Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

“Brutal Kill!” Violent video games as a predictor of aggression

“Brutal Kill!” Videogames Violentos como Preditor da Agressão

“Brutal Kill!” Videojuegos violentos como predictores de agresión

Abstract

In recent years, many international studies have investigated the relationship between violent games and violence, aggressiveness and delinquent behavior, but there are scarce studies in Brazil on the subject. The aim of this research was to investigate the relationship between dispositional behavior and antisocial behavior correlated with the playing of violent content video games. A total of 249 high school students participated in the study, of which 154 were women and 95 were men, aged 13-20 years (M = 15.4, DP = 1.12), who responded the following scales: Big Five Inventory, Buss and Perry Aggression Questionnaire, Antisocial and Criminal Behaviors Questionnaire and a new Scale of Video Games Violence. The regressions performed indicated that the violent games, antisocial behavior, anger and sex were predictors for physical aggression. In conclusion, the study confirms the hypothesis of the General Aggression Model on human aggression in which violent games are associated with aggressive behavior.

Keywords:
violence; video games; risk factors; aggression

Resumo

Nos últimos anos muitos estudos internacionais têm investigado a relação entre jogar jogos violentos e comportamento agressivo e delinquente, mas tem-se verificado uma escassez desses estudos no Brasil. O objetivo da pesquisa foi investigar a relação entre traços disposicionais e comportamentos antissociais com o consumo de videogames violentos. Participaram 249 estudantes do ensino médio, sendo 154 do sexo feminino e 95 do sexo masculino, e idade variando de 13 a 20 anos (M = 15,4; DP = 1,12), tendo respondido às seguintes escalas: Inventário dos Cinco Grandes Fatores, Questionário de Agressão de Buss e Perry, Questionário de Comportamentos Antissociais e Delitivos e a Escala de Violência nos Videogames. A regressão indicou que jogar videogames violentos, comportamento antissocial, raiva e gênero foram preditores da agressão física. Conclui-se que este estudo corrobora com a hipótese do Modelo Geral da Agressão sobre a agressão humana na qual os jogos violentos estão associados ao comportamento agressivo.

Palavras-chave:
violência; videogames; fatores de risco; agressão

Resumen

En los últimos años muchos estudios internacionales han investigado la relación entre jugar juegos violentos y comportamiento agresivo y delincuente, aunque en Brasil se ha verificado escasez de estos estudios. El objetivo de la presente búsqueda fue investigar la relación entre rasgos disposicionales y comportamientos antisociales con el consumo de videojuegos violentos. Para ello se contó con la participación de 249 estudiantes de Enseñanza Secundaria, siendo 154 de sexo femenino y 95 de sexo masculino, y edades variando de 13 a 20 años (M = 15,4, DP = 1,12), que respondieron las siguientes escalas: Inventario de los Cinco Grandes Factores, Cuestionario de Agresión de Buss y Perry, Cuestionario de Comportamientos Antisociales y Delictivos y la Escala de Violencia en los Videojuegos. La regresión indicó que jugar videojuegos violentos, comportamiento antisocial, rabia y género fueron predictores de agresión física. Se concluye que este estudio corrobora con la hipótesis del Modelo General de Agresión sobre agresión humana en la cual los juegos violentos están asociados al comportamiento agresivo.

Palabras clave:
violencia; videojuegos; factores de riesgo; agresión

Introduction

The digital gaming industry has become more conspicuous over the past years. From an economic point of view, its growth began in the 1970s with the emergence of the Atari video game company, created by Nollan Busnell and Ted Dabney (Mora-Cantallops & Sicilia, 2018Mora-Cantallops, M, & Sicilia, M. A. (2018). MOBA games: A literature review. Entertainment Computing, 26, 128-138.), and has now surpassed more established industries such as, for example, the cinema (Pinto, Coronel & Bresolin, 2013Pinto, N. G. M., Coronel, D. A., & Bresolin, R. P. (2013). Análise Comparativa da Evolução das Vendas e do PIB Per Capita dos Principais Mercados da Sétima Geração de Consoles de Videogames no Período de 2006-2011. REUNIR-Revista de Administração, Contabilidade e Sustentabilidade, 3(3), 44-60.). In addition, the inclusion of digital video games in the daily lives of the population has a role in the social, cognitive and emotional development of children and young adults who enjoy this kind of entertainment (Ashinoff, 2014Ashinoff, B. K. (2014). The potential of video games as a pedagogical tool. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1109. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01109
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01109...
; Kühn, Gleich, Lorenz, Lindenberger, & Gallinat, 2014Kühn, S., Gleich, T., Lorenz, R. C., Lindenberger, U., & Gallinat, J. (2014). Playing Super Mario induces structural brain plasticity: Gray matter changes resulting from training with a commercial video game. Molecular Psychiatry, 19(2), 265-271., Stansbury, Wheeler, & Buckingham, 2014Stansbury, J. A., Wheeler, E. A., & Buckingham, J. T. (2014). Can Wii engage college-level learners? Use of commercial off-the-shelf gaming in an introductory statistics course. Computers in the Schools, 31(1-2), 103-115.).

There has been an increasing debate about the consequences of video gaming on people’s behavior and attitudes, as it had once occurred with movies and other forms of media. It is noteworthy that researchers have positively associated video gaming with a set of negative influences, such as greater cognitive accessibility to aggressive content (Anderson, Carnagey, Flanagan, Benjamin, Eubanks & Valentine, 2004Anderson, C. A., Carnagey, N. L., Flanagan, M., Benjamin, A. J., Eubanks, J., & Valentine, J. C. (2004). Violent video games: Specific effects of violent content on aggressive thoughts and behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 199-249.), higher propensity for antisocial behavior (DeLisi, Vaughn, Gentile, Anderson & Shook, 2013DeLisi, M., Vaughn, M. G., Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., & Shook, J. J. (2013). Violent video games, delinquency, and youth violence: New evidence. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 11(2), 132-142.; Gentile & Bushman, 2012Gentile, D. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2012). Reassessing media violence effects using a risk and resilience approach to understanding aggression. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1(3), 138-151.; Mariano, 2016Mariano, T. E. (2016). O impacto dos videogames nas cognições pró e antissociais. Dissertação de Mestrado. Departamento de Psicologia, Universidade Federal da Paraíba, João Pessoa.), greater occurrence of negative affective states (Anderson & Dill, 2000Anderson, C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000). Video Games and Aggressive Thoughts, Feelings, and Behavior in the Laboratory and in Life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 772-790.), and reduction in prosocial behaviors (Anderson et al., 2010Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., & Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and pro social behavior in eastern and western countries: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 151- 173.; Bushman & Huesmann, 2014Bushman, B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2014). Twenty-Five Years of Research on Violence in Digital Games and Aggression Revisited. European Psychologist, 19(1), 47-55.).

Intense debate has aroused among researchers who argue that this influence does not exist or that it is not relevant, such as Elson and Ferguson (2013Elson, M., & Ferguson, C. J. (2013). Twenty-Five Years of Research on Violence in Digital Games and Aggression. European Psychologist, 19(1), 33-46.), and those who confirm an association, such as Bushman and Heusmann (2014Bushman, B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2014). Twenty-Five Years of Research on Violence in Digital Games and Aggression Revisited. European Psychologist, 19(1), 47-55.) and Krahé (2014Krahé, B. (2014). Restoring the Spirit of Fair Play in the Debate About Violent Video Games. European Psychologist, 19(1), 56-59.). The probable association becomes more significant when the most successful games display content related to violence or unfavorable social messages, such as God of War and Red Dead Redemption (Gouveia, Mariano, Nascimento, Grangeiro, Medeiros, 2017Gouveia, V. V., Mariano, T. E., Nascimento, A. M., Grangeiro, A. S. M., & Medeiros, E. D. (2017). Escala de motivações para jogos online: estudo de adaptação à realidade brasileira. Temas em Psicologia, 25(1), 131-141.; Kasumovic, Blake, Dixson, & Denson, 2015Kasumovic, M., Blake, K., Dixson, B., & Denson, T. (2015). Why do people play violent video games? Demographic, status-related, and mating-related correlates in men and women. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 204-211. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.018
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.0...
).

When comparing this discussion with others related to the effects of other media on antisocial behavior, exposure to violence in digital games is different due to permanent attention, interactivity and active psychological participation of players, which according to some authors could easily promote more negative effects, such as aggressive thoughts, than other media (Ferreira, Carneiro, Miguéis, Soares, & Esteves, 2009Ferreira, P. A., Carneiro, M. P., Miguéis, M. L., Soares, S., & Esteves, F. (2009). Jogos de computador violentos e seus efeitos na hostilidade, ansiedade e activação fisiológica. Revista Lusófona de Ciências da Mente e do Comportamento, 1(1), 193-209., Granic, Lobel & Engels, 2014Granic, I., Lobel, A., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2014). The benefits of playing video games. American Psychologist, 69(1), 66-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034857
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034857...
).

Swing, Gentile and Anderson (2009Swing, E. L., Gentile, D. A., & Anderson, C. A. (2009). Learning processes and violent video games. In R. E. Ferdig (Ed.), Handbook of Research on Effective Electronic Gaming in Education (Vol. II, pp. 876-892). New York: Information Science Reference.), following this reasoning, argue that certain characteristics of digital games can potentiate lasting changes in behavior, such as: difficulty level control, allowing greater adjustment to the player’s expertise; recurrent application of specific skills, which characterizes a training situation; existence of an active learning process due to the players’ engagement; reward systems and immediate feedback; possibility of generalization of specific behaviors in broader contexts.

Therefore, it is still relevant to investigate and further understand the phenomena related to the exposure of youth to violent games (Anderson & Bushman, 2001Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: A meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychological science, 12(5), 353-359.; Gee, 2003Gee, J. P. (2003). What video games have to teach us about learning and literacy. Computers in Entertainment, 1(1), 20-20.). To explain and understand the effects of exposure to violent content in entertainment media, we will use the Risk Factor Paradigm that seeks to identify risk factors for delinquent behavior, seeking to design appropriate prevention measures (Farrington, 2000Farrington, D. P. (2000). Explaining and preventing crime: The globalization of knowledge -The American Society of Criminology 1999 presidential address. Criminology, 38(1), 1-24.). Exposure to violence has been identified as a risk factor (among several others such as, for example, previous involvement in physical aggression, gender, and hostile attribution bias) (Bushman & Huesmann, 2006Bushman, B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2006). Short-term and long-term effects of violent media on aggression in children and adults. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 160, 348-352.; Anderson et al., 2010Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., & Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and pro social behavior in eastern and western countries: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 151- 173.). Studies that have included exposure to violence through media as a risk factor for aggressive and antisocial behavior have paved the way for this approach and for expanding knowledge about video game violence in the risk factor paradigm, such as Boxer, Huesmann, Bushman, O’Brien and Moceri (2009Boxer, P., Huesmann, L. R., Bushman, B. J., O’Brien, M., & Moceri, D. (2009). The role of violent media preference in cumulative developmental risk for violence and general aggression. Journal of youth and adolescence, 38(3), 417-428.), Braun, Stopfer, Müller, Beutel and Egloff (2016Braun, B., Stopfer, J. M., Müller, K. W., Beutel, M. E., & Egloff, B. (2016). Personality and video gaming: Comparing regular gamers, non-gamers, and gaming addicts and differentiating between game genres. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 406-412.), DeLisi et al., (2013DeLisi, M., Vaughn, M. G., Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., & Shook, J. J. (2013). Violent video games, delinquency, and youth violence: New evidence. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 11(2), 132-142.), Exelmans, Custers, and Van den Bulck (2015Exelmans, L., Custers, K., & Van den Bulck, J. (2015). Violent video games and delinquent behavior in adolescents: a risk factor perspective. Aggressive Behavior, 41(3), 267-279.), Gentile and Bushman (2012Gentile, D. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2012). Reassessing media violence effects using a risk and resilience approach to understanding aggression. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1(3), 138-151.) and Mariano (2016Mariano, T. E. (2016). O impacto dos videogames nas cognições pró e antissociais. Dissertação de Mestrado. Departamento de Psicologia, Universidade Federal da Paraíba, João Pessoa.).

Considering the existence of this effect triggers a whole series of repercussions. If the nature of the game plays an important role in the probability of manifestation of different behaviors, the knowledge produced by this area can subsidize, on the one hand, as it draws attention of digital game developers to which characteristics may favor the training of different behaviors and lead to a new perspective for the development of serious games (for example, aiming to reduce antisocial behaviors). On the other hand, deepening knowledge on the effects of digital games on behavior has the potential to provide information to the governmental organizations responsible for regulating the content production sector (Funk, Pizzo, & Baker, 2018Funk, D., Pizzo, A., & Baker, B. (2018). eSport management: Embracing eSport education and research opportunities. Sport Management Review, 21(1), 7-13.).

Considering the relevance of this construct, the aim of the present study is to investigate the relationship between violent video games, dispositional traits and antisocial behaviors. The definition of personality used in this study was proposed by McAdams (2006), who conceives it as an individual variation represented by patterns of development, dispositional traits, characteristics of adaptation and self-definition of life, situated in culture and social context.

Next, we shall discuss the General Aggression Model, a widely used theoretical model in the study of this issue, as it is integrative and seeks to explain the processes that are involved in aggression, followed by evidence that supports or questions its assumptions (Arriaga, Monteiro & Esteves, 2008Arriaga, P., Monteiro, M. B., & Esteves, F. (2008). Factores mediadores e moderadores dos efeitos dos jogos electrónicos violentos na agressão interpessoal. Revista Portuguesa de Pedagogia, 203-223.).

Description of the General Aggression Model

The General Aggression Model (GAM) proposed by Anderson and Bushman (2002Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 27-51.) is one of the most recent theoretical models about aggression and it is also the most widely used in research on violence in the media. It emphasizes three subtypes of structures: perceptual schemas, which are used to identify phenomena such as everyday physical objects (chairs) or social events (personal insults); personal schemas, which include beliefs about a particular person or group; and behavioral scripts, which contain information about how people behave under different circumstances. These structures, developed by the experience of individuals, are capable of influencing perceptions at different levels. They are processes that become more automatic as they are used and they are related to affective states and guide behavioral response to environmental demands (Anderson & Bushman, 2002Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 27-51.).

Three aspects are central to this model. The first one is related to several situational and individual factors and contextual characteristics present in a given situation (such as the characteristics of a given digital game) and how the environmental stimuli are interpreted by the individual. Among individual factors, Anderson and Bushman (2002Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 27-51.) highlight personality traits, sex, beliefs, attitudes and values, all of which influence how individuals interpret ambiguous situations and manifest aggressive/violent behavior.

When analyzing the susceptibility to the effects of violent video games, Markey and Markey (2010Markey, P. M., & Markey, C. N. (2010). Vulnerability to violent video games: A review and integration of personality research. Review of General Psychology, 14(2), 82-91.), for example, found that certain personality traits, when analyzed together, can be important moderators of the relationship between exposure to violent video games and aggressive behaviors. As for the analysis of the Five Personality Factors model, the authors emphasize that a high rate of neuroticism and low levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness could be related to the manifestation of aggressive behaviors.

Secondly, information derived from primary inputs (personal and situational, such as sex and provocation in traffic) influences the individual’s current internal state due to the cognitive, affective and arousal dimensions. And finally, information is interpreted by the individual, resulting in impulsive or controlled behaviors manifested by the individual; these behaviors are compatible with the nature of situational and individual factors (Anderson & Bushman, 2002Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 27-51.).

It has a cyclical character in the model because the outcomes of the decisive process are used as input for a later episode. In this sense, repeated exposure to specific digital games can be considered as a training situation, since, at each exposure, specific behavioral scripts and cognitive schemas are reinforced and the chances of these behaviors manifesting themselves in other situations is high (Anderson & Bushman, 2002Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 27-51.).

Studies were carried out with the purpose of directly or indirectly testing the relationships between the variables predicted in the GAM and violent video games, such as those by Arriaga, Monteiro and Esteves (2008Arriaga, P., Monteiro, M. B., & Esteves, F. (2008). Factores mediadores e moderadores dos efeitos dos jogos electrónicos violentos na agressão interpessoal. Revista Portuguesa de Pedagogia, 203-223.) that address the potential effects of violent video games on aggression. Research conducted by Greitemeyer (2013Greitemeyer, T. (2013). Playing violent video games increases intergroup bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(1), 70-78.), Hasan, Bègue and Bushman (2013Hasan, Y., Bègue, L., & Bushman, B. J. (2013). Violent video games stress people out and make them more aggressive. Aggressive Behavior, 39(1), 64-70.) and Kim and Sundar (2013Kim, K. J., & Sundar, S. S. (2013). Can interface features affect aggression resulting from violent video game play? An examination of realistic controller and large screen size. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16(5), 329-334.) also found an increase in aggressive behavior. This explains the use of the GAM in the present study. The hypothesis of the study is that violent video gaming predicts aggressive behavior.

Method

Participants

A total of 249 high school students from two public schools in the city of João Pessoa/PB, aged 13-20 years (M = 15.4, SD = 1.12), of which most were women (62%), single (98%) and in first year of high school (59%), participated in the study. The study used convenience sampling (non-probabilistic).

Instruments

Scale of Violence in Video Games: the scale was developed for the present study; it is composed of three items that aim to quantify the violence of electronic games. It was used to obtain information about the nature and quality of the digital games played by the participants, and the questions were based on the Anderson and Dill (2000Anderson, C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000). Video Games and Aggressive Thoughts, Feelings, and Behavior in the Laboratory and in Life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 772-790.) study, as replicated in several countries (Anderson et al., 2017Anderson, C. A., Suzuki, K., Swing, E. L., Groves, C. L., Gentile, D. A., Prot, S., … Petrescu, P. (2017). Media Violence and Other Aggression Risk Factors in Seven Nations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(7), 986-998.). Participants indicated the quantity of violence in their favorite video game on a scale of 1 (none) to 7 (a lot). The items in the instrument were as follows: “How much violent content is there in your favorite video game?” “How much death is there in your favorite video game?” and “How bloody is your favorite video game?”. Correlations between these items ranged from 0.79 to 0.85 and Cronbach’s alpha was 0.93.

Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire. The 26-item questionnaire was proposed by Buss and Perry (1992Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 452-459.) and it assesses four dimensions of aggression: physical aggression (e.g., If someone hits me, I hit back), verbal aggression (e.g., I cannot keep my mouth shut when people disagree with me), anger (e.g., Some friends say I’m hothead), and hostility (e.g., Sometimes jealousy eats me from the inside). These items are answered on a five-point scale (1 = totally disagree to 5 = strongly agree). It was adapted for the Brazilian context by Gouveia, Chaves, Peregrino, Branco and Gonçalves (2008Gouveia, V. V., Chaves, C. M. C. M., Peregrino, R. R., Branco, A. O. C., & Gonçalves, M. P. (2008). Medindo a agressão: o Questionário de Buss-Perry. Arquivos Brasileiros de Psicologia, 60(3), 92-103.) who found the following coefficients of internal consistency: verbal aggression (α = .72), anger (α = .83), and physical aggression (α = .85) and hostility (α = .77).

Questionnaire on Antisocial Behaviors and Delinquency. A 20-item instrument equally distributed in two factors (antisocial and delinquency), which was adapted for the Brazilian context by Gouveia, Santos, Pimentel, Diniz and Fonseca (2009Gouveia, V.V., Santos, W.S., Pimentel, C.E., Diniz, P.K.C., & Fonseca, P.N. (2009). Questionário de comportamentos antissociais e delitivos: Evidências psicométricas de uma versão reduzida. Psicologia: Reflexão & Crítica, 22(1), 20-28.). Participants rate how often they have felt the behaviors described in each item, and their responses are rated on a scale ranging from 1 = never to 10 = always. Examples are as follows: antisocial factor item ‘refusing to do the requested tasks (at work, at school or at home)’ and the delinquency factor ‘making money threatening weaker people’. Cronbach alpha revealed .84 for the antisocial behavior factor and .82 for the delinquent behavior factor (Gouveia, Santos, Pimentel, Diniz, & Fonseca, 2009Gouveia, V.V., Santos, W.S., Pimentel, C.E., Diniz, P.K.C., & Fonseca, P.N. (2009). Questionário de comportamentos antissociais e delitivos: Evidências psicométricas de uma versão reduzida. Psicologia: Reflexão & Crítica, 22(1), 20-28.).

The Five-Factor Model of Personality - BFI-44 (Big Five Inventory). It was first developed by John, Donahue and Kentle (1991John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five Inventory-Versions 4a and 54. Berkeley: University of California, Institute of Personality and Social Research.) in English, and validated in Brazil by Andrade (2008Andrade, J. M. (2008). Evidências do Inventário dos Cinco Grandes Fatores de Personalidade para o Brasil. Tese de doutorado não-publicada. Departamento de Psicologia, Universidade de Brasília, Brasília, DF.). The questionnaire consists of 34 items, which correspond to five factors, namely: Openness, Conscientiousness, Extroversion, Agreeableness and Neuroticism. In the validation process, the following alpha values were found: .65 to .75; Openness (α = .65), Neuroticism (α = .75), Extroversion (α = .75), Conscientiousness (α = .65), and Agreeableness (α = .69). To respond, the participant indicated how much each attribute characterized him/her on a Likert scale with the following response options: 1 = Strongly disagree 5 = Strongly agree.

Sociodemographic Questionnaire. It was used to characterize the sample with items such as sex, age, marital status and level of education.

Procedures

The present study strictly followed the ethical principles recommended by the Ethics Committee under Resolution 466/2012, which deals with research and human testing (Report number: 1.310.836). To collect data at the school, previous contact was made with the school principals to obtain consent for the research. After confirming the availability of students for voluntary participation in the study, the term of free and informed consent (TFIC, filled out by the legal guardian of the participant when under the age of 18 years) and the term of free and clarified assent (TFCA, filled out by the participant when under the age of 18 years) were signed and the objectives related to the study were explained to the students and the confidentiality of the participants’ answers was assured. During the data collection, participants answered the questionnaires individually, but collectively in a classroom with an average of 30 students per session. The average duration of the questionnaire administration was 30 minutes per classroom, considering all steps, and two psychology students administered the instrument. To reduce the impact on classroom activities, the questionnaire was administered at the end of the lesson when the teacher had already finished the daily class program. The questionnaire was administered at the end of the school day or close to the break time. The teacher remained in the room throughout the administration of the questionnaire, but without any intervention during the process. If the students or parents did not give consent to be in the study, the students were dismissed at the end of the class before the questionnaire was administered.

Data analysis

The research data were organized and statistically analyzed using the SPSS Statistics software, version 22.0 (IBM). Descriptive statistics were performed focusing on the exploratory analysis of the data. Pearson correlation analysis and comparison of means were used to verify possible associations among the variables of the study. After this step, the variables that revealed a relation with the dependent variables of the study (physical aggression and verbal aggression) were the object of simple and multiple regression analyses. In the present study, the factors of physical aggression and verbal aggression will be used as measures of the criterion variable (aggressive behavior), since they consist of the description of behaviors (and not of internal states), which are usually expressed in the opinion of the respondent. The order of magnitude of the correlation effect was based on Dancey and Reidy (2013Dancey, C. P., & Reidy, J. (2013). Estatística sem matemática para psicologia. Penso Editora.), of which weak: r = .10 to .30; moderate: r = .40 to .6; strong: r = .70 to 1.

Results

First we sought to investigate how the variables (VG = Violence in Games, AG = Agreeableness, OP = Openness, C = Conscientiousness, N = Neuroticism, E = Extroversion, AS = Antisocial Behavior, D = Delinquent Behavior, PA = Physical Aggression ; VA = Verbal Aggression, A = Anger; H = Hostility) of the study were related. Table 1 shows the means and standard deviations of the variables and the correlation scores among them.

Table 1
Means, standard deviations, and correlations of all study variables

There was a positive correlation between exposure to violent games and physical aggression (r = .23, p <0.001), the only dimension of the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire related to the Scale of Violence in Video Games. In the questionnaire on Antisocial Behavior and Delinquency, a weak correlation was found between Violence in Video Games and the Antisocial dimension (r = .14, p = .03). For personality traits, there were two negative correlations between the factors Violence in Video Games and Conscientiousness (r = -.13, p = .03) and Agreeableness (r = -.17, p = .01).

The Conscientiousness factor also correlated negatively with Physical Aggression (r = -.14, p = .03). On this latter factor, other significant associations were found, for example, with the trait of Neuroticism (r = .26, p <.01) and Antisocial Behavior (r = .36, p <.01). The Verbal Aggression dimension, in turn, correlated with the Agreeableness (r = .13, p= .04) and Neuroticism (r = .23, p < .01) factors in addition to the Physical Aggression dimension (r = .31, p <.01). However, there was no statistically significant relationship between Verbal Aggression and exposure to violent games. Finally, when analyzing the variable sex, we observed a significant difference between men and women for Physical Aggression, t (247) = 2.09, p = .04, d = .27, and Verbal Aggression, t (247) = 2.27, p = .02, d = .30. Men (M = 2.10) presented higher rates of physical aggression than women (M = 1.90), but women (M = 3.14) presented higher rates of verbal aggression than men (M = 2.80).

In accordance with the correlation findings, we sought to investigate the predictive power of the variables for the Physical Aggression dimension (Dependent Variable) using simple and multiple regression analyses. First the factor Violence in Video Games (Independent Variable), the main objective of the study, was included, followed by the constituent variables of the scales and the sex variable, established as the specific objectives.

When the Violence in Video Games factor (Model 1) was included, a predictive power was observed (β = .23, p <.01). However, this model explains only 5.2% of the variance in Physical Aggression.

In Model 2, the Agreeableness, Openness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, Extroversion, Antisocial Behavior, Delinquent Behavior, Verbal Aggression, Anger and Hostility factors were included. Therefore, controlling these variables, we found that violence in video games still predicts physical aggression (β = .19, p <.01). On the other hand, we found that anger and antisocial behavior had better predictive power (β = .49, p <.01 and β = .22, p <.01, respectively). As shown in Table 2, Model 2 accounts for 42% of the variance explanation, versus 5.2% for the first model.

Table 2
Simple and multiple regression (enter method) for predicting physical aggression

Finally, Model 3 included sex and age as independent variables. The sex variable presented a predictive power of β = -.17, p <.01. This model explains 44.3% of the variance with violent video gaming and it predicts physical aggression, β = .14, p <.01. However, anger (β = .51, p <.01), antisocial behavior (β = .18, p <.01) and sex were the best predictors.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate the relationship between violent video gaming, dispositional traits and antisocial behavior. Evidence suggests that exposure to violent games may explain the manifestation of antisocial behaviors, particularly those related to physical aggression. Moreover, evidence suggests that a set of variables, such as those related to personality, sex, and specific affective states, have considerable explanatory power, and studies that seek to understand these interactions may be promising avenues for further research.

The scientific literature indicates a positive relationship between youth exposure to violent games and aggression, which corroborates the data found in the present study and studies discussed below. Among these studies, the meta-analysis of Greitemeyer and Mügge (2014Greitemeyer, T., & Mügge, D. (2014). Video games do affect social outcomes: A meta-analytic review of the effects of violent and prosocial video dame. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 578-589. doi:10.1177/0146167213520459
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213520459...
) revealed an increase in variables related to aggression. In the study by Gentile, Swing, Anderson, Rinker, and Thomas (2016Gentile, D. A., Swing, E. L., Anderson, C. A., Rinker, D., & Thomas, K. M. (2016). Differential neural recruitment during violent video game play in violent and nonviolent game players. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 5(1), 39-51.), usual players of violent games demonstrated desensitization to aggression.

The data found by Hasan et al., (2013Hasan, Y., Bègue, L., & Bushman, B. J. (2013). Violent video games stress people out and make them more aggressive. Aggressive Behavior, 39(1), 64-70.) showed that violent video game players presented higher levels of aggression. Kim and Sundar (2013Kim, K. J., & Sundar, S. S. (2013). Can interface features affect aggression resulting from violent video game play? An examination of realistic controller and large screen size. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16(5), 329-334.) have identified increased aggressiveness in players who have used weapon-shaped controllers. Finally, in the study by Saleem, Anderson and Gentile (2012Saleem, M., Anderson, C. A., & Gentile, D. A. (2012). Effects of prosocial, neutral, and violent video games on college students’ affect. Aggressive Behavior, 38(4), 263-271.), children playing violent video games presented more offensive behaviors and less prosocial behaviors. In the present study, the preference for violent digital games can be a predictor of physical aggression even if the effect of several variables such as personality traits, anger, antisocial behavior or sex are controlled.

The regression analyses revealed a predictive power of experience with violent digital games regarding physical aggression. These findings support the GAM prediction that violent media content influences the manifestation of aggressive behavior. In this sense, playing violent video games would increase the expectation that aggressive situations should be treated aggressively, leading to increased aggressiveness (Anderson & Bushman, 2002Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 27-51.). Anger, as a predictor of aggressive behavior, would be a facilitator of aggressiveness as it was the best predictor of physical aggression in the present study. In addition, studies point to increased aggressiveness due to cathartic reactions and increase in the likelihood of a new aggressive behavior, which is different from the cathartic hypothesis that reaction causes a decrease in aggressiveness. These findings corroborate the GAM integrative hypothesis on human aggression and its cyclic triggering (Bushman, Baumeister, & Stack, 1999Bushman, B., Baumeister, R., & Stack, A. (1999). Catharsis, aggression, and persuasive influence: Self-fulfilling or self-defeating prophecies? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(3), 367-376.; Tedeschi & Felson, 1994Tedeschi, J. T., & Felson, R. B. (1994). Violence, aggression, & coercive actions. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.).

The correlation between exposure to violent games and antisocial behavior discussed in the results has also been the object of studies on delinquency and electronic gaming. Using the Risk Factor Paradigm, exposure to violent games is a risk factor for delinquent behavior (Boxer et al., 2009Boxer, P., Huesmann, L. R., Bushman, B. J., O’Brien, M., & Moceri, D. (2009). The role of violent media preference in cumulative developmental risk for violence and general aggression. Journal of youth and adolescence, 38(3), 417-428.; DeLisi et al., 2013DeLisi, M., Vaughn, M. G., Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., & Shook, J. J. (2013). Violent video games, delinquency, and youth violence: New evidence. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 11(2), 132-142., Exelmans, Custers, & Van den Bulck, 2015Exelmans, L., Custers, K., & Van den Bulck, J. (2015). Violent video games and delinquent behavior in adolescents: a risk factor perspective. Aggressive Behavior, 41(3), 267-279.; Gentile & Bushman, 2012Gentile, D. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2012). Reassessing media violence effects using a risk and resilience approach to understanding aggression. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1(3), 138-151.). Within this perspective, violent games would be another factor among many others that comprise the problem of aggression and delinquent behavior. The second most important factor to explain physical aggression was antisocial behavior, which was expected, since aggression can be understood as another antisocial behavior (Pimentel, 2004Pimentel, C. E. (2004). Valores humanos, preferência musical, identificação grupal e comportamentos antissociais. Dissertação de Mestrado não-publicada, Departamento de Psicologia, Universidade Federal da Paraíba, João Pessoa, PB.).

An alternative interpretation for the data found may suggest, in agreement with several criticisms to the GAM model, that exposure to violent games, although taking place in the explanatory model of physical aggression, may be overvalued to the detriment of other variables with greater predictive power (Elson & Ferguson, 2013Elson, M., & Ferguson, C. J. (2013). Twenty-Five Years of Research on Violence in Digital Games and Aggression. European Psychologist, 19(1), 33-46.). Therefore, the preference for violent digital games could just be another consequence, for example, of more stable dispositional traits and related to antisocial behaviors. In addition, participants with a high level of Neuroticism and low levels of Agreeableness and Conscientiousness, for example, might be more susceptible to the effect of game content on their behavior (Markey & Markey, 2010Markey, P. M., & Markey, C. N. (2010). Vulnerability to violent video games: A review and integration of personality research. Review of General Psychology, 14(2), 82-91.), leading to a more dispositional interpretation of the effects of media on behavior.

However, as GAM assumes relevant variables of input, individual characteristics (Anderson & Bushman, 2002Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 27-51.) as well as a set of evidence favorable to an interaction relationship between these variables for understanding the relationship between violent game playing and antisocial behavior. The results found with the participants of this study indicated a negative correlation between violent games and Conscientiousness and Agreeableness traits, confirmed by the results found by Anderson et al., (2004Anderson, C. A., Carnagey, N. L., Flanagan, M., Benjamin, A. J., Eubanks, J., & Valentine, J. C. (2004). Violent video games: Specific effects of violent content on aggressive thoughts and behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 199-249.). On the other hand, Chory and Goodboy (2011Chory, R. M., & Goodboy, A. K. (2011). Is basic personality related to violent and non-violent video game play and preferences? Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(4), 191-198.) found no correlation between Conscientiousness and violent video game playing but corroborated the negative correlation with Agreeableness. Thus, further in-depth research must be conducted, since new empirical evidence, particularly focusing on testing these relationships, can promote positive discussions on the subject.

With regard to sex, this variable was also considered one of the risk factors associated with violent games and aggressive and antisocial behaviors, as suggested in the findings of the present study. Therefore, these findings corroborate the literature that boys are more attracted to violent games (Lemmens, Valkenburg, & Peter, 2011Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2011). The effects of pathological gaming on aggressive behavior. Journal of youth and adolescence, 40(1), 38-47.; Vandercammen & Vandenbrande, 2011Vandercammen, M., & Vandenbrande, K. (2011). Jongeren en media. Brussel: Onderzoeks- en Informatiecentrum van de Verbruikersorganisaties.).

However, this study is not without limitations. Among them, three deserve attention. First, the results are based on self-reporting measures that may lead to bias in responses due to social desirability. Second, there may be other important risk factors to better detail the problem that were not included in the research. Finally, the cross-sectional study precludes inference of causality. The results are restricted to the study sample.

Despite the limitations, the study can help to explain the problem involving violent games and aggression by raising questions about possible predictive factors. On the other hand, although the significance of video game violence has been proven to understand physical aggression, the role of anger and antisocial behavior, as well as sex, should be emphasized. These variables proved to be better predictors of aggressive behavior. Therefore, further studies should include these variables in an experimental design to investigate the possible effect of violent games on the behavior of players of different age groups. For example, the difference between the time of exposure to games, which was not considered in this research, and use of specific types of games could be investigated.

Finally, the predictive power of video game violence in aggressive behavior, after controlling for several variables, such as personality traits, was confirmed (Barlett & Anderson, 2012Barlett, C. P., & Anderson, C. A. (2012). Direct and indirect relations between the Big 5 personality traits and aggressive behavior. Personality & Individual Differences, 52, 870-875.; Tang & Fox, 2016Tang, W. Y., & Fox, J. (2016). Men’s harassment behavior in online video games: Personality traits and game factors. Aggressive behavior, 42(6), 513-521.). The present study contributes to those that have investigated the role of violent media in aggressive and antisocial behaviors (Braun, Stopfer, Müller, Beutel, & Egloff, 2016Braun, B., Stopfer, J. M., Müller, K. W., Beutel, M. E., & Egloff, B. (2016). Personality and video gaming: Comparing regular gamers, non-gamers, and gaming addicts and differentiating between game genres. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 406-412.; DeLisi et al., 2013DeLisi, M., Vaughn, M. G., Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., & Shook, J. J. (2013). Violent video games, delinquency, and youth violence: New evidence. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 11(2), 132-142.), promoting the development in the field in Brazil.

References

  • Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: A meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychological science, 12(5), 353-359.
  • Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 27-51.
  • Anderson, C. A., & Dill, K. E. (2000). Video Games and Aggressive Thoughts, Feelings, and Behavior in the Laboratory and in Life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 772-790.
  • Anderson, C. A., Carnagey, N. L., Flanagan, M., Benjamin, A. J., Eubanks, J., & Valentine, J. C. (2004). Violent video games: Specific effects of violent content on aggressive thoughts and behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 199-249.
  • Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., & Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and pro social behavior in eastern and western countries: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 151- 173.
  • Anderson, C. A., Suzuki, K., Swing, E. L., Groves, C. L., Gentile, D. A., Prot, S., … Petrescu, P. (2017). Media Violence and Other Aggression Risk Factors in Seven Nations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(7), 986-998.
  • Andrade, J. M. (2008). Evidências do Inventário dos Cinco Grandes Fatores de Personalidade para o Brasil Tese de doutorado não-publicada. Departamento de Psicologia, Universidade de Brasília, Brasília, DF.
  • Arriaga, P., Monteiro, M. B., & Esteves, F. (2008). Factores mediadores e moderadores dos efeitos dos jogos electrónicos violentos na agressão interpessoal. Revista Portuguesa de Pedagogia, 203-223.
  • Ashinoff, B. K. (2014). The potential of video games as a pedagogical tool. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1109. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01109
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01109
  • Barlett, C. P., & Anderson, C. A. (2012). Direct and indirect relations between the Big 5 personality traits and aggressive behavior. Personality & Individual Differences, 52, 870-875.
  • Boxer, P., Huesmann, L. R., Bushman, B. J., O’Brien, M., & Moceri, D. (2009). The role of violent media preference in cumulative developmental risk for violence and general aggression. Journal of youth and adolescence, 38(3), 417-428.
  • Braun, B., Stopfer, J. M., Müller, K. W., Beutel, M. E., & Egloff, B. (2016). Personality and video gaming: Comparing regular gamers, non-gamers, and gaming addicts and differentiating between game genres. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 406-412.
  • Bushman, B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2006). Short-term and long-term effects of violent media on aggression in children and adults. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 160, 348-352.
  • Bushman, B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2014). Twenty-Five Years of Research on Violence in Digital Games and Aggression Revisited. European Psychologist, 19(1), 47-55.
  • Bushman, B., Baumeister, R., & Stack, A. (1999). Catharsis, aggression, and persuasive influence: Self-fulfilling or self-defeating prophecies? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(3), 367-376.
  • Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 452-459.
  • Chory, R. M., & Goodboy, A. K. (2011). Is basic personality related to violent and non-violent video game play and preferences? Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(4), 191-198.
  • Dancey, C. P., & Reidy, J. (2013). Estatística sem matemática para psicologia Penso Editora.
  • DeLisi, M., Vaughn, M. G., Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., & Shook, J. J. (2013). Violent video games, delinquency, and youth violence: New evidence. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 11(2), 132-142.
  • Elson, M., & Ferguson, C. J. (2013). Twenty-Five Years of Research on Violence in Digital Games and Aggression. European Psychologist, 19(1), 33-46.
  • Elson, M., Mohseni, M. R., Breuer, J., Scharkow, M., & Quandt, T. (2014). Press CRTT to measure aggressive behavior: The unstandardized use of the competitive reaction time task in aggression research. Psychological Assessment, 26(2), 419-32.
  • Exelmans, L., Custers, K., & Van den Bulck, J. (2015). Violent video games and delinquent behavior in adolescents: a risk factor perspective. Aggressive Behavior, 41(3), 267-279.
  • Farrington, D. P. (2000). Explaining and preventing crime: The globalization of knowledge -The American Society of Criminology 1999 presidential address. Criminology, 38(1), 1-24.
  • Ferreira, P. A., Carneiro, M. P., Miguéis, M. L., Soares, S., & Esteves, F. (2009). Jogos de computador violentos e seus efeitos na hostilidade, ansiedade e activação fisiológica. Revista Lusófona de Ciências da Mente e do Comportamento, 1(1), 193-209.
  • Funk, D., Pizzo, A., & Baker, B. (2018). eSport management: Embracing eSport education and research opportunities. Sport Management Review, 21(1), 7-13.
  • Gee, J. P. (2003). What video games have to teach us about learning and literacy. Computers in Entertainment, 1(1), 20-20.
  • Gentile, D. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2012). Reassessing media violence effects using a risk and resilience approach to understanding aggression. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1(3), 138-151.
  • Gentile, D. A., Swing, E. L., Anderson, C. A., Rinker, D., & Thomas, K. M. (2016). Differential neural recruitment during violent video game play in violent and nonviolent game players. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 5(1), 39-51.
  • Gouveia, V. V., Chaves, C. M. C. M., Peregrino, R. R., Branco, A. O. C., & Gonçalves, M. P. (2008). Medindo a agressão: o Questionário de Buss-Perry. Arquivos Brasileiros de Psicologia, 60(3), 92-103.
  • Gouveia, V. V., Mariano, T. E., Nascimento, A. M., Grangeiro, A. S. M., & Medeiros, E. D. (2017). Escala de motivações para jogos online: estudo de adaptação à realidade brasileira. Temas em Psicologia, 25(1), 131-141.
  • Gouveia, V.V., Santos, W.S., Pimentel, C.E., Diniz, P.K.C., & Fonseca, P.N. (2009). Questionário de comportamentos antissociais e delitivos: Evidências psicométricas de uma versão reduzida. Psicologia: Reflexão & Crítica, 22(1), 20-28.
  • Granic, I., Lobel, A., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2014). The benefits of playing video games. American Psychologist, 69(1), 66-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034857
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0034857
  • Greitemeyer, T. (2013). Playing violent video games increases intergroup bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(1), 70-78.
  • Greitemeyer, T., & Mügge, D. (2014). Video games do affect social outcomes: A meta-analytic review of the effects of violent and prosocial video dame. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 578-589. doi:10.1177/0146167213520459
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213520459
  • Hasan, Y., Bègue, L., & Bushman, B. J. (2013). Violent video games stress people out and make them more aggressive. Aggressive Behavior, 39(1), 64-70.
  • John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five Inventory-Versions 4a and 54 Berkeley: University of California, Institute of Personality and Social Research.
  • Kasumovic, M., Blake, K., Dixson, B., & Denson, T. (2015). Why do people play violent video games? Demographic, status-related, and mating-related correlates in men and women. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 204-211. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.018
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.018
  • Kim, K. J., & Sundar, S. S. (2013). Can interface features affect aggression resulting from violent video game play? An examination of realistic controller and large screen size. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16(5), 329-334.
  • Krahé, B. (2014). Restoring the Spirit of Fair Play in the Debate About Violent Video Games. European Psychologist, 19(1), 56-59.
  • Kühn, S., Gleich, T., Lorenz, R. C., Lindenberger, U., & Gallinat, J. (2014). Playing Super Mario induces structural brain plasticity: Gray matter changes resulting from training with a commercial video game. Molecular Psychiatry, 19(2), 265-271.
  • Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2011). The effects of pathological gaming on aggressive behavior. Journal of youth and adolescence, 40(1), 38-47.
  • Mariano, T. E. (2016). O impacto dos videogames nas cognições pró e antissociais. Dissertação de Mestrado. Departamento de Psicologia, Universidade Federal da Paraíba, João Pessoa.
  • Markey, P. M., & Markey, C. N. (2010). Vulnerability to violent video games: A review and integration of personality research. Review of General Psychology, 14(2), 82-91.
  • Mora-Cantallops, M, & Sicilia, M. A. (2018). MOBA games: A literature review. Entertainment Computing, 26, 128-138.
  • Pimentel, C. E. (2004). Valores humanos, preferência musical, identificação grupal e comportamentos antissociais Dissertação de Mestrado não-publicada, Departamento de Psicologia, Universidade Federal da Paraíba, João Pessoa, PB.
  • Pinto, N. G. M., Coronel, D. A., & Bresolin, R. P. (2013). Análise Comparativa da Evolução das Vendas e do PIB Per Capita dos Principais Mercados da Sétima Geração de Consoles de Videogames no Período de 2006-2011. REUNIR-Revista de Administração, Contabilidade e Sustentabilidade, 3(3), 44-60.
  • Retondar, M. J., & Harris, E. R. A. (2013). Jogos eletrônicos e violência. Motrivivência, 25(40), 183-191.
  • Saleem, M., Anderson, C. A., & Gentile, D. A. (2012). Effects of prosocial, neutral, and violent video games on college students’ affect. Aggressive Behavior, 38(4), 263-271.
  • Stansbury, J. A., Wheeler, E. A., & Buckingham, J. T. (2014). Can Wii engage college-level learners? Use of commercial off-the-shelf gaming in an introductory statistics course. Computers in the Schools, 31(1-2), 103-115.
  • Swing, E. L., Gentile, D. A., & Anderson, C. A. (2009). Learning processes and violent video games. In R. E. Ferdig (Ed.), Handbook of Research on Effective Electronic Gaming in Education (Vol. II, pp. 876-892). New York: Information Science Reference.
  • Tang, W. Y., & Fox, J. (2016). Men’s harassment behavior in online video games: Personality traits and game factors. Aggressive behavior, 42(6), 513-521.
  • Tedeschi, J. T., & Felson, R. B. (1994). Violence, aggression, & coercive actions Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  • Vandercammen, M., & Vandenbrande, K. (2011). Jongeren en media Brussel: Onderzoeks- en Informatiecentrum van de Verbruikersorganisaties.
  • Ybarra, M. L., Huesmann, L. R., Korchmaros, J. D., & Reisner, S. L. (2014). Cross-sectional associations between violent video and computer game playing and weapon carrying in a national cohort of children. Aggressive Behavior, 40(4), 345-358.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    03 Aug 2020
  • Date of issue
    Apr-Jun 2020

History

  • Received
    26 June 2018
  • Accepted
    12 Mar 2019
Universidade de São Francisco, Programa de Pós-Graduação Stricto Sensu em Psicologia R. Waldemar César da Silveira, 105, Vl. Cura D'Ars (SWIFT), Campinas - São Paulo, CEP 13045-510, Telefone: (19)3779-3771 - Campinas - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistapsico@usf.edu.br