Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Spatial updating of targets in front and behind

Atualização espacial de alvos a frente e atrás do observador

Abstracts

This research compared the accuracy of spatial updating of targets in front with that of targets behind. The participant viewed a target on the ground several meters away and then, without vision, sidestepped along a guide rope while trying to mentally update the location of the target. On some trials, the target location was in front as the person sidestepped and, on other trials, the target location was behind. Participants responded by facing the updated target location with eyes closed. The results indicate that people are able to update target locations behind them very nearly as well as target locations in front.

Visual perception; space perception; locomotion; action


Esta pesquisa comparou a acurácia da atualização espacial de alvos situados à frente com a de alvos situados atrás do observador. O participante observava um alvo no solo distante vários metros dele e então, sem a visão, andou de lado ao longo de uma corda-guia enquanto tentava atualizar mentalmente a localização do alvo. Em algumas tentativas, a localização do alvo esteve à frente do observador e, em outras tentativas, a localização do alvo foi atrás. Os participantes respondiam encarando a localização atualizada do alvo com os olhos fechados. Os resultados indicaram que as pessoas são capazes de atualizar a localização de alvos localizados atrás delas aproximadamente tão bem quanto localizações de alvos à frente delas.

Percepção visual; percepção espacial; locomoção; ação


Spatial updating of targets in front and behind

Atualização espacial de alvos a frente e atrás do observador

David Horn; Jack Loomis

University of California, Santa Barbara - USA

Endereço para correspondência Endereço para correspondência: David Horn Surgical Resident, Department of Otolaryngology-Head & Neck Surgery Indiana University School of Medicine Indianapolis, IN 46220, USA E-mail: hornears@yahoo.com Jack M. Loomis Department of Psychology University of California, Santa Barbara Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA E-mail: loomis@psych.ucsb.edu

ABSTRACT

This research compared the accuracy of spatial updating of targets in front with that of targets behind. The participant viewed a target on the ground several meters away and then, without vision, sidestepped along a guide rope while trying to mentally update the location of the target. On some trials, the target location was in front as the person sidestepped and, on other trials, the target location was behind. Participants responded by facing the updated target location with eyes closed. The results indicate that people are able to update target locations behind them very nearly as well as target locations in front.

Key-words: Visual perception, space perception, locomotion, action

RESUMO

Esta pesquisa comparou a acurácia da atualização espacial de alvos situados à frente com a de alvos situados atrás do observador. O participante observava um alvo no solo distante vários metros dele e então, sem a visão, andou de lado ao longo de uma corda-guia enquanto tentava atualizar mentalmente a localização do alvo. Em algumas tentativas, a localização do alvo esteve à frente do observador e, em outras tentativas, a localização do alvo foi atrás. Os participantes respondiam encarando a localização atualizada do alvo com os olhos fechados. Os resultados indicaram que as pessoas são capazes de atualizar a localização de alvos localizados atrás delas aproximadamente tão bem quanto localizações de alvos à frente delas.

Palavras-chave: Percepção visual, percepção espacial, locomoção, ação

Spatial updating refers to the ability of a human or non-human agent to mentally keep track of one of more stationary targets, initially localized on the basis of sensory information, as the agent moves about in space while receiving no further sensory information about the target locations. Many non-human species are capable of spatial updating of initially viewed objects (the toad [Collett, 1982] and the jumping spider [Hill, 1979]). Similarly, humans can easily and accurately update one or more target locations initially specified by vision (Farrell & Robertson, 1997; Fukusima, Loomis, & Da Silva, 1997; Loomis, Da Silva, Fujita, & Fukusima, 1992; Philbeck, Loomis, & Beall, 1997; Rieser, 1989; Thomson, 1983), by audition (Ashmead, DeFord, & Northington, 1995; Loomis, Lippa, Klatzky, & Golledge, 2002), or by touch (Hollins & Kelley, 1988) as rotate, translate, or both.

Many of the experiments on spatial updating and all of those on "path integration" which require a person to return to an origin after walking along an outbound path (for review, Loomis, Klatzky, Golledge, & Philbeck, 1999) entail some updating of a target location behind the participant. Because people are quite accurate at updating on these tasks, there is good reason to believe that people can update targets behind them, possibly as well as they can update targets in front. The current study addresses this issue directly by comparing people's ability to update targets in front and behind. The experiment utilized a procedure which varied whether the imaginally updated target was in front or behind the participants as they sidestepped a short distance. The procedure kept constant virtually all other aspects of the participants' task, such as how they viewed the target stimulus and how they reported the direction of the updated target.

Method

Participants. We tested 28 participants, 13 females and 15 males, ages 19-22. All but two were undergraduate students at the University of California, Santa Barbara (UCSB). Each participant reported having normal visual acuity with or without optical correction. None had knowledge about the purpose of the experiment.

Experimental setting, stimulus layout, and materials. The experiment was conducted in a large grassy field on the UCSB campus adjacent to a straight concrete sidewalk. The field was free of any trees, shrubs, or other distinctive objects within 20 m of where the visual targets were placed, and the area in which the targets were placed was very nearly level. All trials were run during daylight hours, thus allowing abundant visual information for perceiving the target locations,

Figure 1 shows the experimental setup including stimulus layout. The 11-m guide rope was parallel to the edge of the sidewalk and attached at its ends to two stanchions. It was kept taut and positioned at waist height. The locations marked Start A, Start B, and Stop were on the sidewalk. The 10 target locations, labeled 1 to 5, were in the grassy field. Depending on the trial, the participant started out at the one of the two Start A or two Start B locations and subsequently sidestepped, while holding the guide rope, to one of the two Stop locations. The paired start and stop locations, on both sides of the rope, were used according to whether the trial involved updating behind or in front. For Behind trials, the participant was on the side of the rope nearer the field facing the rope, and on In Front trials, the participant was on the far side, also facing the rope. The two start locations were separated by 10.2 m and the Stop point was roughly midway between them.


The 10 target locations, 5 for Behind trials and 5 forthe target locations. In Front trials, were positioned approximately 3.3 m from the corresponding Stop locations. On a given trial, a white Styrofoam ball, 30 cm in diameter, was place on the ground at one of the target locations. It was fixed in place by impaling it on a thick wire inserted into the ground. The change in direction of the various targets going from Start A to Stop ranged from 36 to 102 deg, with a mean direction change of 69 deg. The change in direction of the various targets going from Start B to Stop ranged from 22 to 85 deg, with a mean direction change of 50 deg. For the two start locations, the mean direction change was 59.5 deg.

The participant indicated the direction of the imaginally updated target by turning the body and head to face it, all with eyes closed. The heading of the body was measured with a KVH electronic compass (model Azimuth 100) worn on the back. It had a digital display that could be read to the nearest degree. The participant also wore a backpack that carried a 12V battery that powered the electronic compass.

Procedure. There were several slight variations in procedure used in the experiment that proved inconsequential. Participants 1-10 simply closed the eyes when appropriate; whereas, participants 11-28 used a blindfold to prevent vision. Similar updating research by the second author has more often not used a blindfold (Loomis, et al., 1992; Fukusima, et al., 1997) because participants have been observed to comply with the instruction to keep the eyes closed. Also, participants 11-19 used a headset apparatus to prevent localization of incidental environmental sounds; whereas, the remaining participants had the use of normal hearing. Fukusima et al. (1997) found that spatial updating performance was not influenced by blocking the localization of environmental sounds.

Participants were deprived of vision during the experiment other than between trials and during the brief period at the beginning of each trial when the participant viewed the target. The experimenter and an assistant conducted the experiment. The experimenter gave instructions to the participant, led the participant to the appropriate start point on each trial, stopped the participant after the sidestepping traverse to the appropriate stop point, and recorded the compass readings. The assistant placed the target at the appropriate location on the ground prior to each trial.

Prior to each trial, the participant was led to either Start A or Start B on the appropriate side of the rope, depending on whether it was an In Front or Behind trial. In all cases, the trial began with the participant's turning the body (without vision) to face the Stop point while holding onto the rope. The participant used one hand to hold the rope; for those using a blindfold, the other hand was used to raise and lower the blindfold. Once facing the Stop point, the participant was told to view the target by raising the blindfold or opening the eyes. The participant turned the head, but not the body, toward the grassy field and viewed the target until satisfied with knowing its location. When ready, the participant closed the eyes or lowered the blindfold, turned the head toward the Stop point, and, then while holding the rope with one hand, turned the body (and head) toward the rope. On the rare occasion where this sequence was performed incorrectly, the trial was skipped and then reinserted at the end of the experiment.

With the participant now facing the rope, the participant was instructed to "walk" by sidestepping rapidly in the direction of the center of the rope. Those facing the rope and the grassy field were updating an unseen target in front of them; whereas, those facing the rope and facing away from the grassy field were updating an unseen target behind them. As the participant approached the stopping point, the experimenter gently slowed the participant to the correct location while holding on to his/her shoulders.

Once stopped but prior to making a response, the participant then immediately rotated 90 deg in a direction opposite to that which was turned initially. Thus, this post-walk orientation was the same as the pre-walk orientation. The two 90 deg turns before and after sidestepping were used to avoid the confound of larger turns in the Behind trials than in the In Front trials. Once in the post-walk orientation, the participant was instructed to "respond" by turning in place to face the updated target. Participants had been instructed to point toes, body, and nose toward the updated location. Once in the response orientation, participants were told to keep their shoulders parallel with the ground and their backs straight and to refrain from readjusting or swaying. The experimenter then took the compass reading and led the participant to the next starting point. Upon arriving at the new start point, the participant faced away from the grassy field and was allowed to open the eyes while the next target was placed in position.

Prior to the experimental trials, participants were given a number of In Front and Behind practice trials to acquaint them with the procedure. No feedback about updating accuracy was given. The experiment proper then began. After completing 2 trials in one of the two experimental conditions, both from opposite starting points (i.e., Start A and Start B), the participant moved to the starting point on the other side of the rope and then completed 2 trials in the other condition, again from opposite starting points. In other words, the starting point varied in rectangular fashion (Start A, In Front: Start B, In Front: Start B, Behind: Start A, Behind). This procedure was repeated until each of the 4 starting points was used once with each of the 5 targets, thus resulting in 20 trials in total.

Upon completion of the 20 trials, participants stopped in the final location and then faced body and head toward each of the 5 targets using vision. The instructions for facing the targets were identical to those used in the experimental trials, but now the participants no longer had to imagine each target. The headings were recorded for each of the 5 targets, after which the participant moved to the other Stop point and repeated the procedure. The 10 headings for In Front and Behind targets obtained with visually-based facing were used as reference values in computing the updating errors on the experimental trials. Use of these reference values eliminated some of the measurement error associated with variations in how the electronic compass was placed on the back and in how different participants aimed their bodies toward the visible or updated target.

Results

Two of the 28 participants were excused from the experiment because of technical problems. For the remaining participants, signed error scores (in deg) were computed for each trial. For a given target, this error score was the difference between the compass reading of the updating response and the compass reading when the participant faced the same target using vision. For each target, condition, and participant, we obtained two signed error scores, one for Start A and one for Start B. For each pair of scores we computed the standard deviation, which is also equal to one half of the absolute value of the difference score.

The mean signed error for each participant's two responses (for Start A and Start B) was computed for each target in each condition. These individual means were then averaged over participants to give the means plotted in Figure 2, as a function of target and condition. Mean signed error is a measure of any systematic bias away from the correct direction. The grand means for In Front and Behind were 1.7 deg and 0.9 deg, respectively. A mixed-model three-way ANOVA computed on the individual mean constant errors indicated no significant effects.


The two absolute errors for each participant's two responses (for Start A and Start B) were averaged to obtain the individual's mean absolute error for each target in each condition. These individual means were then averaged over participants to give the means plotted in Figure 3, as a function of target and condition. Mean absolute error indicates the magnitude of error, regardless of sign, expected on a typical trial for the typical participant. The grand means for In Front and Behind were 12.6 and 14.8 deg, respectively. A mixed-model three-way ANOVA indicated two statistically significant effects: a main effect of target (F(4, 22) = 3.032, p < .05) and an interaction between target and condition, (F(4, 22) = 4.495, p < .01). The effect of condition was marginally significant, (F(1, 25) = 3.732, p = .065). This analysis indicates that updating behind is worse than updating in front for one or more of the targets.


Figure 4 gives the mean value of the standard deviation (in deg) as a function of target and condition. Each mean was computed on the 26 standard deviations (computed on the two signed errors for each of the 26 participants). The grand means of these standard deviations for In Front and Behind were 9.5 and 10.3 deg, respectively. A mixed-model three-way ANOVA indicated that only the main effect of target was significant, (F(4,22) = 5.084, p < .05).


Discussion

In this experiment, participants sidestepped through space while updating targets that were in front or behind. The mean change in direction of the targets during these traverses was 59.5 deg. The results given in Figure 1 show that the mean signed error, averaged over participants, ranged from 4 to -1 deg in the In Front condition and from to 5 to -3 deg in the Behind condition. Compared to the mean change in directions of the targets, these small mean signed errors signify that participants were, on average, accurate in updating targets both in front and behind. Even the mean absolute errors, which indicate the magnitude of error expected on a given trial and which averaged 12.6 and 14.8 deg for In Front and Behind, respectively, are an impressively small fraction of the mean change in direction.

Although the statistical analysis of absolute error indicates that updating targets behind is significantly worse than updating targets in front for one or more of the targets, the overall difference in performance (14.8 deg vs. 12.6 deg) is of no practical import, and the other two analyses indicate no difference. Thus, the more important message is that updating of targets behind is nearly as good as updating of targets in front. This is theoretically interesting. Contrasting with the literature on visual imagery which shows that visual images are functionally like visual percepts (Farah, 1985; Finke, 1989) and, thus, non-existent in the imaginal space behind the person's head, the spatial images associated with spatial updating definitely do exist in all directions. This study shows that, at least with respect to body translations, spatial updating is comparable in front and in back. Given the critical role of spatial updating in everyday life (keeping track of threatening animals and people), a role that surely goes back beyond the origins of human existence, it is no surprise that spatial updating ought to operate well in all directions relative to the head and body. Whereas visual imagery is known to be associated with early stages of visual processing (V1), spatial updating is generally thought to be associated with amodal or multimodal spatial representations of posterior parietal cortex (PPC), which receives sensory input from vision, hearing, and touch (Andersen, Snyder, Bradley, & Xing, 1997; see also, Philbeck, Behrmann, & Loomis, 2001). Given that we can readily perceive the locations of sound sources and haptically sensed objects that are behind us, it is not surprising that there would be brain areas, like PPC, involving abstract spatial representations of location, including those initiated by visual input, that are independent of direction relative to the head and body.

Agradecimentos:

This research was originally reported in the first author's Honor's thesis for the B.A. degree at the University of California, Santa Barbara in 1996.

Artigo recebido para publicação em 27/08/2003; aceito em 18/12/2003.

  • Andersen, R.A., Snyder, L.H., Bradley, D.C., & Xing, J. (1997). Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 20, 303-330.
  • Ashmead, D.H., DeFord, L.D., & Northington, A. (1995). Contribution of listeners' approaching motion to auditory distance perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 239-256.
  • Collett, T.S. (1982). Do toads plan routes? A study of the detour behavior of Bufo viridis. Journal of Comparative Physiology, 146, 261-271.
  • Farah, M.J. (1985). Psychophysical evidence for a shared representational medium for mental images and percepts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 91-103.
  • Farrell, M.J., & Robertson, I.H. (1997). Mental rotation and the automatic updating of body-centered spatial relationships. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 227-233.
  • Finke, R.A. (1989). Principles of mental imagery. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Fukusima, S.S., Loomis, J.M., & Da Silva, J.A. (1997). Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Psychophysics, 23, 86-100.
  • Hill, D.E. (1979). Orientation by jumping spiders of the genus Phidippus (Araneae: Salticidae) during the pursuit of prey. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 5, 301-322.
  • Hollins, M., & Kelley, E.K. (1988). Spatial updating in blind and sighted people. Perception & Psychophysics, 43, 380-388.
  • Loomis, J.M., Da Silva, J.A., Fujita, N., & Fukusima, S.S. (1992). Visual space perception and visually directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 906-921.
  • Loomis, J.M., Klatzky, R.L., Golledge, R.G., & Philbeck, J.W. (1999). Human navigation by path integration. In R.G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding: cognitive mapping and other spatial processes (pp. 125-151). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins.
  • Loomis, J.M., Lippa, Y., Klatzky, R.L., & Golledge, R.G. (2002). Spatial updating of locations specified by 3-D sound and spatial language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 28, 335-345.
  • Philbeck, J.W., Behrmann, M., & Loomis, J.M. (2001). Updating of locations during whole-body rotations in patients with hemispatial neglect. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 1, 330-343.
  • Philbeck, J.W., Loomis, J.M., & Beall, A.C. (1997). Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 601-612.
  • Rieser, J.J. (1989). Access to knowledge of spatial structure at novel points of observation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1157-1165.
  • Thomson, J.A. (1983). Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 427-443.
  • Endereço para correspondência:

    David Horn
    Surgical Resident, Department of Otolaryngology-Head & Neck Surgery
    Indiana University School of Medicine
    Indianapolis, IN 46220, USA
    E-mail:
    hornears@yahoo.com Jack M. Loomis
    Department of Psychology
    University of California, Santa Barbara
    Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA
    E-mail:
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      11 Feb 2009
    • Date of issue
      Apr 2004

    History

    • Accepted
      18 Dec 2003
    • Received
      27 Aug 2003
    Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Filosofia Ciências e Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Av.Bandeirantes 3900 - Monte Alegre, 14040-901 Ribeirão Preto - São Paulo - Brasil, Tel.: (55 16) 3315-3829 - Ribeirão Preto - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: paideia@usp.br