Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Vygotsky’s “The Psychology of Art”: A foundational and still unexplored text

A “Psicologia da Arte” de Vigotski: seu texto fundacional e ainda inexplorado

ABSTRACT

In the last ten years, new trends in the interpretation of Vygotsky’s work have been developed, many of which have transcended the traditional interpretations that have been hegemonic in Soviet and Western psychology since the 1980s. Nonetheless, Vygotsky’s “The Psychology of Art” is among the most interesting books written by this Soviet psychologist and, paradoxically, has not received enough attention in the study of his legacy. In that book, Vygotsky developed a rich psychology, in dialogue with Philosophy, Sociology and Art. In this paper, some theoretical questions and concepts developed by Vygotsky are discussed, which were not included in the dominant interpretation of his work, neither in Soviet nor Western psychology. The discussion opened by Vygotsky throughout the book shows that philosophy, art, poetry, Sociology and Psychology are interrelated in such a way that they are a living theoretical representation whose epicenter was human motivation and the creative character of human performance.

Keywords
Art; Creativity; Imagination; Motivation; Perezhivanie

Resumo

Nos últimos dez anos, têm sido desenvolvidos trabalhos sobre as novas tendências na interpretação da obra de Vygotsky, muitos dos quais transcenderam as interpretações tradicionais que foram hegemônicas na psicologia Soviética e ocidental desde a década de oitenta. Não obstante, a obra “A Psicologia da Arte”, de Vygotsky, está entre os livros mais interessantes escritos pelo psicólogo soviético, e paradoxalmente, até o momento não recebeu suficiente atenção no estudo de seu legado. Nesse livro, Vygotsky constrói uma rica psicologia, estabelecendo diálogos com a Filosofia, a Sociologia e a Arte. Neste trabalho são discutidas algumas questões teóricas e conceitos desenvolvidos por Vygotsky que não foram incluídos na interpretação dominante de seu trabalho, nem na psicologia Soviética, nem na psicologia Ocidental. A discussão aberta por Vygotsky ao longo do livro mostra que a filosofia, a arte, a poesia, a Sociologia e a Psicologia, estão interrelacionadas, de tal forma que, são uma representação teórica de vive para que o epicentro foi a motivação humana e o criativo personagem do desempenho humano.

Palavras-chave
Arte; Criatividade; Imaginação; Motivação; Perejivanie

Vygotsky may be the only author in the history of psychology whose work was broadly discussed worldwide before many of his writings were actually published in their original language. Such an unprecedented situation was possible due to a psychology that was developed in an environment of high pressure, censorship and distortions, which was the reason why, until very recent times, the history of Soviet psychology could not be used as a source for new constructions, analysis and reflections on Soviet psychology.

This paper aims to reveal some theoretical questions and concepts developed by Vygotsky that were not included in the dominant interpretation of his work, neither in Soviet nor Western psychology, where some of his last ideas and concepts have just recently begun to be discussed. However, it is important to stress that some of his later ideas first appeared in the book, “The Psychology of Art”, in which Vygotsky opened what I called years ago as the “first moment of Vygotsky’s work” (González Rey, 2011González Rey, F. (2011). A re-examination of defining moments in Vygotsky’s work and their implications for his continuing legacy. Mind, Culture & Activity, 18(3), 257-275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10749030903338517
https://doi.org/10.1080/1074903090333851...
), whose main concepts and ideas were taken up again only at the end of his life, between 1932 and 1934.

It is amazing that Vygotsky focused on art at a time when psychology was largely dominated by an empirical way of doing science, within which art and culture were completely excluded. In “The Psychology of Art”, the great merit of Vygotsky is that he was still not under institutional and ideological soviet pressures, or widely influenced by relevant and better-known theories such as Gestalt psychology that usefully and productively influenced the last moment of his work. It is true that the version of “The Psychology of Art” defended as doctoral thesis by Vygotsky in 1925 expressed the influences of Kornilov’s work in some paragraphs, which were completely contradictory with the rest of the text.

More recently, different authors have drawn special attention to the last period of Vygotsky’s work (González Rey, 2011González Rey, F. (2011). A re-examination of defining moments in Vygotsky’s work and their implications for his continuing legacy. Mind, Culture & Activity, 18(3), 257-275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10749030903338517
https://doi.org/10.1080/1074903090333851...
; Leontiev, 1992Leontiev, A. A. (1992). Ecce Homo: Methodological problems of the activity theoretical approach. Multidisciplinary Newsletter for Activity Theory, 11/12, 41-45.; Yasnitsky, 2009Yasnitsky, A. (2009). Vygotsky circle during the decade of 1931-1941: Toward an integrative science of mind, brain and education (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Toronto, Canada., 2012, 2015; Zavershneva, 2010, 2015). However, the link between this period and the ideas discussed by him in “The Psychology of Art” has remained beyond researchers’ attention.

In this paper, it is intended to evidence how some of the seminal ideas advanced by Vygotsky in “The Psychology of Art” were taken up again by himself in 1932, when he continued the pathway which he had begun with in that book and which he focused on until 1934, the year of his death. The paper is organized as follows. Firstly, the main contributions of “The Psychology of Art” are discussed in depth. Secondly, I discuss why this book was only published forty years after its introduction as Vygotsky’s doctoral thesis, as well as the impact that its preface, written by A.N. Leontiev, had in terms of lack of attention given to “The Psychology of Art” in Soviet psychology and consequently in Western psychology. Finally, Vygotsky’s return to the topics of his original agenda between 1931 and 1934, is discussed, as well as the development of these topics towards the discussion of meaning, perezhivanie and the social situation of development, topics that open a new path for extending his legacy to topics that have been historically less developed within the cultural-historical standpoint, such as subjectivity, creativity and motivation.

The theoretical originality of “The Psychology of Art” in the context of Russian psychology of the time

In “The Psychology of Art”, Vygotsky continuously referred to more diverse expressions of psychology and culture of that time. The discussion opened by Vygotsky throughout the book shows that philosophy, art, poetry, sociology and psychology are interrelated in such a way that they are a living theoretical representation whose epicenter was human motivation and the creative character of human performance. Russian poets quoted by Vygotsky in this text did not appear again in psychology or artistic magazines in the Soviet Union for 50 years. Vygotsky’s interest in the psychological functioning of the creative artist, which was a relevant precedent to approach the study of creativity in a different way, was made clear in the next statement:

By its nature, artistic perezhivanie remains incomprehensible and closed to the subject in its course and essence. We never know why we like or dislike a work of art. Everything we intend to explain their influence is later thought to be a complete rationalization of unconscious processes. The very essence of perezhivanie remains a mystery for us

(Vygotsky, 1965Vygotsky, L .S. (1965). Psikhologiya Iskustva [The Psychology of Art] Moscow: Iskustva Isdatelstva., p.25; translated from the Russian version by the author).

Here, Vygotsky emphasizes that perezhivanie is not a simple “emotional experience”, as is commonly assumed in English translations. Perezhivanie has a specific psychological nature, stressing emotions as intrinsic to the creative functions in a process that is not accessible to the human being through consciousness. Complex psychological networks and dynamics lie behind these tendencies and need to be further studied. From the very beginning, Vygotsky’s work recognized the emotional undertones of human actions and performances that are beyond the conscious control of the subject.

Vygotsky advanced a theoretical representation of motivation supported by the concept of perezhivanie as an emotional state of the creator that qualified their performance beyond any conscious proposal. Vygotsky seemed to worry about the subject’s motivational formations rather than about the psychological entities or functions. In this sense, he used the concept of perezhivanie to define a set of emotions inherent to human performance. Perezhivanie was used to define the intrinsic emotional character of creation in art, as well as to explain the perception of the artistic work. Art, from his perspective, was intrinsically associated with feeling, imagination and fantasy.

We can never say exactly why we like one or another art production; words can hardly ever express the essential and most important aspects of perezhivanie and, as Plato stated (in his dialogue Ion), poets know the way by which they create less than anyone else

(Vygotsky, 1965Vygotsky, L .S. (1965). Psikhologiya Iskustva [The Psychology of Art] Moscow: Iskustva Isdatelstva., p.93; translated from the Russian version by the author).

With the concept of perezhivanie, Vygotsky established dialogues with German psychologists devoted to the relations between fantasy and emotions in artistic creation, and with Freud on the basis of mutual interests, which revealed his great curiosity for the emotional inner life of human beings and its unconscious character. However, Vygotsky gradually introduced his own opinions and concepts, like perezhivanie, through which he advanced new demands in his search for an understanding of creative motivation, as we will see below. Vygotsky’s sympathy with Freud was clear in chapter 4 of “The Psychology of Art”, in which Vygotsky established a critical dialogue with Freud. Though stressing his differences with Freud, Vygotsky was, at the same time, very impressed by Freud’s audacious and creative ideas about the unconscious character of some psychological processes and the central place given to fantasy. However, unlike Freud, Vygotsky did not associate perezhivanie, the nature of which he defined as unconscious, with inner human universal forces and defined it as being closely related with action.

Vygotsky’s and Luria’s relations with psychoanalysis extended until the end of the 1920s (Van der Veer & Valsiner, 1991Van Der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1991). Understanding Vygotsky: A quest for synthesis. New York: Blackwell.). The attention drawn by Vygotsky to Freud in “The Psychology of Art” is another argument for the openness of Vygotsky’s original thinking while he was writing the first version of the book. Vygotsky´s orientation to psychology in “The Psychology of Art” was clearly addressed to the study of the affective side of the human being.

“The Psychology of Art” had historically been excluded, as an “immature” moment in Vygotsky’s work. I believe this was a result of the type of psychology proposed by Vygotsky in this book, which represented the opposite of the objectivist path, taken by official versions of Soviet psychology from those years until the mid-1970s (González Rey, 2014González Rey, F. (2014). Advancing further the history of Soviet psychology: Moving forward from dominant representations in Western and Soviet psychology. History of Psychology, 17(1), 60-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035565
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035565...
, 2017González Rey, F. (2017). The topic of subjectivity in psychology: Contradictions, paths and new alternatives. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 47(4), 502-521.). In addition, another fact that contributed to the representation of “The Psychology of Art” as an immature work was Leontiev’s short paper written as the preface of the 1965 Russian edition of “The Psychology of Art”. This introduction can be interpreted as a theoretical critique of the book which, in this case, included political connotations.

The real importance of the concept of perezhivanie in “The Psychology of Art” has long passed unnoticed and many Vygotsky interpreters who are interested in the concept still do not consider the use to perezhivanie given by Vygotsky in this book. It was in “The Psychology of Art” that Vygotsky highlighted perezhivanie as the set of emotional processes that integrates the unit fantasy-emotion as inseparable from artistic creation.

The involvement of emotions in human creation was promising due to its potential for explaining a new qualitative level of the human psyche within which emotions are inseparable from intellectual operations. This position anticipated Vygotsky’s emphasis on the intellectual and emotional unity that characterized his holistic period, between 1932 and 1934 (Yasnitsky, 2015Yasnitsky, A. (2015). A transnational history of “the beginning of a beautiful friendship”: The birth of the Cultural Historical Gestalt Psychology of Alexander Luria, Kurt Lewin, Lev Vygotsky and others. In: A. Yasnitsky & R. Van Der Veer (Eds.), Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies (pp.201-226). London. Routledge.; Zavershneva, 2015Zavershneva, E. (2015). The way to freedom: Vygotsky in 1932. In: A.Yasnitsky & R. Van Der Veer (Eds.), Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies (pp.127-140). London: Routledge.). Concentrated heavily on artistic perezhivanie, Vygotsky could not extend its use to other types of human performance in which the individual is actively involved as the creative subject of the action. However, perezhivanie was a key concept in his emphasis on the emotional side of human life.

“The Psychology of Art” was not only an expression of the broad intellectual and cultural interests of Vygotsky in the first moment of his work, it was also an expression of Vygotsky´s special interest in the topics of emotions, fantasy and imagination, which formed one of the theoretical cores of “The Psychology of Art”. His dialogue with Freud and Ribot also evidenced his interest in the relation of those topics with mental disorders. So, in a dialogue with Ribot, Vygotsky stated:

This new approach can be described approximately as follows: The psychologists proceed from the irrefutable association that exists between emotion and imagination. We know that every emotion has a psychic expression in addition to a physical one. In other words, a feeling “is embodied, fixed in an idea, as is evidenced in cases of persecution mania”, according to Ribot. Consequently, an emotion is expressed by the mimic, pantomimic, secretory, and somatic responses of our organism. It also requires some expression of our imagination. We find the best evidence for this view among the so called objectless emotions. Pathological phobias, persistent fears, and so forth, are always associated with specific ideas, most of which are absolutely false and distort reality, but in so doing, find their “psychic” expression. A patient who suffers from obsessive fear is emotionally sick, his fear is irrational; and so in order to rationalize it, he imagines that everyone is pursuing and persecuting him

(Vygotsky, 1971Vygotsky, L. S. (1971). Psychology of art. New York: MIT Press., p.209).

At that time, Vygotsky was closer to subjectivity than at any other moment of his work. The idea that “feeling is embodied, fixed in an idea”, as stressed by Vygotsky, was an important antecedent of the way he would approach the concept of unit in the last stage of his work, mainly expressed by concepts such as senses and perezhivanie. Nonetheless, in that last stage of his work, Vygotsky was still far from the position that any idea, once it becomes subjectively configured, distorts reality, creating imaginary realities, which is the cornerstone of our proposal of subjectivity. However, Vygotsky’s most important theoretical intuition is that, aside from the different corporal and somatic expressions of emotions, these processes always require the expression of imagination. Art was the path for Vygotsky to advance a new and original representation of the human mind. Following his previous idea, Vygotsky took an audacious step forward:

This means that in essence, all our fantasy experiences take place on a completely real emotional basis. We see, therefore, that emotion and imagination are not two separate processes; on the contrary, they are the same process. We can regard a fantasy as the central expression of an emotional reaction

(Vygotsky, 1971Vygotsky, L. S. (1971). Psychology of art. New York: MIT Press., p.210).

The consideration of fantasy “as the central expression of an emotional reaction” is essential because it integrates emotions with psychological functions. Such integration emphasizes the “fictional character” of psychological functions, the objectivity of which is inseparable from their cultural and emotional character. This is an argument which implies that objectivity should be considered as a culturally produced concept. Properly human processes and realities are fictional, not because they are non-objective, but because they are new realities invented by human beings, which progressively separate them more and more from nature. This human nature is inseparable from a plot of different facts and conditions that are not controlled by individuals or social instances; this fact defines all human processes and realities as objective. Nonetheless, human realities, processes and facts share a qualitative attribute that does not exist in the rest of natural phenomena; their subjective character turns human beings into creators, making them capable of creating new, original realities and processes within which, in turn, subjectivity emerges.

Taking the prior statements as starting points, it is possible to advance in the recognition that our “imaginary situations” are founded on a “completely real emotional basis”, which implies in recognizing fantasy experiences as a new kind of human phenomenon. Human realities and their objectivity are inseparable from human actions, and therefore, for human beings, objectivity is always subjectively produced in human relations. The relevance of this process is that the fictional reality of culture is part of the genesis and development of the human mind, and the human mind defines new moments in the production of culture, in an endless process within which neither culture nor human mind are objectified by one another, something that Vygotsky never made explicit in his work.

Vygotsky´s definition of culture was still very narrow, identifying culture mainly with language, without considering human institutions, and other human domains like science, politics and other socially given phenomena as cultural instances. The topic of the symbolic was very narrowly treated by Vygotsky, who mainly emphasized the sign among the many diverse symbols, symbolical devices and realities (Zinchenko, 1993Zinchenko, V. P. (1993). Kulturno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya. Opyt amplifikatsii. [Cultural-historical psychology: The experience of amplification]. Voprosy Psychologii [Questions of Psychology], 4, 5-19.).

In “The Psychology of Art” Vygotsky expressed his concern at the absence of a psychological theory capable of advancing the study of sentiments and fantasies, topics that for a long time had been monopolized by psychoanalysis:

It is necessary to say, however, that there are not any more obscure topics than these two (Vygotsky is referring to sentiments and fantasy) and although they have been subject to more development and examination in recent times, at least until today, unfortunately, we have no general recognized and elaborated system for the study of sentiments and fantasy

(Vygotsky, 1965Vygotsky, L .S. (1965). Psikhologiya Iskustva [The Psychology of Art] Moscow: Iskustva Isdatelstva., p.256, translated from the Russian version by the author).

Vygotsky’s concern with the absence of theoretical systems capable of bringing light to topics such as sentiments and fantasy, clearly evidenced his interest in advancing a psychology capable of studying these phenomena. Focused on these topics, Vygotsky created the basis for advancing new ways in the study of motivation and creativity. On such a different psychological system, Vygotsky attempted to advance it in the last moment of his work through a new definition of consciousness (González Rey, 2009González Rey, F. (2009). Historical relevance of Vygotsky’s work: Its significance for a new approach to the problem of subjectivity in psychology. Outlines: Critical Practice Studies, 11(1), 59-73., 2011, 2017González Rey, F. (2009). Historical relevance of Vygotsky’s work: Its significance for a new approach to the problem of subjectivity in psychology. Outlines: Critical Practice Studies, 11(1), 59-73.; Leontiev, 1992Leontiev, A. A. (1992). Ecce Homo: Methodological problems of the activity theoretical approach. Multidisciplinary Newsletter for Activity Theory, 11/12, 41-45.; Zavershneva, 2015Zavershneva, E. (2015). The way to freedom: Vygotsky in 1932. In: A.Yasnitsky & R. Van Der Veer (Eds.), Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies (pp.127-140). London: Routledge.).

“The Psychology of Art” also brought to light some methodological insights that dealt closely with its theoretical proposal.

For this reason, I think it is necessary to propose another method for the psychology of art, which needs a clear methodological basis. Against this proposal, I will frequently object to what is often said in relation to the study of the unconscious: the unconscious is, according to its own meaning, something not recognized by us and therefore not clear for us, and for this reason, it could not become the object of scientific research. Starting from this erroneous premise that “we can study only (and in general can only know) what we directly recognize has no support because we study and know many things that we do not directly know and what we know only with the support of analogies, constructions, hypotheses, conclusions, deductions and so on, in general by indirect ways”

(Vygotsky, 1965Vygotsky, L .S. (1965). Psikhologiya Iskustva [The Psychology of Art] Moscow: Iskustva Isdatelstva., pp.32-33; translated from the Russian version by the author).

Vygotsky acutely perceived that to further advance on the questions he raised in “The Psychology of Art”, it was important to use indirect routes, analogies and assumptions as methodological resources for following the complex processes of human creation. This relevant epistemological assumption has been completely unnoticed by Vygotsky’s followers within and outside Soviet psychology. In the Soviet Union, epistemological discussions in particular were taboo due to their philosophical implications for a science ruled by a strict objectivity based on the empirical correspondence between theory and empirical facts.

We stated in the first chapter that this point of view was wrong and that practice magnificently denies it. This shows that science studies not only immediate and recognized facts, but also a series of phenomena and events that can be studied only indirectly by means of footsteps and vestiges, and with the help of material that is not only completely different from what we study but which is often false

(Vygotsky, 1965Vygotsky, L .S. (1965). Psikhologiya Iskustva [The Psychology of Art] Moscow: Iskustva Isdatelstva., p.94; translated from Russian version by the author).

These methodological assumptions advanced in the opposite direction to the positivist path taken by the instrumental and experimental positions that characterized first the researchers in Kornilov’s group, and later the experimental studies of psychological functions that were conducted following the Activity Theory framework. These natural and objective methodological positions were also defended by Vygotsky between 1927 and 1931 (Vygotsky, 2012Vygotsky, L. S. (2012). The science of psychology. Journal of Russian and East European Journal, 50(4), 85-106.).

The Psychology of Art”: its first publication 40 years after its presentation as Vygotsky’s doctoral thesis

This first edition of “The Psychology of Art” was published in 1965 with a short “introductory paper” by A. N. Leontiev, a fact that, taken together with the omission of Soviet psychology regarding the topics discussed by Vygotsky in that book, contributes to explaining the lack of attention for this book in Soviet psychology. Vygotsky’s work was centered on art, but his reflections had relevant implications to the development of a general psychology, since the motivational and creative processes discussed by him in relation to art are general to all human motivated performances. In that introduction, Leontiev made a presentation/wrote an introduction stressing the idea that the book represented a historical work with little theoretical value, considering the advances of Soviet psychology in the 40 years after the book had been written. That edition had little impact in the Soviet Union.

No matter how distant this prologue was written in relation to the original version of the book, to some extent it permits an explanation of why the book was published so late, as well as of the slight impact it had in Soviet psychology. Since 1951, Leontiev had been the Chair of Psychology in the Faculty of Philosophy of the University of Moscow. He enjoyed the peak of his intellectual and political prestige in 1963, when he received the “Lenin Prize”. Why then, instead of using the publication of “The Psychology of Art” as a first step to the introduction of Vygotsky in the West, did he write such a highly critical introductory paper to a book that was extremely difficult to access, given the small numbers of this edition?

Leontiev’s preface to this edition of “The Psychology of Art” seemed to have a political proposal that announced the main trends of Soviet psychology in the 1960s. His introductory paper represented a written testimony of the integration of Vygotsky’s ideas within the Activity Theory as it was developed by Leontiev, which represented the new dominant official psychology of the Soviet Union in the 1960s.

From the very beginning of his preface, Leontiev diminished the moment of Vygotsky’s ideas with political argumentation:

Vygotsky wrote “The Psychology of Art” forty years ago in the years of the establishment of Soviet psychological science. At that time a battle was still being waged with the idealistic psychology that dominated the most important psychological research center of the country – the Institute of Psychology of the University of Moscow, headed by professor Chelpanov. ... At that time, Vygotsky was still a young man within scientific psychology, and it is also possible to say an unexpected man

(Leontiev, 1965Leontiev, A. N. (1965). Psikhologiya Iskustva [The Psychology of Art] Moscow: Iskustva Isdatelstva., p.iii-iv; translated from the Russian version by the author).

It is curious from a historical perspective that, even after Stalinism was officially overcome, the political discourse of Leontiev continued the same arguments developed by Soviet psychology in the 1930s, an expression of pressure and institutional political control at a time when social fear strongly characterized the social subjectivity of the country. The arguments given by Leontiev in 1965 are similar to those that supported the most conservative sector of Soviet psychology in the 1920s and 30s. Leontiev’s ideological orthodoxy at that time was a clear evidence of his political position, which was impossible to separate from his theoretically conservative position in psychology. Leontiev invalidated Vygotsky and the best Russian poets and intellectuals of the time by stressing that socialist realism was not yet an option when “The Psychology of Art” was introduced. In 1965, Leontiev continued to defend socialist realism.

After all the criticism, as exemplified above, Leontiev emphatically invalidated Vygotsky as a serious author by drawing a completely different picture of psychology than the one defended by Vygotsky in “The Psychology of Art”: “For this reason Vygotsky frequently speaks as an author but still not through his own words; he quotes many authors, even authors who are strange to him in their more general basis” (Leontiev, 1965Leontiev, A. N. (1965). Psikhologiya Iskustva [The Psychology of Art] Moscow: Iskustva Isdatelstva., p.viii).

In conclusion, Leontiev stated that:

After forty years of claiming that Soviet psychologists had done much with Vygotsky and after him, many of the positions in this psychological book should be interpreted in another way – from the position of contemporary representations of activity and human consciousness.

(Leontiev, 1965Leontiev, A. N. (1965). Psikhologiya Iskustva [The Psychology of Art] Moscow: Iskustva Isdatelstva., p.x; translated from the Russian version by the author).

According to Leontiev, Vygotsky developed a few ideas of his own in the book. For this reason, Vygotsky’s position needs to be updated in light of the advances in Soviet psychology made after his death, which were reduced by Leontiev to the works about consciousness and activity. The focus of Activity Theory at that time was activity itself, and consciousness was understood as the epiphenomenon of this focus (Zinchenko, 2002Zinchenko, V. P. (2002). Problematika myshleniya v razvivayushevo obucheniya [The problem of thinking in developmental education]. In Razvivayushee obrazovanie. Dialog c V.V. Davydovym [Developmental education. Dialog with V. V. Davydov] (pp.46-102). Moscow: Akademiya., 2009Zinchenko, V. P. (2009). Consciousness as the subject matter and task of psychology. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 47(5), 44-75.).

That “short introductory paper” was written two decades after the virulent attack made by Leontiev against the ideological deviation of Vygotsky in regards to pedology (Leontiev, 1937/1998Leontiev, A. N. (1998). Ushenie o srede vpedologisheskix rabotax L. S. Vygotskogo [Theory on environment in the pedological works of Vygotsky]. Voprosy Psychologii, 1, 108-124. (Original work published 1937).), which remained unknown until 1998, when it was published in Russian in the journal “Voprosy Psychologii”. This critique addressed by Leontiev to Vygotsky did not represent an isolated fact; the differences and reciprocal criticisms between them became increasingly more acute during Vygotsky’s life, particularly after the decision by Vygotsky to advance a theory of consciousness in 1933 (Zavershneva, 2015Zavershneva, E. (2015). The way to freedom: Vygotsky in 1932. In: A.Yasnitsky & R. Van Der Veer (Eds.), Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies (pp.127-140). London: Routledge.). Leontiev’s short introductory paper to “The Psychology of Art” and the various references to Vygotsky, as well as Vygotsky’s quotations used by Leontiev after his death, including the absence of references to Vygotsky in his last book “Activity, Consciousness and Personality”, leads to the conclusion that Leontiev’s main proposal regarding Vygotsky was to relegate him to the past, as a mere historical source of the Activity Theory, which represented the most mature theory of Soviet psychology since the 1960s.

Behavior and social determinism became central to the definition of an objective psychology, which Kornilov and his group considered as a Marxist psychology. Under the new political and scientific conditions in which Vygotsky worked since his entry to Kornilov’s group in 1924, he took a completely different path from that of “The Psychology of Art”. The central place given to behavior by Kornilov in those years was explicitly defended by Luria as follows: “The psychologist as a rule shares the objective position of physiologists, but carry on their work on a much broader basis, approaching psychology from the perspective of that structural behavior which is determined by social conditions” (Luria, 1928Luria, A. R. (1928). Psychology in Russia. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 35(3), 347-355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1928.10533070
https://doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1928.10...
, p.347). It was Vygotsky’s turn to the study of higher forms of human behaviors, used by him indistinctively as higher psychological functions that have as their main function the control of behavior, as will be discussed below, a clear expression of the winds that blew in Kornilov’s group at that time.

The return of Vygotsky to some of his main topics in “The Psychology of Art”

In what is termed the third stage of Vygotsky’s work, in the period between 1931 and 1934 (González Rey, 2011González Rey, F. (2011). A re-examination of defining moments in Vygotsky’s work and their implications for his continuing legacy. Mind, Culture & Activity, 18(3), 257-275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10749030903338517
https://doi.org/10.1080/1074903090333851...
, 2016González Rey, F., & Mitjans Martínez, A. (2016). Perezhivanie: Advancing on its implications for the cultural-historical approach. International Research in Early Childhood Education Journal, 7(1), 143-160.), he transcended some positions that dominated his works between 1927-1931, a period that has been defined by different authors as an instrumental period (Leontiev, 1984Leontiev, A. A. (1984). The productive career of Aleksei Nikolaevich Leont’ev. Soviet Psychology, 23(1), 6-56.; Yasnitsky, 2015Yasnitsky, A. (2015). A transnational history of “the beginning of a beautiful friendship”: The birth of the Cultural Historical Gestalt Psychology of Alexander Luria, Kurt Lewin, Lev Vygotsky and others. In: A. Yasnitsky & R. Van Der Veer (Eds.), Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies (pp.201-226). London. Routledge.; Zavershneva, 2015Zavershneva, E. (2015). The way to freedom: Vygotsky in 1932. In: A.Yasnitsky & R. Van Der Veer (Eds.), Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies (pp.127-140). London: Routledge.). The concepts of higher psychological functions, sign, mediation and internalization, which were central in this instrumental period were replaced by concepts like perezhivanie, sense and social situation of development. It is amazing that these concepts were largely overlooked by both Soviet and Western psychology until very recent times. In Soviet psychology only Bozhovich (1968)Bozhovich, L. I. (1968). Lichnost i ee formirovanie v detskom vosraste [Personality and its formation in childhood]. Moscow: Pedagogika. drew attention to the concepts of perezhivanie and social situation of development, advancing forward on Vygotsky’s definition on perezhivanie.

However, the concept of sense may be a result of its very short and fast formulation by Vygotsky, and its little integration to the rest of the concepts has been considered as relevant by very few authors (González Rey, 2002González Rey, F. (2002). Sujeto y subjetividad: una aproximación histórico-cultural. México: Thomson Learning., 2005, 2009; Leontiev, 1992Leontiev, A. A. (1992). Ecce Homo: Methodological problems of the activity theoretical approach. Multidisciplinary Newsletter for Activity Theory, 11/12, 41-45.; Zavershneva, 2015Zavershneva, E. (2015). The way to freedom: Vygotsky in 1932. In: A.Yasnitsky & R. Van Der Veer (Eds.), Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies (pp.127-140). London: Routledge.). The concept of word sense, as formulated by Vygotsky opened a new path to advance on consciousness as a psychological system. Sense, as defined by Vygotsky, is:

A word’s sense is the aggregate of all psychological facts that arise in our consciousness as a result of the word. Sense is a dynamic, fluid, and complex formation which has several zones that vary in their stability. Meaning is only one of these zones of the sense that the word acquires in the context of speech

(Vygotsky, 1987Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In R. Rieber, & A. Carton (Eds.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky (Vol. 1, pp.43-287). New York: Plenum., p.279).

As sense was defined by Vygotsky as a quality of the word, in fact the word itself is transformed into a psychological unit, embodying several psychological facts that arise in consciousness as a result of its emergence. His definition of sense, which was strongly influenced by the French psychologist Frédric Paulhan, followed the principle of integration between emotions and ideas already discussed by him in “The Psychology of Art”, when he still had not been in contact with Lewin and his group. The idea of unit was in embryo in “The Psychology of Art” when Vygotsky was at the very beginning of his work. The relations stressed by him in “The Psychology of Art”, between emotions, imagination and fantasy, were topics that he completely abandoned during his instrumental period.

Undoubtedly, the influence of K. Lewin and his group on Vygotsky was strong in that last stage of his work (Bozhovich, 1968Bozhovich, L. I. (1968). Lichnost i ee formirovanie v detskom vosraste [Personality and its formation in childhood]. Moscow: Pedagogika.; Yarochevsky, 2007Yarochevsky, M. G. (2007). L. S. Vygotsky: V poiskax novoi psykjologii [L. S. Vygotsky: In the search for a new psychology]. Moscow: L. K. I.; Yasnitsky, 2012Yasnitsky, A. (2012). Revisionist revolution in Vygotskian science: Toward cultural-historical Gestalt Psychology. Journal of Russian and East European, 50(4), 3-15. http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/RPO1061-0405500400
https://doi.org/10.2753/RPO1061-04055004...
, 2015; Zavershneva, 2015Zavershneva, E. (2015). The way to freedom: Vygotsky in 1932. In: A.Yasnitsky & R. Van Der Veer (Eds.), Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies (pp.127-140). London: Routledge.). Lewin’s advances on the inseparability of human needs and the social environment were closely associated with the inseparability of personality and environment. Lewin’s position on the matter helped to understand the environment, not as a reality per se, but in its close relation to individuals. Individuals were understood as inseparable from the environment and the concept of relationship became central for the understanding of the relationship between individuals and their social environment. For Vygotsky, perezhivanie appears to be the psychological term to explain that unit. The impossibility of analyzing social environment outside of individual motivation and personality influenced Vygotsky’s definition of perezhivanie through which he attempted to overcome the mechanical social determinism as understood by Soviet psychology at that time.

The impossibility to split social environment from a child’s personality was clearly expressed by Vygotsky as follows:

To state a certain, general, formal position, it would be correct to say that the environment determines the development of the child through perezhivanie of the environment. Most essential, therefore, is rejection of the absolute indicators of the environment; the child is part of the social situation, and the relationship between the child and the environment and between the environment and the child occurs through perezhivanie and the activity of the child himself

(Vygotsky, 1998Vygotsky, L. S. (1998). The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky (Vol. 5). New York: Plenum., p.294).

Perezhivanie is used by Vygotsky to understand the child’s relations with the environment as the real force for their development. Nonetheless, the concept as such is vaguely defined in its psychological nature, leaving many theoretical gaps to be filled. (González Rey, 2015aGonzález Rey, F. (2015a). Human motivation in question: Discussing emotions, motives, and subjectivity from a cultural-historical standpoint. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 45(4), 419-439. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jtsb.12073
https://doi.org/10.1111/jtsb.12073...
, 2016bGonzález Rey, F. (2016b). Advances in subjectivity from a cultural-historical perspective: Unfolding and consequences for cultural studies today. In M. Fleer, F. González Rey, & N. Veresov (Eds.), Perezhivanie, emotions and subjectivity: Advancing Vygotsky´s legacy (pp.173-194). Singapore: Springer.). Be that as it may, his focus seemed to be concentrated on the rejection of the absolute indicators of the environment, something that was extremely revolutionary in relation to the way social environment was understood by behavioral psychology and by Soviet psychology as well, that always had important convergences with a behavioral representation of psychology.

Vygotsky´s turn toward the emotional side of human psyche, between 1931 and 1934, aside from some promising statements about emotions and his advances on some important concepts such as sense and perezhivanie, in fact resulted in contradicting his emphasis on the cognitive genesis of perezhivanie. Bozhovich’s critique of the concepts of perezhivanie in that last period of Vygotsky’s work made that contradiction very explicit. Bozhovich expressed this failure by Vygotsky as follows:

If the concept of experience as raised by him (concept that expressed the child’s affective relations to the environment) brings us closer to the interpretation of the true causes of child development, the subsequent search for the link that determines this development, a search that ends in the concept of generalization, has again made us return to intellectualist positions

(Bozhovich, 1981Bozhovich, L. I. (1981). La personalidad y su formación en la edad infantil. Habana. Pueblo y Educación., p.125; translated from the Spanish version by the author).

Bozhovich, without making it explicit and perhaps without being clearly conscious of this, in fact, got deeper into Vygotsky’s positions in “The Psychology of Art”. Her search for concepts through which to understand personality as a motivational system was closer to Vygotsky’s attempt to integrate emotions, imagination and fantasy, which he stressed as the basis for the psychological genesis of art and mental disorders. This path was evident in Bozhovich’s effort to find concepts that were impossible to reduce to cognitive genesis or functioning. That effort was clear in this next assumption by Bozhovich:

In other words, what underlies perezhivanie, as we see it, is the world of children’s needs – their impulses, desires, intentions, complexly intertwined with one another and interrelated with possibilities for meeting these needs. And this entire complex system of connections, the entire world of a child’s needs and impulses, must be deciphered so that we can understand the nature of the influence external circumstances exert on children’s mental development

(Bozhovich, 2009Bozhovich, L. I. (2009). The social situation of child development. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 47(4), 59-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/RPO1061-0405470403
https://doi.org/10.2753/RPO1061-04054704...
, p.70).

As result of the lack of a new ontological definition related to human psychological processes in Soviet psychology, these processes continued to be vaguely defined by the concept of psyche, and Bozhovich defined perezhivanie as an affective formation2 2 Bozhovich’s concept of psychological formation of personality stressed the idea that motives are not entities, but complex systems within which different needs and motives organized around one dominant core of motives, attempting to define a hierarchy of motives that she defined as “orientation of personality”. These promising concepts were not completely developed by her and in the end were reduced to some dominant contents. So, the orientations of personality were reduced by her to individualistic and collectivistic actions, and to actions addressed to praxis. Perezhivanie, as defined above, is explicitly defined by her as a formation. . A new definition of motivation was in process in Bozhovich’s work; motivation was understood rather as a system of formations that define the psychological core of personality, instead of being defined by specific concrete motives, as the concept of motivation has been historically understood by psychology. However, for this representation to advance forward, it would be necessary to transcend the taxonomy of concepts by which human motivation has historically been explained, such as needs, desires, among others.

The necessary step forward to be done demands a theoretical link capable to explain how emotions become symbolical processes having a cultural genesis; body and culture become inseparable through this possible connection. Nonetheless, that connection was impossible to be found in a psychology that treated the symbolic processes through a very narrow notion of individual mediation of psychological functions through signs as Vygotsky did. The narrow representation of the symbolical processes and realities was in some extent responsible for the narrow comprehension of culture and social processes in Soviet psychology (González Rey, 2016aGonzález Rey, F. (2016a). Advancing the topics of social reality, culture, and subjectivity from a cultural-historical standpoint: Moments, paths, and contradictions. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 36(3), 175-189., 2017Mitjans Martínez, A., & González Rey, F. (2017). Psicologia, educação e aprendizagem escolar. São Paulo: Cortez.; González Rey & Mitjans Martínez, 2016González Rey, F., & Mitjans Martínez, A. (2016). Perezhivanie: Advancing on its implications for the cultural-historical approach. International Research in Early Childhood Education Journal, 7(1), 143-160.).

Bozhovich was aware of the need to explain motives as self-oriented systems, and not as drives defined by external objects, as defined by A.N. Leontiev.

Children may therefore strive to once again relate to something they experienced previously that became appealing to them. In this case, perezhivanie is transformed from being a means of orientation to a goal in and of itself and leads to the emergence of new needs – the need for perezhivanii themselves. However, in this regard as well, perezhivanii are not the exception. In the process of development, the entire human mind ceases to be a mere apparatus of orientation and adaptation

(Bozhovich, 2009Bozhovich, L. I. (2009). The social situation of child development. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 47(4), 59-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/RPO1061-0405470403
https://doi.org/10.2753/RPO1061-04054704...
, p.74-75)3 3 This quotation, which was taken from the English version of one of the chapters of her book “Personality and its formation in childhood” repeats the mistake of translating perezhivanie as experience. As result of this, I replace experience with “perezhivanie”. .

In the paragraph above, Bozhovich pointed out an important question without which the problem of human motivation could not be advanced; human motivation is intrinsic to a concept of mind and understood as a generative and creative system. The prior statement is not a simple call to advance a new concept of motivation, but an idea to advance a new concept of human mind; Bozhovich regarded the human mind as a creative system, capable of producing new realities. Bozhovich was the only Soviet author to embody the main ideas that were interrupted in “The Psychology of Art”.

Aside from my personal vindication in previous studies that Vygotsky, in the final years of his life returned to his origins, defined by me as a qualitative moment, not as chronological one, because “Pedagogical Psychology” was very close chronologically, but very different from a theoretical perspective. In fact today I don’t agree with my own vindication. Vygotsky, at the last moment of his work, despite his strong theoretical advances discussed above, fails in assembling these concepts within a theoretical system. Because of this, these concepts overlapped each other, and were not used in theoretical constructions in which they could have been pertinent at that time. One example of this is the last chapter of Thinking and Speech, where, instead of using the concept of sense, Vygotsky used the traditional taxonomy, like needs, motives and interests to refer to the whole character of the individual thinker.

The development of the topic of subjectivity from a cultural-historical perspective has been our theoretical, epistemological and methodological focus in the last twenty years (González Rey, 1997González Rey, F. (1997). Epistemología cualitativa y subjectividad. São Paulo: EDUC., 2002González Rey, F. (2002). Sujeto y subjetividad: una aproximación histórico-cultural. México: Thomson Learning., 2005González Rey, F. (2005). O social na psicologia e a psicologia social. Petrópolis: Vozes., 2014González Rey, F. (2014). Advancing further the history of Soviet psychology: Moving forward from dominant representations in Western and Soviet psychology. History of Psychology, 17(1), 60-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035565
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035565...
, 2015aGonzález Rey, F. (2015a). Human motivation in question: Discussing emotions, motives, and subjectivity from a cultural-historical standpoint. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 45(4), 419-439. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jtsb.12073
https://doi.org/10.1111/jtsb.12073...
, 2016a, 2016b, 2017; González Rey & Mitjans Martínez, 2016González Rey, F., & Mitjans Martínez, A. (2016). Perezhivanie: Advancing on its implications for the cultural-historical approach. International Research in Early Childhood Education Journal, 7(1), 143-160., 2017González Rey, F. (2017). The topic of subjectivity in psychology: Contradictions, paths and new alternatives. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 47(4), 502-521.; Mitjans Martínez & González Rey, 2017González Rey, F., & Mitjans Martínez, A. (2017). Subje-tividade: teoria, epistemologia e método. Campinas: Alínea.). This proposal on subjectivity is one of the paths through which the challenge opened up by the Vygotsky-Bozhovich line has been advanced toward a new conception of human mind. The concepts of subjective sense, as qualitative units, within which symbolical processes and emotions turn into one and the same process, is what we defined as the ontological definition of subjectivity from a cultural-historical perspective. Subjective senses represent that link between culture and body that Soviet psychology could not find. The constant interweaving between subjective senses and configurations4 4 “Subjective configurations emerge as a self-regulative and generative organization of subjective senses. Subjective configurations are dynamic, but have a relative stability due to the congruency of the subjective senses that they generate” (2017, p.515). defined a new ontological domain for the study of human phenomena.

Final Considerations

Vygotsky’s “The Psychology of Art” represented a foundational proposal for a new psychology centered on the integration of emotions, imaginations and fantasy, opening an avenue to advance on a different psychology that he had not been able to achieve when alive. The first ideas by the author related to that focus were addressed toward philosophical and theoretical questions that were completely omitted for decades within Soviet psychology, such as the matter of unconscious processes and the methodological demands of its study, the creation of new emotionally-based realities, the integration of emotions and imagination as one and the same process. All these questions continued to be out far from the focus of most of academic psychology until today.

The paths taken by the new Soviet state, which became an official Marxist State, turned Marxism into a political doctrine. This process led to a unilateral emphasis of the materialism over dialectics, omitting the anthropological side of the Marx’s thought. As result, Soviet sciences were politicized and, in psychology, this polarization appears through the imperative of becoming an objective science. There was no room in this kind of science for Vygotsky’s fecund ideas such as the ones presented in “The Psychology of Art”.

“The Psychology of Art” was not only directed to finding psychological processes involved with the artistic creation; it was, first of all, a theoretical platform for advancing a new psychology centering the possibility to understand human mind as a creative system culturally and historically engendered. The topics of fantasy, unsconscious and fantasy, advanced by Freud, captivated the young Vygotsky. Nonetheless,he oposed the universal and ahistorical character given by Freud to the Unconscious.

  • 2
    Bozhovich’s concept of psychological formation of personality stressed the idea that motives are not entities, but complex systems within which different needs and motives organized around one dominant core of motives, attempting to define a hierarchy of motives that she defined as “orientation of personality”. These promising concepts were not completely developed by her and in the end were reduced to some dominant contents. So, the orientations of personality were reduced by her to individualistic and collectivistic actions, and to actions addressed to praxis. Perezhivanie, as defined above, is explicitly defined by her as a formation.
  • 3
    This quotation, which was taken from the English version of one of the chapters of her book “Personality and its formation in childhood” repeats the mistake of translating perezhivanie as experience. As result of this, I replace experience with “perezhivanie”.
  • 4
    “Subjective configurations emerge as a self-regulative and generative organization of subjective senses. Subjective configurations are dynamic, but have a relative stability due to the congruency of the subjective senses that they generate” (2017Mitjans Martínez, A., & González Rey, F. (2017). Psicologia, educação e aprendizagem escolar. São Paulo: Cortez., p.515).

Como citar este artigo/How to cite this article

References

  • Bozhovich, L. I. (1968). Lichnost i ee formirovanie v detskom vosraste [Personality and its formation in childhood]. Moscow: Pedagogika.
  • Bozhovich, L. I. (1981). La personalidad y su formación en la edad infantil Habana. Pueblo y Educación.
  • Bozhovich, L. I. (2009). The social situation of child development. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 47(4), 59-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/RPO1061-0405470403
    » https://doi.org/10.2753/RPO1061-0405470403
  • González Rey, F. (1997). Epistemología cualitativa y subjectividad São Paulo: EDUC.
  • González Rey, F. (2002). Sujeto y subjetividad: una aproximación histórico-cultural México: Thomson Learning.
  • González Rey, F. (2005). O social na psicologia e a psicologia social Petrópolis: Vozes.
  • González Rey, F. (2009). Historical relevance of Vygotsky’s work: Its significance for a new approach to the problem of subjectivity in psychology. Outlines: Critical Practice Studies, 11(1), 59-73.
  • González Rey, F. (2011). A re-examination of defining moments in Vygotsky’s work and their implications for his continuing legacy. Mind, Culture & Activity, 18(3), 257-275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10749030903338517
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/10749030903338517
  • González Rey, F. (2014). Advancing further the history of Soviet psychology: Moving forward from dominant representations in Western and Soviet psychology. History of Psychology, 17(1), 60-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035565
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035565
  • González Rey, F. (2015a). Human motivation in question: Discussing emotions, motives, and subjectivity from a cultural-historical standpoint. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 45(4), 419-439. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jtsb.12073
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/jtsb.12073
  • González Rey, F. (2015b). A new path for the discussion of social representations: Advancing the topic of subjectivity from a cultural-historical standpoint. Theory of Psychology, 25(4), 1-19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0959354315587783
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354315587783
  • González Rey, F. (2016a). Advancing the topics of social reality, culture, and subjectivity from a cultural-historical standpoint: Moments, paths, and contradictions. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 36(3), 175-189.
  • González Rey, F. (2016b). Advances in subjectivity from a cultural-historical perspective: Unfolding and consequences for cultural studies today. In M. Fleer, F. González Rey, & N. Veresov (Eds.), Perezhivanie, emotions and subjectivity: Advancing Vygotsky´s legacy (pp.173-194). Singapore: Springer.
  • González Rey, F. (2017). The topic of subjectivity in psychology: Contradictions, paths and new alternatives. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 47(4), 502-521.
  • González Rey, F., & Mitjans Martínez, A. (2016). Perezhivanie: Advancing on its implications for the cultural-historical approach. International Research in Early Childhood Education Journal, 7(1), 143-160.
  • González Rey, F., & Mitjans Martínez, A. (2017). Subje-tividade: teoria, epistemologia e método Campinas: Alínea.
  • Leontiev, A. A. (1984). The productive career of Aleksei Nikolaevich Leont’ev. Soviet Psychology, 23(1), 6-56.
  • Leontiev, A. A. (1992). Ecce Homo: Methodological problems of the activity theoretical approach. Multidisciplinary Newsletter for Activity Theory, 11/12, 41-45.
  • Leontiev, A. N. (1965). Psikhologiya Iskustva [The Psychology of Art] Moscow: Iskustva Isdatelstva.
  • Leontiev, A. N. (1998). Ushenie o srede vpedologisheskix rabotax L. S. Vygotskogo [Theory on environment in the pedological works of Vygotsky]. Voprosy Psychologii, 1, 108-124. (Original work published 1937).
  • Luria, A. R. (1928). Psychology in Russia. The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, 35(3), 347-355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1928.10533070
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/08856559.1928.10533070
  • Mitjans Martínez, A., & González Rey, F. (2017). Psicologia, educação e aprendizagem escolar São Paulo: Cortez.
  • Van Der Veer, R., & Valsiner, J. (1991). Understanding Vygotsky: A quest for synthesis New York: Blackwell.
  • Vygotsky, L .S. (1965). Psikhologiya Iskustva [The Psychology of Art] Moscow: Iskustva Isdatelstva.
  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1971). Psychology of art New York: MIT Press.
  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In R. Rieber, & A. Carton (Eds.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky (Vol. 1, pp.43-287). New York: Plenum.
  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1998). The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky (Vol. 5). New York: Plenum.
  • Vygotsky, L. S. (2012). The science of psychology. Journal of Russian and East European Journal, 50(4), 85-106.
  • Yarochevsky, M. G. (2007). L. S. Vygotsky: V poiskax novoi psykjologii [L. S. Vygotsky: In the search for a new psychology]. Moscow: L. K. I.
  • Yasnitsky, A. (2009). Vygotsky circle during the decade of 1931-1941: Toward an integrative science of mind, brain and education (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Toronto, Canada.
  • Yasnitsky, A. (2012). Revisionist revolution in Vygotskian science: Toward cultural-historical Gestalt Psychology. Journal of Russian and East European, 50(4), 3-15. http://dx.doi.org/10.2753/RPO1061-0405500400
    » https://doi.org/10.2753/RPO1061-0405500400
  • Yasnitsky, A. (2015). A transnational history of “the beginning of a beautiful friendship”: The birth of the Cultural Historical Gestalt Psychology of Alexander Luria, Kurt Lewin, Lev Vygotsky and others. In: A. Yasnitsky & R. Van Der Veer (Eds.), Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies (pp.201-226). London. Routledge.
  • Zavershneva, E. (2015). The way to freedom: Vygotsky in 1932. In: A.Yasnitsky & R. Van Der Veer (Eds.), Revisionist Revolution in Vygotsky Studies (pp.127-140). London: Routledge.
  • Zinchenko, V. P. (1993). Kulturno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya. Opyt amplifikatsii. [Cultural-historical psychology: The experience of amplification]. Voprosy Psychologii [Questions of Psychology], 4, 5-19.
  • Zinchenko, V. P. (2002). Problematika myshleniya v razvivayushevo obucheniya [The problem of thinking in developmental education]. In Razvivayushee obrazovanie. Dialog c V.V. Davydovym [Developmental education. Dialog with V. V. Davydov] (pp.46-102). Moscow: Akademiya.
  • Zinchenko, V. P. (2009). Consciousness as the subject matter and task of psychology. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 47(5), 44-75.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Oct-Dec 2018

History

  • Received
    12 Mar 2018
  • Reviewed
    06 June 2018
  • Accepted
    19 June 2018
Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia, Pontifícia Universidade Católica de Campinas Núcleo de Editoração SBI - Campus II, Av. John Boyd Dunlop, s/n. Prédio de Odontologia, 13060-900 Campinas - São Paulo Brasil, Tel./Fax: +55 19 3343-7223 - Campinas - SP - Brazil
E-mail: psychologicalstudies@puc-campinas.edu.br