticas-vitais/
    » https://datasus.saude.gov.br/estatisticas-vitais/
  • 8
    Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant and toddler development. 3rd ed. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment; 2014.
  • 9
    Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa [homepage on the Internet]. Critério de classificação econômica Brasil 2015 [cited 2017 Feb 02]. Available from: https://www.abep.org/Servicos/Download.aspx?id=09
    » https://www.abep.org/Servicos/Download.aspx?id=09
  • 10
    Rose SA, Feldman JF, Jankowski JJ, Van Rossem R. A cognitive cascade in infancy: pathways from prematurity to later mental development. Intelligence. 2008;36:367-78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.07.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.07.003
  • 11
    Carmo AL, Fredo FW, Bruck I, Lima JR, Janke RN, Fogaça TG, et al. Avaliação neurológica, cognitiva e de aprendizagem de escolares nascidos prematuros. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2022;40:e2020252. http://doi.org/10.1590/1984-0462/2022/40/2020252
    » http://doi.org/10.1590/1984-0462/2022/40/2020252
  • 12
    Verreschi MQ, Cáceres-Assenço AM, Krebs VL, Carvalho WB, Befi-Lopes DM. Pré-escolares nascidos prematuros apresentam desempenho adequado em vocabulário expressivo e memória de curto prazo verbal? CoDAS. 2020;32:e20180107. https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20192018107
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20192018107
  • 13
    Breeman LD, Jaekel J, Baumann N, Bartmann P, Wolke D. Preterm cognitive function into adulthood. Pediatrics. 2015;136:415-23. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-0608
    » https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-0608
  • 14
    Fenoglio A, Georgieff MK, Elison JT. Social brain circuitry and social cognition in infants born preterm. J Neurodev Disord. 2017;9:27. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-017-9206-9
    » https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-017-9206-9
  • 15
    Feldman R, Eidelman AI. Biological and environmental initial conditions shape the trajectories of cognitive and social-emotional development across the first years of life. Dev Sci. 2009;12:194-200. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00761.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00761.x
  • 16
    Cheong JL, Doyle LW, Burnett AC, Lee KJ, Walsh JM, Potter CR, et al. Association between moderate and late preterm birth and neurodevelopment and social-emotional development at age 2 years. JAMA Pediatr. 2017;171:e164805. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.4805
    » https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.4805
  • 17
    Jong M, Verhoeven M, van Baar AL. School outcome, cognitive functioning, and behaviour problems in moderate and late preterm children and adults: a review. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2012;17:163-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2012.02.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2012.02.003
  • 18
    Cheong JL, Spittle AJ, Burnett AC, Anderson PJ, Doyle LW. Have outcomes following extremely preterm birth improved over time? Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. 2020;25:101114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2020.101114
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.siny.2020.101114
  • 19
    Luu TM, Mian MO, Nuyt AM. Long-term impact of preterm birth: neurodevelopmental and physical health outcomes. Clin Perinatol. 2017;44:305-14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clp.2017.01.003
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clp.2017.01.003
  • 20
    Eickmann SH, Malkes NF, Lima MC. Psychomotor development of preterm infants aged 6 to 12 months. São Paulo Med J. 2012;130:299-306. https://doi.org/10.1590/s1516-31802012000500006
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/s1516-31802012000500006
  • 21
    Ream AM, Lehwald L. Neurologic consequences of preterm birth. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2018;18:48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-018-0862-2
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-018-0862-2
  • 22
    Imren C, Vlug LE, Koning BA, Diertens T, Snel HE, Suurland J, et al. Necrotizing enterocolitis in a dutch cohort of very preterm infants: prevalence, mortality, and long-term outcomes. Eur J Pediatr Surg. 2022;32:111-9. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0041-1741544
    » https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0041-1741544
  • 23
    Evensen KA, Skranes J, Brubakk AM, Vik T. Predictive value of early motor evaluation in preterm very low birth weight and term small for gestational age children. Early Hum Dev. 2009;85:511-8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2009.04.007
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2009.04.007
  • 24
    Romeo DM, Ricci M, Mirra F, Venezia I, Mallardi M, Pede E, et al. Longitudinal cognitive assessment in low-risk very preterm infants. Medicina (Kaunas). 2022;58:133. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58010133
    » https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina58010133
  • 25
    Sansavini A, Pentimonti J, Justice L, Guarini A, Savini S, Alessandroni R, et al. Language, motor and cognitive development of extremely preterm children: modeling individual growth trajectories over the first three years of life. J Commun Disord. 2014;49:55-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.02.005
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.02.005
  • 26
    Hadders-Algra M. Early human brain development: starring the subplate. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;92:276-90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.017
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.017
  • 27
    Chugani HT. A critical period of brain development: studies of cerebral glucose utilization with PET. Prev Med. 1998;27:184-8. https://doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1998.0274
    » https://doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1998.0274
  • 28
    DeMaster D, Bick J, Johnson U, Montroy JJ, Landry S, Duncan AF. Nurturing the preterm infant brain: leveraging neuroplasticity to improve neurobehavioral outcomes. Pediatr Res. 2019;85:166-75. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-018-0203-9
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/s41390-018-0203-9
  • 29
    Taylor HG. Multiple risks for long-term cognitive impairments following preterm birth. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104:218-20. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.12900
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.12900
  • 30
    Vasconcelos LT, Irineu ME, Santos JN, Modesto TS. Estimulação precoce multiprofissional em crianças com defasagem no desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor: revisão integrativa. Rev Pesqui Fisioter. 2019;9:284-92. https://doi.org/10.17267/2238-2704rpf.v9i2.2302
    » https://doi.org/10.17267/2238-2704rpf.v9i2.2302

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    25 Aug 2023
  • Date of issue
    2024

History

  • Received
    19 Aug 2022
  • Accepted
    04 Apr 2023
Sociedade de Pediatria de São Paulo R. Maria Figueiredo, 595 - 10o andar, 04002-003 São Paulo - SP - Brasil, Tel./Fax: (11 55) 3284-0308; 3289-9809; 3284-0051 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: rpp@spsp.org.br