Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

HIGHLY INTELLIGENT CHILDREN WITH DIFFICULTY IN ARITHMETIC: AN EFFECT OF MATH ANXIETY?

ABSTRACT

Mathematical anxiety (MA) is a set of beliefs, physical and emotional symptoms that some people experience when dealing with mathematical problems. The study aimed to investigate the effect of MA on the arithmetic performance of children with high intelligence. Fifty-two children with lower performance on the STP arithmetic subtest and classification “above average” or “intellectually superior” in the Raven Matrices participated in the study. The results showed a strong correlation between intelligence and arithmetic EDT (r = 0.82; p < 0.000); and moderate correlations between arithmetic DET and MAQ (r = 0.32; p = 0.02) and MAQ-B (r = 0.35; p = 0.01). Multiple linear regression analysis showed an 8% prediction of the MAQ-A subscale and 11% of the MAQ-B as predictors of arithmetic performance. Mathematical anxiety interferes with arithmetic performance, even in children with higher than expected fluid intelligence, suggesting an important effect of emotional mechanisms.

Keywords:
fluid intelligence; mathematical anxiety; arithmetic

RESUMO

Ansiedade matemática (AM) é um conjunto de crenças, sintomas físicos e emocionais que algumas pessoas experimentam ao lidar com problemas matemáticos. O estudo objetivou investigar o efeito da AM no desempenho aritmético de crianças com alta inteligência. Participaram do estudo 52 crianças com desempenho inferior no subteste aritmética do TDE e classificação “acima da média” ou “intelectualmente superior” nas Matrizes de Raven. Os resultados demonstraram correlação forte entre inteligência e TDE aritmética (r = 0,82; p < 0,000); e correlações moderadas entre TDE aritmética e QAM-A (r = 0,32; p = 0,02) e QAM-B (r = 0,35; p = 0,01). A análise de regressão linear múltipla demonstrou uma predição de 8% da subescala QAM-A e de 11 % da QAM-B como preditores do desempenho aritmético. A ansiedade matemática interfere no desempenho em aritmética, mesmo em crianças com a inteligência fluída acima do esperado, sugerindo um importante efeito dos mecanismos emocionais.

Palavras-chave:
inteligência fluída; ansiedade matemática; aritmética

RESUMEN

Ansiedad matemática (AM) es un conjunto de creencias, síntomas físicos y emocionales que algunas personas experimentan al lidiar con problemas matemáticos. El estudio tuvo por objetivo investigar el efecto de la AM en el rendimiento aritmético de niños con alta inteligencia. Participaron del estudio 52 niños con rendimiento inferior en la subprueba aritmética del TDE y clasificación “superior de la media” o “intelectualmente superior” en las Matrices de Raven. Los resultados apuntan fuerte correlación entre inteligencia y TDE aritmética (r = 0,82; p < 0,000); y correlaciones moderadas entre TDE aritmética y QAM-A (r = 0,32; p = 0,02) y QAM-B (r = 0,35; p = 0,01). El análisis de regresión linear múltiple demostró una predicción del 8% de la subescala QAM-A y del 11 % de la QAM-B como predictores del rendimiento aritmético. La ansiedad matemática interfiere en el rendimiento en aritmética, incluso en niños con la inteligencia fluida superior al esperado, sugiriendo un importante efecto de los mecanismos emocionales.

Palabras clave:
inteligencia fluida; ansiedad matemática; aritmética

INTRODUCTION

School performance is the result of the adequate acquisition of educational skills. In its initial acts, the schooling process has ambitious goals related to the acquisition of reading, writing, and arithmetic (Soares, 2004Soares, M. (2004). Letramento e alfabetização: as muitas facetas. Revista Brasileira de Educação, 25, 5 - 17. https://doi.org/10.1590/s1413-24782004000100002
https://doi.org/10.1590/s1413-2478200400...
). Studies point out that difficulties in school education can be explained by cognitive alterations such as deficits in fluid intelligence (Porto, 2018Porto, S. B. (2018). Relações entre inteligência, criatividade e funções executivas: um estudo multicasos. Dissertação (Mestrado em Psicologia) - Centro de Ciências Humanas, Letras e Artes, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal. Recuperado de: https://repositorio.ufrn.br/jspui/handle/123456789/29149
https://repositorio.ufrn.br/jspui/handle...
), and deficits in operational memory (Peng et al., 2018Peng, P.; Fuchs, D.; Fuchs, L. S.; Elleman, A. M.; Kearns, D. M.; Gilbert, J. K.; Compton, D. L.; Cho, E.; Patton, S. (2018). A Longitudinal Analysis of the Trajectories and Predictors of Word Reading and Reading Comprehension Development Among At-Risk Readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 002221941880908. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219418809080
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219418809080...
).

According to the cognitive perspective, learning is the result of consolidation of information in memory in the long term. It depends on the availability of environmental stimuli and neurocognitive capabilities by individuals when it comes to handling such information. The process gets started with the gathering of information by means of organs of the senses, which is codified and decodified in a continuous process that involves levels that are increasingly complex (Eysenck & Keane, 2017Eysenck, M. W.; Keane, M. T. (2017). Manual de Psicologia Cognitiva. In Manual de Psicologia Cognitiva. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2014.04.017
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2014.04...
). This is the mechanism by means of which information is interpreted, organized, integrated, and stored before an answer is produced (Simões & Nogaro, 2018Simões, E. M. S.; Nogaro, A. (2018). Neurociência Cognitiva Para Educadores - Aprendizagem e Prática Docente no Século XXI. Curitiba: CRV. https://doi.org/10.24824/978854441017.2
https://doi.org/10.24824/978854441017.2...
).

Evidence shows that there is an effect of fluid intelligence in performance in mathematical skills (Colom, 2006Colom, R. (2006). O que é inteligência? In: Flores-Mendoza, C.; Colom, R. (Eds.), Introdução à psicologia das diferenças individuais (59-72). Porto Alegre: Artmed.; Ribeiro & Freitas, 2018Ribeiro, D. O.; Freitas, P. M. (2018). Inteligência e desempenho escolar em crianças entre 6 e 11 anos. Revista Psicologia Em Pesquisa, 12(1), 84-91. https://doi.org/10.24879/201800120010085
https://doi.org/10.24879/201800120010085...
), characterizing intelligence as an important predictor of educational performance (Roth et al., 2015Roth, B.; Becker, N.; Romeyke, S.; Schäfer, S.; Domnick, F.; Spinath, F. M. (2015). Intelligence and school grades: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 53, 118-137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.09...
). Fluid intelligence is linked to the capacity of individuals to realize tasks that involve reasoning, abstraction, planning, and the management of problems (Neubert, Mainert, Kretzschmar, & Greiff, 2015Neubert, J. C.; Mainert, J.; Kretzschmar, A.; Greiff, S. (2015). The Assessment of 21st Century Skills in Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Complex and Collaborative Problem Solving. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 8(2), 238-268. https://doi.org/10.1017/iop.2015.14
https://doi.org/10.1017/iop.2015.14...
). The elaboration of new strategies, processing speed, and the resolution of problems are necessary skills for the accomplishment of numerous math tasks (Luculano, Moro, & Butterworth, 2011Luculano, T., Moro, R.; Butterworth, B. (2011). Updating Working Memory and arithmetical attainment in school. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 655-661. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.12.002
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.12...
).

Another factor whose influence on performance in arithmetic has been tested is math anxiety (MA). Studies point at a negative correlation between mathematical development and mathematics anxiety (Cargnelutti, Tomasetto, & Passolunghi, 2017Cargnelutti, E.; Tomasetto C.; Passolunghi, M. C. (2017). How is anxiety related to math performance in young students? A longitudinal study of Grade 2 to Grade 3 children. Cognition And Emotion, 31(4), 755-764. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1147421
https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.11...
; Monteiro, Peixoto, Mata, & Sanches, 2017Monteiro, V.; Peixoto, F.; Mata, L.; Sanches, C. (2017). Mathematics: I don’t like it! I like it! Very much, a little, not at all... Social support and emotions in students from 2nd and 3rd cycles of education. Aná. Psicológica [online]. 35(3), 281-296. http://dx.doi.org/10.14417/ap.1156
https://doi.org/10.14417/ap.1156...
; Reali, Jiménez-Leal, Maldonado-Carreño, Devine, & Szücs, 2016Reali, F.; Jiménez-Leal, W.; Maldonado-Carreño, C.; Devine, A.; Szücs, D. (2016). Examining the link between math anxiety and math performance in Colombian students. Revista Colombiana de Psicologia, 25(2). https://doi.org/10.15446/rcp.v25n2.54532
https://doi.org/10.15446/rcp.v25n2.54532...
; Vukovic, Kieffer, Bailey, & Harari, 2013Vukovic, R. K.; Kieffer, M. J.; Bailey, S. P.; Harari, R. R. (2013). Mathematics anxiety in young children: Concurrent and longitudinal associations with mathematical performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.09.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012....
). MA is a term that is used for denominating a set of beliefs, physical and emotional symptoms that some children and adults experience regarding math problems both in the school environment and in everyday situations (Carey, Hill, Devine, & Szücs, 2016Carey, E.; Hill, F.; Devine, A.; Szücs, D. (2016). The Chicken or the Egg? The Direction of the Relationship Between Mathematics Anxiety and Mathematics Performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01987
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01987...
; Dowker, Sarkar, & Looi, 2016Dowker, A.; Sarkar, A.; Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics Anxiety: What Have We Learned in 60 Years? Frontiers in Psychology, 7(APR). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508...
). In this sense, the situation generally gets started during the first years of elementary school or even in child education and goes all the way as far as adulthood (Ashcraft, 2002Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math Anxiety: Personal, Educational, and Cognitive Consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 181-185. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00196
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00196...
), without any identification by teachers, caretakers, or even by the students themselves, which can be justified by the fact that studies on mathematical anxiety are quite recent.

MA comprehends beliefs of incapacity and failure, negative assessment on the subject and on performance itself; emotional reactions such as fear, anger, tension, and physiological symptoms like tachycardia, sweating, intestinal colic, and shivers. In order to avoid these sensations, people might develop behaviors of escape (skip class, be absent on test days, fail to participate in papers and activities) and of avoidance (doing homework hastily, being late for class). By doing that, she feels relieved, which is something that reinforces the avoidance pattern. On the other hand, when students do not experience doing these activities, they miss out on opportunities to practice and develop mathematical skills, which might lead to poorer educational performance, when compared to peers that do not suffer from math anxiety (Carmo, 2011Carmo, J. S. (2011). Ansiedade à matemática: identificação, descrição operacional e estratégias de intervenção. In F. Capovilla (Ed.), Transtornos de aprendizagem: progressos em avaliação e intervenção preventiva e remediativa (249-255). São Paulo: Memnon. ISBN. 978-85-7954-015-8; Haase et al., 2013Haase, V. G.; Lopes-Silva, J.; Starling-Alves, I.; Antunes, A.; Júlio-Costa, A.; Oliveira, L.; Pinheiro-Chagas, P.; Moura, R.; Wood, G. (2013). Com quantos bytes se reduz a ansiedade matemática? A inclusão digital como uma possível ferramenta na promoção do capital mental. In L. E. R. do Valle; Mattos, M. J. V. M.; Costa, J. W. (Eds.), Educação digital. A tecnologia a favor da inclusão (1st ed., Issue January, pp. 188-202). ARTMED. ISBN. 9788565848572).

Reactions to math anxiety can manifest themselves during or before the realization of activities that involve math and in common occasions in everyday life and work (Beilock & Maloney, 2015Beilock, S. L.; Maloney, E. A. (2015). Math Anxiety: A Factor in Math Achievement Not to Be Ignored. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2(1), 4 - 12. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215601438
https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215601438...
). The consequences of this problem include school evasion and a limitation in the number of professional choices for areas that do not involve mathematics (Suárez-Pellicioni, Núñez-Peña, & Colomé, 2016Suárez-Pellicioni, M.; Núñez-Peña, M. I.; Colomé, À. (2016). Math anxiety: A review of its cognitive consequences, psychophysiological correlates, and brain bases. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 16, 3-22. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0370-7
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0370-...
). Some research works investigate the relation between gender, the presence and the level of MA. Ashcraft (2002Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math Anxiety: Personal, Educational, and Cognitive Consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 181-185. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00196
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00196...
) demonstrated that MA is more frequent among females and added that such fact can be explained by the women’s tendency to speak more openly about their feelings. Dowker et al. (2016Dowker, A.; Sarkar, A.; Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics Anxiety: What Have We Learned in 60 Years? Frontiers in Psychology, 7(APR). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508...
) discuss the possibility that the increase in the levels of MA among women might be related to lesser exposure of women to mathematics because the social expectation is that females present poorer performance in mathematics when compared to males; exposure might also lead them to adapt to the existing expectations concerning their group. Some studies assess the impact of mathematics anxiety in executive functions, especially in work memory and attention components (Hartwright et al., 2018Hartwright, C. E.; Looi, C. Y.; Sella, F.; Inuggi, A.; Santos, F. H.; González-Salinas, C.; Santos, J. M. G.; Kadosh, R. C.; Fuentes, L. J. (2018). The Neurocognitive Architecture of Individual Differences in Math Anxiety in Typical Children. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-26912...
; Passolunghi, Caviola, Agostini, Perin, & Mammarella, 2016Passolunghi, M. C.; Caviola, S.; De Agostini, R.; Perin, C.; Mammarella, I. C. (2016). Mathematics anxiety, working memory, and mathematics performance in secondary-school children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(FEB), 1-8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00042
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00042...
; Soultanlou et al., 2019).

According to studies that have already been realized, it is possible to consider that MA produces effects that still need to be investigated in specific groups. The objective of the study was to investigate the influence of math anxiety among children that present high scores of fluid intelligence and still present poor performance in arithmetic. The central hypotheses is that MA has a predicting effect for arithmetic performance when fluid intelligence is preserved.

METHOD

The employed method was based on a cross-sectional, quantitative, quasi-experimental outline with a sample by convenience.

Participants

The databank that was the basis for the sample contained 412 children between the ages of 7 and 10 years (M = 8.41, SD= 1.15), boys (50%) and girls (50%) from public (70.6%) and from private (29.4%) schools in the city of Vitória da Conquista - BA. By means of this databank researchers selected two groups of children who corresponded to the inclusion criteria for this study. The criteria for Group 1 were: a) intelligence as high as or above 75 % (equivalent to classifications that are ‘definitely above average” and “intellectually superior”); and b) poor performance in arithmetic, where the only considered children were the ones with a performance in the STP arithmetic subtest that was inferior to what was expected for the school year. Criteria for Group 2 were: a) intelligence as high as or above 75 % (equivalent to classifications “definitely above average” and “intellectually superior”); and b) performance in arithmetic that was above expectancy for the school year in the STP arithmetic subtest. Thus, the number of participants in Group 1 was 52 children, while 31 (59.6%) were male and 21 (40.4%) were female, between the ages of 7 and 10 years (M = 8.65 years, SD= 1.10 years), out of which 41 (78.8%) came from public schools and 11 (21.2%) came from private schools. The number of participants of Group 2 was 28 children, while 16 (57.1%) were male and 12 (42.9%) were female, between the ages of 7 and 10 years (M = 7.61 years, SD= 0.79 years), out of which 10 (35.7%) came from public schools and 18 (64.3%) came from private schools.

Instruments

Arithmetic subtest of the Educational Performance Test (STP) (Stein, 1994Stein, L. M. (1994). TDE - Teste de Desempenho Escolar: manual para aplicação e interpretação. São Paulo, SP: Casa do Psicólogo.): this subtest consists of arithmetic calculations with increasing levels of difficulty, while three questions were oral and 15 were written operations. The subtest makes it possible to assess the educational performance of children from the 1st to the 6th in this area based on school year as well as on age. During the operation, the child is required to solve as many calculations as possible, according to what has already been learned in the classroom, while only calculations that have not been learned should be left unsolved. Finally, each right answer was marked with a punctuation period; then, the children’s performance is compared to the normative tables for age and school year and classified as “above”/”within”/”below” the expected result.

Raven’s Colored Matrices (Angelini, Alves, Custódio, Duarte, & Duarte, 1999Angelini, A. L.; Alves, I. C. B.; Custódio, E. M.; Duarte, W. F.; Duarte, J. L. M. (1999). Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas de Raven: Escala Especial. Manual. São Paulo: CETEPP.): an instrument that assesses non-verbal fluid intelligence by means of logical reasoning. It is a book that is divided into three series with 12 items each. Items/series follow an increasing order or difficulty. For each item, children are shown a matrix with a missing part and six alternatives for completion. Participants are required to choose the option that they believe follows the same pattern as the matrix in order to complete it correctly. By means of the Raven results, it is possible to identify the intellectual level of children from the age of 4 years and 9 months until 12.

The Math Anxiety Questionnaire (MAQ) (Wood et al., 2012Wood, G.; Pinheiro-Chagas, P.; Júlio-Costa, A.; Micheli, L. R.; Krinzinger, H.; Kaufmann, L.; Haase, V. G. (2012). Math anxiety questionnaire: Similar latent structure in Brazilian and German school children. Child Development Research, 1-10 https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/610192
https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/610192...
): is an instrument that assesses the level of math anxiety among children during their school education. It is made up of four base questions related to six subscales: general mathematics, easy calculations, difficult calculations, written calculations, mental calculations and math homework, adding up to a total sum of 24 items. The base questions assess the following categories: (MAQ-A) performance self-perception: “how good are you in…”; (MAQ-B) attitudes concerning math: “how do you like…”; (MAQ-C) unhappiness due to math problems: “how happy or unhappy do you feel when you have problems with…”; (MAQ-D) anxiety related to math problems: “how worried do you get when you have difficulties or problems with …”. For example, item 1 is “how good are you in general math?”. Each item is provided with a Likert scale going from 1 to 5 points, five illustrations that symbolize the intensity of the level of anxiety of students regarding the examination. The total sum of scores obtained by the children in the six items of each base question corresponds to the level of anxiety for each one of the categories.

Procedures

The research was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Bahia - Multi-disciplinarian Health Institute, under decision no. 3.082.420. The study proposal was presented in meetings realized at the schools. The parents that got interested signed the Free Informed Consent Term (TCLE), authorizing participation by their children. After authorization, the gathering of data was realized with children by means of individual sessions, with a duration between 30 and 50 minutes, with the signing of the Free Informed Consent Term (TALE) by the child.

Data analysis

After being collected, data were sent to a databank for statistical analysis. This analysis was made by means of the Statistical Package of Social Science (SPSS) software, version 25. In addition to the descriptive analysis (for the purpose of characterizing the sample), researchers used Student t and Mann-Whitney’s U tests for comparison between groups, as well as Pearson’s correlation test and the analysis of multiple linear regression (stepwise method).

RESULTS

The math anxiety of the children from Group 1 (high intelligence and poor arithmetic performance) was above average when compared to the standard norm, but this difference was not significant. Only category B (attitudes regarding math), the experimental group presented a level of anxiety below the general average (Table 1).

Table 1
Comparison of the level of math anxiety between standard norm children and children with poor performances in arithmetic.

When we compare the children from Group 1 to the children from Group 2 (high intelligence and high arithmetic performance), it was possible to observe that Group 1 presented less MA in the B categories (attitudes regarding math) and C (unhappiness due to math problems) of the MAQ, whereas group 2 presented less MA in the A categories (performance self-perception) and D (anxiety related to math problems). However, differences were not statistically significant (Table 2).

Table 2
Comparison of the level of math anxiety between children with low performance in arithmetic and children with high performance in arithmetic.

Regarding the gender difference, Table 3 shows that the girls from Group 1 presented higher levels of anxiety in all categories when compared to boys. The result for test 1 indicated that there are significant differences between boys and girls in category D (anxiety related to math problems) of the MAQ.

Table 3
Comparison of the level of math anxiety by gender and type of school.

The type of choice was also assessed with a comparison of the anxiety scores among children from public and private schools in Group 1. Table 3, the obtained results demonstrated that the children from private schools presented higher scores of math anxiety than the children from public schools, except for category D of the MAQ. No significant differences were found between the types of school.

A strong, significant correlation between intelligence and performance in arithmetic. Also, moderate correlations were found between the STP arithmetic subtest and the MAQ-A categories (performance self-perception) and MAQ-B (attitudes regarding math), according to Table 4.

Table 4
Correlation between intelligence, performance in arithmetic and math anxiety.

It is also possible to observe the A and B categories of the MAQ as predictors of arithmetic performance. The first model presents the MAQ-A (performance self-perception) predicting 8% of the arithmetic performance (Table 5).

Table 5
Regression analysis by the stepwise method for verifying the prediction of math anxiety (MAQ-A) on arithmetic performance.

The second model shows that the MAQ-B (attitude regarding math) predicts 11% (Table 6). No results were presented for categories C (unhappiness due to math problems) and D (anxiety related to math problems) of the MAQ.

Table 6
Regression analysis by the stepwise method in order to verify the prediction of math anxiety (MAQ-B) on arithmetic performance.

DISCUSSION

The effects of MA have been considered for a better understanding of the difficulties for learning by children with low arithmetic performance. The negative emotions triggered by the fear might hinder cognitive processes and interfere in performance even for children that do not present cognitive deficits. However, Fassis, Mendes and Carmo (2014Fassis, D.; Mendes, A. C.; Carmo, J. dos S. (2014). Diferentes graus de ansiedade à matemática e desempenho escolar no ensino fundamental. Psicologia Da Educação, 39, 47-61. ISSN 2175-3520. https://revistas.pucsp.br/psicoeduca/article/view/26704/0
https://revistas.pucsp.br/psicoeduca/art...
) suggest that it is not possible to say that high levels of math anxiety are linked to low performance in the subject. In a research developed by these authors with students from elementary school, the students with higher levels of MA were the ones who presented better performance in the discipline. In this study, the objective was to verify the influence of MA in children that present high scores in fluid intelligence in combination with low arithmetic performance.

The results of this study provided evidence for the presence of MA in children who present lower-than-expected performance in math but present high scores in fluid intelligence. However, the levels of MA were not significatively different from the results of the more comprehensive sample. On its turn, fluid intelligence proved a strong predictor of performance in arithmetic skills.

The correlation analysis evidenced two categories related to arithmetic performance, the MAQ-A (performance self-perception) and the MAQ-B (attitude regarding math). No moderate or strong correlations were found between arithmetic performance and the categories that assess the MAQ-C (unhappiness due to math problems) and the MAQ-D (anxiety due to math problems). These results demonstrate the bidimensional nature of MA, with a more cognitive dimension (MAQ-A and MAQ-B) and a more emotional dimension (MAQ-C and MAQ-D) (Haase et al., 2013Haase, V. G.; Lopes-Silva, J.; Starling-Alves, I.; Antunes, A.; Júlio-Costa, A.; Oliveira, L.; Pinheiro-Chagas, P.; Moura, R.; Wood, G. (2013). Com quantos bytes se reduz a ansiedade matemática? A inclusão digital como uma possível ferramenta na promoção do capital mental. In L. E. R. do Valle; Mattos, M. J. V. M.; Costa, J. W. (Eds.), Educação digital. A tecnologia a favor da inclusão (1st ed., Issue January, pp. 188-202). ARTMED. ISBN. 9788565848572). These results also provide evidence that the way children assess their own performance and their own attitude regarding math produce a deeper effect in their performance than the unhappiness or anxiety they face when they have to handle discipline problems.

Multiple Linear Regression Analysis confirmed these results by providing evidence that the MAQ-A category is a predictor of 10 % of performance and category MAQ-B as a predictor of 12%. Such result suggests that performance self-perception explains a variation of 10 % of arithmetic performance, and that attitude regarding math explains 12 % of performance.

These results are in accordance with the findings of Silva, Paixão, Machado and Miguel (2017Silva, J.; Paixão, M.; Machado, T. M.; Miguel, J. (2017). Rendimento escolar na matemática: Efeito diferencial da ansiedade e das crenças de autoeficácia. Revista de Estudios e Investigación en Psicología y Educación, Extr. (1), A1-058. https://doi.org/10.17979/reipe.2017.0.01.2214
https://doi.org/10.17979/reipe.2017.0.01...
), which demonstrate that the belief in self-efficacy for learning math (a person’s confidence in their own capacity to accomplish math tasks successfully), they are inversely related to MA. This suggests that students with low expectations regarding their own performance in the discipline present higher levels of MA, which have a negative impact on performance. Núñez-Peña and Suárez-Pellicioni (2015Núñez-Peña, M. I.; Suárez-Pellicioni, M. (2015). Processing of multi-digit additions in high math-anxious individuals: psychophysiological evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1268. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01268
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01268...
) verify that people with higher levels of MA tend to make more mistakes, provide less accurate answers, and need more time to solve the same math problem proposed to individuals with lower levels of MA. Individuals with high levels of math anxiety also tend to overload their work memory by concentrating on negative emotions and expectations regarding their own performance in the task they have to accomplish.

Another study realized by Dowker et al. (2016Dowker, A.; Sarkar, A.; Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics Anxiety: What Have We Learned in 60 Years? Frontiers in Psychology, 7(APR). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508...
) with the objective to discuss research works realized in the last 60 years on MA suggests that people who recurrently present low performance in math tasks tend to avoid them, which diminishes the possibility to learn the subject in the short and in the long term, wears out cognitive resources that should be employed in the solution of math problems, and compromises performance. According to the authors, there is also the possibility that math anxiety interferes in operational memory, with a negative impact on arithmetic performance, especially in operations with several digits and transport operations. It is an important construct to be studied.

Concerning the gender comparison, significant differences were found between boys and girls only in category MAQ-D, which assesses the anxiety related to math problems. This result suggests that girls with high intelligence and low arithmetic performance are not different from boys when it comes to performance self-perception, attitude regarding the discipline, 0and unhappiness due to math problems, but present higher levels of anxiety regarding these problems. Some studies found different results when comparing the attitude and performance self-perception for math by gender. Pinxten, Marsh, De Fraine, Van Den Noortgate and Van Damme (2014Pinxten, M.; Marsh, H. W.; De Fraine, B.; Van Den Noortgate, W.; Van Damme, J. (2014). Enjoying mathematics or feeling competent in mathematics? Reciprocal effects on mathematics achievement and perceived math effort expenditure. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(1), 152-174. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12028
https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12028...
), in a longitudinal study realized with students from the 4th to the 7th school year, verified that boys report that they like the subject more than girls and they also tend to assess their own math performance more positively. Besides that, a study realized by Loos-Sant’ana and Brito (2017Loos-Sant’Ana, H.; Brito, M. R. F. de. (2017). Atitude e Desempenho em Matemática, Crenças Autorreferenciadas e Família: uma path-analysis. Bolema: Boletim de Educação Matemática, 31(58), 590-613. https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v31n58a03
https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v31n58a...
) indicated that boys tend to believe that girls present an inferior performance in the discipline and girls do not commonly believe their own performance in the discipline might be better than the one by boys, even in situations in which they actually perform better than the boys.

Concerning the comparison of the level of MA of the children in the sample by type of school, the children from the private education system presented higher levels of math anxiety for three of the four categories of the Math Anxiety Questionnaire. However no statistically significant differences were found. This result is in accordance with the results found by Fassis et al. (2014Fassis, D.; Mendes, A. C.; Carmo, J. dos S. (2014). Diferentes graus de ansiedade à matemática e desempenho escolar no ensino fundamental. Psicologia Da Educação, 39, 47-61. ISSN 2175-3520. https://revistas.pucsp.br/psicoeduca/article/view/26704/0
https://revistas.pucsp.br/psicoeduca/art...
), in which the average level of MA of the students from public schools was lower than the level presented by the students from private schools. This difference also failed to show any significance in the statistical assessments though.

Concerning the relation between intelligence and arithmetic performance, a strong correlation between the two variables was found, which suggests that students with higher scores in intelligence tend to present better performance in arithmetic. A study realized by Ribeiro and Freitas (2018Ribeiro, D. O.; Freitas, P. M. (2018). Inteligência e desempenho escolar em crianças entre 6 e 11 anos. Revista Psicologia Em Pesquisa, 12(1), 84-91. https://doi.org/10.24879/201800120010085
https://doi.org/10.24879/201800120010085...
) chose intelligence as a variable to explain 40 % of the variation in school performance. Another study developed by Mecca, Dias, Seabra, Jana and Macedo (2016Mecca, T. P.; Dias, N. M.; Seabra, A. G.; Jana, T. A.; Macedo, E. C. (2016). Relação entre habilidades cognitivas de processamento visual e inteligência fluida com o desempenho em aritmética. Psico(Porto Alegre) [online]. 47(1), 35-45. http://dx.doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2016.1.18924
https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2016....
) indicated that children with higher scores in reasoning tend to present better performance in arithmetic tasks.

Fluid intelligence is a more general component of intelligence and is linked to our capacity for problem solution, planning and the production of strategies (Carrol, 1993Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities. In Human Cognitive Abilities. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511571312
https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511571312...
). The assessed children presented capacities that were above average for these functions, but they presented a performance that was below average in math. Math anxiety proved a factor for explaining this variation. However, it is not the only one. We must remember that empty spaces in education might lead to MA, as well as specific difficulties stemming from low arithmetic performance.

The study provides a representative sample with over 400 children assessed and, based on this group, 12.6% of the children were identified as part of a specific group that presents difficulty in learning math. This percentage is compatible with studies of prevalence that indicate that approximately 10 % of the Brazilian population during the school phase present difficulty in reading and calculus tasks (Garcia, 2003Garcia, J. N. (2003). Las dificultades de aprendizage y otros trastornos del desarrollo. In Futuro Eventos(Ed.), Livro do 6º Simpósio Nacional sobre Distúrbios de Aprendizagem. São Paulo, 157-176.; Giacheti, 2002Giacheti, C. M. (2002). Diagnóstico e intervenção multiprofissional das crianças com dificuldades de aprendizagem. In Futuro Eventos (Ed.), Livro do 6º Simpósio Nacional sobre Distúrbios da aprendizagem. São Paulo, 37-44.). As a limitation to this study, we can highlight the fact that there was no investigation of math skills in a comprehensive way in order to define whether such children present dyscalculia or not. Besides that, there was no investigation of possible environmental causes, such as educational blanks. These limitations can be explored in future studies, including methodological refinement by means of longitudinal studies.

Despite the aforementioned limitations, the realization of this study is important for the comprehension of factors that interfere in the arithmetic performance of children and also in order to amplify the discussion on math anxiety.

Scientific literature has presented an increasing number of research works in the area of number cognition. In this context, the present study collaborates with the results that reflect direct and indirectly in the realm of education, while using scientific foundations in order to point at factors that influence the school performance of children.

The obtained results denotte the impact of emotional variations on arithmetic behavior, promoting a discussion on possible interventions for these problems. Thus, it is important that studies continue to be done in order to investigate arithmetic performance from different perspectives.

REFERÊNCIAS

  • Angelini, A. L.; Alves, I. C. B.; Custódio, E. M.; Duarte, W. F.; Duarte, J. L. M. (1999). Matrizes Progressivas Coloridas de Raven: Escala Especial. Manual São Paulo: CETEPP.
  • Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math Anxiety: Personal, Educational, and Cognitive Consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 181-185. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00196
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00196
  • Beilock, S. L.; Maloney, E. A. (2015). Math Anxiety: A Factor in Math Achievement Not to Be Ignored. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2(1), 4 - 12. https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215601438
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/2372732215601438
  • Carey, E.; Hill, F.; Devine, A.; Szücs, D. (2016). The Chicken or the Egg? The Direction of the Relationship Between Mathematics Anxiety and Mathematics Performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01987
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01987
  • Cargnelutti, E.; Tomasetto C.; Passolunghi, M. C. (2017). How is anxiety related to math performance in young students? A longitudinal study of Grade 2 to Grade 3 children. Cognition And Emotion, 31(4), 755-764. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1147421
    » https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1147421
  • Carmo, J. S. (2011). Ansiedade à matemática: identificação, descrição operacional e estratégias de intervenção. In F. Capovilla (Ed.), Transtornos de aprendizagem: progressos em avaliação e intervenção preventiva e remediativa (249-255). São Paulo: Memnon. ISBN. 978-85-7954-015-8
  • Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities. In Human Cognitive Abilities Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511571312
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511571312
  • Colom, R. (2006). O que é inteligência? In: Flores-Mendoza, C.; Colom, R. (Eds.), Introdução à psicologia das diferenças individuais (59-72). Porto Alegre: Artmed.
  • Dowker, A.; Sarkar, A.; Looi, C. Y. (2016). Mathematics Anxiety: What Have We Learned in 60 Years? Frontiers in Psychology, 7(APR). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00508
  • Eysenck, M. W.; Keane, M. T. (2017). Manual de Psicologia Cognitiva. In Manual de Psicologia Cognitiva https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2014.04.017
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envres.2014.04.017
  • Fassis, D.; Mendes, A. C.; Carmo, J. dos S. (2014). Diferentes graus de ansiedade à matemática e desempenho escolar no ensino fundamental. Psicologia Da Educação, 39, 47-61. ISSN 2175-3520. https://revistas.pucsp.br/psicoeduca/article/view/26704/0
    » https://revistas.pucsp.br/psicoeduca/article/view/26704/0
  • Garcia, J. N. (2003). Las dificultades de aprendizage y otros trastornos del desarrollo. In Futuro Eventos(Ed.), Livro do Simpósio Nacional sobre Distúrbios de Aprendizagem São Paulo, 157-176.
  • Giacheti, C. M. (2002). Diagnóstico e intervenção multiprofissional das crianças com dificuldades de aprendizagem. In Futuro Eventos (Ed.), Livro do Simpósio Nacional sobre Distúrbios da aprendizagem São Paulo, 37-44.
  • Haase, V. G.; Lopes-Silva, J.; Starling-Alves, I.; Antunes, A.; Júlio-Costa, A.; Oliveira, L.; Pinheiro-Chagas, P.; Moura, R.; Wood, G. (2013). Com quantos bytes se reduz a ansiedade matemática? A inclusão digital como uma possível ferramenta na promoção do capital mental. In L. E. R. do Valle; Mattos, M. J. V. M.; Costa, J. W. (Eds.), Educação digital. A tecnologia a favor da inclusão (1st ed., Issue January, pp. 188-202). ARTMED. ISBN. 9788565848572
  • Hartwright, C. E.; Looi, C. Y.; Sella, F.; Inuggi, A.; Santos, F. H.; González-Salinas, C.; Santos, J. M. G.; Kadosh, R. C.; Fuentes, L. J. (2018). The Neurocognitive Architecture of Individual Differences in Math Anxiety in Typical Children. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
    » https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5
  • Luculano, T., Moro, R.; Butterworth, B. (2011). Updating Working Memory and arithmetical attainment in school. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 655-661. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.12.002
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.12.002
  • Loos-Sant’Ana, H.; Brito, M. R. F. de. (2017). Atitude e Desempenho em Matemática, Crenças Autorreferenciadas e Família: uma path-analysis. Bolema: Boletim de Educação Matemática, 31(58), 590-613. https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v31n58a03
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v31n58a03
  • Mecca, T. P.; Dias, N. M.; Seabra, A. G.; Jana, T. A.; Macedo, E. C. (2016). Relação entre habilidades cognitivas de processamento visual e inteligência fluida com o desempenho em aritmética. Psico(Porto Alegre) [online]. 47(1), 35-45. http://dx.doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2016.1.18924
    » https://doi.org/10.15448/1980-8623.2016.1.18924
  • Monteiro, V.; Peixoto, F.; Mata, L.; Sanches, C. (2017). Mathematics: I don’t like it! I like it! Very much, a little, not at all... Social support and emotions in students from 2nd and 3rd cycles of education. Aná. Psicológica [online]. 35(3), 281-296. http://dx.doi.org/10.14417/ap.1156
    » https://doi.org/10.14417/ap.1156
  • Neubert, J. C.; Mainert, J.; Kretzschmar, A.; Greiff, S. (2015). The Assessment of 21st Century Skills in Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Complex and Collaborative Problem Solving. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 8(2), 238-268. https://doi.org/10.1017/iop.2015.14
    » https://doi.org/10.1017/iop.2015.14
  • Núñez-Peña, M. I.; Suárez-Pellicioni, M. (2015). Processing of multi-digit additions in high math-anxious individuals: psychophysiological evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1268. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01268
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01268
  • Passolunghi, M. C.; Caviola, S.; De Agostini, R.; Perin, C.; Mammarella, I. C. (2016). Mathematics anxiety, working memory, and mathematics performance in secondary-school children. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(FEB), 1-8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00042
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00042
  • Peng, P.; Fuchs, D.; Fuchs, L. S.; Elleman, A. M.; Kearns, D. M.; Gilbert, J. K.; Compton, D. L.; Cho, E.; Patton, S. (2018). A Longitudinal Analysis of the Trajectories and Predictors of Word Reading and Reading Comprehension Development Among At-Risk Readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 002221941880908. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219418809080
    » https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219418809080
  • Pinxten, M.; Marsh, H. W.; De Fraine, B.; Van Den Noortgate, W.; Van Damme, J. (2014). Enjoying mathematics or feeling competent in mathematics? Reciprocal effects on mathematics achievement and perceived math effort expenditure. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(1), 152-174. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12028
    » https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12028
  • Porto, S. B. (2018). Relações entre inteligência, criatividade e funções executivas: um estudo multicasos Dissertação (Mestrado em Psicologia) - Centro de Ciências Humanas, Letras e Artes, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal. Recuperado de: https://repositorio.ufrn.br/jspui/handle/123456789/29149
    » https://repositorio.ufrn.br/jspui/handle/123456789/29149
  • Reali, F.; Jiménez-Leal, W.; Maldonado-Carreño, C.; Devine, A.; Szücs, D. (2016). Examining the link between math anxiety and math performance in Colombian students. Revista Colombiana de Psicologia, 25(2). https://doi.org/10.15446/rcp.v25n2.54532
    » https://doi.org/10.15446/rcp.v25n2.54532
  • Ribeiro, D. O.; Freitas, P. M. (2018). Inteligência e desempenho escolar em crianças entre 6 e 11 anos. Revista Psicologia Em Pesquisa, 12(1), 84-91. https://doi.org/10.24879/201800120010085
    » https://doi.org/10.24879/201800120010085
  • Roth, B.; Becker, N.; Romeyke, S.; Schäfer, S.; Domnick, F.; Spinath, F. M. (2015). Intelligence and school grades: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 53, 118-137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.002
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.09.002
  • Silva, J.; Paixão, M.; Machado, T. M.; Miguel, J. (2017). Rendimento escolar na matemática: Efeito diferencial da ansiedade e das crenças de autoeficácia. Revista de Estudios e Investigación en Psicología y Educación, Extr (1), A1-058. https://doi.org/10.17979/reipe.2017.0.01.2214
    » https://doi.org/10.17979/reipe.2017.0.01.2214
  • Simões, E. M. S.; Nogaro, A. (2018). Neurociência Cognitiva Para Educadores - Aprendizagem e Prática Docente no Século XXI Curitiba: CRV. https://doi.org/10.24824/978854441017.2
    » https://doi.org/10.24824/978854441017.2
  • Soares, M. (2004). Letramento e alfabetização: as muitas facetas. Revista Brasileira de Educação, 25, 5 - 17. https://doi.org/10.1590/s1413-24782004000100002
    » https://doi.org/10.1590/s1413-24782004000100002
  • Soltanlou, M.; Artemenko, C.; Dresler, T.; Fallgatter, A. J.; Ehlis, A.-C.; Nuerk, H. C. (2019). Math anxiety in combination with low visuospatial memory impairs math learning in children. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 89. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00089
    » https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00089
  • Suárez-Pellicioni, M.; Núñez-Peña, M. I.; Colomé, À. (2016). Math anxiety: A review of its cognitive consequences, psychophysiological correlates, and brain bases. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 16, 3-22. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0370-7
    » https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0370-7
  • Stein, L. M. (1994). TDE - Teste de Desempenho Escolar: manual para aplicação e interpretação São Paulo, SP: Casa do Psicólogo.
  • Vukovic, R. K.; Kieffer, M. J.; Bailey, S. P.; Harari, R. R. (2013). Mathematics anxiety in young children: Concurrent and longitudinal associations with mathematical performance. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.09.001
    » https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.09.001
  • Wood, G.; Pinheiro-Chagas, P.; Júlio-Costa, A.; Micheli, L. R.; Krinzinger, H.; Kaufmann, L.; Haase, V. G. (2012). Math anxiety questionnaire: Similar latent structure in Brazilian and German school children. Child Development Research, 1-10 https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/610192
    » https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/610192
  • This paper was translated from Portuguese by Régis Lima.
  • This study was financed in part by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior- Brasil (CAPES) - Finance Code 001

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    21 Aug 2023
  • Date of issue
    2023

History

  • Received
    14 Sept 2020
  • Accepted
    02 Aug 2021
Associação Brasileira de Psicologia Escolar e Educacional (ABRAPEE) Associação Brasileira de Psicologia Escolar e Educacional (ABRAPEE), Rua Mirassol, 46 - Vila Mariana , CEP 04044-010 São Paulo - SP - Brasil , Fone/Fax (11) 96900-6678 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revista@abrapee.psc.br