Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Comparison of pain threshold and duration of pain perception in men and women of different ages

Comparação do limiar de dor e tempo de percepção de dor em homens e mulheres de diferentes faixas etárias

Abstracts

Introduction

Pain is a sensory and emotional experience that occurs with the presence of tissue injury, actual or potential. Pain is subjective, and its expression is primarily determined by the perceived intensity of the painful sensation, called the pain threshold.

Objective

To evaluate whether there are differences in pain threshold (LD) and time to pain perception (TPED) between the gender in different age groups and to analyze the correlation between age and pain threshold in each gender.

Methods and procedures

Participants were 60 volunteers divided into 6 groups (n = 10 each) according to gender and age (18–33, 34–49, and 50–64 years). The evaluation of perception and pain tolerance was performed by immersing the container with one hand in water at a temperature of 0 °C–2 °C; the latency to withdrawal of the hand from ice water was measured in seconds and was considered a measure of LD. The TPED was reported by each participant as the start time of the painful stimulus.

Results

We found differences between the LD for G1 (men aged between 18 and 33 years) and G2 (women aged 18 to 33 years) with greater LD for G1 (p = 0.0122) and greater LD for women (p = 0.0094); for other comparisons of LD and TPED, there were no differences (p > 0.05 for all comparisons). Low correlation was found between age progression with increased LD and the TPED only in men (p = 0.01 and r = 0.45 and p = 0.05 and r = 0.34, respectively).

Conclusion

We conclude that women have a higher pain threshold than men especially when these groups are aged between 18 and 33 years, and in men increasing age correlates with increased TPED and LD.

Pain; Pain threshold; Pain perception


Introdução

A dor é uma experiência sensorial e emocional que aparece com a presença de lesões teciduais, reais ou potenciais. A dor tem caráter subjetivo e sua exteriorização é determinada, principalmente, pela intensidade percebida da sensação dolorosa, chamada limiar de dor.

Objetivo

Avaliar se há diferença no limiar de dor (LM) e no tempo de percepção a dor (TPED) entre os sexos em diferentes faixas etárias, e analisar se há correlação entre faixa etária e limiar de dor em cada sexo.

Métodos e procedimentos

Fizeram parte do estudo 60 voluntários, divididos em 6 grupos (n = 10 cada) de acordo com o sexo e faixa etária (18 a 33, 34 a 49 e 50 a 64 anos). A avaliação da percepção e tolerância à dor foi realizada pela imersão da mão em um recipiente com água na temperatura de 0 °C–2 °C, a latência para retirada da mão da água gelada foi medida em segundos e considerada como medida do LD, o TPED foi relatado por cada participante como o instante de início do estímulo doloroso.

Resultados

Foram observadas diferenças entre o LD para os grupos G1 (homens com idade entre 18 e 33 anos) e G2 (mulheres com idade entre 18 e 33 anos), com maior LD para o G1 (p = 0,0122); e entre os sexos, maior LD para mulheres (p=0,0094), para as demais comparações do LD e do TPED não houve diferenças ( > 0,05 para todas as comparações). Foi encontrada baixa correlação entre progressão da idade com aumento do LD; quanto ao TPED, tal correlação foi detectada apenas nos homens (respectivamente p = 0,01 e r = 0,45; e p = 0,05 e r = 0,34).

Conclusão

Concluímos que as mulheres apresentam maior limiar doloroso que os homens, principalmente quando estes grupos estão com idade entre 18 e 33 anos; e que nos homens o aumento da idade se correlaciona tanto com o aumento do TPED quanto do LD.

Dor; Limiar da dor; Percepção da dor


Introduction

Pain is a multi-factorial phenomenon influenced by tissue injury and emotional, socio-cultural, and environmental aspects and is conceptualized as “an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience that is described in terms of actual or potential tissue injuries” (1Turner JA, Franklin G, Heagerty PJ, Wu R, Egan K, Fulton-Kehoe D, et al. The association between pain and disability. Pain. 2004;112(3):307-14.). It has subjective characteristics, and each individual learns and uses this term based on their previous experiences (2Oliveira AS, Bermudez CC, Souza RA, Souza CMF, Dias EM, Castro CE, et al. Impacto da dor na vida de portadores de disfunção temporomandibular. J Appl Oral Sci. 2003;112(2):138-43.). We consider this sensation to be a protective mechanism of the body against tissue damage, acting as a psychological adjunct to a protective reflex that causes the affected tissue to diverge from harmful and/or noxious stimuli.

Pain is part of everyday life for most people and is closely related to individuals’ quality of life (3Pessoa CP, Barreto MB, Santos LB, Alves TDB, Oliveira MC, Martins AG. Instrumentos utilizados na avaliação do impacto da dor na qualidade de vida de pacientes com dor profacial e disfunção temporomandibular. Rev Baiana Saúde Pública. 2007;31:267-93.). It is one of the symptoms that most often leads individuals to seek professional care in health services (4Budó MLD, Nicolini D, Resta DG, Büttenbender E, Pippi MC, Ressel LB. A cultura permeando os sentimentos e as reações frente à dor. Rev Esc Enfer USP. 2007;41(1):36-43.). It can lead to other health problems, such as eating disorders, sleep irregularities, and systemic bodily fatigue (5Roth-Isigkeit A, Thyen U, Stöven H, Schwarzenberger J, Schmucker P. Pain among children and adolescents: restrictions in daily living and triggering factors. Pediatrics. 2005;115(2):152-62., 6Karoly P, Ruehlman LS. Psychosocial aspects of pain-related life task interference: an exploratory analysis in a general population sample. Pain Med. 2007;8(7):563-72.). In recent decades, the incidence has been increasing, probably due to changes in lifestyle, increased survival of severely ill patients, and increased life expectancy (7Mailis A, Papagapiou M. Prolife of patients admitted to the pain facility of a university affiliated acute care hospital. Pain Clinic. 1993;6:71-82.).

Intense stimuli, whether thermal, mechanical, or chemical, can activate nociceptors. When tissue is injured or irritated, chemical substances (histamine, prostaglandins, bradykinin) are released, causing the activation of pain receptors and making the tissue hypersensitive; thus, after the initial insult, the tissue will become irritable and sensitive (8Klatzkin RR, Mechlin B, Girdler SS. Menstrual cycle phase does not influence gender differences in experimental pain sensitivity. Eur J Pain. 2010;14(1):77.).

There are several studies that examine differences between men and women in the perception and tolerance of pain. However, the results are controversial and find no consensus as to which gender is more sensitive to pain. According to some authors, women experience more frequent conditions of pain, a better ability to describe pain, a lower sensory threshold, less tolerance to pain, and a more unpleasant sensation (8Klatzkin RR, Mechlin B, Girdler SS. Menstrual cycle phase does not influence gender differences in experimental pain sensitivity. Eur J Pain. 2010;14(1):77., 9Vallerand AH, Poloma RC. The relationship of gender to pain. Pain Manag Nurs. 2000;19(3 Suppl 1):8-15., 1010 Myers CD, Riley JL, Robinson ME. Psychosocial contributions to sex-correlated differences in pain. Clin J Pain. 2003;19(4):225-32., 1111 Wiesenfeld-Hallin Z. Sex differences in pain perception. Gend Med. 2005;2(3):137-45., 1212 Ramage-Morin PL, Gilmour H. Chronic pain at ages 12 to 44. Health Rep. 2010;21(4):53-61.). On the other hand, for Budó and colleagues (4Budó MLD, Nicolini D, Resta DG, Büttenbender E, Pippi MC, Ressel LB. A cultura permeando os sentimentos e as reações frente à dor. Rev Esc Enfer USP. 2007;41(1):36-43.), women support more pain than men, because they live with menstrual cramps and go through the pain of childbirth.

Another factor that should be taken into account when analyzing the perception of pain is age. Studies with the young and the old, attempting to assess the sensitivity to and tolerance of pain, demonstrate that there may be differences in pain sensitivity between age groups (1212 Ramage-Morin PL, Gilmour H. Chronic pain at ages 12 to 44. Health Rep. 2010;21(4):53-61., 1313 Melzack R, Gagliese L. Age differences in nociception and pain behaviours in the rat. Rev Neurosci Biobehav. 2000;24(8):843-54., 1414 Andrade FA, Pereira LV, Souza FAEF. Mensuração da dor no idoso: uma revisão. Rev Latino-Am Enfermagem. 2006;14(2):271-6.)

In order to assist in obtaining parameters on pain, this study aims to investigate differences in pain threshold between the genders in different age groups and to analyze whether there is a correlation between age and pain threshold.

Methods and procedures

Experimental design and ethical aspects

The study is characterized as longitudinal, experimental consecutive sampling. All subjects agreed to participate by signing the Informed Consent Form, in accordance with the provisions of Resolution 196/96. The development of the research was approved by the Ethics Committee in Research of the Faculty of Philosophy and Sciences at Marília - Unesp, protocol nº 1866/2010.

Subjects

A total of 60 volunteers took part in the study, 30 female and 30 male residents from the city of Marília, SP, Brazil. The volunteers were divided, according to gender and age group, into six groups of 10 subjects each: Group 1 (G1), composed of female volunteers aged 18–33 years; Group 2 (G2), consisting of male volunteers aged 18–33 years; Group 3 (G3), composed of female volunteers aged 34–49 years; Group 4 (G4), consisting of male volunteers aged 34–49 years; Group 5 (G5), composed of female volunteers aged 50–64 years; and Group 6 (G6), consisting of male volunteers aged 50–64 years. The presence of peripheral neuropathy or previous surgery on the dominant hand, cardiac or neuromuscular disorders, or premenstrual period or catamenial flow were used as exclusion criteria (1515 Santos PCP, Ramos JLO, Motta MA, Montenegro EJN. Efeitos da TENS acupuntural no limiar da dor induzida pelo frio. Fisioter Mov. 2008;21(4):4-49.).

Procedures

The volunteers were properly accommodated in an acclimatized room in a comfortable posture and free of any situation that could cause distraction and thus interfere with the results of the experiment. They were measured for their weight and height using an anthropometric scale (JB Scales®, from São Paulo, SP, Brazil) for subsequent calculation of Body Mass Index (BMI). All underwent 15 minutes of habituation to the environment in which the experiments were conducted.

Initially, the volunteers immersed the dominant hand in a container with warm water at 37 °C for a period of five minutes (1616 Johnson MI, Tabasam G. A single-blind investigation into the hypoalgesic effects of different swing patterns of interferential currents on cold-induced pain in healthy volunteers. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2003;84(3):350-7.). Temperature control was achieved by means of a water boiler (Western®, from São Paulo, SP, Brazil) and verified using a mercury thermometer (WalMur®, from Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil). After removing the hand from the warm water, it was immediately immersed in a container with ice-cold water at 0 °C–2 °C (1515 Santos PCP, Ramos JLO, Motta MA, Montenegro EJN. Efeitos da TENS acupuntural no limiar da dor induzida pelo frio. Fisioter Mov. 2008;21(4):4-49., 1717 Jonhson M, Din A. Ethnocultural differences in the analgecic effects of placebo transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation on cold-induced pain in healthy subjects: a preliminary study. Complement Ther Med. 1997;5:74-9.). Temperature control was maintained by adding ice cubes and monitored by means of a mercury thermometer (WalMur®), thereby ensuring the maintenance of the stimulus at the originally determined temperature. Volunteers remained with their hands immersed until the discomfort caused by the low temperature reached a maximum subjective intensity, forcing them to withdraw their hands. The permanence time in the ice-cold water was measured using a stopwatch. The time until the verbal report of pain was taken as the time of perception to painful stimuli (TPED), and the time between immersion and withdrawal of the limb was considered the pain threshold (LD).

Statistical analysis

For data analysis, the Shapiro-Wilk normality test and Levene's test were used to check the homogeneity of variance. Then, the Kruskal-Wallis test was applied followed by the multiple comparisons Student-Newman-Keuls test. For correlation analysis, the Spearman correlation test was used. The magnitude of the correlations was established at: low, from 0.26 to 0.49; moderate, from 0.50 to 0.69; high, from 0.70 to 0.89; and very high, from 0.90 to 1.00. The level of significance was set at 5% (p ≤ 0.05).

Results

Groups as well as individuals in their totality, divided between male participants and female participants, were shown to be homogeneous in age and BMI. During the comparison between groups of the same age, statistically significant differences were only evident for LD between G1 and G2 (p = 0.0122); for other comparisons, G3 with G4 (p = 0.0695) and G5 with G6 (p = 0.8203), there were no significant differences. For comparisons between different age groups, we observed significant differences between G2 vs. G3 (p = 0.0065), G2 vs. G5 (p = 0.0190), and G2 vs. G6 (p = 0.0283); for the other comparisons, no statistically significant differences were observed (G1 vs. G3 [p = 0.4720], G1 vs. G4 [p = 0.1847], G1 vs. G5 [p = 0.6761], G1 vs. G6 [p = 1], G2 vs. G4 [p = 0.2261], G3 vs. G5 [p = 1], G3 vs. G6 [p = 0.4717], G4 vs. G5 [p = 0.1852] and G4 vs. G6 [p = 0.2897]).

The comparison between groups of the same age for the TPED showed no statistically significant differences for G1 vs. G2 (p = 0.4214), G3 vs. G4 (p = 0.4932), and G5 vs. G6 (p = 0.6197). For comparisons between different age groups, we observed significant differences between G2 vs. G4 (p = 0.0292) and G2 vs. G6 (p = 0.0468); for the other comparisons, no statistically significant differences were observed (G1 vs. G3 [p = 0.4919], G1 vs. G4 [p = 0.1807], G1 vs. G5 [p = 0.1488], G1 vs. G6 [p = 0.2883], G2 vs. G3 [p = 0.1817], G2 vs. G5 [p = 0.202], G3 vs. G5 [p = 0.5941], G3 vs. G6 [p = 0.7030], G4 vs. G5 [p = 0.8491], and G4 vs. G6 [p = 0.5169]). Table 1 shows the characteristics of the sample regarding gender, age, LD, TPED, and BMI within the groups assessed.

Table 1
Characterization of the subjects studied

Table 2 details the characteristics of participants grouped according to gender. We observed differences between men and women only for LD (p = 0.0094); differences between the genders were not observed for TPED (p = 0.8118).

Table 2
Representation of the subjects divided according to gender

Table 3 shows the correlations between the total scores of LD, TPED, and age.

Table 3
Correlations between LD, TPED, and age

Table 4 describes the correlations between the representative scores of the gender-related characteristics of the participants.

Table 4
Correlations between characteristics referring to the gender of the participants

Discussion

The aim of our study was to analyze differences in the pain threshold between the genders in different age groups and to analyze whether there is a correlation between the age group with the pain threshold of men and women. These aspects related to pain modulation in accordance with gender and age should be taken into consideration to tailor treatments and medications to the specifications of each patient. Research on the differences based on the experience of pain between the genders could allow the adaptation of the treatment for pain to the characteristics of the individuals (1818 Paller CJ, Campbell CM, Edwards RR, Dobs AS. Sex-based differences in pain perception and treatment. Pain Med. 2009;10(2):289-99.). Our methodology combines knowledge, as it proposes to seek under-explored differences in literature regarding LD, TPED, and their modulation by both age and gender.

In this study, we evaluated the sensitivity of LD and TPED for the dominant upper extremity immersed in water at approximately zero degrees, reproducing an acute nociceptive pain characteristic (1919 Lacerda PF, Godoy LF, Cobianchi MG, Bachion MM. Estudo da ocorrência de “Dor Crônica” em idosos de uma comunidade atendida pelo orograma saúde da família em goiânia. Rev Eletr Enf. 2005;7(1):29-40.). LD was regarded as the greatest intensity of time in which the stimulus is perceived as being uncomfortable to the point of the individual retracting his or her hand and/or requesting discontinuation, and TPED was considered the first report of pain after immersion.

The discrimination of different degrees of temperature is perceived by thermoreceptors located on the surface of the skin as well as free nerve endings. The cold receptors are linked to medium-diameter myelinated fibers in group Aδ, although there are some fibers attached to small calibrated non-myelinated fibers in group C. Nerve fibers react differently to each thermal stimulus. When the temperature is very low, close to 0 °C, only cold-pain fibers are stimulated; it is reported that, when immersing a region in cold water, the first perceived sensation is the cold, replaced in up to one minute by a deep sense of pain and discomfort, triggering sensations of pricking, stinging and tingling, intensity, and gradual decline of one to three minutes when anesthesia of the region occurs (feeling of numbness), i.e., partial loss of thermal, tactile sensation among others. However, even after termination of the thermal stimulus, the sensations of deep discomforting pain, throbbing, or burning are still perceived (2020 Liu NJ, Schnell S, Wessendorf MW, Gintzler AR. Sex, pain, and opioids: interdependent influences of sex and pain modality on dynorphin-mediated antinociception in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2013; 344(2):522-30.).

During our study protocol, the maximum time of 35 seconds for TPED was obtained by an individual from G4, and for LD 93 seconds was obtained by a subject from G3. However, in our experiment, a significant difference was found only among participants in G1 and G2 and when males and females were separated for LD. As for the TPED, no significant differences were observed. The results observed between males and females differ from those of other studies in which the authors indicate that female patients were more prone to complaints of pain, explaining such an assertion with the greatest variety of painful conditions assigned to women, such as migraines, temporomandibular disorders, fibromyalgia complaints, pregnancy, and childbirth, besides having more severe levels of pain with more frequent episodes and of a longer duration than men (2121 Unruh AM. Gender variations in clinical pain experience. Pain. 1996;65(2-3):123-67., 2222 Wiljnhven HA, de Vet HC, Picavet HS. Prevalence of musculoskeletal disorders is systematically higher in women than in men. Clin J Pain. 2006;22(8):717-24., 2323 Berkley KJ. Sex differences in pain. Behav Brain Sci. 1997;20(3):371-80., 2424 Little JM, Qin C, Farber JP, Foreman RD. Spinal cord processing of cardiac nociception: are there sex differences between male and proestrous female rats? Brain Res. 2011; 1413:24-31.).

It is noteworthy that the differences in the sensation of pain between genders can vary greatly across studies because of the different methods used for its measurement as well as the body area stimulated and the number of repetitions used during the experiment (1818 Paller CJ, Campbell CM, Edwards RR, Dobs AS. Sex-based differences in pain perception and treatment. Pain Med. 2009;10(2):289-99.). Other factors that are difficult to control and can influence the results are the psychological characteristics of each participant; different levels of anxiety, depression, or previous painful experiences of each subject can alter the reports of pain (2525 Greenspan JD, Craft RM, Le Resche L, Arendt-Nielsen L, Berkley KJ, Fillingim RB, et al. Studying sex and gender differences in pain and analgesia: a consensus report. Pain. 2007;132(Suppl 1):26-45., 2626 Carstensen TB,  Frostholm L,  Oernboel E,  Kongsted A,  Kasch H,  Jensen TS, et al. Are there gender differences in coping with neck pain following acute whiplash trauma? A 12-month follow-up study. Eur J Pain. 2012;16(1):49-60., 2727 Hirsh AT,  Bockow TB,  Jensen MP. Catastrophizing, pain, and pain interference in individuals with disabilities. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 2011;90(9):713-22.).

Correlation analysis between TPEDt and AGEt identifies an increase in the time of pain perception with increasing age. Andrade and colleagues (1414 Andrade FA, Pereira LV, Souza FAEF. Mensuração da dor no idoso: uma revisão. Rev Latino-Am Enfermagem. 2006;14(2):271-6., 2828 Lautenbacher S. Experimental approaches in the study of pain in the elderly. Pain Med. 2012;13(2):44-50.) found that advanced age results in loss of pain perception; among the physiological factors pointed out, it is believed that the degree of conservation of the peripheral nervous system is lower in older people, which predisposes damages in the afferents related to pain perception.

The moderate correlation directly proportional between TPEDt and LDt indicates the longer the perception of painful stimuli, the more the pain threshold will be. However, LDt is not related to AGEt. Based on these results, we hypothesized that the modulation of pain sensation may occur on an organization of levels where the stimuli are added, and, even though the pain perception is initially reduced due to advanced age, the summation of stimuli occurring triggers protective responses based on individual previous experiences.

When we analyzed these responses and related them to the participants’ gender, we realized that LD continues to correlate with TPED in both men and women. Nevertheless, differing from the overall results, the age in males plays a relevant role in modulating both TPED and LD, whereas women's age is not correlated with TPED or LD. Even though there is no consensus regarding the modulation of the menstrual cycle phase on pain sensitivity, a likely explanation is that female hormonal changes predispose women to episodes of pain with characteristics from the menstrual cycle phase, regardless of age (8Klatzkin RR, Mechlin B, Girdler SS. Menstrual cycle phase does not influence gender differences in experimental pain sensitivity. Eur J Pain. 2010;14(1):77., 2929 Pahim LS, Menzes AMB, Lima R. Prevalência e fatores associados à enxaqueca na população adulta de Pelotas, RS. Rev Saúde Pública. 2006;40(4):692-8., 3030 Mogil JS. Sex differences in pain and pain inhibition: multiple explanations of a controversial phenomenon. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(12):859-66.).

The continuity of research that relates pain to gender should encompass different age groups, including subjects under 18 years of age and those over 60 years of age. Another point that should be taken into consideration is the menstrual cycle to better understand the relationship between hormonal variation and threshold of pain.

Conclusion

We conclude that, for the experimental condition proposed, women have a higher pain threshold than men, especially when these groups are aged between 18 and 33 years. We also conclude that, in men, increased age correlates well with the increase of TPED and LD. However, in women, age group is not related to TPED or LD. There may be other factors related to the perception of pain, for example, hormonal changes due to the menstrual cycle phase.

References

  • 1
    Turner JA, Franklin G, Heagerty PJ, Wu R, Egan K, Fulton-Kehoe D, et al. The association between pain and disability. Pain. 2004;112(3):307-14.
  • 2
    Oliveira AS, Bermudez CC, Souza RA, Souza CMF, Dias EM, Castro CE, et al. Impacto da dor na vida de portadores de disfunção temporomandibular. J Appl Oral Sci. 2003;112(2):138-43.
  • 3
    Pessoa CP, Barreto MB, Santos LB, Alves TDB, Oliveira MC, Martins AG. Instrumentos utilizados na avaliação do impacto da dor na qualidade de vida de pacientes com dor profacial e disfunção temporomandibular. Rev Baiana Saúde Pública. 2007;31:267-93.
  • 4
    Budó MLD, Nicolini D, Resta DG, Büttenbender E, Pippi MC, Ressel LB. A cultura permeando os sentimentos e as reações frente à dor. Rev Esc Enfer USP. 2007;41(1):36-43.
  • 5
    Roth-Isigkeit A, Thyen U, Stöven H, Schwarzenberger J, Schmucker P. Pain among children and adolescents: restrictions in daily living and triggering factors. Pediatrics. 2005;115(2):152-62.
  • 6
    Karoly P, Ruehlman LS. Psychosocial aspects of pain-related life task interference: an exploratory analysis in a general population sample. Pain Med. 2007;8(7):563-72.
  • 7
    Mailis A, Papagapiou M. Prolife of patients admitted to the pain facility of a university affiliated acute care hospital. Pain Clinic. 1993;6:71-82.
  • 8
    Klatzkin RR, Mechlin B, Girdler SS. Menstrual cycle phase does not influence gender differences in experimental pain sensitivity. Eur J Pain. 2010;14(1):77.
  • 9
    Vallerand AH, Poloma RC. The relationship of gender to pain. Pain Manag Nurs. 2000;19(3 Suppl 1):8-15.
  • 10
    Myers CD, Riley JL, Robinson ME. Psychosocial contributions to sex-correlated differences in pain. Clin J Pain. 2003;19(4):225-32.
  • 11
    Wiesenfeld-Hallin Z. Sex differences in pain perception. Gend Med. 2005;2(3):137-45.
  • 12
    Ramage-Morin PL, Gilmour H. Chronic pain at ages 12 to 44. Health Rep. 2010;21(4):53-61.
  • 13
    Melzack R, Gagliese L. Age differences in nociception and pain behaviours in the rat. Rev Neurosci Biobehav. 2000;24(8):843-54.
  • 14
    Andrade FA, Pereira LV, Souza FAEF. Mensuração da dor no idoso: uma revisão. Rev Latino-Am Enfermagem. 2006;14(2):271-6.
  • 15
    Santos PCP, Ramos JLO, Motta MA, Montenegro EJN. Efeitos da TENS acupuntural no limiar da dor induzida pelo frio. Fisioter Mov. 2008;21(4):4-49.
  • 16
    Johnson MI, Tabasam G. A single-blind investigation into the hypoalgesic effects of different swing patterns of interferential currents on cold-induced pain in healthy volunteers. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2003;84(3):350-7.
  • 17
    Jonhson M, Din A. Ethnocultural differences in the analgecic effects of placebo transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation on cold-induced pain in healthy subjects: a preliminary study. Complement Ther Med. 1997;5:74-9.
  • 18
    Paller CJ, Campbell CM, Edwards RR, Dobs AS. Sex-based differences in pain perception and treatment. Pain Med. 2009;10(2):289-99.
  • 19
    Lacerda PF, Godoy LF, Cobianchi MG, Bachion MM. Estudo da ocorrência de “Dor Crônica” em idosos de uma comunidade atendida pelo orograma saúde da família em goiânia. Rev Eletr Enf. 2005;7(1):29-40.
  • 20
    Liu NJ, Schnell S, Wessendorf MW, Gintzler AR. Sex, pain, and opioids: interdependent influences of sex and pain modality on dynorphin-mediated antinociception in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2013; 344(2):522-30.
  • 21
    Unruh AM. Gender variations in clinical pain experience. Pain. 1996;65(2-3):123-67.
  • 22
    Wiljnhven HA, de Vet HC, Picavet HS. Prevalence of musculoskeletal disorders is systematically higher in women than in men. Clin J Pain. 2006;22(8):717-24.
  • 23
    Berkley KJ. Sex differences in pain. Behav Brain Sci. 1997;20(3):371-80.
  • 24
    Little JM, Qin C, Farber JP, Foreman RD. Spinal cord processing of cardiac nociception: are there sex differences between male and proestrous female rats? Brain Res. 2011; 1413:24-31.
  • 25
    Greenspan JD, Craft RM, Le Resche L, Arendt-Nielsen L, Berkley KJ, Fillingim RB, et al. Studying sex and gender differences in pain and analgesia: a consensus report. Pain. 2007;132(Suppl 1):26-45.
  • 26
    Carstensen TB,  Frostholm L,  Oernboel E,  Kongsted A,  Kasch H,  Jensen TS, et al. Are there gender differences in coping with neck pain following acute whiplash trauma? A 12-month follow-up study. Eur J Pain. 2012;16(1):49-60.
  • 27
    Hirsh AT,  Bockow TB,  Jensen MP. Catastrophizing, pain, and pain interference in individuals with disabilities. Am J Phys Med Rehabil. 2011;90(9):713-22.
  • 28
    Lautenbacher S. Experimental approaches in the study of pain in the elderly. Pain Med. 2012;13(2):44-50.
  • 29
    Pahim LS, Menzes AMB, Lima R. Prevalência e fatores associados à enxaqueca na população adulta de Pelotas, RS. Rev Saúde Pública. 2006;40(4):692-8.
  • 30
    Mogil JS. Sex differences in pain and pain inhibition: multiple explanations of a controversial phenomenon. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(12):859-66.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    jan-mar 2014

History

  • Received
    21 May 2013
  • Accepted
    07 Dec 2013
Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Paraná Rua Imaculada Conceição, 1155 - Prado-Velho -, Curitiba - PR - CEP 80215-901, Telefone: (41) 3271-1608 - Curitiba - PR - Brazil
E-mail: revista.fisioterapia@pucpr.br