Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Mothers-Offspring Resemblance in Intelligence and Its Relationship to Socioeconomic Status

Semelhança Entre Mães e Filhos em Inteligência e sua Relação com o Nível Socioeconômico

Semejanza Entre Madres e Hijos e su Relacion con el Nivel Socioeconómico

Abstract:

Kinship studies have shown that most cognitive ability variances are attributable to genotypic variance. Additionally, kinship correlations are generally increased when tasks that are highly g loaded are considered, a result known as the "Jensen effect". Alternatively, some studies have suggested socioeconomic status as an important factor for explaining differences in cognition. The present study investigated these premises in a sample of 141 mothers (mean age = 36.6; SD = 6.0) and their offspring (53.2% girls; mean age = 11.0; SD = 2.1). The Standard Progressive Matrices of Raven (mothers and offspring) and six other cognitive measures (offspring only) were administered. The findings suggested evidence of g heritability (Jensen effect), which is consistent with published literature on intelligence. However, kinship correlation coefficients were lower (.041 to .177) when compared to other published findings. Finally, the difference between the mother's IQ score and that of her child was influenced by SES and mother's education level.

Keywords:
genetic; intelligence; family study; socioeconomic status; educational status

Resumo:

Estudos familiares demonstram que grande parte da variância das habilidades cognitivas é atribuída à variância genotípica. Além disso, correlações de parentesco possuem, geralmente, maiores valores quando as tarefas são altamente carregadas de g, fenômeno conhecido como "efeito Jensen". No entanto, em outra linha de investigação, estudos sugerem que o nível socioeconômico é um fator importante para explicar as diferenças cognitivas. O presente estudo investigou essas premissas em uma amostra de 141 mães (idade media = 36,6; DP = 6,0) e seus filhos (53,2% meninas;idade média = 11,0; DP = 2,1). Administrou-se as Matrizes Progressivas de Raven (em mães e filhos) e seis outras medidas cognitivas (nos filhos). Os resultados sugeriram uma evidência de herdabilidade em g (efeito Jensen). No entanto, os coeficientes de correlações familiares foram menores (0,041 a 0,177) a outros comumemente publicados. Por outro lado, a diferença entre os scores de QI das mães e de seus filhos foi influenciada pelo nível socioeconômico (NSE) e pelo nível de escolaridade da mãe. No geral, advoga-se por uma interação gene x ambiente das diferenças de habilidades cognitivas dependente do contexto socio-econômico.

Palavras-chave:
genética; inteligência; estudo de família; nível socioeconômico; escolaridade

Resumen:

Los estudios familiares han mostrado que gran parte de la varianza de las habilidades cognitivas es dada por la varianza genotípica. Además, las correlaciones de parentesco generalmente tienen mayor valor cuando las tareas demandan alto g, fenómeno conocido como "efecto Jensen". Sin embargo, considerando otra línea de investigación, los estudios sugieren que el nivel socioeconómico es un factor importante para explicar las diferencias cognitivas. El presente estudio investigó estas premisas en una muestra de 141 madres (edad media = 36,6; DT = 6,0) y sus hijos (53,2% niñas; edad media = 11,0; DT = 2,1). Fue administrado las Matrices Progresivas de Raven (en madres y niños) y otras seis medidas cognitivas (en niños). Los resultados sugieren evidencia de herdabilidad en g (efecto Jensen), hallazgo consistente con la literatura. Con todo, los coeficientes de correlación de parentesco fueron inferiores (0.041 a 0.177) en comparación con otras publicaciones. Finalmente, la diferencia entre las puntuaciones de CI de madres y sus hijos fue influenciado por el nivel socioeconómico (SES) y el nivel educativo de la madre.

Palabras clave:
genética; inteligencia; estudio de familia; nivel socioeconómico; escolaridad

Intelligence is defined by the ability to plan, reason, comprehend, perform abstract thinking, and learn (Gottfredson, 1997Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 24(1), 79-132. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90014-3
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90...
). Because of its positive association with success in education, profession, income and welfare (Deary, 2012Deary, I. J. (2012). Intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology 63, 453-482. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100353
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-12...
), intelligence is considered the most important trait in psychology (Hunt, 2010Hunt, E. (2010). Human intelligenceNew York, NY: Cambridge University Press.). Heritability (h2) , in turn, has been considered one of the longest standing assumptions about the nature of human intelligence (Bouchard & McGue, 1981Bouchard, T. J. Jr., , & McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: A review. Science 212(4498), 1055-1059.doi:10.1126/science.7195071
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7195071...
; Brouwer et al., 2014Brouwer, R. M., van Soelen, I. L. C., Swagerman, S. C., Schnack, H. G., Ehli, E. A., Kahn, R. S., Boomsma, D. I. (2014). Genetic associations between intelligence and cortical thickness emerge at the start of puberty. Human Brain Mapping 35(8), 3760-3773. doi:10.1002/hbm.22435
https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22435...
; Erlenmeyer-Kimling & Jarvik, 1963Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., & Jarvik, L. F. (1963). Genetics and intelligence: A review. Science 142(3598), 1477-1478. doi:10.1126/science.142.3598.1477
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.142.3598...
; Halsey, 1958Halsey, A. H. (1958). Genetics, social structure and intelligence. The British Journal of Sociology 9(1), 15-28. doi:10.2307/587619
https://doi.org/10.2307/587619...
; Hanscombe et al., 2012Hanscombe, K. B., Trzaskowski, M., Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2012). Socioeconomic status (SES) and children's intelligence (IQ): In a large UK-representative sample SES moderates the environmental, not genetic, effect on IQ. PloS One 7(2), e30320. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030320
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.003...
; Harden, Turkheimer, & Loehlin, 2007Harden, K. P., Turkheimer, E., & Loehlin, J. C. (2007). Genotype by environment interaction in adolescents' cognitive aptitude. Behavior Genetics 37(2), 273-283. doi:10.1007/s10519-006-9113-4
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-006-9113-...
; Johnson & Bouchard, 2007Johnson, W., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (2007). Sex differences in mental abilities: G masks the dimension on which they lie. Intelligence 35(1), 23-39. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.03.012
https://doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.intell.200...
; Plomin & DeFries, 1980Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1980). Genetics and intelligence: Recent data. Intelligence 4(1), 15-24. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(80)90003-3
https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(80)900...
; Scarr-Salapatek, 1971Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1971). Race, social class, and IQ. Science 174(4016), 1285-1295. doi:10.1126/science.174.4016.1285
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.174.4016...
).

Considerable empirical evidence exists regarding the heritability of intelligence. Several studies have suggested that heritable intelligence can vary between 0.2 and 0.86. In other words, 20 to 86% of the phenotypic variance observed in measured cognitive abilities is attributed to genetic factors (Bouchard & McGue, 1981Bouchard, T. J. Jr., , & McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: A review. Science 212(4498), 1055-1059.doi:10.1126/science.7195071
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7195071...
; Brouwer et al., 2014Brouwer, R. M., van Soelen, I. L. C., Swagerman, S. C., Schnack, H. G., Ehli, E. A., Kahn, R. S., Boomsma, D. I. (2014). Genetic associations between intelligence and cortical thickness emerge at the start of puberty. Human Brain Mapping 35(8), 3760-3773. doi:10.1002/hbm.22435
https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22435...
; Deary, Johnson, & Houlihan, 2009Deary, I. J., Johnson, W., & Houlihan, L. M. (2009).Genetic foundations of human intelligence.Human Genetics 126(1), 215-232. doi:10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00439-009-0655-...
; Erlenmeyer-Kimling & Jarvik, 1963Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., & Jarvik, L. F. (1963). Genetics and intelligence: A review. Science 142(3598), 1477-1478. doi:10.1126/science.142.3598.1477
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.142.3598...
; Lynn & Hattori, 1990Lynn, R., & Hattori, K. (1990). The heritability of intelligence in Japan. Behavior Genetics 20(4)545-546. doi:10.1007/BF01067719
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067719...
; Nisbett et al., 2012Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2012). Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist 67(2), 130-159. doi:10.1037/a0026699
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026699...
; Plomin & DeFries, 1980Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1980). Genetics and intelligence: Recent data. Intelligence 4(1), 15-24. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(80)90003-3
https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(80)900...
; Turkheimer, Haley, Waldron, D'Onofrio, & Gottesman, 2003Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science 14(6), 623-628. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003...
). For instance, in a fundamental review that compared 111 studies worldwide, investigating familial IQ correlations (Bouchard & McGue, 1981), the authors suggested an association of .86 for monozygotic (MZ) twins raised together and .72 for monozygotic twins raised separately. Decades later, similar observations were also reported (Lynn & Hattori, 1990; Turkheimer et al., 2003). As expected, investigation of dizygotic twins demonstrated a positive, however lower correlation compared to MZ's results. For same-sex dizygotic pairs, correlation values were estimated to be between .43 and .62 (Bouchard & McGue, 1981; Lynn & Hattori, 1990).

Additionally, parent-offspring correlations have been reported at approximately .50 (Bouchard & McGue, 1981Bouchard, T. J. Jr., , & McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: A review. Science 212(4498), 1055-1059.doi:10.1126/science.7195071
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7195071...
; Caruso, 1983Caruso, D. R. (1983). Sample differences in genetics and intelligence data: Sibling and parent-offspring studies. Behavior Genetics 13(5), 453-458. doi:10.1007/BF01065921
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01065921...
; Erlenmeyer-Kimling & Jarvik, 1963Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., & Jarvik, L. F. (1963). Genetics and intelligence: A review. Science 142(3598), 1477-1478. doi:10.1126/science.142.3598.1477
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.142.3598...
; McCall, 1970McCall, R. B. (1970). Intelligence quotient pattern over age: Comparisons among siblings and parent-child pairs. Science 170(3958), 644-648. doi:10.1126/science.170.3958.644
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.170.3958...
; Plomin & DeFries, 1980Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1980). Genetics and intelligence: Recent data. Intelligence 4(1), 15-24. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(80)90003-3
https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(80)900...
). While Bouchard and McGue (1981) found a correlation of .41 between father or mother and their offspring, a review consisting of 52 familial IQ correlation studies (Erlenmeyer-Kimling & Jarvik, 1963) indicated a median correlation of .50 for parent-child raised together. Similarly, Caruso (1983) demonstrated an N-weighted mean correlation of 0.51 after re-analyzing data from 15 samples, referred to parent-offspring pairs.

McCall (1970McCall, R. B. (1970). Intelligence quotient pattern over age: Comparisons among siblings and parent-child pairs. Science 170(3958), 644-648. doi:10.1126/science.170.3958.644
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.170.3958...
) also analyzed the cognitive performance correlation between parents and their children, of which both samples were evaluated at the same age, from 3 until 12 years of age. The highest correlation was .50, at age 4 and the lowest was .17 at age 11. In general, most of the analyzed pairs demonstrated a low correlation, with a median of .29. The author suggested that the many years separating the parent-child assessment, which was taken at the same age, was what resulted in the low correlations.

Cousins are less biologically related individuals and, as expected, their results in cognitive tests display lower correlations. Bouchard and McGue (1981Bouchard, T. J. Jr., , & McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: A review. Science 212(4498), 1055-1059.doi:10.1126/science.7195071
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7195071...
) have indicated a correlation coefficient of .15. When considering unrelated individuals raised together (e.g. adopted children), the correlation was .23 and close to 0 for unrelated people that were reared apart (such as children living in the same orphanage or foster home from an early age) (Erlenmeyer-Kimling & Jarvik, 1963Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., & Jarvik, L. F. (1963). Genetics and intelligence: A review. Science 142(3598), 1477-1478. doi:10.1126/science.142.3598.1477
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.142.3598...
).

These results are in agreement with polygenic inheritance, which predicts that a pair of individuals who have a stronger genetic similarity will also have similarly higher IQs correlations (Bouchard & McGue, 1981Bouchard, T. J. Jr., , & McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: A review. Science 212(4498), 1055-1059.doi:10.1126/science.7195071
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7195071...
). No statistically significant effect of sex from parents or their children on parents-offspring resemblance in intelligence was observed (McAskie & Clarke, 1976McAskie, M., & Clarke, A. M. (1976). Parents-offspring resemblances in intelligence: Theories and evidence. British Journal of Psychology67(2), 243-273. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1976.tb01514.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1976...
; Reed & Rich, 1982Reed, S.C., & Rich, S. S. (1982). Parent-offspring resemblances and regressions for IQ. Behavior Genetics 12, 535-542.).

On the other hand, there is evidence that kinship correlations differ according to the variation of the tests' g loadings (Colom, Juan-Espinosa, & Garcia, 2001Colom, R., Juan-Espinosa, M., & Garcia, L. F. (2001). The secular increase in test scores is a "Jensen effect". Personality and Individual Differences 30(4), 553-559.doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00054-4
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00...
; Jensen, 1998Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental abilityWestport, CT: Praeger.). For instance, it has been suggested that when a more complex cognitive test is applied, the more the contribution of g is observed. Therefore, what is g? In this situation, g is the general intelligence that influences all mental abilities. In statistical terms, g is the principal factor accounted for by the variance within diverse mental tests. It is the "working definition of intelligence", as Gottfredson (1997Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 24(1), 79-132. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90014-3
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90...
) asserts. And according to Jensen, g is the most heritable cognitive factor (Jensen, 1998). Thus, stronger kinship correlations are expected when tasks that are highly g loaded are considered. Additionally, evidence of g loading on the magnitude of human heritability tests (Luo, Petrill, & Thompson, 1994Luo, D., Petrill, S. A., & Thompson, L. A. (1994). An exploration of genetic g: Hierarchical factor analysis of cognitive data from the Western Reserve Twin Project. Intelligence 18(3), 335-347. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(94)90033-7
https://doi.org/doi:10.1016/0160-2896(94...
), and non-human heritability tests (Woodley, Fernandes, & Hopkins, 2015Woodley, M. A., Fernandes, H. B. F., & Hopkins, W. D. (2015). The more g-loaded, the more heritable, evolvable, and phenotypically variable: Homology with humans in chimpanzee cognitive abilities. Intelligence50, 159-163. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.04.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.04...
) have previously been reported. Thus, g loading is the first indicator that must be considered in studies regarding parents-offspring resemblance in intelligence.

Beyond g, current psychometric literature (Carroll, 1993; McGrew, Flanagan, Keith, & Vanderwood, 1997McGrew, K. S., Flanagan, D. P., Keith, T. Z., & Vanderwood, M. (1997). Beyond g: The impact of Gf-Gc specific cognitive abilities research on the future use and interpretation of intelligence tests in the schools. School Psychology Review26(2), 189-210.) recognizes the importance of two broad cognitive abilities, Fluid Intelligence (Gf) and Crystallized Intelligence (Gc), originally identified by Raymond Cattelland and further developed by his student, Horn (1994Horn, J. L. (1994).Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human intelligence (pp. 443-451). New York, NY: MacMillan .). While Gf is the ability to reason, form concepts and problem solve using novel information,Gc is the ability to use general and cultural knowledge, including verbal communication with previously learned procedures. It can be noted that Gf instead Gc could be less sensitive to environmental intervention, and, in principle, Gf may impact the resemblance between parents-offspring in intelligence. However, until now the only study linking heritability across cognitive abilities was the study of Kan, Wicherts, Dolan and van der Maas (2013Kan, K.-J., Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2013). On the nature and nurture of intelligence and specific cognitive abilities: The more heritable, the more culture dependent. Psychological Science 24(12), 2420-2428. doi:10.1177/0956797613493292
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613493292...
). Contrary to traditional assumptions, this study identified that most heritable abilities are in fact the most culture-dependent abilities, i.e., Gcwould be more heritable than Gf. This is an indicator that must be further investigated in cross-cultural research, especially in developing countries.

On the other hand, the majority of behavioral genetic studies in human, of which the majority were conducted in developed countries, did not take into account the influence of environmental aspects, such as parental income, parental education and parental occupation, factors that, together, refers to familial socioeconomic status (SES). For these reasons, Turkheimer et al. (2003Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science 14(6), 623-628. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003...
) evaluated the influence of low-SES on mothers and twin children cognition. For the high-SES group, heritability was estimated at .72, as previously showed by Bouchard and McGue (1981Bouchard, T. J. Jr., , & McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: A review. Science 212(4498), 1055-1059.doi:10.1126/science.7195071
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7195071...
), but for the low-SES group, surprisingly, the h2 decreased to .10. Similar results were reported by Rowe, Jacobson and Van den Oord (1999Rowe, D.C., Jacobson, K.C., & Van den Oord, E.J.C.G. (1999). Genetic and Environmental Influences on Vocabulary IQ: Parental Education Level as Moderator. Child development, 70, 1151-1162.) regarding the moderating effect that parental attained educational level has on the vocabulary level in 16-year old children.

On the contrary, Nagoshi and Johnson (2005Nagoshi, C. T., & Johnson, R. C. (2005). Socioeconomic status does not moderate the familiality of cognitive abilities in the Hawaii family study of cognition. Journal of Biosocial Science 37(6), 773-781. doi:10.1017/S0021932004007023
https://doi.org/10.1017/S002193200400702...
), when controlling for the effect of socioeconomic status (SES) on parent-child cognitive interaction, did not confirm that low-SES acts to minimize the influence of heredity on intelligence.

Harden et al. (2007Harden, K. P., Turkheimer, E., & Loehlin, J. C. (2007). Genotype by environment interaction in adolescents' cognitive aptitude. Behavior Genetics 37(2), 273-283. doi:10.1007/s10519-006-9113-4
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-006-9113-...
) reanalysis of Turkheimer et al. (2003Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science 14(6), 623-628. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003...
) study from 839 twin pairs found the same pattern reported by Turkheimer et al. However non-significant differences in heritability were observed between the SES (40% of variance accounted for additive genetics for families from the lowest incomes and 55% for families from the highest incomes.) Perhaps this result was due to the fact that most of the samples were not actually considered low SES.

Recently, a meta-analysis was published by Tucker-Drob and Bates (2016Tucker-Drob, E.M., & Bates, T.C. (2016). Large Cross-National Differences in Gene x Socioeconomic Status Interaction on Intelligence. Psychological Science , 27, 138-149.) with the aim of understanding the paradox: genex SES interaction effects on individual differences in intelligence and school performance. Fourteen independent studies comprising a total of 24,926 pairs of twins and siblings were analyzed. The results indicated a significant gene x SES effect (a′ = .074, SE = .020, p< .0005) for studies conducted in USA; but no significant gene x SES effect (a′ = −.027, SE = .022, p = .223) was observed in non-USA studies (Western Europe and Australia). No other mediators, such as type of cognitive or school performance test, single or composite measure of SES, were statistically significant. That author's hypothesis aimed to explain that those results were related to the access to high-quality education and health care, which is more uniform in Western Europe and Australia than in USA. Tucker-Drob and Bates asserted that effects of family SES on cognitive development must be carefully considered in genetic studies.

On the other hand, a better understanding of gene x SES interaction allows for possible explanations for the cognitive gains across generations (also known as Flynn effect). To date, there is no conclusive answer as to why the current generation (e.g. offspring) displays higher scores in intelligence test than past generations (e.g. parents) (Pietschnig & Voracek, 2015Pietschnig, J., & Voracek, M. (2015). One century of global IQ gains: A formal meta-analysis of the Flynn effect (1909-2013). Perspectives on Psychological Science 10(3), 282-306.doi:10.1177/1745691615577701
https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615577701...
). These gains were estimated around 3 points of IQ per decade (Flynn, 1994), or 7.5 points of IQ in a generation (25 years). This rapid increase cannot be due to genetic factors. There would be a "X" factor, which permits the increase of intelligence (Flynn, 1999). In fact, Dickens and Flynn (2001Dickens, W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2001). Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved. Psychological Review108(2), 346-369. doi:10.1037//0033295X.108.2.346
https://doi.org/10.1037//0033295X.108.2....
) proposed the "hypothesis of social multipliers" according to which even slight environmental advantages (e.g. good social policies) could lead to improve individual performance in cognitive and school measures, and this improved performance will lead to a better environment, and so on. Other studies has denied this hypothesis (Rowe & Rodgers, 2002Rowe, D. C., & Rodgers, J. L. (2002). Expanding variance and the case of historical changes in IQ means: A critique of Dickens and Flynn (2001). Psychological Review 109(4), 759- 763. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.759
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.4....
). However, this is an indicator that should be investigated further in order to understand the cognitive gains, as seen from the results of Tucker-Drob and Bates (2016Tucker-Drob, E.M., & Bates, T.C. (2016). Large Cross-National Differences in Gene x Socioeconomic Status Interaction on Intelligence. Psychological Science , 27, 138-149.) and Turkheimer et al. (2003Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science 14(6), 623-628. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003...
). If these results are robust, we can expect that correlations between parents-offspring (irrespective of treatment of intelligence as g or Gf/ Gc) must tend to be statistically non-significant in samples with low SES, favoring the operation of "X" factor. Additionally, There is forecast for today heavy rain and lightning. If there are problems with the transmission we will let you know. If there is a loss of the broadcast, do not worry that another speaker will use the time of your presentation, and we will inform to the audience that your presentation (PPT) will be available on our website (our plan B). OK? cognitive differences between parents and offspring, favoring offspring, should therefore be higher in families with low SES.

Brazil is a country with high levels of social iniquity (http://data.worldbank.org/indicator/SI.POV.GINI), where gene x SES interaction effects can be better tracked. For instance, a Brazilian study has indicated that, as expected, SES affects more Gc than Gf (Flores-Mendoza, Widaman, Mansur-Alves, Bacelar, & Saldanha, 2013Flores-Mendoza, C., Widaman, K., Mansur-Alves, M., Dias Bacelar, T., & Saldanha, R. (2013). Psychoticism and disruptive behavior can be also good predictors of school achievement. Spanish Journal of Psychology 16, E13.doi:10.1017/sjp.2013.3
https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2013.3...
). However, to date, there is no information regarding how SES could affect the heritability of intelligence using a parent-offspring design. Thus the aims of this study was to verify: 1) kinship correlations (mother-offspring) and the effect Jensen (g loading) in these correlations, 2) correlations of heritability across cognitive abilities, such as Gf and Gc, 3) the impact of SES on kinship correlations (mother-offspring), and 4) cognitive differences between mother and their offspring in the same cognitive measure, taking into account SES and education level of mothers.

To the extent of our knowledge, it is the first study in Brazil presenting information about resemblance in intelligence from parents and offspring.

Method

Participants

The present study was part of a larger project entitled, "Psychological Assessment of cognitive and school performance of children from an urban and rural context". The study was conducted in 2006 in the city of Perdões, in the state of Minas Gerais, Brazil. Perdões is 211 kilometers from Belo Horizonte (the capital city of Minas Gerais) with 270,657 km² of total area and approximately 20,000 inhabitants (2010 census).

There were 374 students (196 girls and 178 boys) that participated in this project, of which 15.5% were enrolled in seven rural schools (which represents all rural schools that exist in the city) and 84.5% were enrolled in three urban schools. All parents were invited to participate of the study, however only 156 parents (15 fathers and 141 mothers) were available for the parental assessment. Parents that were unable to attend the psychological assessment (around 60% of the initial sample) cited work conflicts. Thus, in the present study, data was reported from 141 paired (mothers and children) participants, 16.5% of pairs related to students enrolled in three rural schools and 83.5% enrolled in three urban schools. According to the social questionnaire filled out by parents, all mothers were biological mothers of their kids and there was no twins or siblings among students. Descriptive data are shown in Table 1.

Table 1
Sample description of mothers and offspring

Instruments

Intelligence was measured by the Standard Progressive Matrices - (SPM). (Raven, 2001Raven, J. C. (2001). Standard progressive matrices: Sets A-E, manual Rio de Janeiro, RJ: CEPA .). SPM is considered to be a measure of fluid intelligence, or Gf (Jensen, 1998Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental abilityWestport, CT: Praeger.). The internal consistency (alpha Cronbach) of SPM for mothers was .934 and for their offspring, .936. Furthermore, the offspring sample was also assessed by using five subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III subtest (Wechsler, 2002Wechsler, D. (2002). WISC III: Escala de inteligência Wechsler para crianças [WISC III: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children: Manual] (V. L. M. Figueiredo, Trans.). São Paulo, SP: Casa do Psicólogo.). Two subtests considered as crystallized intelligence measurements, or Gc (Jensen, 1998), included: information and similarities (alpha Cronbach of .80 and .76, respectively). Three subtests considered as fluid intelligence measures included: Blocks, Symbol Search and Code subtest (reliability re-test, r = .80, .63, and .70 respectively). Additionally, the Toulouse test (Rainho, 2001Rainho, O. (2001). Bateria fatorial CEPA: Testes de Aptidões Específicas. Manual[CEPA Factorial Battery : Specific Abilities Tests] (2a ed.). Rio de Janeiro, RJ: CEPA.) for measuring attention reliability by re-test of .66 according study of Araujo (2011Araujo, R. S. (2011). Teste de atençãoconcentrada Toulouse-Pierón: Atualização dos estudos de padronização, validade e precisão[Toulouse-Pieron Concentrated Attention Test: Updating of standardization, validity and reliability studies]. Unpublished master's dissertation. Universidade de Sao Paulo, Sao Paulo, Sp. Retrieved from http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/47/47131/publico
http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponivei...
) was also applied to offspring.

Family SES was estimated using CriterioBrasil(AssociaçãoBrasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa [ABEP], 2008) which is based on two sources: availability and quantity of resources within the home (e.g. TV, washing machine, refrigerator, etc.), and parents' education level. The points accumulated by each item are calculated and converted into a classification scale (e.g., 1 TV = 1 point, whereas 3 TVs = 3 points; 1 bathroom = 4 points, whereas 2 bathroom = 5 points). Levels of SES ranged from A1 (highest SES level) to E (lowest SES level). This information was available to 121 mothers (or 86% of total sample) and its distribution was: 0.8% for B1; 9.1% for B2; 25.6% for C1 level; 38.8% for C2 and 25.6% for D level. Thus, the sample of the present study was predominantly concentrated (64%) between low (D) and low-middle class (C1) SES. Additionally, the social questionnaire included items regarding level of kinship between parents and children (biological, relative or adopted), and other information not analyzed for the present study (e.g. race, level of religiosity, and political orientation of parents).

Procedure

Data collection. The Secretary of Education from Perdões County previously invited public school principals from rural and urban zones to participate in this study. The school's principals, in turn, invited the parents to participate. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants. A team of psychology students and two professors (authors of the present study) remained in the city for a week, thanks to the support of the county's Education Secretary, in order to perform the evaluations. The SPM test was administered collectively in school classrooms for children enrolled in the 3rd grad or above. The subtests of WISC III (Information, Similarities, Block, Symbols Search and Code), and the SPM test (only for children attending the 1st and 2nd grade), were administered individually in a special room provided by the school. Parents were collectively assessed in the last day, in two school classrooms.

Data analysis. Careful effort was given to control a developmental effect on cognitive measures (for offspring this includes the correlations between SPM, information, similarities, blocks, symbol search, code Toulouse with age, revealed values of .595, .639, .590, .520, .604, .633, and .582, respectively; while for mothers, the correlation between SPM and age was -.221). All raw scores were normalized to a z score, controlling for age. Additionally, an exploratory factorial analysis through principal axis factoring was conducted, which considers only common variance when extracting the factorial load of each cognitive measure based on the principal factor (g). The correlation values were disattenuated for reliability of the measurements, using the following formula:.In this equation, r 12 is the observed correlation, and r 11 and r 22 are the reliability estimates of measures 1 and measures 2. The normalization of the raw SPM score (from mothers and offspring) was converted to an IQ scale [(z*15)+100]. Finally, a linear model (Montgomery, Peck, & Vining, 2006Montgomery, D. C., Peck, E. A., & Vining, G. G. (2006). Introduction to linear regression analysis. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Interscience.) that contrasted the groups was estimated to verify the interaction between SES and education level of mothers with regards to IQ differences. Analysis was performed using software R, version 3.2.0 (Venables, Smith, & R Core Team, 2015Venables, W. N., Smith, D. M., & R Core Team. (2015). An introduction to R. Notes on R: A programming environment for data analysis and graphics: Version 3.2.2. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/doc/manuals/R-intro.pdf
https://cran.r-project.org/doc/manuals/R...
).

Ethical Considerations

Ethical approval (Process n. 089/06) was obtained by the ethical committee at the Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais.

Results

Table 2 indicates the load of each cognitive measure on the principal factor (g) and the cognitive performance correlation between mothers (SPM test) and their offspring.

Table 2
Correlation between cognitive performance of offspring and mothers

Table 3
Performance of mothers and offspring according to SES on the SPM test using an IQ scale

The Pearson correlation between g load and the relationship between cognitive performance of mothers and their offspring (r = .842, or .778, when corrected for attenuation) was statistically significant (p= .02), and approaching significance when Spearman correlation was considered (rho = .750, p = .052; same value when corrected for attenuation). Thus, the correlation between the cognitive performance of mothers and offspring, decrease (or increase) when decrease (or increase) the g load of cognitive measures. Additionally, only crystallized measures, such as information (r = .192; p = .005) and similarities (r = .201; p = .005) were significantly associated with cognitive performance of mothers. For fluid measures (SPM or Block Design) or attention measures (Code and Toulouse), no significant correlation between cognitive performance of mothers and their children were found.

In addition, genetic x environmental interaction was estimated. The difference between the mother's IQ subtest score and that of her child was estimated (Table 3), and then the differences were adjusted for SES. Using a linear model, the results indicate a tendency for children to have a higher IQ than their mothers in low-SES families, when compared to middle-high SES families. However this result was only marginally significant (p = .084) for mothers that had attained university level education (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Mean differences in IQ points (using standardized scores from the SPM test), between mothers and their offspring, according to education of mother from low and middle-high SES (negative differences favoring offspring; positive differences favoring mothers).

Discussion

Three interesting points emerged from the results of the present study. First, as Jensen had previously indicated (Jensen, 1998Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental abilityWestport, CT: Praeger.), the column vector of g (composed of the g loadings from a set of cognitive measures) had a significant correlation with the heritability vector from a set of cognitive measures (mother-offspring correlation on each test). This results means that cognitive similarity between mothers-offspring may depend on g loadings of tests employed in the assessment.

Second, despite the fact that all correlations between cognitive performance of mothers and their offspring were positive, all values were very low (the highest correlation was .177 or .201, corrected for attenuation). This result contradicts previously published studies demonstrating parent-offspring resemblance on intelligence tests (Bouchard & McGue, 1981Bouchard, T. J. Jr., , & McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: A review. Science 212(4498), 1055-1059.doi:10.1126/science.7195071
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7195071...
; Erlenmeyer-Kimling &Jarvik, 1963Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., & Jarvik, L. F. (1963). Genetics and intelligence: A review. Science 142(3598), 1477-1478. doi:10.1126/science.142.3598.1477
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.142.3598...
; Reed & Rich, 1982Reed, S.C., & Rich, S. S. (1982). Parent-offspring resemblances and regressions for IQ. Behavior Genetics 12, 535-542.; van Leeuwen, van den Berg, & Boomsma, 2008van Leeuwen, M., van den Berg, S. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). A twin-family study of general IQ. Learning and Individual Differences18(1), 76-88. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2007.04.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2007.04...
). In these studies, moderate correlations (approximately .50) were measured. In this regard, it must be noted that Caruso (1983Caruso, D. R. (1983). Sample differences in genetics and intelligence data: Sibling and parent-offspring studies. Behavior Genetics 13(5), 453-458. doi:10.1007/BF01065921
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01065921...
) had observed a heterogeneity of parent-offspring IQ correlations. The correlations for the samples included in his meta-analysis ranged from .15 to .80. According to Caruso, sampling error, attenuation due to unreliability, and attenuation due to restriction of range, usually account for variability in the correlations. In the present study, sampling error could account for the low correlations inso far as our sample, apart small size, was mostly composed by children (70% until 12 years old), few adolescents (30%) and no adult offspring. Age is an important variable in studies investigating familial cognitive resemblance since heritability appears to increase with age, i.e., the influence of shared environment is stronger during childhood and disappears during adolescence when genetic factors begin to play a more important role (Scarr & Weinberg, 1983Scarr, S., & Weinberg, R. A. (1983). The Minnesota Adoption Studies: Genetic differences and malleability. Child Development 54(2), 260-267.doi:10.2307/1129689
https://doi.org/10.2307/1129689...
; van Leeuwen et al., 2008). However, after dividing our sample into two age groups (≤12 years old, and ≥ 13 years old), no evidence of increased correlation was observed in the older group.

Another possible interpretation for explaining the results obtained is related to the impact of the SES variable. Our results were similar to those obtained by Turkheimer et al. (2003Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science 14(6), 623-628. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003...
), where heritability was estimated at .10 in twins from low-SES mothers. Our study refers to individuals that are less biological related than twins. It refers to mothers and their children, where approximately 67% were categorized as low-SES families (the remainder, 33%, was middle-high class. No participating families were from high or extreme low SES). Thus, it is possible to infer that cognitive ability heritability is sensitive to environmental conditions.

Third, the kinship correlation was statistically significant, only for crystallized measures (Information and Similarities), but not fluid measures (SPM or Block Design) or attention measures (Code, Toulouse Test, and Symbol Search). This result seems to be counter intuitive considering the mainstream psychometric intelligence literature, which indicates that fluid intelligence (the closest psychological construct to g) is supposedly culture-free (or, at least, less influenced by cultural factors). Thus, h2 of fluid intelligence should be higher than crystallized intelligence. However, our results are consistent with Kan et al. (2013Kan, K.-J., Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2013). On the nature and nurture of intelligence and specific cognitive abilities: The more heritable, the more culture dependent. Psychological Science 24(12), 2420-2428. doi:10.1177/0956797613493292
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613493292...
) who observed the same surprising result after analyzing 23 twin studies (N = 7,852). These authors found that the heritability coefficients of culture-loaded tests (crystallized measures) tended to be larger than those of culture-reduced tests (fluid measures). Kan et al., considered their own results to be difficult to interpret, without appreciating the role of education, culture and experience, in the development of heritable intelligence. According to these authors, societal demands could determine the load g of tests and, pressing the inclusion of the effects of genotype-environment covariance on heritability coefficients.

Moreover, a meta-analysis recently conducted by Pietschnig and Voracek (2015Pietschnig, J., & Voracek, M. (2015). One century of global IQ gains: A formal meta-analysis of the Flynn effect (1909-2013). Perspectives on Psychological Science 10(3), 282-306.doi:10.1177/1745691615577701
https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615577701...
) the trajectories of the Flynn effect across cognitive abilities, from 1909 to 2013, indicated strongest gains for Gf than Gc, which in some way could be linked to the study of Kan et al. (2013Kan, K.-J., Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2013). On the nature and nurture of intelligence and specific cognitive abilities: The more heritable, the more culture dependent. Psychological Science 24(12), 2420-2428. doi:10.1177/0956797613493292
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613493292...
), and to our present study. These last two studies found more heritability for Gc than Gf, which implies that cognitive gains through generations can be better expressed in changes in Gf than in Gc. Assuming that Gc is more heritable, it is possible to expect a larger correlation between mothers and offspring if both were assessed with crystallized measures, which did not happen in our study. According to Pietschnig and Voracek, the reason why Gf would be less heritable than Gc is that Gf, over time, could be being incremented by exposition of students to tests with multiple-choice response, improved nutrition and reduced pathogen stress. These factors could be differentiated among participants (mothers and offspring) of our study.

If the hypothesis of influence of genotype-environment covariance (through abilities Gf /Gc and SES) on heritability coefficients of the intelligence is true or not, the results obtained in the present study seem to validate it.

Finally, the differences in cognitive performance (controlling for age) between mothers and offspring from the same cognitive measure (the SPM test) were estimated. The results (Figure 1) indicated improved cognitive performance in offspring when compared to their mothers from low-SES families, when compared to children from middle-high-SES families, especially children from mothers with university level education. To verify the interaction between education and mother's SES in reference to "IQ differences" (IQ was entered as a categorical variable - positive or negative), a logistic model (Agresti, 2002Agresti, A. (2002). Categorical data analysisNew York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.) was implemented. The following was observed: 89% of mothers with university education from middle-high-SES versus 25% from low-SES had an IQ score higher than their offspring. This proportional difference was significant (p = .045). Hence, the results seem to indicate that rising intelligence test performance across generations in families (Flynn effect) presents a greater probability in low-SES families. These results are similar to those found in cognitive comparison studies between countries, where Flynn effects are observed in developing nations, while they are diminishing in developed countries (Meisenberg & Woodley, 2013Meisenberg, G., & Woodley, M. A. (2013). Are cognitive differences between countries diminishing? Evidence from TIMSS and PISA. Intelligence41(6), 808-816. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.009
https://doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.intell.201...
).

The present study was a kinship study, in which Brazilian mothers and their offspring (N = 141) were administered the SPM test. Additionally, six other cognitive measures were administered, only to the offspring. The results indicated evidence of heritability of g (the greater the loaded g test, the greater the association between cognitive performance of mother and their offspring), which is supported by others in the literature. Nevertheless, heritability coefficients of each test were low, and significance was only observed for crystallized (Information and Similarities) instead fluid measures (SPM, Block Design, Code), which is apparently contrary to the literature. On the other hand, IQ differences between mothers-offspring in the same measure (SPM test) tended to be negative in low-SES families (offspring had higher performance than mothers) and positive in middle-high-SES families (offspring had less performance than mothers). The authors of this study are inclined to consider the hypothesis of Kan et al. (2013Kan, K.-J., Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2013). On the nature and nurture of intelligence and specific cognitive abilities: The more heritable, the more culture dependent. Psychological Science 24(12), 2420-2428. doi:10.1177/0956797613493292
https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613493292...
) and the studies of Harden et al. (2007Harden, K. P., Turkheimer, E., & Loehlin, J. C. (2007). Genotype by environment interaction in adolescents' cognitive aptitude. Behavior Genetics 37(2), 273-283. doi:10.1007/s10519-006-9113-4
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-006-9113-...
), Tucker-Drob and Bates (2016Tucker-Drob, E.M., & Bates, T.C. (2016). Large Cross-National Differences in Gene x Socioeconomic Status Interaction on Intelligence. Psychological Science , 27, 138-149.) and Turkheimer et al. (2003Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science 14(6), 623-628. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x
https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003...
) to explain the obtained results. The genotype-environment covariance through SES could influence the coefficients of cognitive heritability, where Gc would be more inheritable than Gf. However, we recognize that the size of sample employed (relatively small), the absence of high and extreme low social class families, and the administration of just one cognitive measure to mothers (the SPM test) do not permit to robust conclusions, and they are recognized as limitation of the present study. It is not easy to design a study in which parents and their children are tested simultaneously. For this reason, recruiting representative samples remains a challenge for researchers. Despite these limitations, this study does provide new information regarding intelligence in respect to nature versus nurture.

References

  • Agresti, A. (2002). Categorical data analysisNew York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Araujo, R. S. (2011). Teste de atençãoconcentrada Toulouse-Pierón: Atualização dos estudos de padronização, validade e precisão[Toulouse-Pieron Concentrated Attention Test: Updating of standardization, validity and reliability studies]. Unpublished master's dissertation. Universidade de Sao Paulo, Sao Paulo, Sp. Retrieved from http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/47/47131/publico
    » http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/47/47131/publico
  • Associação Brasileira de Empresas de Pesquisa. (2008). Critério de classificação econômica Brasil [Brazil economic classification criterion]. Retrieved from http://www.abep.org/criterioBrasil.aspx
    » http://www.abep.org/criterioBrasil.aspx
  • Bouchard, T. J. Jr., , & McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: A review. Science 212(4498), 1055-1059.doi:10.1126/science.7195071
    » https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7195071
  • Brouwer, R. M., van Soelen, I. L. C., Swagerman, S. C., Schnack, H. G., Ehli, E. A., Kahn, R. S., Boomsma, D. I. (2014). Genetic associations between intelligence and cortical thickness emerge at the start of puberty. Human Brain Mapping 35(8), 3760-3773. doi:10.1002/hbm.22435
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22435
  • Caruso, D. R. (1983). Sample differences in genetics and intelligence data: Sibling and parent-offspring studies. Behavior Genetics 13(5), 453-458. doi:10.1007/BF01065921
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01065921
  • Colom, R., Juan-Espinosa, M., & Garcia, L. F. (2001). The secular increase in test scores is a "Jensen effect". Personality and Individual Differences 30(4), 553-559.doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00054-4
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00054-4
  • Deary, I. J. (2012). Intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology 63, 453-482. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100353
    » https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100353
  • Deary, I. J., Johnson, W., & Houlihan, L. M. (2009).Genetic foundations of human intelligence.Human Genetics 126(1), 215-232. doi:10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4
  • Dickens, W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2001). Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved. Psychological Review108(2), 346-369. doi:10.1037//0033295X.108.2.346
    » https://doi.org/10.1037//0033295X.108.2.346
  • Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., & Jarvik, L. F. (1963). Genetics and intelligence: A review. Science 142(3598), 1477-1478. doi:10.1126/science.142.3598.1477
    » https://doi.org/10.1126/science.142.3598.1477
  • Flores-Mendoza, C., Widaman, K., Mansur-Alves, M., Dias Bacelar, T., & Saldanha, R. (2013). Psychoticism and disruptive behavior can be also good predictors of school achievement. Spanish Journal of Psychology 16, E13.doi:10.1017/sjp.2013.3
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2013.3
  • Flynn, J. R. (1994). IQ gains over time. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human intelligence (pp. 617-623). New York, NY: MacMillan.
  • Flynn, J. R. (1999). Searching for justice: The discovery of IQ gains over time. American Psychologist 54(1), 5-20. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.54.1.5
    » https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066X.54.1.5
  • Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence 24(1), 79-132. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90014-3
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90014-3
  • Hanscombe, K. B., Trzaskowski, M., Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2012). Socioeconomic status (SES) and children's intelligence (IQ): In a large UK-representative sample SES moderates the environmental, not genetic, effect on IQ. PloS One 7(2), e30320. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030320
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030320
  • Halsey, A. H. (1958). Genetics, social structure and intelligence. The British Journal of Sociology 9(1), 15-28. doi:10.2307/587619
    » https://doi.org/10.2307/587619
  • Harden, K. P., Turkheimer, E., & Loehlin, J. C. (2007). Genotype by environment interaction in adolescents' cognitive aptitude. Behavior Genetics 37(2), 273-283. doi:10.1007/s10519-006-9113-4
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-006-9113-4
  • Horn, J. L. (1994).Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human intelligence (pp. 443-451). New York, NY: MacMillan .
  • Hunt, E. (2010). Human intelligenceNew York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental abilityWestport, CT: Praeger.
  • Johnson, W., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (2007). Sex differences in mental abilities: G masks the dimension on which they lie. Intelligence 35(1), 23-39. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.03.012
    » https://doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.03.012
  • Kan, K.-J., Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Maas, H. L. J. (2013). On the nature and nurture of intelligence and specific cognitive abilities: The more heritable, the more culture dependent. Psychological Science 24(12), 2420-2428. doi:10.1177/0956797613493292
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613493292
  • Luo, D., Petrill, S. A., & Thompson, L. A. (1994). An exploration of genetic g: Hierarchical factor analysis of cognitive data from the Western Reserve Twin Project. Intelligence 18(3), 335-347. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(94)90033-7
    » https://doi.org/doi:10.1016/0160-2896(94)90033-7
  • Lynn, R., & Hattori, K. (1990). The heritability of intelligence in Japan. Behavior Genetics 20(4)545-546. doi:10.1007/BF01067719
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067719
  • McAskie, M., & Clarke, A. M. (1976). Parents-offspring resemblances in intelligence: Theories and evidence. British Journal of Psychology67(2), 243-273. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1976.tb01514.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1976.tb01514.x
  • McCall, R. B. (1970). Intelligence quotient pattern over age: Comparisons among siblings and parent-child pairs. Science 170(3958), 644-648. doi:10.1126/science.170.3958.644
    » https://doi.org/10.1126/science.170.3958.644
  • McGrew, K. S., Flanagan, D. P., Keith, T. Z., & Vanderwood, M. (1997). Beyond g: The impact of Gf-Gc specific cognitive abilities research on the future use and interpretation of intelligence tests in the schools. School Psychology Review26(2), 189-210.
  • Meisenberg, G., & Woodley, M. A. (2013). Are cognitive differences between countries diminishing? Evidence from TIMSS and PISA. Intelligence41(6), 808-816. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.009
    » https://doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.009
  • Montgomery, D. C., Peck, E. A., & Vining, G. G. (2006). Introduction to linear regression analysis. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Interscience.
  • Nagoshi, C. T., & Johnson, R. C. (2005). Socioeconomic status does not moderate the familiality of cognitive abilities in the Hawaii family study of cognition. Journal of Biosocial Science 37(6), 773-781. doi:10.1017/S0021932004007023
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/S0021932004007023
  • Nisbett, R. E., Aronson, J., Blair, C., Dickens, W., Flynn, J., Halpern, D. F., & Turkheimer, E. (2012). Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist 67(2), 130-159. doi:10.1037/a0026699
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026699
  • Pietschnig, J., & Voracek, M. (2015). One century of global IQ gains: A formal meta-analysis of the Flynn effect (1909-2013). Perspectives on Psychological Science 10(3), 282-306.doi:10.1177/1745691615577701
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691615577701
  • Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1980). Genetics and intelligence: Recent data. Intelligence 4(1), 15-24. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(80)90003-3
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/0160-2896(80)90003-3
  • Rainho, O. (2001). Bateria fatorial CEPA: Testes de Aptidões Específicas. Manual[CEPA Factorial Battery : Specific Abilities Tests] (2a ed.). Rio de Janeiro, RJ: CEPA.
  • Raven, J. C. (2001). Standard progressive matrices: Sets A-E, manual Rio de Janeiro, RJ: CEPA .
  • Reed, S.C., & Rich, S. S. (1982). Parent-offspring resemblances and regressions for IQ. Behavior Genetics 12, 535-542.
  • Rowe, D. C., & Rodgers, J. L. (2002). Expanding variance and the case of historical changes in IQ means: A critique of Dickens and Flynn (2001). Psychological Review 109(4), 759- 763. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.759
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.759
  • Rowe, D.C., Jacobson, K.C., & Van den Oord, E.J.C.G. (1999). Genetic and Environmental Influences on Vocabulary IQ: Parental Education Level as Moderator. Child development, 70, 1151-1162.
  • Scarr, S., & Weinberg, R. A. (1983). The Minnesota Adoption Studies: Genetic differences and malleability. Child Development 54(2), 260-267.doi:10.2307/1129689
    » https://doi.org/10.2307/1129689
  • Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1971). Race, social class, and IQ. Science 174(4016), 1285-1295. doi:10.1126/science.174.4016.1285
    » https://doi.org/10.1126/science.174.4016.1285
  • Tucker-Drob, E.M., & Bates, T.C. (2016). Large Cross-National Differences in Gene x Socioeconomic Status Interaction on Intelligence. Psychological Science , 27, 138-149.
  • Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B., & Gottesman, I. I. (2003). Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science 14(6), 623-628. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1475.x
  • van Leeuwen, M., van den Berg, S. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). A twin-family study of general IQ. Learning and Individual Differences18(1), 76-88. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2007.04.006
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2007.04.006
  • Venables, W. N., Smith, D. M., & R Core Team. (2015). An introduction to R Notes on R: A programming environment for data analysis and graphics: Version 3.2.2 Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/doc/manuals/R-intro.pdf
    » https://cran.r-project.org/doc/manuals/R-intro.pdf
  • Wechsler, D. (2002). WISC III: Escala de inteligência Wechsler para crianças [WISC III: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children: Manual] (V. L. M. Figueiredo, Trans.). São Paulo, SP: Casa do Psicólogo.
  • Woodley, M. A., Fernandes, H. B. F., & Hopkins, W. D. (2015). The more g-loaded, the more heritable, evolvable, and phenotypically variable: Homology with humans in chimpanzee cognitive abilities. Intelligence50, 159-163. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.04.002
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.04.002

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Jan-Apr 2017

History

  • Received
    09 Nov 2015
  • Reviewed
    25 Mar 2016
  • Accepted
    12 Apr 2016
Universidade de São Paulo, Faculdade de Filosofia Ciências e Letras de Ribeirão Preto, Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Av.Bandeirantes 3900 - Monte Alegre, 14040-901 Ribeirão Preto - São Paulo - Brasil, Tel.: (55 16) 3315-3829 - Ribeirão Preto - SP - Brazil
E-mail: paideia@usp.br