Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Language development profile of children in Belem, according to Denver developmental screening test

Abstract:

PURPOSES:

to assess the language development of children enrolled in elementary schools in Belém, according to the Denver Developmental Screening Test, and to test the correlation between language development and family background, personal characteristics, and characteristics of the environment.

METHODS:

this study is cross-sectional and descriptive exploratory. A questionnaire was applied to parents to collect personal, contextual and family data. The socioeconomic level was measured using an instrument to measure the level of poverty of the family.

RESULTS:

from the 319 children assessed, 59.2% presented result of suspect delay in language. The variables that showed statistically significant association with language development were: paternal education (p=0.003), maternal age (p=0.03) and poverty level of the family (p=0.003).

CONCLUSION:

this study highlights the importance of implementing stimulation and systematic monitoring programs, moreover it alerts about the negative interference of the risk factors in this process.

Keywords:
Child Development; Language; Risk Factors

Resumo:

OBJETIVO:

avaliar o desenvolvimento da linguagem, segundo o Teste de Triagem de Denver II, de crianças que frequentavam a educação infantil em Belém e verificar fatores associados do desfecho com as características familiares, ambientais e pessoais.

MÉTODOS:

trata-se de uma pesquisa transversal e de caráter descritivo exploratório. Foi aplicado um questionário aos genitores para coletar os dados pessoais, contextuais e familiares e um instrumento para medição do nível de pobreza familiar.

RESULTADOS:

das 319 crianças avaliadas, 59,2% apresentaram resultado suspeito de atraso na linguagem. As variáveis que mostraram associação estatisticamente significante com o nível de desenvolvimento da linguagem foram escolaridade paterna (p=0,003), idade materna (p=0,03) e o nível de pobreza urbana (p=0,003).

CONCLUSÃO:

destaca-se a importância de implementar programas de estimulação e monitoramento sistemático, além de alertar para a interferência negativa dos fatores de risco nesse processo.

Descritores:
Infantil; Linguagem; Fatores de Risco

Introduction

The human development is characterized by constant changes in physical aspects, and in neurological, behavioral, cognitive, and social maturation. This phenomenon takes place gradually along the cycle of life, and its outcome is to make the human being competent to attend its own necessities and those from the environment 1. Bronfenbrenner U. Bioecologia do desenvolvimento humano: tornando os seres humanos mais humanos. Porto Alegre: Artmed. Carvalho-Barreto, A. Trad.; 2011. 2. Figueiras AC, Souza ICN, Rios VG, Benguigui Y. Manual para vigilância do desenvolvimento infantil no contexto do AIDPI. Washington: Organização Pan Americana de Saúde; 2005.. During childhood a continuous and progressive process of acquisition of skills happens. These aspects, however, may be influenced by a set of factors 2. Figueiras AC, Souza ICN, Rios VG, Benguigui Y. Manual para vigilância do desenvolvimento infantil no contexto do AIDPI. Washington: Organização Pan Americana de Saúde; 2005., nonetheless, the developmental process doesn't happen the same way for children who were subjected to different socio-cultural contexts due to multiple causes as: gestational history, biological characteristics, the family's socio-economic conditions 3. Gallo PR, Leone C, Amigo H. Tendência de crescimento de filhos de mães adolescentes. Rev Bras Crescimento Desenvolv Hum. 2009;19(2):297-305. 5. Issler RMS, Giugliani ERJ. Identificação de grupos mais vulneráveis à desnutrição infantil pela medição do nível de pobreza. J Pediatr. 1997;73(2):101-5., exposure to contextual factors 6. Eickmann SH, Maciel AMS, Lira PIC, Lima MC. Fatores associados ao desenvolvimento mental e motor de crianças de quatro creches públicas de Recife, Brasil. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2009;27(3):282-8. 9. Maria-Mengel MRS, Linhares MBM. Fatores de risco para problemas de desenvolvimento infantil. Rev Latino-Am Enfermagem. 2007;15:837-42., besides stressful events at early ages 1010 . Evans GW, Kim P. Childhood poverty and young adults' allostatic load: the mediating role of childhood cumulative risk exposure. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(9):979-83. 1111 . Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Ricter L, Strupp B. Developmental potential in the ?rst 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369(9555):60-70.. That means to say that these factors can lead to changes in the development, hence, may facilitate or impair the language acquisition.

Hoff 1212 . Hoff E. Language Development. Belmont: Wadsworth/Cengage Learning; 2014. states that language is the systematic and conventional use of sounds and symbols for communication or self-expression. In the same direction Puyelo 1313 . Puyelo M. Comunicação e Linguagem: desenvolvimento normal e alterações no decorrer do ciclo vital. In: Puyelo M, Rondal JA. (Eds.), Manual do desenvolvimento e alterações da linguagem na criança e no adulto. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 2007. P. 87-99. points out that language is the communication means of human beings, and it works as a means for transmission, categorization, association, and synthesis of complex information between people. Therefore, it is an essential capacity for socialization, learning, and integration to the interlocutor's culture. The linguistic development gathers biological conditions 1212 . Hoff E. Language Development. Belmont: Wadsworth/Cengage Learning; 2014., though it depends on the influence of environmental and social factors that are present in the primary context people are inserted in, such as the family, the day care center, and shelters 8. Cachapuz RF, Halpern R. A influência das variáveis ambientais no desenvolvimento da linguagem em uma amostra de crianças. Rev AMRIGS. 2006;50(4):292-301. 1414 . Nóbrega JN, Minervino CASM. Análise do nível de desenvolvimento da linguagem em crianças abrigadas. Psicol Argum. 2011;29(65):219-26. 1515 . Ramos DD, Salomão NMR. Interação educadora-criança em creches públicas: estilos linguísticos. Psicol Estud. 2012;17(1):15-25..

The elements from the physical and social environment in which the child is inserted are important for the lexical and phonologic development, in other words, in an stimulating and enabler environment, language may progressively develop 1212 . Hoff E. Language Development. Belmont: Wadsworth/Cengage Learning; 2014. 1616 . Schmitt BM, Pentimonti JM, Justice LM. Teacher-child relationships, behavior regulation, and language gain among at-risk preschoolers. J Sch Psychol. 2012;50(5):681-99. 1717 . Schoon I, Parsons S, Rush R, Law J. Children's language ability and psychosocial development: a 29-year follow-up study. Pediatrics. 2010;126(1):73-80.. On the other hand, if the child lives in an environment that doesn't motivate the use communication or its expression in different forms, linguistic dysfunction or delays may occur. Therefore, children that present deficits in comprehension and verbal expression have the tendency to demonstrate difficulties in psychosocial and cognitive aspects, even after they became adults 1717 . Schoon I, Parsons S, Rush R, Law J. Children's language ability and psychosocial development: a 29-year follow-up study. Pediatrics. 2010;126(1):73-80..

Studies have shown that linguistic development may present meaningful changes due to biological risk factors 1818 . Song L, Spier ET, Tamis-Lemonda CS. Reciprocal influences between maternal language and children's language and cognitive development in low-income families. J Child Lang. 2014;41(51):305-26. 1919 . Stolt S, Haataja L, Lapinleimu H, Lehtonen L. The early lexical development and its predictive value to language skills at 2 years in very-low-birthweight children. J Commun Disord. 2009;42(2):107-23. in addition to socio-environmental ones 8. Cachapuz RF, Halpern R. A influência das variáveis ambientais no desenvolvimento da linguagem em uma amostra de crianças. Rev AMRIGS. 2006;50(4):292-301. 2020 . Basílio CS, Puccini RF, Silva EMK, Pedromônico MRM. Condições de vida e vocabulário receptivo em crianças de dois a cinco anos. Rev Saúde Publ. 2005;39(5):725-30. 2121 . Richels CG, Johnson KN, Conture EG. Socioeconomic status, parental education, vocabulary and language skills of children who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2013;46(4):361-74. 2222 . Mousinho R, Schmid E, Pereira J, Lyra L, Mendes L, Nóbrega V. Aquisição e desenvolvimento da linguagem: dificuldades que podem surgir neste percurso. Rev Psicopedagogia. 2008;25(78):297-306., which may cause delays in this area. Among the several language damages here are highlighted the simple delay, phonological delays, specific disorder, difficulties in fluency, and semantic-pragmatic changes, which interfere in the intellectual and academic fields 2222 . Mousinho R, Schmid E, Pereira J, Lyra L, Mendes L, Nóbrega V. Aquisição e desenvolvimento da linguagem: dificuldades que podem surgir neste percurso. Rev Psicopedagogia. 2008;25(78):297-306.. Impairments in this field represent socio-economic problem both for the individual and society because it may increase the years of schooling, decrease professional insertion, and lead to extra expenses on special education or interventions 8. Cachapuz RF, Halpern R. A influência das variáveis ambientais no desenvolvimento da linguagem em uma amostra de crianças. Rev AMRIGS. 2006;50(4):292-301. 2323 . Isotani SM, Azevedo MF, Chiari BM, Perissinoto J. Linguagem expressiva de crianças nascidas pré-termo e termo aos dois anos de idade. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2009;21(2):155-60.. Therefore, it is observed the need to assess and follow the broad development of language, mainly in emergent countries.

There are several strategies mentioned in the literature to detect problems that might appear in early childhood, such as triage and assessments. According to Sigolo and Aiello 2424 . Sigolo ARL, Aiello ALR. Análise de instrumentos para triagem do desenvolvimento infantil. Paidéia (Ribeirão Preto). 2011;21(48):51-60., triage consists in applying tests at a population of children at different ages and it aims at tracking those that might have risks of development delay. Among the triage tools the Denver Developmental Screening Test II (DDST-II) is one of the most used ones, even in clinical and epidemiological researches. This test was developed by Frankenbug and Dodds in 1967 and readapted in 1992 2525 . Frankenburg WK, Dodds J, Archer P, Shapiro H, Bresnick B. The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver Developmental Screening Test. Pediatrics. 1992;89:91-7.. It is among the most common in Brazil and several countries. It is important to mention that DDST-II is a screening test, hence it is not a definite predictor tool of adaptative or intellectual skills 2525 . Frankenburg WK, Dodds J, Archer P, Shapiro H, Bresnick B. The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver Developmental Screening Test. Pediatrics. 1992;89:91-7.. Then, its results cannot be used for diagnostic purposes.

Several studies have investigated the profile of the Neuropsychomotor Development (NPMD) in children that attended Young Children Educational Units (YCEU), making use of DDST-II. Many studies showed language to be the most affected area among pre-schoolers. The influence of socioeconomic factors from the family and the city, along with biological aspects and how they are set may increase the probability for developmental delays to happen. In this sense, this study aims at assessing the language development of children (according to the DDST-II) that attend YCEUs in Belém, and verify possible connections between the outcome and their family's environmental and personal characteristics.

Methods

This research was authorized by the Education State Secretary (SEMEC) and approved by the Ethics Committee on Human Research from the Tropical Medicine Center (NMT/UFPA), by the protocol No. 167.271/2012. The procedures used obeyed the recommendations of the Resolution No. 196/96 from the National Health Council and National Committee of Ethics in Research, in agreement with the Resolution 466/2012. Only the children whose mothers or legal representative accepted to partake in the research and signed the Consent Form were included in the study. In addition, a pilot project was performed with the five different applications of each tool, which enabled to have the same conditions of this study and the training sessions of the research team that was composed by three master's students and seven graduate students.

This consists in a descriptive exploratory and transversal study. 319 children that attended the YCEU from Belém were assessed from August to December, 2012, from which 56% (178) were boys and 44% (141) were girls with ages that varied from 36 to 48 months. The cluster sampling method was used. The margin of error for the sample size calculation was 5% and the level of confidence represented 95%. The YCEUs involved in the research were sorted according to the total number in each district and according to the amount of children in each researched age. Therefore, this study gathered 19 YCEUs out of 35 in the district. Some children were not selected to take part for presenting disorders that affected speech, sensory alterations, sequelae from impairments in the central nervous system, and malformations.

DDST-II 2525 . Frankenburg WK, Dodds J, Archer P, Shapiro H, Bresnick B. The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver Developmental Screening Test. Pediatrics. 1992;89:91-7. was used In order to assess the NPMD, which comprises the ages from 0 to 6. The protocol is composed by 125 tasks, subdivided in four areas: personal-social contact, fine motricity, language, and gross motricity. The test was performed based on the examiner's observation of the child, however some data may be rated based on the parents or caretakers report.

As regards the interpretation of the test, at first the individual data were analyzed, then the test as a whole. Individual items are interpreted as "passed", "failed", "no opportunity available", and "refusal". Afterwards four indicators are created: "Normal" when there is no delay or only one area ranked as caution; "Risk" for two or more cautions and/or delay in at least one area; "Delay" when there are two or more delay items, indicating that the child might have alterations on its development; and "Non-testable" if there is any register of refusal in one or more items that must already be part of the child's repertoire.

The validity of the DDST-II is established by the precision each item and subgroup was determined by crossing the data between ages and percentage of correct answers in the test that can be of 25%, 50%, 75%, and 90% of precision. Therefore, each item would be ranked "normal" when the child would pass or fail between the variation limits from 25% to 75% of right answers for the reference population; "precaution" when the failure would be between 75-90%; and delay when the failures surpassed 90%. Based on the test, the outcomes would be threefold: delay, at risk, or normal. However, in this study a different setting was used, taking into consideration the children at risk or with delay being called suspicious of developmental delay in order to perform the inferential statistical analysis and basing on different studies.

In order to verify family, personal, and environmental characteristics of the children it was used the Biopsychosocial Characteristics of Children Questionnaire (Questionário de Características Biopsicossociais da Criança - QCBC) created for this study (Attachment 1 Attachment 1: Biopsychosocial characteristics of children questionnaire (qcbc) ). This instrument was based on the literature about the determinant factors for the development. It is composed by 48 questions (19 open-ended and 29 closed-ended) structured from the following categories: children and parents identification; pre-, peri-, and post-natal history; Socioeconomic and environmental conditions; and play environment. However, this instrument was used only for characterization purpose, it was not validated.

To measure the level of poverty of the family it was used the Instrument to Measure the Level of Poverty 2626 . Alvarez ML, Muzzo S, Ivanovic D. Escala para medición del nível socioeconómico, en el área de la salud. Rev Med Chile. 1985;113(3):243-9. (Attachment 2 Attachment 2: Instrument to measure the level of poverty* ) translated and adapted in Brazil by Issler and Giugliani 5. Issler RMS, Giugliani ERJ. Identificação de grupos mais vulneráveis à desnutrição infantil pela medição do nível de pobreza. J Pediatr. 1997;73(2):101-5., which enables the analysis of several socioeconomic descriptors of the unfortunate urban population. It has the objective to obtain an appropriate measure to assess its variability, not limited to the family's income. It is composed by 13 items that gathers some variables recognized in the literature as factors that have influence on the child's development. The scores in each item vary in a scale from zero to four, the total minimum possible is seven and the maximum 52 points. The total sum of each item establishes the level of urban poverty of the family. For statistical purposes it is recommended to divide the population studied in quartiles, according to the score obtained in the level of poverty rank. Each quartile is equivalent to 25% of the data distribution.

The data obtained by the instruments were added in a database through SPSS 19. Due to the nature of the variables it was performed descriptive and inferential statistical analyses. The dependent variable of the study was the development score obtained by DDST-II, which was dealt as a dichotomic outcome variable (normal or suspected delay). The independent variables were obtained through other instruments. To verify the associations between the outcome - normal, or suspected of language delay - and the independent variables it was used the Qui-Squared test, considering the level of significance at 5% (p-value <0.05). As it was the same database used in Guerreiro's study2727 . Guerreiro TBF. Desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor de crianças de Belém: associação com características pessoais e variáveis do seu ambiente ecológico [Dissertação]. Belém, PA: Universidade Federal do Pará; 2013., only the results from the language area were analyzed and discussed, for it was found a great level of cases of suspected delay.

Results

Among the 319 participants assessed in this study the majority of the children had suspected delay in the language development, which corresponded to 59.2% (189), from which 62.4% (111) were boys and 55.3% (78) were girls. Table 1 presents the frequency and percentage of the main variables, according to the results of the DDST-II in the language area.

Table 1:
Frequency and association of personal and family variables according to the results of the language development (ddst-ii)

According to Table 1there was a significant statistical associations between the results of the language development, according to DDST-II, and the following variables: mother younger than 19; and the father's schooling under eight years. Regarding the mothers' age, there was a significant statistical correlation (X²= 8.78; gl= 3; p= 0.03) to the outcome studied, it was observed that the mothers aging ≤ 19 were the ones with greater suspected language delay. Regarding the fathers' schooling it was identified that this variable was also linked to the suspected delay (= 13.83; gl= 3; p= 0.003), the higher risk was noted among the participants that had fathers that studied ≤ 8 years.

Table 2 shows the scores of language with normal development and those with suspected delay in relation to the level of poverty. The children who lived in less fortunate environments presented higher percentage (19.7%) of suspected delay in this area, and a significant statistical association among the variables (X2=8.588; gl=1; p<0.003).

Table 2:
Percentage of poverty level distribution correlated to the results in the language area

Discussion

Language development profile according to the DDST-II

The language development analysis showed that 59.2% from the 319 participants presented results that suggested delay. In less economically fortunate populations the YCEUs are vital option for the care of children and may be environments that facilitate a healthy development 6. Eickmann SH, Maciel AMS, Lira PIC, Lima MC. Fatores associados ao desenvolvimento mental e motor de crianças de quatro creches públicas de Recife, Brasil. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2009;27(3):282-8. 2828 . Rodovalho JC, Braga AKP, Formiga CKMR. Diferenças no crescimento e desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor de crianças em centros de educação infantil de Goiânia/GO Rev Eletrônica Enf. 2012;14(1):122-32. 2929 . Braga AKP, Rodovalho JC, Formiga CKMR. Evolução do crescimento e desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor de crianças pré-escolares de zero a dois anos do município de Goiânia (GO). Rev Bras Crescimento Des Hum. 2011;21(2):230-9.. When considering the YCEUs from Belém it was seen that many children are in extreme poverty and at social risk, however, they spend most of their time under the care of the institution. Therefore, the educator's actions work as protection mechanisms for they are considered important communicative reference for those that attend these places 1414 . Nóbrega JN, Minervino CASM. Análise do nível de desenvolvimento da linguagem em crianças abrigadas. Psicol Argum. 2011;29(65):219-26. 1616 . Schmitt BM, Pentimonti JM, Justice LM. Teacher-child relationships, behavior regulation, and language gain among at-risk preschoolers. J Sch Psychol. 2012;50(5):681-99.. Nonetheless, in this study the relationship caretaker-child wasn't investigated.

Personal and environmental profile characteristics

The connection between the low socioeconomic condition and the impairments in the child development is known in the literature 1010 . Evans GW, Kim P. Childhood poverty and young adults' allostatic load: the mediating role of childhood cumulative risk exposure. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(9):979-83. 1111 . Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Ricter L, Strupp B. Developmental potential in the ?rst 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369(9555):60-70.. In this sense, most participants were members of families with an income lower than the minimum wage. However, the income variable was not significantly linked to the studied outcome. As a result, the socioeconomic situation cannot be reduced only to the income, but other variables should be considered such as the parents' education and occupation.

On the subject of poverty, the results about the ecologic context in which the children were inserted were demonstrated to be unfavorable for the linguistic development. Data from Guerreiro 2727 . Guerreiro TBF. Desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor de crianças de Belém: associação com características pessoais e variáveis do seu ambiente ecológico [Dissertação]. Belém, PA: Universidade Federal do Pará; 2013. reveal that poverty is disseminated throughout the city of Belém. Such evidences may have contributed for strengthening the hypothesis that a condition of misery lived by the families may have increased the chances of suspected language delay. Nonetheless, an expressive number of participants (57%) was receiving benefits from the government by the Bolsa Família.

There are severe consequences to the child development occasioned by an unfavorable socioeconomic environment, especially for the language. When compared to their peers with a better economic situation, those children in a poverty situation were facing disparities that dealt with the family, the school, and the community they belonged to 5. Issler RMS, Giugliani ERJ. Identificação de grupos mais vulneráveis à desnutrição infantil pela medição do nível de pobreza. J Pediatr. 1997;73(2):101-5. 1010 . Evans GW, Kim P. Childhood poverty and young adults' allostatic load: the mediating role of childhood cumulative risk exposure. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(9):979-83.. The level of poverty experienced in childhood is one of the main risk factors to reach the family and the neurodevelopment. This variable may take place or aggravate multiple risk factors, besides generating the deprivation of opportunities that enable the potential growth 1111 . Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Ricter L, Strupp B. Developmental potential in the ?rst 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369(9555):60-70.. In this study, the level of poverty was significantly linked to the suspect of language delay. Such data supports the findings of other investigations that applied the DDST-II 2828 . Rodovalho JC, Braga AKP, Formiga CKMR. Diferenças no crescimento e desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor de crianças em centros de educação infantil de Goiânia/GO Rev Eletrônica Enf. 2012;14(1):122-32. 3434 . Torquato JA, Paes JB, Bento MC, Saikai GMPN, Souto JN, Lima EAM, Abreu LC. Prevalência de atraso do desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor em pré-escolares. Rev Bras Crescimento Desenvolv Hum. 2011;21(2):259-68..

Regarding the age of the parents, the mother's age variable (inferior to 19) showed statistical significance with the outcome. In fact, other studies showed that adolescent mothers had children with inferior development when considering growth and neurodevelopment 3. Gallo PR, Leone C, Amigo H. Tendência de crescimento de filhos de mães adolescentes. Rev Bras Crescimento Desenvolv Hum. 2009;19(2):297-305. 2929 . Braga AKP, Rodovalho JC, Formiga CKMR. Evolução do crescimento e desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor de crianças pré-escolares de zero a dois anos do município de Goiânia (GO). Rev Bras Crescimento Des Hum. 2011;21(2):230-9.. Therefore, some questions are raised about the anticipation of sexual relations and motherhood, the presence or absence of a spouse, and family negligence. To Figueiras 2. Figueiras AC, Souza ICN, Rios VG, Benguigui Y. Manual para vigilância do desenvolvimento infantil no contexto do AIDPI. Washington: Organização Pan Americana de Saúde; 2005., teenage pregnancy may lead to risks in the child development. Adolescent mothers compared to the adult ones are less interactive and communicative with their children. These characteristics may be related to the possible explanations of the results of previous studies, which relate the influence of social factors to the lack of stimulation or interaction of the mother and the child suspected of language delay.

Some studies support the idea that the level of education of mothers works as a protective factor for the child development 8. Cachapuz RF, Halpern R. A influência das variáveis ambientais no desenvolvimento da linguagem em uma amostra de crianças. Rev AMRIGS. 2006;50(4):292-301. 2020 . Basílio CS, Puccini RF, Silva EMK, Pedromônico MRM. Condições de vida e vocabulário receptivo em crianças de dois a cinco anos. Rev Saúde Publ. 2005;39(5):725-30. 3131 . Brito CML, Vieira GO, Costa MCO, Oliveira NF. Desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor: o teste de Denver na triagem dos atrasos cognitivos e neuromotores de pré-escolares. Cad Saúde Pública. 2011;27(7):1403-14.. In the present study there were no statistical significance in the correlation between the mothers' level of education and the language area score. Maybe that has happened due to the nature of the sample, which was composed by children who studied in public institutions that attend mostly families with low socioeconomic levels.

However, paternal schooling equal or inferior to eight years was shown to have influence on the children' language. A higher level of the father's education may lead to better conditions of employment and increase the family's income, and improve the quality of adequate stimulus to the development 9. Maria-Mengel MRS, Linhares MBM. Fatores de risco para problemas de desenvolvimento infantil. Rev Latino-Am Enfermagem. 2007;15:837-42. 2121 . Richels CG, Johnson KN, Conture EG. Socioeconomic status, parental education, vocabulary and language skills of children who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2013;46(4):361-74.. Studies suggest that parents or caretakers with a better socioeconomic level and higher education are more communicative with their children, using broad and varied vocabulary in the interactions 1515 . Ramos DD, Salomão NMR. Interação educadora-criança em creches públicas: estilos linguísticos. Psicol Estud. 2012;17(1):15-25. 2121 . Richels CG, Johnson KN, Conture EG. Socioeconomic status, parental education, vocabulary and language skills of children who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2013;46(4):361-74. 3535 . Hoff E. How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev Rev. 2006;26(1):55-88.. On the other hand, those that have unfavorable socioeconomic and educational conditions, as in the studied sample, are likely to use a less diversified language pattern and to read less to their children, depriving them of complex verbal strategies 1818 . Song L, Spier ET, Tamis-Lemonda CS. Reciprocal influences between maternal language and children's language and cognitive development in low-income families. J Child Lang. 2014;41(51):305-26. 3535 . Hoff E. How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev Rev. 2006;26(1):55-88..

In fact, the parents' schooling is shown to be a protective factor. It is understood that a better education is associated to the parents cognitive skills used to stimulate the children. Besides, this variable tends to increase the chances of a better education to children, conditioned to the care practices and to the ecologic environment provided to the children. This context may amplify the physical and socio-cultural experiences in childhood, motivating a better adjustment 9. Maria-Mengel MRS, Linhares MBM. Fatores de risco para problemas de desenvolvimento infantil. Rev Latino-Am Enfermagem. 2007;15:837-42. 2121 . Richels CG, Johnson KN, Conture EG. Socioeconomic status, parental education, vocabulary and language skills of children who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2013;46(4):361-74.. However, it is necessary to clarify that being underprivileged doesn't mean denying opportunities or depriving the children from stimulus facilitator of the development.

A higher degree of the parents' education enables a quality stimulation of the children, even if sometimes the amount of time dedicated to this interaction is smaller. Besides, they are the primary communicative peers, and through this relation the first forms of language take place 1515 . Ramos DD, Salomão NMR. Interação educadora-criança em creches públicas: estilos linguísticos. Psicol Estud. 2012;17(1):15-25. 1818 . Song L, Spier ET, Tamis-Lemonda CS. Reciprocal influences between maternal language and children's language and cognitive development in low-income families. J Child Lang. 2014;41(51):305-26.. When the child is inserted in an educative and caring environment the caretakers are the main reference and the stimulators of this domain. This enables the children to learn new words and their meanings, in addition to perceiving how adults organize the information from their physical and social environment 1616 . Schmitt BM, Pentimonti JM, Justice LM. Teacher-child relationships, behavior regulation, and language gain among at-risk preschoolers. J Sch Psychol. 2012;50(5):681-99. 2020 . Basílio CS, Puccini RF, Silva EMK, Pedromônico MRM. Condições de vida e vocabulário receptivo em crianças de dois a cinco anos. Rev Saúde Publ. 2005;39(5):725-30..

However, depending on the level of education and socioeconomic condition of these professionals they may use simpler and poorer linguistic styles 1515 . Ramos DD, Salomão NMR. Interação educadora-criança em creches públicas: estilos linguísticos. Psicol Estud. 2012;17(1):15-25.. Other characteristics that interfere in the quality of these institutions and the language stimulation in these environments are the proportion caretaker-children, qualification and continuous improvement of the professionals, and interpersonal responsiveness 1515 . Ramos DD, Salomão NMR. Interação educadora-criança em creches públicas: estilos linguísticos. Psicol Estud. 2012;17(1):15-25. 2929 . Braga AKP, Rodovalho JC, Formiga CKMR. Evolução do crescimento e desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor de crianças pré-escolares de zero a dois anos do município de Goiânia (GO). Rev Bras Crescimento Des Hum. 2011;21(2):230-9.. The caretaker-children proportion is predicted by the National Curricular Parameters for Child Education 3636 . Brasil. Parâmetros Nacionais de Qualidade para a Educação Infantil. Brasília: MEC/SEB/DPE/COEDI; 2006.. For that reason it is a necessary data for the analysis of language development, for the educator need to interact with the child in a peculiar way. In this study, these variables were not investigated, however they should, in future researches.

It must be highlighted that most YCEUs involved in the research are placed in suburban areas, where social problems may be noted everywhere. Even though some units presented good structure most of them were damaged and lacked physical and educational resources, possibly interfering on the linguistic abilities. Despite that, these places might work as protective factors of the development, for the children spend most of their time there and establish relationships and interactions outside the family circle.

The varied type of job occupation the fathers had showed a connection slightly significant in relation to the results suspected to have delay in the language area. Such result may be in agreement with the hypothesis that a higher education level may lead to a better job for the father, providing greater opportunities and better developmental stimulation.

Through the analysis and interpretation of the results it was possible to assess the high prevalence of suspected delay in the participant's language. It is emphasized that the DDST-II test is a triage test, in other words, it doesn't make a clinical diagnosis. Therefore, the participants with altered performance should be reassessed, and if the results persist they should be lead to specific evaluation. The characteristics identified as predictors of suspected delay in the language development were: children who live in poverty; their mothers were 19 years old or younger; and the father's schooling period was less than eight years. In other words, children with this profile are exposed to risk factors and vulnerability that might bring negative effects for their development.

The DDST-II was shown to be a good tool to screen the child development through simple methodology, low cost, and easily applied by professionals from the fields of education and health. In addition to being an important mean of early detection of disorders. Likewise, the instrument used to assess the level of poverty of the families was able to identify and link the data about ecological conditions present in the family environment of the researched children. Therefore, it is considered that the suspected delay in the language development has a multi-factor character.

Conclusion

The results from the present study draw attention to the necessity of promoting improvements in the ecological conditions of the children assessed along with their families, reducing the threats to the development they are exposed to. This type of research and hence its discussion are necessary and essential to contribute to the adaptation of public policies related to the children's education and health in the city of Belém, in addition to support and motivate follow-up programs and vigilance of the development by a multidisciplinary team. It is suggested that new longitudinal studies with bigger sample, and different age range are carried out in order to investigate more precisely the findings. Other assessment tools may also be used, including ones to assess the influence of the care environment and the caretakers have on the child's development, in addition to qualification courses for these professionals.

Referências

  • 1
    Bronfenbrenner U. Bioecologia do desenvolvimento humano: tornando os seres humanos mais humanos. Porto Alegre: Artmed. Carvalho-Barreto, A. Trad.; 2011.
  • 2
    Figueiras AC, Souza ICN, Rios VG, Benguigui Y. Manual para vigilância do desenvolvimento infantil no contexto do AIDPI. Washington: Organização Pan Americana de Saúde; 2005.
  • 3
    Gallo PR, Leone C, Amigo H. Tendência de crescimento de filhos de mães adolescentes. Rev Bras Crescimento Desenvolv Hum. 2009;19(2):297-305.
  • 4
    Escarce AG, Camargos TV, Souza VC, Mourão MP, Lemos SMA. Escolaridade materna e desenvolvimento da linguagem em crianças de 2 meses à 2 anos. Rev CEFAC. 2011;14(6):1139-45.
  • 5
    Issler RMS, Giugliani ERJ. Identificação de grupos mais vulneráveis à desnutrição infantil pela medição do nível de pobreza. J Pediatr. 1997;73(2):101-5.
  • 6
    Eickmann SH, Maciel AMS, Lira PIC, Lima MC. Fatores associados ao desenvolvimento mental e motor de crianças de quatro creches públicas de Recife, Brasil. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2009;27(3):282-8.
  • 7
    Ribeiro DG, Perosa GB, Padovani FHP. Fatores de risco para o desenvolvimento de crianças atendidas em Unidades de Saúde da Família, ao final do primeiro ano de vida: aspectos sociodemográficos e de saúde mental materna. Cien Saude Colet. 2014;19(1):215-26.
  • 8
    Cachapuz RF, Halpern R. A influência das variáveis ambientais no desenvolvimento da linguagem em uma amostra de crianças. Rev AMRIGS. 2006;50(4):292-301.
  • 9
    Maria-Mengel MRS, Linhares MBM. Fatores de risco para problemas de desenvolvimento infantil. Rev Latino-Am Enfermagem. 2007;15:837-42.
  • 10
    Evans GW, Kim P. Childhood poverty and young adults' allostatic load: the mediating role of childhood cumulative risk exposure. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(9):979-83.
  • 11
    Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Ricter L, Strupp B. Developmental potential in the ?rst 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369(9555):60-70.
  • 12
    Hoff E. Language Development. Belmont: Wadsworth/Cengage Learning; 2014.
  • 13
    Puyelo M. Comunicação e Linguagem: desenvolvimento normal e alterações no decorrer do ciclo vital. In: Puyelo M, Rondal JA. (Eds.), Manual do desenvolvimento e alterações da linguagem na criança e no adulto. Porto Alegre: Artmed; 2007. P. 87-99.
  • 14
    Nóbrega JN, Minervino CASM. Análise do nível de desenvolvimento da linguagem em crianças abrigadas. Psicol Argum. 2011;29(65):219-26.
  • 15
    Ramos DD, Salomão NMR. Interação educadora-criança em creches públicas: estilos linguísticos. Psicol Estud. 2012;17(1):15-25.
  • 16
    Schmitt BM, Pentimonti JM, Justice LM. Teacher-child relationships, behavior regulation, and language gain among at-risk preschoolers. J Sch Psychol. 2012;50(5):681-99.
  • 17
    Schoon I, Parsons S, Rush R, Law J. Children's language ability and psychosocial development: a 29-year follow-up study. Pediatrics. 2010;126(1):73-80.
  • 18
    Song L, Spier ET, Tamis-Lemonda CS. Reciprocal influences between maternal language and children's language and cognitive development in low-income families. J Child Lang. 2014;41(51):305-26.
  • 19
    Stolt S, Haataja L, Lapinleimu H, Lehtonen L. The early lexical development and its predictive value to language skills at 2 years in very-low-birthweight children. J Commun Disord. 2009;42(2):107-23.
  • 20
    Basílio CS, Puccini RF, Silva EMK, Pedromônico MRM. Condições de vida e vocabulário receptivo em crianças de dois a cinco anos. Rev Saúde Publ. 2005;39(5):725-30.
  • 21
    Richels CG, Johnson KN, Conture EG. Socioeconomic status, parental education, vocabulary and language skills of children who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2013;46(4):361-74.
  • 22
    Mousinho R, Schmid E, Pereira J, Lyra L, Mendes L, Nóbrega V. Aquisição e desenvolvimento da linguagem: dificuldades que podem surgir neste percurso. Rev Psicopedagogia. 2008;25(78):297-306.
  • 23
    Isotani SM, Azevedo MF, Chiari BM, Perissinoto J. Linguagem expressiva de crianças nascidas pré-termo e termo aos dois anos de idade. Pró-Fono R Atual Cient. 2009;21(2):155-60.
  • 24
    Sigolo ARL, Aiello ALR. Análise de instrumentos para triagem do desenvolvimento infantil. Paidéia (Ribeirão Preto). 2011;21(48):51-60.
  • 25
    Frankenburg WK, Dodds J, Archer P, Shapiro H, Bresnick B. The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver Developmental Screening Test. Pediatrics. 1992;89:91-7.
  • 26
    Alvarez ML, Muzzo S, Ivanovic D. Escala para medición del nível socioeconómico, en el área de la salud. Rev Med Chile. 1985;113(3):243-9.
  • 27
    Guerreiro TBF. Desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor de crianças de Belém: associação com características pessoais e variáveis do seu ambiente ecológico [Dissertação]. Belém, PA: Universidade Federal do Pará; 2013.
  • 28
    Rodovalho JC, Braga AKP, Formiga CKMR. Diferenças no crescimento e desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor de crianças em centros de educação infantil de Goiânia/GO Rev Eletrônica Enf. 2012;14(1):122-32.
  • 29
    Braga AKP, Rodovalho JC, Formiga CKMR. Evolução do crescimento e desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor de crianças pré-escolares de zero a dois anos do município de Goiânia (GO). Rev Bras Crescimento Des Hum. 2011;21(2):230-9.
  • 30
    Biscegli TS, Polis LB, Santos LM, Vicentin M. Avaliação do estado nutricional e do desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor em crianças frequentadoras de creche. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2007;25(4):337-42.
  • 31
    Brito CML, Vieira GO, Costa MCO, Oliveira NF. Desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor: o teste de Denver na triagem dos atrasos cognitivos e neuromotores de pré-escolares. Cad Saúde Pública. 2011;27(7):1403-14.
  • 32
    Sabatés AL, Mendes LC. Perfil do crescimento e desenvolvimento de crianças entre 12 e 36 meses de idade que frequentam uma creche municipal da cidade de Guarulhos. Cienc Cuid Saúde. 2007;6:164-70.
  • 33
    Saccani R, Brizola E, Giordani AP, Bach S, Resende TL, Almeida CS. Avaliação do desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor em crianças de um bairro da periferia de Porto Alegre. Sci Med. 2007;17(3):130-7.
  • 34
    Torquato JA, Paes JB, Bento MC, Saikai GMPN, Souto JN, Lima EAM, Abreu LC. Prevalência de atraso do desenvolvimento neuropsicomotor em pré-escolares. Rev Bras Crescimento Desenvolv Hum. 2011;21(2):259-68.
  • 35
    Hoff E. How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev Rev. 2006;26(1):55-88.
  • 36
    Brasil. Parâmetros Nacionais de Qualidade para a Educação Infantil. Brasília: MEC/SEB/DPE/COEDI; 2006.
  • Financial support: This manuscript is part of a master's degree project whose student received scholarship from Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior - CAPES

Attachment 1: Biopsychosocial characteristics of children questionnaire (qcbc)

Attachment 2: Instrument to measure the level of poverty*

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Aug 2015

History

  • Received
    30 Oct 2014
  • Accepted
    29 Jan 2015
ABRAMO Associação Brasileira de Motricidade Orofacial Rua Uruguaiana, 516, Cep 13026-001 Campinas SP Brasil, Tel.: +55 19 3254-0342 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistacefac@cefac.br