Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Psychometric properties of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX): a study with Brazilian older adults

Propriedades Psicométricas do Questionário Disexecutivo (DEX): Um Estudo com Adultos Idosos Brasileiros

Propiedades psicométricas del Cuestionario Disejecutivo (DEX): un estudio con adultos mayores brasileños

Abstract

This study aimed to verify the psychometric properties of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) through exploratory factor analysis (EFA), evidence of reliability, and convergent validity, in a sample of neurologically preserved older adults. Participants were 345 older adults who answered, in addition to DEX, a sociodemographic and clinical questionnaire, the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), and two verbal fluency tasks. The EFA was conducted through Parallel Analysis based on the generation of a polychoric correlation matrix, as well as Pearson’s correlation between the DEX scores, age, education, MMSE, and verbal fluency tasks. According to the EFA, the extraction of two factors (“Inhibition” and “Social Regulation and Planning”) was suggested and DEX was negatively associated with age and MMSE. In conclusion, DEX presented a satisfactory factorial structure for older adults, which can be considered a reliable self-report measure for complaints of executive functions.

Keywords:
factor analysis; aging; cognition; symptom assessment; executive function

Resumo

O objetivo deste estudo foi verificar as propriedades psicométricas do Questionário Disexecutivo (DEX) por meio de análise fatorial exploratória (AFE), evidências de confiabilidade e de validade convergente, em uma amostra de adultos idosos neurologicamente preservados. Participaram 345 adultos idosos que responderam, além do DEX, um questionário sociodemográfico e clínico, o Miniexame do Estado Mental (MEEM) e duas tarefas de fluência verbal. A AFE foi conduzida por meio de análise paralela com base na geração de matriz de correlação policórica, bem como correlação de Pearson entre os escores do DEX, idade, escolaridade, MEEM e tarefas de fluência verbal. A AFE sugeriu a extração de dois fatores (“Inibição” e “Regulação Social e Planejamento”) e o DEX associou-se negativamente com a idade e com o MEEM. Conclui-se que o DEX apresentou estrutura fatorial satisfatória para adultos idosos, podendo ser considerado uma medida confiável de autorrelato para queixas de funções executivas.

Palavras-chave:
análise fatorial; envelhecimento; cognição; avaliação de sintomas; função executiva

Resumen

El objetivo de este estudio fue verificar las propiedades psicométricas del Cuestionario Disejecutivo (DEX) a través del análisis factorial exploratorio (AFE), evidencias de confiabilidad y validez convergente, en una muestra de adultos mayores preservados neurológicamente. Participaron 345 adultos mayores que respondieron, además del DEX, un cuestionario sociodemográfico y clínico, el Mini Examen del Estado Mental (MEEM) y dos tareas de fluidez verbal. La AFE se realizó mediante Análisis Paralelo basado en la generación de una matriz de correlación policórica, así como la correlación de Pearson entre las puntuaciones del DEX, edad, nivel de escolarización, MEEM y tareas de fluidez verbal. La AFE sugirió la extracción de dos factores (“Inhibición” y “Regulación y Planificación Social”) y el DEX se asoció negativamente con la edad y con el MEEM. Se concluyó que el DEX presentó una estructura factorial satisfactoria para adultos mayores, lo que puede considerarse una medida de autoinforme fiable para las quejas de funciones ejecutivas.

Palabras clave:
análisis factorial; envejecimiento; cognición; evaluación de síntomas; función ejecutiva

Executive dysfunction, also known as dysexecutive syndrome, refers to deficits in executive functions observed when performing daily activities (Chaytor & Schmitter-Edgecombe, 2007Chaytor, N., & Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2007). Fractionation of the dysexecutive syndrome in a heterogeneous neurological sample: Comparing the Dysexecutive Questionnaire and the Brock Adaptive Functioning Questionnaire. Brain Injury, 21(6), 615-621. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050701426949
https://doi.org/10.1080/0269905070142694...
). Frontal lobe injuries are often associated with executive dysfunction, as this region is considered the superior cortical center for decision-making, planning, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility (Chan, 2001Chan, R. C. K. (2001). Dysexecutive symptoms among a non-clinical sample: A study with the use of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire. British Journal of Psychology, 92(Part 3), 551-565. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712601162338
https://doi.org/10.1348/000712601162338...
). Thus, there is an occurrence of executive dysfunction in individuals who have suffered injuries in the frontal lobes resulting from traumatic brain injury (Ozga et al., 2018Ozga, J. E., Povroznik, J. M., Engler-Chiurazzi, E. B., & Vonder Haar, C. (2018). Executive (dys)function after traumatic brain injury: Special considerations for behavioral pharmacology. Behavioural Pharmacology, 29(7), 617-637. https://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0000000000000430
https://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.000000000000...
; Wood & Worthington, 2017Wood, R. L., & Worthington, A. (2017). Neurobehavioral abnormalities associated with executive dysfunction after traumatic brain injury. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00195
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00195...
) and brain stroke (Povroznik et al., 2018Povroznik, J. M., Ozga, J. E., Vonder Haar, C., & Engler-Chiurazzi, E. B. (2018). Executive (dys)function after stroke: Special considerations for behavioral pharmacology. Behavioural Pharmacology, 29(7), 638-653. https://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0000000000000432
https://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.000000000000...
; Veldsman et al., 2020Veldsman, M., Werden, E., Egorova, N., Khlif, M. S., & Brodtmann, A. (2020). Microstructural degeneration and cerebrovascular risk burden underlying executive dysfunction after stroke. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75074-w
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75074...
). However, there are reports of this condition also in older adults with Mild Cognitive Decline (Junquera et al., 2020Junquera, A., García-Zamora, E., Olazarán, J., Parra, M. A., & Fernández-Guinea, S. (2020). Role of executive functions in the conversion from mild cognitive impairment to dementia. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 77(2), 641-653. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-200586
https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-200586...
; Lee et al., 2019Lee, S. J., Kim, H., Kim, L. S., Kim, J. H., & Park, K. W. (2019). Effects of frontal-executive dysfunction on self-perceived hearing handicap in the elderly with mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One, 14(3), e0210014. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210014
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.021...
), Alzheimer’s Disease (Amanzio et al., 2020Amanzio, M., Bartoli, M., Cipriani, G. E., & Palermo, S. (2020). Executive dysfunction and reduced self-awareness in patients with neurological disorders. A mini-review. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1-8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01697
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01697...
; Guarino et al., 2019Guarino, A., Favieri, F., Boncompagni, I., Agostini, F., Cantone, M., & Casagrande, M. (2019). Executive functions in Alzheimer Disease: A systematic review. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 10, 1-24. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00437
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00437...
), and Frontotemporal Dementia (Amanzio et al., 2020Amanzio, M., Bartoli, M., Cipriani, G. E., & Palermo, S. (2020). Executive dysfunction and reduced self-awareness in patients with neurological disorders. A mini-review. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1-8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01697
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01697...
; Baez et al., 2017Baez, S., Pinasco, C., Roca, M., Ferrari, J., Couto, B., García-Cordero, I., Ibañez, A., Cruz, F., Reyes, P., Matallana, D., Manes, F., Cetcovich, M., & Torralva, T. (2017). Brain structural correlates of executive and social cognition profiles in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and elderly bipolar disorder. Neuropsychologia, 126, 159-169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.012
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsycholog...
), suggesting that this syndrome is related to neurodegenerative conditions that affect the frontal lobes (Mooney et al., 2006Mooney, B., Walmsley, C., & McFarland, K. (2006). Factor analysis of the self-report Dysexecutive (DEX-S) Questionnaire. Applied Neuropsychology, 13(1), 12-18. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1301_2
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1301_...
).

The Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) was developed by Wilson et al. (1996Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. J. (1996). Behavioral Assessment of Dysexecutive Syndrome. Thames Valley Test Company.) in order to be a scale of complaints of executive functioning based on daily activities. DEX is part of the Behavioral Assessment of Dysexecutive Syndrome, an instrument consisting of ecological activities that assess impairments in different components of executive functions in adults with frontal lobe injury. DEX has a self-report version, and another aimed at informants (for example, family members or caregivers). Both versions comprise 20 items that are answered using a five-point Likert scale, whose high scores indicate a higher frequency of executive losses. The items represent problems related to emotion/personality, behavior, cognition, and motivation, which are frequent in patients with frontal lesions (Fellows, 2019Fellows, L. K. (2019). The functions of the frontal lobes: Evidence from patients with focal brain damage. In M. D’Esposito, & J. H. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology (pp. 19-34). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804281-6.00002-1
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804281...
; Stuss & Knight, 2002Stuss, D. T., & Knight, R. T. (2002). Principles of frontal lobe function. Oxford University Press.). However, since the publication of DEX, few studies have reported its psychometric properties in clinical and non-clinical populations (Mooney et al., 2006Mooney, B., Walmsley, C., & McFarland, K. (2006). Factor analysis of the self-report Dysexecutive (DEX-S) Questionnaire. Applied Neuropsychology, 13(1), 12-18. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1301_2
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1301_...
), with no consensus regarding its factor structure (Pedrero-Pérez et al., 2015Pedrero-Pérez, E. J., Ruiz-Sánchez-de-León, J. M., & Winpenny-Tejedor, C. (2015). Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX): Unrestricted structural analysis in large clinical and non-clinical samples. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 25(6), 879-894. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2014.993659
https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2014.99...
).

The first study that investigated the factorial structure of DEX was conducted by Burgess et al. (1998Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie, H., & Wilson, B. A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 547-558. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798466037
https://doi.org/10.1017/s135561779846603...
), which included in the sample neurological patients and controls who responded to the self-report version of the instrument. From the analysis of the main components, a solution of five factors was found (inhibition, intentionality, executive memory, positive affect, and negative affect). Other studies have identified a five-factor structure similar to that reported by Burgess et al. (1998Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie, H., & Wilson, B. A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 547-558. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798466037
https://doi.org/10.1017/s135561779846603...
), also conducted through principal components analysis (PCA), in a non-clinical population and with neurological conditions based on the self-report version (Amieva et al., 2003Amieva, H., Phillips, L., & Della Sala, S. (2003). Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain and Cognition, 53(2), 129-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00094-0
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00...
; Luna-Lario et al., 2012Luna-Lario, P., Seijas-Gómez, R., Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., Hernáez-Goñi, P., & Mata-Pastor, I. (2012). Estructura factorial del cuestionario disejecutivo en una muestra de población española con daño cerebral adquirido y quejas de déficit de memoria. Revista de Neurología, 55(11), 641-650. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5511.2012549
https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5511.2012549...
) and the informant version (Chan, 2001Chan, R. C. K. (2001). Dysexecutive symptoms among a non-clinical sample: A study with the use of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire. British Journal of Psychology, 92(Part 3), 551-565. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712601162338
https://doi.org/10.1348/000712601162338...
; Chaytor & Schmitter-Edgecombe, 2007Chaytor, N., & Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2007). Fractionation of the dysexecutive syndrome in a heterogeneous neurological sample: Comparing the Dysexecutive Questionnaire and the Brock Adaptive Functioning Questionnaire. Brain Injury, 21(6), 615-621. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050701426949
https://doi.org/10.1080/0269905070142694...
). However, factorial structures of six (Pedrero-Pérez et al., 2009Pedrero-Pérez, E. J., Ruiz-Sánchez-de-León, J. M., Rojo-Mota, G., Llanero-Luque, M., Olivar-Arroyo, A., Bouso-Saiz, J. C., & Puerta-García, C. (2009). Versión española del cuestionario disejecutivo (DEX-Sp): Propiedades psicométricas en adictos y población no clínica. Adicciones, 21(2), 155-166. https://doi.org/10.20882/adicciones.243
https://doi.org/10.20882/adicciones.243...
), four (Bodenburg & Dopslaff, 2008Bodenburg, S., & Dopslaff, N. (2008). The Dysexecutive Questionnaire advanced: Item and test score characteristics, 4-factor solution, and severity classification. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 196(1), 75-78. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e31815faa2b
https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e31815f...
; Mooney et al., 2006Mooney, B., Walmsley, C., & McFarland, K. (2006). Factor analysis of the self-report Dysexecutive (DEX-S) Questionnaire. Applied Neuropsychology, 13(1), 12-18. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1301_2
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1301_...
; Yang et al., 2018Yang, Z. Y., Sun, S. F., Lui, S. S. Y., Shi, H. S., Xie, D. J., Xie, W. L., Wang, Y., Cheung, E. F. C., Shum, D. H. K., & Chan, R. C. K. (2018). An attempt at revisiting the factor structure of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire in the Chinese setting. PsyCh Journal, 7(1), 25-30. https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.198
https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.198...
), three (Shinagawa et al., 2007Shinagawa, Y., Nakaaki, S., Hongo, J., Murata, Y., Sato, J., Matsui, T., Tatsumi, H., Akechi, T., & Furukawa, T. A. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) in Alzheimer’s disease: Validation of a behavioral rating scale to assess dysexecutive symptoms in Japanese patients with Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 951-956. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768
https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768...
; Simblett & Bateman, 2011Simblett, S. K., & Bateman, A. (2011). Dimensions of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) examined using Rasch analysis. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 21(1), 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2010.531216
https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2010.53...
; Wilson et al., 2003Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. J. (2003). Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS). Journal of Occupational Psychology, Employment and Disability, 5(2), 33-37. https://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20121105170305/http://www.dwp.gov.uk/docs/no2-sum-03-test-review-2.pdf
https://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov....
), two dimensions (Pedrero-Pérez et al., 2011Pedrero-Pérez, E. J., Ruiz-Sánchez-de-León, J. M., Lozoya-Delgado, P., Llanero-Luque, M., Rojo-Mota, G., & Puerta-García, C. (2011). Evaluación de los síntomas prefrontales: Propiedades psicométricas y datos normativos del cuestionario disejecutivo (DEX) en uma muestra de población española. Revista de Neurología, 52(7), 394-404. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5207.2010731
https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5207.2010731...
), and one-dimensional (Takeuchi et al., 2013Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y, Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. (2013). Brain structures associated with executive functions during everyday events in a non-clinical sample. Brain Structure and Function, 218(4), 1017-1032. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0444-z
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0444-...
) are found in the literature.

Specifically in aging, to date, only two studies aimed to identify the factor structure of DEX in the older adult population (Amieva et al., 2003Amieva, H., Phillips, L., & Della Sala, S. (2003). Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain and Cognition, 53(2), 129-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00094-0
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00...
; Shinagawa et al., 2007Shinagawa, Y., Nakaaki, S., Hongo, J., Murata, Y., Sato, J., Matsui, T., Tatsumi, H., Akechi, T., & Furukawa, T. A. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) in Alzheimer’s disease: Validation of a behavioral rating scale to assess dysexecutive symptoms in Japanese patients with Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 951-956. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768
https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768...
). Amieva et al. (2003Asparouhov, T., & Muthen, B. (2010). Simple second order chi-square correction. https://www.statmodel.com/download/WLSMV_new_chi21.pdf
https://www.statmodel.com/download/WLSMV...
) investigated a sample of neurologically healthy older adults who responded to the self-report version of the instrument, while Shinagawa et al. (2007Shinagawa, Y., Nakaaki, S., Hongo, J., Murata, Y., Sato, J., Matsui, T., Tatsumi, H., Akechi, T., & Furukawa, T. A. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) in Alzheimer’s disease: Validation of a behavioral rating scale to assess dysexecutive symptoms in Japanese patients with Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 951-956. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768
https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768...
) included in their sample older adults with Alzheimer’s Disease, who responded to the DEX version for informants. The study by Amieva et al. (2003Asparouhov, T., & Muthen, B. (2010). Simple second order chi-square correction. https://www.statmodel.com/download/WLSMV_new_chi21.pdf
https://www.statmodel.com/download/WLSMV...
) suggested a five-factor solution for DEX, while the study by Shinagawa et al. (2007Shinagawa, Y., Nakaaki, S., Hongo, J., Murata, Y., Sato, J., Matsui, T., Tatsumi, H., Akechi, T., & Furukawa, T. A. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) in Alzheimer’s disease: Validation of a behavioral rating scale to assess dysexecutive symptoms in Japanese patients with Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 951-956. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768
https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768...
) suggested a three-factor solution.

In addition to the factor structure, some international studies searched evidence of convergent validity of DEX with clinical aspects or other instruments. For example, Emmanouel et al. (2014Emmanouel, A., Mouza, E., Kessels, R. P., & Fasotti, L. (2014). Validity of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX). Ratings by patients with brain injury and their therapists. Brain Injury, 28(12), :1581-1589. http://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2014.942371.
http://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2014.942...
) demonstrated a relationship between DEX results and the presence of brain injury and perception of the severity condition by the professional team. In addition, in the study by Azouvi et al. (2015Azouvi, P., Vallat-Azouvi, C., Millox, V., Darnoux, E., Ghout, I., Azerad, S., Ruet, A., Bayen, E., Pradat-Diehl, P., Aegerter, P., Weiss, J. J., & Jourdan, C. (2015). Ecological validity of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire: Results from the PariS-TBI study. Neuropsychol Rehability, 25(6), 864 -878. http://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2014.990907.
http://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2014.990...
), DEX was related to years of education, cognitive deficit, dependence on activities of daily living, anxiety, depression, and inability to return to work.

However, the small sample size, the heterogeneity of data collection (self-report or informant), and the data analysis techniques of these studies (Amieva et al., 2003Amieva, H., Phillips, L., & Della Sala, S. (2003). Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain and Cognition, 53(2), 129-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00094-0
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00...
; Shinagawa et al., 2007Shinagawa, Y., Nakaaki, S., Hongo, J., Murata, Y., Sato, J., Matsui, T., Tatsumi, H., Akechi, T., & Furukawa, T. A. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) in Alzheimer’s disease: Validation of a behavioral rating scale to assess dysexecutive symptoms in Japanese patients with Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 951-956. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768
https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768...
) suggest the fragility of the results with the older adult population, especially regarding the factor structure of the instrument. Thus, this study aimed to verify the psychometric properties of DEX, through exploratory factor analysis (EFA) from a polychoric correlation matrix, evidence of reliability, and convergent validity, in a sample of neurologically preserved older adults.

Method

Participants

Initially, 381 community older adults, aged 60 years and over, were recruited for convenience from cities in the northern region of Rio Grande do Sul and in the Southwest of Santa Catarina. Among them, 13 were excluded due to a history of neurological or psychiatric illnesses, or primary sensory alterations that were not corrected at the time of the assessment (for example, absence of hearing aids or glasses). Still, another 23 participants were excluded for showing signs of cognitive decline assessed by the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) (adapted by Chaves & Izquierdo, 1992Chaves, M. L, & Izquierdo, I. (1992). Differential diagnosis between dementia and depression: A study of efficiency increment. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 85(6), 378-382. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.1992.tb06032.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.1992...
) considering the cutoff points for education for southern Brazil (Kochhann et al., 2010Kochhann, R., Varela, J. S., Lisboa, C. S. M., & Chaves, M. L. F. (2010). The Mini Mental State Examination: Review of cutoff points adjusted for schooling in a large Southern Brazilian sample. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 4(1), 35-41. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642010DN40100006
https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642010DN...
): ≥ 21 for illiterates, ≥ 22 for low education, ≥ 23 for intermediate education and ≥ 24 for high education. Thus, the final sample included 345 older adults.

Regarding years of age and education, the means were 74.51 (SD = 9.14, ranging between 60 and 104 years) and 7.85 (SD = 5.90, ranging between 0 and 27 years), respectively. The sample consisted of 105 men (30%) and 240 women (70%), of which 301 (87%) were retired. The written informed consent was obtained from all participants through the Informed Consent Form, which was individually assessed in a single session of approximately 60 minutes. The research received approval from the Research Ethics Committee removed by the IMED Ethics Committee (CAAE: 73088917.5.0000.5319).

Instruments

Sociodemographic and clinical questionnaire. Composed of questions regarding age, education, gender, current occupation, history of neurological or psychiatric diseases, and medication use.

Dysexecutive Questionnaire - DEX (adapted byMacuglia et al., 2016Macuglia, G. R., Almeida, R. M. M., Santos, F. C., & Giacomoni, C. H. (2016). Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS): Adaptação e evidências de validade. Psico-USF, 21(2), 219-231. https://doi.org/10.1590/1413-82712016210201
https://doi.org/10.1590/1413-82712016210...
). DEX is a questionnaire answered through self-report or from an informant, consisting of 20 items that investigate the frequency of behaviors associated with dysexecutive problems present in daily activities. In this study, the self-report version was used. Items are answered using a five-point Likert scale ranging from “never” (0 points) to “almost always” (4 points). Thus, the minimum scale score is 0 and the maximum is 80 points, and higher scores suggest a greater occurrence of executive alterations. In its Brazilian version, DEX presented evidence of content validity from the analysis of expert judges (> .80), with a mean Kappa index of .75 (Macuglia et al., 2016Macuglia, G. R., Almeida, R. M. M., Santos, F. C., & Giacomoni, C. H. (2016). Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS): Adaptação e evidências de validade. Psico-USF, 21(2), 219-231. https://doi.org/10.1590/1413-82712016210201
https://doi.org/10.1590/1413-82712016210...
).

Phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks (Strauss et al., 2006Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests. Oxford University Press.). Verbal fluency tasks are brief measures of cognitive flexibility and inhibition. In the phonemic modality (FAS), participants had 60 seconds to evoke the greatest number of words that started with the letters, F, A, and S. In the semantic modality (Animals), types of animals should be evoked, also in 60 seconds. The score considered for both modalities is the total number of words evoked correctly. In Brazil, the instrument presents evidence of validity for older adults of different age groups and educational levels (Esteves et al., 2015Esteves, C. S., Oliveira, C. R., Moret-Tatay, C., Navarro-Pardo, E., Carli, G. A., Silva, I. G., Irigaray, T. Q., & Argimon, I. I. L. (2015). Phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks: Normative data for elderly Brazilians. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 28(2), 350-355. https://doi.org/10.1590/1678-7153.201528215
https://doi.org/10.1590/1678-7153.201528...
).

Data analysis

Descriptive analysis for variables related to the sociodemographic characteristics of participants (mean, standard deviation, and percentages), data normality, Pearson correlation between the total score and DEX factors with age, education, MMSE, and verbal fluency tasks. The alpha coefficient calculation was performed using SPSS software version 23 for Windows. The interpretation of the strength of associations in the Pearson correlation was based on the classification of Rosenthal (1996Rosenthal, J. A. (1996). Qualitative descriptors of strength of association and effect size. Journal of Social Service Research, 21(4), 37-59. https://doi.org/10.1300/j079v21n04_02
https://doi.org/10.1300/j079v21n04_02...
): weak ≤ .10, moderate ≥ .30, or strong ≥ .50. The EFA was conducted based on the generation of a polychoric correlation matrix, which was performed with the Factor software (v.10.02; Lorenzo-Seva & Ferrando, 2006Lorenzo-Seva, U., Fernando, P. J. (2006). FACTOR: A computer program to fit the exploratory factor analysis model. Behavior Research Methods, 38(1), 88-91. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192753
https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192753...
).

The procedure adopted to determine the number of factors was Parallel Analysis with random permutation of the observed data (Timmerman, & Lorenzo-Seva, 2011Timmerman, M. E., & Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2011). Dimensionality assessment of ordered polytomous items with parallel analysis. Psychological Methods, 16(2), 209-220. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023353
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023353...
), Robust Diagonally Weighted Least Squares method (RDWLS, Asparohov & Muthen, 2010), and Robust Promin rotation (Lorenzo-Seva & Ferrando, 2019Lorenzo-Seva, U., & Ferrando, P. J. (2019). Robust Promin: A method for diagonally weighted factor rotation. Liberabit, 25(1), 99-106. http://dx.doi.org/10.24265/liberabit.2019.v25n1.08
http://dx.doi.org/10.24265/liberabit.201...
). Unidimensional Congruence (UniCo) and Explained Common Variance (ECV) were used as complementary analyzes in order to verify the occurrence of unidimensionality in the instrument. The reference values to indicate unidimensionality were > .95 for UniCo, > .85 and for ECV (Ferrando & Lorenzo-Seva, 2018Ferrando, P. J., & Lorenzo-Seva U. (2018). Assessing the quality and appropriateness of factor solutions and factor score estimates in exploratory item factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 78(5), 762-780. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308...
).

As a complement to the analyses, the stability of the factors was assessed using the H index (Ferrando & Lorenzo-Seva, 2018Ferrando, P. J., & Lorenzo-Seva U. (2018). Assessing the quality and appropriateness of factor solutions and factor score estimates in exploratory item factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 78(5), 762-780. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308...
), which verified how well the factor is represented by the items, consisting of the H-Latent indices (capacity of the factor to be identified by the latent variable) and H-Observed (expected replicability for future studies). The H index values range from 0 to 1, with values above .80 suggesting a well-defined latent variable and greater stability between studies (Hancock & Mueller, 2001Hancock, G. R., & Mueller, R. O. (2001). Rethinking construct reliability within latent variable systems. In R. Cudek, S. Du Toit, D. Sorbum, & K. G. Joreskog (Eds.), Factor analysis and structural equation modeling: Perspectives and vision; Structural equation modeling present and future a festschrift in honor of Karl Joreskog (pp. 195-216). Scientific Software International. https://www.tib.eu/en/search/id/BLCP:CN039007976/Rethinking-construct-reliability-within-latent?cHash=78afdf4ff256545a5a2a507e3920cea4
https://www.tib.eu/en/search/id/BLCP:CN0...
).

In addition, the quality and effectiveness analysis of the factor estimation was verified by Bayes Expected a Posteriori (EAP, expected values > .80), Factor Determinacy Index (FDI, expected values > .90), Sensitivity Ratio (SR, expected values > 2.00) and Expected Percentage of True Differences (EPTD, expected values > 90%) (Ferrando & Lorenzo-Seva, 2018Ferrando, P. J., & Lorenzo-Seva U. (2018). Assessing the quality and appropriateness of factor solutions and factor score estimates in exploratory item factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 78(5), 762-780. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308...
). Composite reliability calculation was based on the formula proposed by Raykov (1997Raykov, T. (1997). Estimation of composite reliability for congeneric measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 21(2), 173-184. doi: 10.1177/01466216970212006).

Results

The average obtained in the MMSE was 27.25 points (SD = 2.24, ranging between 21 and 30 points), while in FAS was 27.49 words (SD = 14.24, ranging between 3 and 77 words) and in Animals was 13.42 words (SD = 5.33, ranging between 3 and 29 words). Bartlett’s sphericity tests (2217.6, gl = 190, p < .001) and KMO (.80) suggested interpretability of the correlation matrix of the items. The Parallel Analysis (Table 1), according to the RDWLS extraction method, indicated two factors as being the most representative for the data from the recommendation based on the 95th percentile for explained variance, which is a more accurate parameter than the recommendation based on the average (Timmerman & Lorenzo-Seva, 2011Timmerman, M. E., & Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2011). Dimensionality assessment of ordered polytomous items with parallel analysis. Psychological Methods, 16(2), 209-220. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023353
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023353...
). The explained variance, considering the two factors, was approximately 38%. The UniCo (.840) and ECV (.743) indices did not suggest the unidimensionality of the instrument.

Table 1
Factors Extracted in the Parallel Analysis by the RDWLS Method (n = 345)

The means, standard deviation, factor loadings of items, quality estimates and measures of internal consistency of the DEX are presented in Table 2, as well as estimates of replicability of the factor scores (H index; Ferrando & Lorenzo-Seva, 2018Ferrando, P. J., & Lorenzo-Seva U. (2018). Assessing the quality and appropriateness of factor solutions and factor score estimates in exploratory item factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 78(5), 762-780. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308...
). The analysis of the quality and effectiveness of estimating factor scores was considered adequate based on the EAP indexes (Factor 1 = .82 and Factor 2 = .86), FDI (Factor 1 = .90; Factor 2 = .93), SR (Factor 1 = 2.10 and Factor 2 = 2.51) and EPTD (Factor 1 = 90% and Factor 2 = 91%). Factor 1 was composed of six items (5, 6, 8, 10, 15, and 16), while Factor 2 was composed of nine items (2, 4, 7, 9, 12, 13, 14, 19, and 20). Items 1, 3, 11, 17, and 18 did not show factor loadings above 0.40 for any of the two factors.

Table 2
DEX Descriptive Data and Factor Structure (n = 345)

In general, the items had high factor loadings, with no pattern of cross-loadings being found (for example, items with factor loadings above .40 in more than one factor). The composite reliability of the factors was also adequate (> .70). Analysis of the H index, a measure of replicability of the factor structure, suggested that Factor 1 might not be replicable in future studies (H < .80). The DEX showed satisfactory internal consistency considering the Composite Reliability values (> .70), which is a reliability measure that considers the contribution of the factor loadings of each item (Table 2).

The data regarding the correlation between the factors and total DEX score with sociodemographic variables, MMSE, PVFT, and SVFT are presented in Table 3. Factor 1 was negatively associated (weak intensity) with education, MMSE, and verbal fluency tasks. Factor 2 was negatively associated (weak intensity) with education and verbal fluency tasks, in addition to positively (moderate intensity) with age. The DEX total score was negatively associated with the MMSE and positively associated with age, both with weak intensity.

Table 3
Correlation Coefficients between DEX, Age, Education, MMSE, PVFT, and SVFT (n = 345)

Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the psychometric properties of DEX in a sample of neurologically preserved older adults. In this study, a two-factor solution was found, which explained 38% of the variance, in addition to reliability indices between the factors that ranged from .74 to .83 (Composite reliability) and .66 to .71 (alpha coefficient). The first factor, consisting of six items, was called “Inhibition” and included questions related to euphoria, temporal sequencing problems, apathy, variable motivation, agitation, and response inhibition. The second factor, consisting of nine items, was called “Social Regulation and Planning”, presenting issues related to planning, lack of insight, aggression, lack of concern, perseveration, decision-making, no concern for social rules, and social awareness

The observed two-factor solution differs from that identified in the original study by Burguess et al. (1998), which obtained five factors in a sample of adults. It also differs from other studies with the self-report version of the instrument, which identified between one and six factors with varied samples (Bodenburg & Dopslaff, 2008Bodenburg, S., & Dopslaff, N. (2008). The Dysexecutive Questionnaire advanced: Item and test score characteristics, 4-factor solution, and severity classification. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 196(1), 75-78. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e31815faa2b
https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e31815f...
; Hellebrekers et al., 2017Hellebrekers, D., Winkens, I., Kruiper, S., & Van Heugten, C. (2017). Psychometric properties of the awareness questionnaire, patient competency rating scale and Dysexecutive Questionnaire in patients with acquired brain injury. Brain Injury, 31(11), 1469-1478. http://doi.org/10.1080/02699052.2017.1377350.
http://doi.org/10.1080/02699052.2017.137...
; Mooney et al., 2006Mooney, B., Walmsley, C., & McFarland, K. (2006). Factor analysis of the self-report Dysexecutive (DEX-S) Questionnaire. Applied Neuropsychology, 13(1), 12-18. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1301_2
https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1301_...
; Pedrero-Pérez et al., 2009Pedrero-Pérez, E. J., Ruiz-Sánchez-de-León, J. M., Rojo-Mota, G., Llanero-Luque, M., Olivar-Arroyo, A., Bouso-Saiz, J. C., & Puerta-García, C. (2009). Versión española del cuestionario disejecutivo (DEX-Sp): Propiedades psicométricas en adictos y población no clínica. Adicciones, 21(2), 155-166. https://doi.org/10.20882/adicciones.243
https://doi.org/10.20882/adicciones.243...
; Simblett & Bateman, 2011Simblett, S. K., & Bateman, A. (2011). Dimensions of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) examined using Rasch analysis. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 21(1), 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2010.531216
https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2010.53...
; Takeuchi et al., 2013Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y, Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. (2013). Brain structures associated with executive functions during everyday events in a non-clinical sample. Brain Structure and Function, 218(4), 1017-1032. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0444-z
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0444-...
; Wilson et al., 2003Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. J. (2003). Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS). Journal of Occupational Psychology, Employment and Disability, 5(2), 33-37. https://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20121105170305/http://www.dwp.gov.uk/docs/no2-sum-03-test-review-2.pdf
https://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov....
; Yang et al., 2018Yang, Z. Y., Sun, S. F., Lui, S. S. Y., Shi, H. S., Xie, D. J., Xie, W. L., Wang, Y., Cheung, E. F. C., Shum, D. H. K., & Chan, R. C. K. (2018). An attempt at revisiting the factor structure of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire in the Chinese setting. PsyCh Journal, 7(1), 25-30. https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.198
https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.198...
). However, the findings corroborate the model identified by Pedrero-Pérez et al. (2011Pedrero-Pérez, E. J., Ruiz-Sánchez-de-León, J. M., Lozoya-Delgado, P., Llanero-Luque, M., Rojo-Mota, G., & Puerta-García, C. (2011). Evaluación de los síntomas prefrontales: Propiedades psicométricas y datos normativos del cuestionario disejecutivo (DEX) en uma muestra de población española. Revista de Neurología, 52(7), 394-404. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5207.2010731
https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5207.2010731...
), in which two factors were also identified: planning (disorganization/apathy) and ability to inhibit inappropriate behavior (disinhibition/impulsivity).

Specifically in older adults, dissonant results are also observed when comparing data from Amieva et al. (2003Amieva, H., Phillips, L., & Della Sala, S. (2003). Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain and Cognition, 53(2), 129-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00094-0
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00...
) and Shinagawa et al. (2007Shinagawa, Y., Nakaaki, S., Hongo, J., Murata, Y., Sato, J., Matsui, T., Tatsumi, H., Akechi, T., & Furukawa, T. A. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) in Alzheimer’s disease: Validation of a behavioral rating scale to assess dysexecutive symptoms in Japanese patients with Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 951-956. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768
https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768...
), who presented, respectively, solutions of five (interference management, intentionality, inhibition, planning, and social regulation) and three (apathy, planning and monitoring, and hyperactivity) factors from PCA. There is a possibility that the differences between the results are due to the sample size used between the surveys. The study by Amieva et al. (2003Amieva, H., Phillips, L., & Della Sala, S. (2003). Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain and Cognition, 53(2), 129-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00094-0
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00...
) included only 20 neurologically preserved elderly adults and that of Shinagawa et al. (2007Shinagawa, Y., Nakaaki, S., Hongo, J., Murata, Y., Sato, J., Matsui, T., Tatsumi, H., Akechi, T., & Furukawa, T. A. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) in Alzheimer’s disease: Validation of a behavioral rating scale to assess dysexecutive symptoms in Japanese patients with Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 951-956. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768
https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768...
) included 122 older adults with Alzheimer’s Disease. The adoption of a sample with less than 10 participants per item of the evaluated instrument contradicts literature indication for psychometric or inferential analyzes (Pasquali, 2010Pasquali, L. (2010) Instrumentação psicológica - fundamentos e práticas. Artmed.; Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6ª ed). Pearson.). These characteristics, in addition to the factor extraction technique used, may have influenced the high value of explained variance between these studies, which ranged from 65% to 76%. Furthermore, Amieva et al. (2003Amieva, H., Phillips, L., & Della Sala, S. (2003). Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain and Cognition, 53(2), 129-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00094-0
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00...
) used the self-report version and Shinagawa et al. (2007Shinagawa, Y., Nakaaki, S., Hongo, J., Murata, Y., Sato, J., Matsui, T., Tatsumi, H., Akechi, T., & Furukawa, T. A. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) in Alzheimer’s disease: Validation of a behavioral rating scale to assess dysexecutive symptoms in Japanese patients with Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 951-956. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768
https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768...
) the informant’s version.

Although in this sample DEX presented two factors, these resemble some of the original factors proposed by Burgess et al. (1998Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie, H., & Wilson, B. A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 547-558. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798466037
https://doi.org/10.1017/s135561779846603...
), which indicates the usefulness of DEX as an instrument for the assessment of executive dysfunctions in older adults. Furthermore, Pedrero-Pérez et al. (2011Pedrero-Pérez, E. J., Ruiz-Sánchez-de-León, J. M., Lozoya-Delgado, P., Llanero-Luque, M., Rojo-Mota, G., & Puerta-García, C. (2011). Evaluación de los síntomas prefrontales: Propiedades psicométricas y datos normativos del cuestionario disejecutivo (DEX) en uma muestra de población española. Revista de Neurología, 52(7), 394-404. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5207.2010731
https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5207.2010731...
) demonstrated that executive control varies throughout the aging process. Thus, extreme ages, such as children (Fish & Wilson, 2021Fish, J., & Wilson, F. C. (2021). Assessing children’s executive function: BADS-C validity. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 1-11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.626291
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.62629...
) and older adults (Shinagawa et al., 2007Shinagawa, Y., Nakaaki, S., Hongo, J., Murata, Y., Sato, J., Matsui, T., Tatsumi, H., Akechi, T., & Furukawa, T. A. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) in Alzheimer’s disease: Validation of a behavioral rating scale to assess dysexecutive symptoms in Japanese patients with Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 951-956. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768
https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768...
), may present different groupings of factors due to changes in this construct throughout the life cycle.

Correlation analyzes between DEX factors, sociodemographic characteristics, and cognitive measures suggested that higher Inhibition Factor scores were associated with less education time, lower overall cognitive ability (MMSE), and lower cognitive fluency/flexibility (PVFT and SVFT). While higher results in Social Regulation and Planning showed an association with lower education level and cognitive fluency/flexibility, in addition to older age. These data are in line with the literature, identifying different executive components present changes in the aging process (Hirsiger et al., 2017Hirsiger, S., Koppelmans, V., Mérillat, S., Erdin, C., Narkhede, A., Brickman, A. M., & Jäncke, L. (2017). Executive functions in healthy older adults are differentially related to macro- and microstructural white matter characteristics of the cerebral lobes. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00373
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00373...
; Turner & Spreng, 2012Turner, G. R., & Spreng, R. N. (2012). Executive functions and neurocognitive aging: Dissociable patterns of brain activity. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(4), 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging...
), susceptible to variables such as years of education (Azouve et al., 2015; Farina et al., 2018Farina, M., Paloski, L. H., Oliveira, C. R., Argimon, I. I. L., & Irigaray, T. Q. (2018). Cognitive reserve in elderly and its connection with cognitive performance: A systematic review. Ageing International, 43(4), 496-507. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12126-017-9295-5
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12126-017-9295-...
; Thow et al., 2017Thow, M. E., Summers, M. J., Saunders, N. L., Summers, J. J., Ritchie, K., & Vickers, J. C. (2017). Further education improves cognitive reserve and triggers improvement in selective cognitive functions in older adults: The Tasmanian Healthy Brain Project. Alzheimer’s & Dementia (Amsterdam, Netherlands), 10, 22-30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2017.08.004
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2017.08.0...
). The association between DEX, MMSE, and fluency tasks demonstrates evidence of convergent validity between scores of screening instruments for assessing general cognitive aspects, and executive functions and subjective complaints scale, indicating its usefulness for assessing aspects that impact functional capacity to perform daily activities.

Among the limitations, the non-inclusion of other gold standard instruments for executive functions is highlighted, for example, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and the Five Digit Test, in order to verify other validity evidence based on external variables. For future studies, factor analyzes conducted with different populations simultaneously are suggested, in order to test the impact of the external characteristics of the sample and to conduct comparisons between the self-report version and the informant version. Furthermore, it is possible to perform investigations of convergent validity between DEX and emotional and personality aspects, since executive difficulties may be present in neuropsychiatric conditions.

In general, the results of this study indicated that DEX had a satisfactory factor structure for older adults, in addition to its score being associated with formal measures of executive functions and cognition in general, suggesting clinical applicability. As far as known, this is the first study with a significant sample of older adults to conduct EFA, using statistical techniques more appropriate for the type of response scale of the instrument. Thus, DEX was an appropriate tool for the Brazilian context for the cognitive assessment that complements instruments of executive function performance.

References

  • Amanzio, M., Bartoli, M., Cipriani, G. E., & Palermo, S. (2020). Executive dysfunction and reduced self-awareness in patients with neurological disorders. A mini-review. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1-8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01697
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01697
  • Amieva, H., Phillips, L., & Della Sala, S. (2003). Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain and Cognition, 53(2), 129-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00094-0
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00094-0
  • Asparouhov, T., & Muthen, B. (2010). Simple second order chi-square correction. https://www.statmodel.com/download/WLSMV_new_chi21.pdf
    » https://www.statmodel.com/download/WLSMV_new_chi21.pdf
  • Azouvi, P., Vallat-Azouvi, C., Millox, V., Darnoux, E., Ghout, I., Azerad, S., Ruet, A., Bayen, E., Pradat-Diehl, P., Aegerter, P., Weiss, J. J., & Jourdan, C. (2015). Ecological validity of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire: Results from the PariS-TBI study. Neuropsychol Rehability, 25(6), 864 -878. http://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2014.990907
    » http://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2014.990907
  • Baez, S., Pinasco, C., Roca, M., Ferrari, J., Couto, B., García-Cordero, I., Ibañez, A., Cruz, F., Reyes, P., Matallana, D., Manes, F., Cetcovich, M., & Torralva, T. (2017). Brain structural correlates of executive and social cognition profiles in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and elderly bipolar disorder. Neuropsychologia, 126, 159-169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.012
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.012
  • Bodenburg, S., & Dopslaff, N. (2008). The Dysexecutive Questionnaire advanced: Item and test score characteristics, 4-factor solution, and severity classification. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 196(1), 75-78. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e31815faa2b
    » https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e31815faa2b
  • Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie, H., & Wilson, B. A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 547-558. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798466037
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617798466037
  • Chan, R. C. K. (2001). Dysexecutive symptoms among a non-clinical sample: A study with the use of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire. British Journal of Psychology, 92(Part 3), 551-565. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712601162338
    » https://doi.org/10.1348/000712601162338
  • Chaves, M. L, & Izquierdo, I. (1992). Differential diagnosis between dementia and depression: A study of efficiency increment. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 85(6), 378-382. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.1992.tb06032.x
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0404.1992.tb06032.x
  • Chaytor, N., & Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2007). Fractionation of the dysexecutive syndrome in a heterogeneous neurological sample: Comparing the Dysexecutive Questionnaire and the Brock Adaptive Functioning Questionnaire. Brain Injury, 21(6), 615-621. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050701426949
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/02699050701426949
  • Emmanouel, A., Mouza, E., Kessels, R. P., & Fasotti, L. (2014). Validity of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX). Ratings by patients with brain injury and their therapists. Brain Injury, 28(12), :1581-1589. http://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2014.942371
    » http://doi.org/10.3109/02699052.2014.942371
  • Esteves, C. S., Oliveira, C. R., Moret-Tatay, C., Navarro-Pardo, E., Carli, G. A., Silva, I. G., Irigaray, T. Q., & Argimon, I. I. L. (2015). Phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tasks: Normative data for elderly Brazilians. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 28(2), 350-355. https://doi.org/10.1590/1678-7153.201528215
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/1678-7153.201528215
  • Farina, M., Paloski, L. H., Oliveira, C. R., Argimon, I. I. L., & Irigaray, T. Q. (2018). Cognitive reserve in elderly and its connection with cognitive performance: A systematic review. Ageing International, 43(4), 496-507. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12126-017-9295-5
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s12126-017-9295-5
  • Fellows, L. K. (2019). The functions of the frontal lobes: Evidence from patients with focal brain damage. In M. D’Esposito, & J. H. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology (pp. 19-34). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804281-6.00002-1
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804281-6.00002-1
  • Ferrando, P. J., & Lorenzo-Seva U. (2018). Assessing the quality and appropriateness of factor solutions and factor score estimates in exploratory item factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 78(5), 762-780. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308
  • Fish, J., & Wilson, F. C. (2021). Assessing children’s executive function: BADS-C validity. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 1-11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.626291
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.626291
  • Guarino, A., Favieri, F., Boncompagni, I., Agostini, F., Cantone, M., & Casagrande, M. (2019). Executive functions in Alzheimer Disease: A systematic review. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 10, 1-24. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00437
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2018.00437
  • Hancock, G. R., & Mueller, R. O. (2001). Rethinking construct reliability within latent variable systems. In R. Cudek, S. Du Toit, D. Sorbum, & K. G. Joreskog (Eds.), Factor analysis and structural equation modeling: Perspectives and vision; Structural equation modeling present and future a festschrift in honor of Karl Joreskog (pp. 195-216). Scientific Software International. https://www.tib.eu/en/search/id/BLCP:CN039007976/Rethinking-construct-reliability-within-latent?cHash=78afdf4ff256545a5a2a507e3920cea4
    » https://www.tib.eu/en/search/id/BLCP:CN039007976/Rethinking-construct-reliability-within-latent?cHash=78afdf4ff256545a5a2a507e3920cea4
  • Hellebrekers, D., Winkens, I., Kruiper, S., & Van Heugten, C. (2017). Psychometric properties of the awareness questionnaire, patient competency rating scale and Dysexecutive Questionnaire in patients with acquired brain injury. Brain Injury, 31(11), 1469-1478. http://doi.org/10.1080/02699052.2017.1377350
    » http://doi.org/10.1080/02699052.2017.1377350
  • Hirsiger, S., Koppelmans, V., Mérillat, S., Erdin, C., Narkhede, A., Brickman, A. M., & Jäncke, L. (2017). Executive functions in healthy older adults are differentially related to macro- and microstructural white matter characteristics of the cerebral lobes. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00373
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00373
  • Junquera, A., García-Zamora, E., Olazarán, J., Parra, M. A., & Fernández-Guinea, S. (2020). Role of executive functions in the conversion from mild cognitive impairment to dementia. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 77(2), 641-653. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-200586
    » https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-200586
  • Kochhann, R., Varela, J. S., Lisboa, C. S. M., & Chaves, M. L. F. (2010). The Mini Mental State Examination: Review of cutoff points adjusted for schooling in a large Southern Brazilian sample. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 4(1), 35-41. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642010DN40100006
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642010DN40100006
  • Lee, S. J., Kim, H., Kim, L. S., Kim, J. H., & Park, K. W. (2019). Effects of frontal-executive dysfunction on self-perceived hearing handicap in the elderly with mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One, 14(3), e0210014. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210014
    » https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210014
  • Lorenzo-Seva, U., Fernando, P. J. (2006). FACTOR: A computer program to fit the exploratory factor analysis model. Behavior Research Methods, 38(1), 88-91. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192753
    » https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192753
  • Lorenzo-Seva, U., & Ferrando, P. J. (2019). Robust Promin: A method for diagonally weighted factor rotation. Liberabit, 25(1), 99-106. http://dx.doi.org/10.24265/liberabit.2019.v25n1.08
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.24265/liberabit.2019.v25n1.08
  • Luna-Lario, P., Seijas-Gómez, R., Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., Hernáez-Goñi, P., & Mata-Pastor, I. (2012). Estructura factorial del cuestionario disejecutivo en una muestra de población española con daño cerebral adquirido y quejas de déficit de memoria. Revista de Neurología, 55(11), 641-650. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5511.2012549
    » https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5511.2012549
  • Macuglia, G. R., Almeida, R. M. M., Santos, F. C., & Giacomoni, C. H. (2016). Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS): Adaptação e evidências de validade. Psico-USF, 21(2), 219-231. https://doi.org/10.1590/1413-82712016210201
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/1413-82712016210201
  • Mooney, B., Walmsley, C., & McFarland, K. (2006). Factor analysis of the self-report Dysexecutive (DEX-S) Questionnaire. Applied Neuropsychology, 13(1), 12-18. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1301_2
    » https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1301_2
  • Ozga, J. E., Povroznik, J. M., Engler-Chiurazzi, E. B., & Vonder Haar, C. (2018). Executive (dys)function after traumatic brain injury: Special considerations for behavioral pharmacology. Behavioural Pharmacology, 29(7), 617-637. https://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0000000000000430
    » https://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0000000000000430
  • Pasquali, L. (2010) Instrumentação psicológica - fundamentos e práticas. Artmed.
  • Pedrero-Pérez, E. J., Ruiz-Sánchez-de-León, J. M., Lozoya-Delgado, P., Llanero-Luque, M., Rojo-Mota, G., & Puerta-García, C. (2011). Evaluación de los síntomas prefrontales: Propiedades psicométricas y datos normativos del cuestionario disejecutivo (DEX) en uma muestra de población española. Revista de Neurología, 52(7), 394-404. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5207.2010731
    » https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5207.2010731
  • Pedrero-Pérez, E. J., Ruiz-Sánchez-de-León, J. M., Rojo-Mota, G., Llanero-Luque, M., Olivar-Arroyo, A., Bouso-Saiz, J. C., & Puerta-García, C. (2009). Versión española del cuestionario disejecutivo (DEX-Sp): Propiedades psicométricas en adictos y población no clínica. Adicciones, 21(2), 155-166. https://doi.org/10.20882/adicciones.243
    » https://doi.org/10.20882/adicciones.243
  • Pedrero-Pérez, E. J., Ruiz-Sánchez-de-León, J. M., & Winpenny-Tejedor, C. (2015). Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX): Unrestricted structural analysis in large clinical and non-clinical samples. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 25(6), 879-894. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2014.993659
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2014.993659
  • Povroznik, J. M., Ozga, J. E., Vonder Haar, C., & Engler-Chiurazzi, E. B. (2018). Executive (dys)function after stroke: Special considerations for behavioral pharmacology. Behavioural Pharmacology, 29(7), 638-653. https://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0000000000000432
    » https://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0000000000000432
  • Raykov, T. (1997). Estimation of composite reliability for congeneric measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 21(2), 173-184. doi: 10.1177/01466216970212006
  • Rosenthal, J. A. (1996). Qualitative descriptors of strength of association and effect size. Journal of Social Service Research, 21(4), 37-59. https://doi.org/10.1300/j079v21n04_02
    » https://doi.org/10.1300/j079v21n04_02
  • Shinagawa, Y., Nakaaki, S., Hongo, J., Murata, Y., Sato, J., Matsui, T., Tatsumi, H., Akechi, T., & Furukawa, T. A. (2007). Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) in Alzheimer’s disease: Validation of a behavioral rating scale to assess dysexecutive symptoms in Japanese patients with Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22(10), 951-956. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1768
  • Simblett, S. K., & Bateman, A. (2011). Dimensions of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) examined using Rasch analysis. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 21(1), 1-25. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2010.531216
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2010.531216
  • Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests. Oxford University Press.
  • Stuss, D. T., & Knight, R. T. (2002). Principles of frontal lobe function. Oxford University Press.
  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6ª ed). Pearson.
  • Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y, Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., & Kawashima, R. (2013). Brain structures associated with executive functions during everyday events in a non-clinical sample. Brain Structure and Function, 218(4), 1017-1032. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0444-z
    » https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0444-z
  • Thow, M. E., Summers, M. J., Saunders, N. L., Summers, J. J., Ritchie, K., & Vickers, J. C. (2017). Further education improves cognitive reserve and triggers improvement in selective cognitive functions in older adults: The Tasmanian Healthy Brain Project. Alzheimer’s & Dementia (Amsterdam, Netherlands), 10, 22-30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2017.08.004
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2017.08.004
  • Timmerman, M. E., & Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2011). Dimensionality assessment of ordered polytomous items with parallel analysis. Psychological Methods, 16(2), 209-220. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023353
    » https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023353
  • Turner, G. R., & Spreng, R. N. (2012). Executive functions and neurocognitive aging: Dissociable patterns of brain activity. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(4), 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.005
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.005
  • Veldsman, M., Werden, E., Egorova, N., Khlif, M. S., & Brodtmann, A. (2020). Microstructural degeneration and cerebrovascular risk burden underlying executive dysfunction after stroke. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75074-w
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-75074-w
  • Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. J. (1996). Behavioral Assessment of Dysexecutive Syndrome. Thames Valley Test Company.
  • Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H., & Evans, J. J. (2003). Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS). Journal of Occupational Psychology, Employment and Disability, 5(2), 33-37. https://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20121105170305/http://www.dwp.gov.uk/docs/no2-sum-03-test-review-2.pdf
    » https://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20121105170305/http://www.dwp.gov.uk/docs/no2-sum-03-test-review-2.pdf
  • Wood, R. L., & Worthington, A. (2017). Neurobehavioral abnormalities associated with executive dysfunction after traumatic brain injury. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00195
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00195
  • Yang, Z. Y., Sun, S. F., Lui, S. S. Y., Shi, H. S., Xie, D. J., Xie, W. L., Wang, Y., Cheung, E. F. C., Shum, D. H. K., & Chan, R. C. K. (2018). An attempt at revisiting the factor structure of the Dysexecutive Questionnaire in the Chinese setting. PsyCh Journal, 7(1), 25-30. https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.198
    » https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.198

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    22 Apr 2022
  • Date of issue
    Dec 2021

History

  • Received
    31 May 2021
  • Reviewed
    13 Aug 2021
  • Accepted
    13 Aug 2021
Universidade de São Francisco, Programa de Pós-Graduação Stricto Sensu em Psicologia R. Waldemar César da Silveira, 105, Vl. Cura D'Ars (SWIFT), Campinas - São Paulo, CEP 13045-510, Telefone: (19)3779-3771 - Campinas - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistapsico@usf.edu.br