Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Analysis of phonological processes in children from the metropolitan region of recife

Abstract:

PURPOSE:

to describe the phonological development of not standard Portuguese speaking children in the metropolitan region of Recife-PE from the phonological processes model.

METHODS:

202 children of both sexes, aged from 2:0 to 6:11 years, attending day care centers and public schools, took part in the study. The Phonological Assessment Test was used for evaluation. Data were recorded, transcribed, coded and tabulated in a database, which enabled the realization of descriptive analysis (calculation of means and standard deviations).

RESULTS:

the most frequent phonological processes were consonant cluster simplification, liquid consonant simplification, syllable reduction and simplification of final consonant. There was a delay in the elimination of syllable reduction processes, consonant harmony, plosivation of fricatives, simplification of velar fricatives and liquid consonant simplification. It was also noted that the consonant cluster simplification process was not eliminated in the studied population, taking into account the age of elimination as described in the literature, which may be indicative of the influence of linguistic variety on phonological acquisition.

CONCLUSION:

speakers of non-standard Portuguese of the Recife metropolitan region have a different phonological acquisition from that described in the literature, which may be indicative of the influence of linguistic variety. The results draw attention to the fact that such variety should be considered in phonological evaluations. It is essential that new studies explore phonological acquisition in different regions of the country.

Keywords:
Language; Language Development; Speech; Child; Preschool; Family Relations

Resumo:

OBJETIVO:

descrever o desenvolvimento fonológico de crianças falantes do português não padrão na Região Metropolitana do Recife- PE, a partir do modelo dos processos fonológicos.

MÉTODOS:

participaram do estudo 202 crianças de ambos os sexos, na faixa etária de 2:0 a 6:11 anos, matriculadas em creches e escolas públicas. Utilizou-se como instrumento de avaliação a Prova de Avaliação Fonológica. Os dados foram gravados, transcritos, codificados e tabulados em um banco de dados, que possibilitou a realização de análises descritivas (cálculo de médias e desvios padrão).

RESULTADOS:

os processos fonológicos mais frequentes foram simplificação de encontro consonantal, simplificação de líquida, redução de sílaba e simplificação de consoante final. Observou-se o atraso na eliminação dos processos de redução de sílaba, harmonia consonantal, plosivação de fricativa, simplificação de fricativa velar e simplificação de liquida. Observou-se, ainda, que o processo de simplificação de encontro consonantal não foi eliminado na população estudada, levando-se em consideração as idades de eliminação descritas na literatura pesquisada, o que pode ser revelador da influência da variedade linguística sobre a aquisição fonológica.

CONCLUSÃO:

crianças falantes do português não padrão da Região Metropolitana do Recife apresentam uma aquisição fonológica diferente da descrição da literatura na área, o que pode ser indicativo da influência da variedade linguística. Os resultados chamam a atenção para o fato que tal variedade deve ser considerada na avaliação fonológica, sendo fundamental que novos estudos explorem a aquisição fonológica em diferentes regiões do país.

Descrtores:
Linguagem; Desenvolvimento da Linguagem; Fala; Criança; Pré-Escolar; Relações Familiares

Introduction

The phonological development of a linguistic system occurs gradually, to the extent that the child experiences different situations in which it must learn which sounds are used in its language and how they are organized 11 Lamprecht RR. Sobre os desvios fonológicos. In: Lamprecht RR. (org.) Aquisição fonológica do português: perfil de desenvolvimento e subsídios para terapia. Porto Alegre: ARTMED, 2004. p.193-212. 22 Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Análise dos processos fonológicos em crianças com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(1):36-40.. This development is not linear. It has individual variations, since it is fully linked to social relations, experiences and communicative interactions that the child establishes with the environment. The social aspect develops jointly vocabulary and cognition from social interactions11 Lamprecht RR. Sobre os desvios fonológicos. In: Lamprecht RR. (org.) Aquisição fonológica do português: perfil de desenvolvimento e subsídios para terapia. Porto Alegre: ARTMED, 2004. p.193-212. 99 Ghisleni MRL, Keske-Soares M, Mezzomo CL. O uso das estratégias de reparo considerando a gravidade do desvio fonológico evolutivo. Rev CEFAC. 2010;12(5):766-71..

For some authors, the establishment of the age group for phonological development is widely discussed, being found in the literature ranges between 4:0 and 6:0 years1010 Acosta VM, Moreno A, Ramos V, Quintana A, Espino O. V. Avaliação da linguagem: teoria e prática do processo de avaliação do comportamento linguístico infantil. São Paulo: Santos, 2003. 214 p. and between 4:0 and 7:0 years1111 Wertzner HF. Fonologia: Desenvolvimento e Alterações. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. Ed. Roca, 2004. p. 772-86.. For some authors22 Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Análise dos processos fonológicos em crianças com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(1):36-40., the phase of greatest expansion of the phonological system is between 1:6 and 4:0 years, when there is an increase in the phonetic inventory of children, enabling the production of polysyllabic words and more complex syllabic structures. However, this period is characterized by substitutions and omissions of sounds.

During development, the child increases its phonetic inventory and dominates the phonological rules of its own linguistic system considering phonemes, their distribution and the type of syllabic structure where they occur22 Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Análise dos processos fonológicos em crianças com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(1):36-40.. Children use resources in place of the segment or the syllabic structure that they do not know yet, or whose production they do not master. As this process unfolds, the used resources also change due to the proximity of child's and adult's phonological systems. These resources are observed both in normal as in deviant phonological development processes, with the difference that, in deviations, these features will last longer22 Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Análise dos processos fonológicos em crianças com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(1):36-40. 55 Spíndola RA, Payão LMC, Bandini HHM. Abordagem fonoaudiológica em desvios fonológicos fundamentada na hierarquia dos traços distintivos e na consciência fonológica. Rev CEFAC. 2007;9(2):180-9. 77 Patah LK, Takiuchi N. Prevalência das alterações fonológicas e uso de processos fonológicos em escolares aos 7 anos. Rev CEFAC. 2008;10(2):158-67. 99 Ghisleni MRL, Keske-Soares M, Mezzomo CL. O uso das estratégias de reparo considerando a gravidade do desvio fonológico evolutivo. Rev CEFAC. 2010;12(5):766-71. 1212 Vítor RM, Cardoso-Martins C. Desenvolvimento fonológico de crianças pré-escolares da Região Noroeste de Belo Horizonte. Psicol Rev. 2007;13(2):383-98. 1313 Caumo DTM, Ferreira MIDC. Relação entre desvios fonológicos e processamento auditivo. Rev. soc. bras. fonoaudiol. [periódico na internet]. 2009 [Acesso em 14 de janeiro de 2010]; 14(2):234-40. Disponível em: www.scielo.br/scielo.php?pid=S151680342009000200015&script=sci_arttext
http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php?pid=S151...
.

To evaluate phonological development, it is crucial to analyze the speech of the child considering the phonological system of the adult. A model that has long been used in the literature to describe a child's phonological system is phonological processes, which concerns the simplification of phonological rules involving sequences of sounds in the pronunciation of words. Most of these are part of a typical speech development, eliminated over the years. If a child shows phonological processes beyond the expected age, it is regarded as having phonological disorders1414 Nunes DA, Payão LMC, Costa RCC. Desvios fonológicos na educação infantil. Rev. CEFAC [periódico na Internet]. 2010 [acesso em 10 de maio de 2011]; 12(2):331-6. Disponível em: www.scielo.br/pdf/rcefac/v12n2/21.pdf
http://www.scielo.br/pdf/rcefac/v12n2/21...
1616 Haupt C, Aguiar PG. Variações fonético-fonológicas e desvios fonológicos - um estudo de caso. Revista do Curso de Letras da UNIABEU [periódico na internet]. 2013 [Acesso em 5 de junho de 2013]; 4(2):12-25. Disponível em: www.uniabeu.edu.br/publica/index.php/RE/article/view/716/pdf_374
http://www.uniabeu.edu.br/publica/index....
.

Phonological processes can be classified into simplification of consonant cluster, liquid consonant simplification, simplification of final consonants, devoicing of fricatives, devoicing of plosives, frontalization to palatal, velar fricative simplification, posteriorization to palatal, frontalization of velar fricatives and plosivization of fricatives1717 Wertzner HF. Distúrbio Fonológico. In: Andrade, CRF, Marcondes, E. Fonoaudiologia em Pediatria. São Paulo: Sarvier, 2003, p. 70-79.. That classification was used in this study, although other research1313 Caumo DTM, Ferreira MIDC. Relação entre desvios fonológicos e processamento auditivo. Rev. soc. bras. fonoaudiol. [periódico na internet]. 2009 [Acesso em 14 de janeiro de 2010]; 14(2):234-40. Disponível em: www.scielo.br/scielo.php?pid=S151680342009000200015&script=sci_arttext
http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php?pid=S151...
1818 Lima LM, Queiroga BAM. Aquisição fonológica em crianças com antecedentes de desnutrição. Rev CEFAC. 2007;9(1):13-20. present other classifications.

In general, studies that explore children's language and socio-cultural aspects report that most cognitive and/or language assessment tests are favorable to socio-culturally dominant groups and, with some frequency, the differences are interpreted as reflecting inadequate development conditions1919 Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Aquisição fonológica de crianças de classe sócio econômica alta. Rev CEFAC. 2008;10(4):452-60. 2020 Silva MK, Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Aquisição fonológica do Português Brasileiro em crianças do Rio de Janeiro. J. Soc. Bras. Fonoaudiol. [periódico na internet]. 2012 [Acesso em 20 de abril de 2013]; 24(3):248-54. Disponível em: www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S2179-64912012000300010
http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=s...
. However, studies identify linguistic variations in populations of specific social classes. Such variations can be attributed in part to social factors such as age, gender, social class and maternal education. They can also be correlated to independent linguistic variables such as phonological context, word length, tone, among others2020 Silva MK, Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Aquisição fonológica do Português Brasileiro em crianças do Rio de Janeiro. J. Soc. Bras. Fonoaudiol. [periódico na internet]. 2012 [Acesso em 20 de abril de 2013]; 24(3):248-54. Disponível em: www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S2179-64912012000300010
http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=s...
2222 Queiroga BAM, Alves JM, Cordeiro AAA, Montenegro ACA, Asfora R. Aquisição dos encontros consonantais por crianças falantes do português não padrão da região metropolitana do Recife. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13(2):214-26..

Although Brazilian Portuguese is relatively consistent, there is a diversity of linguistic variations, as pointed out by the literature2222 Queiroga BAM, Alves JM, Cordeiro AAA, Montenegro ACA, Asfora R. Aquisição dos encontros consonantais por crianças falantes do português não padrão da região metropolitana do Recife. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13(2):214-26., so that particular studies of a region cannot automatically be generalized to other Brazilian regions, being necessary to consider phonological specificities of various dialects and then propose generalizations valid for Brazilian Portuguese. It would therefore be inadmissible to evaluate the language of a child regardless of the model spoken in its language community. However, not infrequently, the speech-language pathologist uses tests involving spontaneous speech, appointment and/or repetition of words, referencing just what would be expected for each age group.

This study has a practical relevance, since it expands the prospects for a greater understanding of the child's language development. Moreover, it provide subsidies to the speech-language pathologist for a precise clinical evaluation and a more appropriated therapeutic intervention for cases of phonological changes, as well as for activities that indicate the promotion of language development. Based on the foregoing, the present study aimed to describe the phonological development of non-standard Portuguese speaking children in the metropolitan region of Recife-PE from the model of phonological processes.

Methods

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee for Research involving Human Beings of the Health Sciences Center (CCS) of the Federal University of Pernambuco under protocol No. 115/09 from the National Health Council, thus allowing its conducting.

The methodological path suitable for the present research prioritized a quantitative analysis and the study was characterized as descriptive, exploratory and crossed.

The participants were 202 children of both genders, aged from 2:0 to 6:11 years old, enrolled in day care centers and public schools in the metropolitan region of Recife - Pernambuco, divided in ten age groups, with a six-month interval: G1 (2:0-2:5); G2 (2:6-2:11); G3 (3:0-3:5); G4 (3:6-3:11); G5 (4:0-4:5); G6 (4:6-4:11); G7 (5:0-5:5); G8 (5:6-5:11); G9 (6:0-6:5); G10 (6:0-6:11). This age subdivision is justified by children's major language acquisitions in this stage of development, as some authors describe2222 Queiroga BAM, Alves JM, Cordeiro AAA, Montenegro ACA, Asfora R. Aquisição dos encontros consonantais por crianças falantes do português não padrão da região metropolitana do Recife. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13(2):214-26.. Thus, longer intervals in age-group analysis could cover leaps in the language development of children.

The sample exclusion criteria were the presence of communication, learning, special educational needs, hearing impairment, neurological problems or other obvious aspect that might interfere with language development.

Data were collected from August 2009 to July 2011. Initially, individual data from the student identification card at school were obtained. Subsequently, additional information was obtained through a psychosocial questionnaire made with parents or guardians through interviews. The purpose of the questionnaire was to characterize the sample and trace a description of the reality in which children were in. Parents or guardians received an information letter containing the study objectives and ensuring the confidentiality of information. All those who agreed to participate signed the Informed Consent - IC.

Initially, children were assessed by a clinical orofacial motor examination based on the MBGR PROTOCOL2323 Queiroga BAM, Alves JM, Cordeiro AAA, Montenegro ACA, Asfora R. Aquisição dos encontros consonantais por crianças falantes do português não padrão da região metropolitana do Recife. Rev. CEFAC. 2011;13(2):214-26., observing the mobility and tone of oral myofunctional structures. The results were registered in a protocol. The purpose of this evaluation was to identify and exclude from the sample children that had phonetic changes.

Following, the assessment of phonological acquisition was carried out through a Phonological Evaluation Test (Prova de Avaliação Fonológica - PAFon)2222 Queiroga BAM, Alves JM, Cordeiro AAA, Montenegro ACA, Asfora R. Aquisição dos encontros consonantais por crianças falantes do português não padrão da região metropolitana do Recife. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13(2):214-26. 2424 Marchesan IQ, Genaro KF, Berretin-Felix G, Rehder MIBC. Avaliação miofuncional orofacial - protocolo MBGR. Rev CEFAC. 2009;11(2):237-55.. This test aims to appoint 215 target-words selected in order to include Portuguese phonemes by controlling some linguistic variables such as syllabic structure and tone. The instrument is divided into six categories: AF1- Animals, AF2- Food, AF3- Body and personal items, AF4- Home objects, AF5- Child objects and AF6- Nature and transport. When the child did not spontaneously name the figures, it is asked to repeat the target-word from the model offered by the examiner. Registration considers whether naming or repetition was performed.

Upon completion of the collection, the results were coded in numeric variables to enable the data collection for a database using the SPSS 13.0 software, which enabled the realization of descriptive statistical analysis (calculation of means and standard deviations).

Studies on phonological acquisition have considered the percentage of 80% standard production as an indication that the child has mastered the acquisition of a phonological segment2222 Queiroga BAM, Alves JM, Cordeiro AAA, Montenegro ACA, Asfora R. Aquisição dos encontros consonantais por crianças falantes do português não padrão da região metropolitana do Recife. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13(2):214-26. 2424 Marchesan IQ, Genaro KF, Berretin-Felix G, Rehder MIBC. Avaliação miofuncional orofacial - protocolo MBGR. Rev CEFAC. 2009;11(2):237-55.. Similarly, in this study, it was considered as a framework for the elimination of processes a 80% reduction in the occurrence of each phonological process for the age group.

Results

As previously stated, 202 preschool children with typical phonological development were distributed in ten age groups as shown in Table 1. The difficulty in applying the test with young children is highlighted, which explains a lower number of participants in the range of two and three years.

Table 1:
Distribution of children surveyed in age groups, Recife, 2014. (N=202)

The mean and the standard deviation for phonological processes and their relation to each age group (group) can be seen in Table 2. It is possible to see that the most frequent phonological processes in all age groups were simplification of consonant cluster, liquid consonant simplification, syllable reduction and simplification of final consonant. It is noteworthy that the process of simplification of consonant cluster was never eliminated in the investigated age group.

Table 2:
Distribution of relative results of phonological processes evaluation (means and standard deviations), by age group, of preschool children from 2:0 to 6:11 years old, Recife, 2012. (N=203)

Figure 1 shows a comparison of the expected phonological acquisition for each age group, according to Wertzner1717 Wertzner HF. Distúrbio Fonológico. In: Andrade, CRF, Marcondes, E. Fonoaudiologia em Pediatria. São Paulo: Sarvier, 2003, p. 70-79., with the age found in this study, indicating a delayed elimination of syllable reduction, consonant harmony, plosivation of fricatives, velar fricative simplification and liquid simplification.

Figure 1:
Comparison of phonological acquisition ages observed in this study and ages proposed by Wertzner (2003)

Discussion

The results show that the phonological processes that operated most in the speech of surveyed children were consonant cluster simplification, liquid consonant simplification, syllable reduction and simplification of final consonant. Another survey conducted in the metropolitan area of ​​Recife with children from public and private schools showed similar results, indicating consonant cluster simplification, liquid consonant simplification, final consonant simplification and syllable reduction as the most frequent processes2424 Marchesan IQ, Genaro KF, Berretin-Felix G, Rehder MIBC. Avaliação miofuncional orofacial - protocolo MBGR. Rev CEFAC. 2009;11(2):237-55.. It is worth noting that the population of the other study2424 Marchesan IQ, Genaro KF, Berretin-Felix G, Rehder MIBC. Avaliação miofuncional orofacial - protocolo MBGR. Rev CEFAC. 2009;11(2):237-55. was composed of children from public and private schools, and not from public schools only, as in this study.

Other studies22 Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Análise dos processos fonológicos em crianças com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(1):36-40. 2020 Silva MK, Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Aquisição fonológica do Português Brasileiro em crianças do Rio de Janeiro. J. Soc. Bras. Fonoaudiol. [periódico na internet]. 2012 [Acesso em 20 de abril de 2013]; 24(3):248-54. Disponível em: www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S2179-64912012000300010
http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=s...
indicate that children with a typical phonological development between the ages of three and four years, use more frequently consonant cluster reduction, lateralization, and final consonant deletion processes. It is worth noting that, in the case of this study, the reduced children sample aged two to three years may limit interpretation of the results.

A study that investigated the prevalence of phonological disorders in seven-year-old children showed that the most frequent processes in children with phonological changes are simplification of consonant cluster, liquid consonant simplification, devoicing of fricatives, devoicing of plosives and elimination of final consonant77 Patah LK, Takiuchi N. Prevalência das alterações fonológicas e uso de processos fonológicos em escolares aos 7 anos. Rev CEFAC. 2008;10(2):158-67.. These data demonstrate similarities with typical phonological development observed in children in this study, but they also ratify important differences such as the absence of devoicing of fricatives and plosives that only happen in the event of a deviant phonological development1111 Wertzner HF. Fonologia: Desenvolvimento e Alterações. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. Ed. Roca, 2004. p. 772-86. 1717 Wertzner HF. Distúrbio Fonológico. In: Andrade, CRF, Marcondes, E. Fonoaudiologia em Pediatria. São Paulo: Sarvier, 2003, p. 70-79..

As can be seen in Table 2, syllable reduction, consonant harmony, plosivation of fricative, velar fricative simplification and liquid simplification phonological processes were overcome late if compared to the expected age according to the literature used as reference1111 Wertzner HF. Fonologia: Desenvolvimento e Alterações. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. Ed. Roca, 2004. p. 772-86. 1717 Wertzner HF. Distúrbio Fonológico. In: Andrade, CRF, Marcondes, E. Fonoaudiologia em Pediatria. São Paulo: Sarvier, 2003, p. 70-79..

When performing a comparison between the results found and the literature, it can be seen that the elimination of the syllable reduction process proved to be very late in this study (6:0 to 6:5 years range). For some authors, this process must be overcome at around 2:6 years1717 Wertzner HF. Distúrbio Fonológico. In: Andrade, CRF, Marcondes, E. Fonoaudiologia em Pediatria. São Paulo: Sarvier, 2003, p. 70-79.. Other authors1818 Lima LM, Queiroga BAM. Aquisição fonológica em crianças com antecedentes de desnutrição. Rev CEFAC. 2007;9(1):13-20., who investigated phonological acquisition in children with malnutrition antecedents, stated that the syllable reduction process was eliminated at about 5:6 years of age. Thus, it can be stated that, in the case of this study, there was a significant delay in the elimination of this process by the studied population.

Similarly, a significant delay was also observed in relation to the elimination of the fricative plosivation process, which happened in the speech of children in this study until 4:11 years old. According to the literature, this process must be overcome at around 2:6 years1717 Wertzner HF. Distúrbio Fonológico. In: Andrade, CRF, Marcondes, E. Fonoaudiologia em Pediatria. São Paulo: Sarvier, 2003, p. 70-79.. Other authors claim that this process will only disappear after the age of 32525 Rosal AGC, Cordeiro AAA, Queiroga BAM. Consciência fonológica e o desenvolvimento do sistema fonológico e crianças de escolas públicas e particulares. Rev CEFAC. 2013;15(4):837-46..

In the case of velar fricative simplification process, it was surpassed by around 4:0 to 4:5 years. There are references in the literature indicating that velar simplification is eliminated at about three years old1717 Wertzner HF. Distúrbio Fonológico. In: Andrade, CRF, Marcondes, E. Fonoaudiologia em Pediatria. São Paulo: Sarvier, 2003, p. 70-79. 2626 Pena-Brooks A, Hedge MN. Assessment and treatment of articulation and phonological disorders in children - A dual-level text. Pro-ed an international Publisher. Austin, Texas, 2000..

The results of this study indicate that posteriorization to velar, posteriorization to palatal, frontalization to velar and frontalization of palatal processes showed to have means too low to be considered as typical phonological development processes1111 Wertzner HF. Fonologia: Desenvolvimento e Alterações. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. Ed. Roca, 2004. p. 772-86.. It is noteworthy that the instrument used for the phonological evaluation (FonET) has target-words selected in order to include all phonemes of Portuguese, considering the position in the syllable and tone, having thus the possibility of the occurrence of such processes.

According to the literature, these processes are typical of development and are eliminated at about 4:6 years1717 Wertzner HF. Distúrbio Fonológico. In: Andrade, CRF, Marcondes, E. Fonoaudiologia em Pediatria. São Paulo: Sarvier, 2003, p. 70-79.. For other authors1818 Lima LM, Queiroga BAM. Aquisição fonológica em crianças com antecedentes de desnutrição. Rev CEFAC. 2007;9(1):13-20., investigating the phonological acquisition in children with malnutrition antecedents, the frontalization of velar, frontalization of palatal, posteriorization to velar and posteriorization for palatal processes disappear for about 4 years.

The devoicing of plosives and devoicing of fricatives processes also showed a greatly reduced occurrence. These, respectively, are commonly found in children with phonological disorders77 Patah LK, Takiuchi N. Prevalência das alterações fonológicas e uso de processos fonológicos em escolares aos 7 anos. Rev CEFAC. 2008;10(2):158-67. 2727 Yavas M. Padrões de aquisição da fonologia do português. Letras de Hoje. 1988;23(3):7-30. and, when surveyed in other regions of the country22 Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Análise dos processos fonológicos em crianças com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(1):36-40., did not influence typical phonological development.

Regarding consonant cluster simplification process, it was observed that it was not overcome in any of the age groups studied. According to the literature, this process should disappear around the age of 52626 Pena-Brooks A, Hedge MN. Assessment and treatment of articulation and phonological disorders in children - A dual-level text. Pro-ed an international Publisher. Austin, Texas, 2000.. It may appear in children over 7 years22 Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Análise dos processos fonológicos em crianças com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(1):36-40.. It is important to note that the acquisition of consonant cluster is very influenced by the socio-cultural environment. Phonological productions such as [fror] instead of /flor/, [peda] instead of /pedra/, [pranta] instead of /planta/ can be observed.

These results point to the importance of the knowledge of a normal phonological development in populations with socio-cultural specificities, especially in different linguistic varieties of the same language, so that misinterpretations between what is linguistic variety and what is phonological disorders are avoided in the diagnosis of speech disorders. With this, it is fundamental to conduct further studies in order to verify the influence of language varieties spoken in Brazil on the process of phonological acquisition.

Conclusion

The most frequent phonological processes were consonant cluster simplification, liquid consonant simplification, syllable reduction and simplification of final consonant. It was observed that syllable reduction, consonant harmony and plosivation of fricative phonological processes were overcome late. On the other hand, liquid consonant simplification, frontalization of palatal and posteriorization of palatal processes were eliminated before the expected age. The consonant cluster simplification process was not overcome in any group, showing the influence of socio-cultural issues.

The data suggest a possible influence of linguistic variety, which should be considered in the evaluation of phonological development, especially in children of the metropolitan region of Recife, being essential that studies further explore this acquisition in different regions of the country.

Given the above, it is concluded that the sociolinguistic context should be considered in children's phonological evaluation processes to prevent any mistaken diagnosis of phonological disorders or regional linguistic variety prejudices, as difficulties related to phonology can hinder the development of language in general, affecting one or more of its subsystems. Therefore, it is important to evaluate and understand each of these subsystems separately, considering the importance of the influence of phonological development to the success of both the understanding and the expression of language.

Referências

  • 1
    Lamprecht RR. Sobre os desvios fonológicos. In: Lamprecht RR. (org.) Aquisição fonológica do português: perfil de desenvolvimento e subsídios para terapia. Porto Alegre: ARTMED, 2004. p.193-212.
  • 2
    Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Análise dos processos fonológicos em crianças com desenvolvimento fonológico normal. Rev Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2009;14(1):36-40.
  • 3
    Othero GA. Processos fonológicos na aquisição da linguagem pela criança. ReVEL [periódico na internet]. 2005 [acesso em 10 de março]; 3(5): 1-13. Disponível em: http://www.revel.inf.br/files/artigos/revel_5_processos_fonologicos.pdf
    » http://www.revel.inf.br/files/artigos/revel_5_processos_fonologicos.pdf
  • 4
    Befi-Lopes DM, Cáceres AM, Araújo K. Aquisição de verbos em pré-escolares falantes do português brasileiro. Rev CEFAC. 2007;9(4):444-52.
  • 5
    Spíndola RA, Payão LMC, Bandini HHM. Abordagem fonoaudiológica em desvios fonológicos fundamentada na hierarquia dos traços distintivos e na consciência fonológica. Rev CEFAC. 2007;9(2):180-9.
  • 6
    Mezzomo CL, Mota HB, Dias RF, Giacchini V; O uso da estratégia de alongamento compensatório em crianças com desenvolvimento fonológico normal e desviante. Letras de Hoje. 2008;43(3):35-41.
  • 7
    Patah LK, Takiuchi N. Prevalência das alterações fonológicas e uso de processos fonológicos em escolares aos 7 anos. Rev CEFAC. 2008;10(2):158-67.
  • 8
    Toreti G, Ribas LP. Aquisição fonológica: descrição longitudinal dos dados de fala de uma criança com desenvolvimento típico. Letrônica. 2010;3(1):42-61.
  • 9
    Ghisleni MRL, Keske-Soares M, Mezzomo CL. O uso das estratégias de reparo considerando a gravidade do desvio fonológico evolutivo. Rev CEFAC. 2010;12(5):766-71.
  • 10
    Acosta VM, Moreno A, Ramos V, Quintana A, Espino O. V. Avaliação da linguagem: teoria e prática do processo de avaliação do comportamento linguístico infantil. São Paulo: Santos, 2003. 214 p.
  • 11
    Wertzner HF. Fonologia: Desenvolvimento e Alterações. In: Ferreira LP, Befi-Lopes DM, Limongi SCO. Tratado de Fonoaudiologia. Ed. Roca, 2004. p. 772-86.
  • 12
    Vítor RM, Cardoso-Martins C. Desenvolvimento fonológico de crianças pré-escolares da Região Noroeste de Belo Horizonte. Psicol Rev. 2007;13(2):383-98.
  • 13
    Caumo DTM, Ferreira MIDC. Relação entre desvios fonológicos e processamento auditivo. Rev. soc. bras. fonoaudiol. [periódico na internet]. 2009 [Acesso em 14 de janeiro de 2010]; 14(2):234-40. Disponível em: www.scielo.br/scielo.php?pid=S151680342009000200015&script=sci_arttext
    » http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php?pid=S151680342009000200015&script=sci_arttext
  • 14
    Nunes DA, Payão LMC, Costa RCC. Desvios fonológicos na educação infantil. Rev. CEFAC [periódico na Internet]. 2010 [acesso em 10 de maio de 2011]; 12(2):331-6. Disponível em: www.scielo.br/pdf/rcefac/v12n2/21.pdf
    » http://www.scielo.br/pdf/rcefac/v12n2/21.pdf
  • 15
    Wiethan FM, Mota HB. Propostas terapêuticas para os desvios fonológicos: diferentes soluções para o mesmo problema. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13(3):541-51.
  • 16
    Haupt C, Aguiar PG. Variações fonético-fonológicas e desvios fonológicos - um estudo de caso. Revista do Curso de Letras da UNIABEU [periódico na internet]. 2013 [Acesso em 5 de junho de 2013]; 4(2):12-25. Disponível em: www.uniabeu.edu.br/publica/index.php/RE/article/view/716/pdf_374
    » http://www.uniabeu.edu.br/publica/index.php/RE/article/view/716/pdf_374
  • 17
    Wertzner HF. Distúrbio Fonológico. In: Andrade, CRF, Marcondes, E. Fonoaudiologia em Pediatria. São Paulo: Sarvier, 2003, p. 70-79.
  • 18
    Lima LM, Queiroga BAM. Aquisição fonológica em crianças com antecedentes de desnutrição. Rev CEFAC. 2007;9(1):13-20.
  • 19
    Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Aquisição fonológica de crianças de classe sócio econômica alta. Rev CEFAC. 2008;10(4):452-60.
  • 20
    Silva MK, Ferrante C, Borsel JV, Pereira MMB. Aquisição fonológica do Português Brasileiro em crianças do Rio de Janeiro. J. Soc. Bras. Fonoaudiol. [periódico na internet]. 2012 [Acesso em 20 de abril de 2013]; 24(3):248-54. Disponível em: www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S2179-64912012000300010
    » http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S2179-64912012000300010
  • 21
    Schneider MN. Variação e discriminação linguística no ensino e aprendizagem de línguas em comunidades bilíngues. Calidoscópio. 2009;7:76-82.
  • 22
    Queiroga BAM, Alves JM, Cordeiro AAA, Montenegro ACA, Asfora R. Aquisição dos encontros consonantais por crianças falantes do português não padrão da região metropolitana do Recife. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13(2):214-26.
  • 23
    Queiroga BAM, Alves JM, Cordeiro AAA, Montenegro ACA, Asfora R. Aquisição dos encontros consonantais por crianças falantes do português não padrão da região metropolitana do Recife. Rev. CEFAC. 2011;13(2):214-26.
  • 24
    Marchesan IQ, Genaro KF, Berretin-Felix G, Rehder MIBC. Avaliação miofuncional orofacial - protocolo MBGR. Rev CEFAC. 2009;11(2):237-55.
  • 25
    Rosal AGC, Cordeiro AAA, Queiroga BAM. Consciência fonológica e o desenvolvimento do sistema fonológico e crianças de escolas públicas e particulares. Rev CEFAC. 2013;15(4):837-46.
  • 26
    Pena-Brooks A, Hedge MN. Assessment and treatment of articulation and phonological disorders in children - A dual-level text. Pro-ed an international Publisher. Austin, Texas, 2000.
  • 27
    Yavas M. Padrões de aquisição da fonologia do português. Letras de Hoje. 1988;23(3):7-30.
  • 28
    Wertzner HF, Pagan LO, Galea DES, Papp ACCS. Características fonológicas de crianças com transtorno fonológico com e sem histórico de otite média. Rev. Soc. Bras. Fonoaudiol [periódico na internet]. 2007 [Acesso em 13 de janeiro de 2013]; 12(1): 41-7. Disponível em: www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1516-80342007000100009.
    » http://www.scielo.br/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1516-80342007000100009
  • Source of Research Grants: CAPES; FACEPE

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Oct 2015

History

  • Received
    11 Nov 2014
  • Accepted
    21 Apr 2015
ABRAMO Associação Brasileira de Motricidade Orofacial Rua Uruguaiana, 516, Cep 13026-001 Campinas SP Brasil, Tel.: +55 19 3254-0342 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistacefac@cefac.br