Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

The performance of elementary public and private school students pre and post phonological intervention

ABSTRACT

Purpose:

this study aims to compare the performance, pre and post phonological intervention, of 2nd year students in public and private education, with and without learning disabilities.

Methods:

30 students from the 2nd year of elementary education, public and private participated in these study, distributed in: GI, GII and GIII, composed of 15 public school students, submitted to pre and post testing and phonological intervention; and GIV, GV and GVI, comprising 15 private school students, submitted to pre and post testing and phonological intervention. In the pre and post testing moment, the Linguistic Cognitive Skills Assessment Protocol Adaptive was performed. For the intervention, the phonological tasks of letter/sound relationship, analysis, synthesis and manipulation of phonemes and syllables were conducted.

Results:

there was a statistically significant performance for the students submitted to the phonological intervention program of the public and private education, in all skills analyzed.

Conclusion:

the intervention was effective in both scopes, public and private, however, private school students did better in a greater number of skills analyzed as compared to public school ones, suggesting the influence of the intervention associated with the stimuli offered in the process of schooling.

Keywords:
Child Language; Learning; Intervention; Reading; Child

RESUMO

Objetivo:

comparar o desempenho pré e pós intervenção fonológica de escolares do 2° ano do ensino público e privado, com e sem dificuldades de aprendizagem.

Método

s:participaram deste estudo 30 escolares do 2° ano do ensino fundamental, público e privado, distribuídos em: GI, GII e GIII, composto por 15 escolares do ensino público, submetidos à pré e pós testagem e intervenção fonológica; e GIV, GV e GVI, composto por 15 escolares do ensino privado, submetidos à pré e pós testagem e intervenção fonológica. Em situação de pré e pós testagem foi realizada aplicação do Protocolo de Avaliação das Habilidades Cognitivo Linguística adaptado. O processo de intervenção envolveu tarefas fonológicas de relação letra/som, análise, síntese e manipulação de fonemas e sílabas.

Resultados:

houve melhor desempenho, com diferença estatisticamente significante, para os escolares submetidos ao programa de intervenção fonológica do ensino público e privado, em todas as habilidades analisadas.

Conclusão:

a intervenção foi eficaz em ambos os âmbitos, no entanto, os escolares de ensino privado apresentaram desempenho superior em maior número de habilidades em comparação aos escolares de ensino público, sugerindo a influência da intervenção associada aos estímulos ofertados no processo de escolarização.

Descritores:
Linguagem Infantil; Aprendizagem; Intervenção; Leitura; Criança

Introduction

Learning is a continuous process that lasts throughout the life of an individual and can be divided basically into formal and informal learning. Among these processes can be mentioned the literacy, which is understood as a formal learning, obtained in most cases, in the educational field11. Tabile AF, Jacometo MCD. Fatores influenciadores no processo de aprendizagem: um estudo de caso. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2017;34(103):75-86..

The distinction between public and private education in national territory has raised some questions, such as on the form and content offered to students, quality of education, capacity and pedagogical professionalism. As well, the maneuvers and paths followed by educators to deal with students who cannot keep up with the literacy process, whether for sociocultural and/or neurological issues22. Marques ACTL, Marandino M. Alfabetização científica, criança e espaços de educação não formal: diálogos possíveis. Educação e Pesquisa. 2018;44: e170831..

In fact, during the literacy process, some students may have learning difficulties that prevent them from following the performance of their peers, making it necessary to identify what may be delaying this process. This demand has been identified as growing in recent years, both in public and private schools33. Novaes CB, Mishima F, Santos PL. Treinamento breve de consciência fonológica: impacto sobre a alfabetização. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2013;30(93):189-200..

Individuals with learning difficulties tend to respond appropriately, when the causative factors of the same are worked, so that they can be adapted to meet the specific needs of each school. Getting success often in conclusion of the literacy process without the need for long periods of intervention44. Pinheiro L, Correa J, Mousinho R. A eficácia de estratégias de remediação fonoaudiológica na avaliação das dificuldades de aprendizagem. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2012;29(89):215-25.

5. Neefa NE, Müller B, Liebig J, Schaadt G, Grigutsch M, Gunter TC et al. Dyslexia risk gene relates to representation of sound in the auditory brainstem. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;24:63-71.
-66. Michelino SM, Cardoso AD, Silva PB, Macedo EC. Desempenho em testes psicopedagógicos e neuropsicológicos de crianças e adolescentes com dislexia do desenvolvimento e dificuldade de aprendizagem. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2017;34(104):111-25..

Learning disabilities are characterized by changes of more comprehensive, being present in changes of one or more skills, such as auditory processing, visual processing, logical reasoning, processing speed, and metalinguistic skills. It may or may not be associated with previous phonological alterations, as well as other comorbidities, becoming of fundamental importance to the early identification of the symptoms to obtain efficacy in the treatment66. Michelino SM, Cardoso AD, Silva PB, Macedo EC. Desempenho em testes psicopedagógicos e neuropsicológicos de crianças e adolescentes com dislexia do desenvolvimento e dificuldade de aprendizagem. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2017;34(104):111-25.,77. Lonigan CJ, Purpura DJ, Wilson SB, Walker PM, Clancy-Menchetti J. Evaluating the components of an emergent literacy intervention for preschool children at risk for reading difficulties. J Exp Child Psychol. 2013;114(1):111-30..

In this way, the main objective of the intervention is to maximize the process of reading and writing learning, with the aim of reducing the damages caused by these deficits during literacy. In addition to encouraging students with learning difficulties, favoring the overcoming of mishaps through activities that can be developed in the educational field, also allows the early identification of those who need a targeted clinical intervention or, even, a multidisciplinary evaluation for the closing of future diagnosis88. Silva NSM, Crennite PAP. Performance of children at risk for reading difficulties submitted to an intervention program. CoDAS. 2016;28(5):517-25.,99. Nicolielo-Carrilho AP, Hage SRV. Metacognitive reading strategies of children with learning disabilities. CoDAS. 2017;29(3):e20160091..

The intervention in the initial years of literacy, based on the stimulation of predictive abilities for learning, provides better performance in this process, aiming the development of phonological awareness. These skills include letter/sound, rhyme, alliteration and syllabic segmentation, as well as auditory and visual discrimination of sounds, processing speed for stimuli, letter sequence naming and fluent reading1010. Fukuda MTM, Capellini SA. Programa de intervenção fonológica associado à correspondência grafema-fonema em escolares de risco para a dislexia. Psicol Refl Crít. 2012;4(25):783-90.

11. Toll AWM, Van Luit JEW. Accelerating the early numeracy development of kindergartners with limited working memory skills through remedial education. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(2):745-55.

12. Lawton T. Improving dorsal stream function in dyslexics by training figure/ground motion discrimination improves attention, reading fluency, and working memory. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:397.
-1313. Männel C, Schaadt G, Illner FK, van der Meer E, Friederici AD. Phonological abilities in literacy-impaired children: brain potentials reveal deficient phoneme discrimination, but intact prosodic processing. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;23:14-25..

Students with learning difficulties can be identified in the initial literacy process, which is completed only in the third year of elementary school. The implementation of early intervention in education setting can provide an evolution or overcoming the frame of learning difficulties, since it makes it possible to identify among students with below-expected performance, in relation to their class-group, those who have learning difficulties and those who are at risk for some specific learning disorder77. Lonigan CJ, Purpura DJ, Wilson SB, Walker PM, Clancy-Menchetti J. Evaluating the components of an emergent literacy intervention for preschool children at risk for reading difficulties. J Exp Child Psychol. 2013;114(1):111-30.,1414. Silva C, Capellini SA. Efficacy of phonological intervention program in students at risk for dyslexia. Rev. CEFAC. 2015;17(6):1827-37..

In view of the above, this study aimed to compare pre and post phonological intervention performance of students from the 2nd year of public and private education, with and without learning difficulties.

Methods

This research project was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Federal Fluminense University - CEP/ Polo of Nova Friburgo-RJ under protocol number 1.800.368. The Informed Consent Term was signed by those responsible for the children, according to resolution of the National Health Council CNS 466/12, and the Informed Agreement Term, signed by the participants of the research.

This study included 30 students from the 2nd year of elementary school attending public and private education, of both genders, aged 7 to 8 years, distributed in the following groups:

  • Group I (GI): five students without learning difficulties, regularly enrolled in public education, submitted to pre and post testing;

  • Group II (GII): five students without learning difficulties, regularly enrolled in public education, submitted to pre-testing, phonological intervention and post testing;

  • Group III (GIII): five students with learning difficulties, regularly enrolled in public education, submitted to pre-testing, phonological intervention and post testing;

  • Group IV (GIV): five students without learning difficulties, regularly enrolled in private education, submitted to pre and post testing;

  • Group V (GV): five students without learning difficulties, regularly enrolled in private education, submitted to pre-testing, phonological intervention and post testing;

  • Group VI (GVI): five students with learning difficulties, regularly enrolled in private education, submitted to pre-testing, phonological intervention and post testing

All groups were matched according to schooling. The inclusion criteria were absence of related complaints or indicators of hearing, vision or neurological disorders, behavioral or cognitive. The exclusion criteria were the non-signing of informed consent term by parents or guardians, presence of complaints of auditory and/or visual alterations, and presence of neurological, behavioral or cognitive disorders. The information regarding the criteria was taken from school records.

The students were submitted to pre-testing, phonological intervention and post testing, according to the division of the groups previously described. All data collection was carried out in the educational setting, at regular school hours, with the authorization of the principal and teachers.

The collection was made in the second semester, allowing the students to have prior knowledge of the school contents proposed in literacy. The pre and post tests were conducted in four sessions, two for the pre and two for post testing, the phonological intervention done in 10 sessions, twice a week, for two months. Both the evaluation and the intervention were performed, individually, by the researcher, with intervention activities directed to assessment items.

In the pre-testing moment, the Cognitive-Linguistic Skills Assessment Protocol was used, in its collective and individual version1515. Capellini SA, Smythe I, Silva C. Protocolo de avaliação das habilidades cognitivo-linguísticas. Ribeirão Preto: Booktoy, 2017., adapted for this population. The collective version of the instrument is composed by the subtests of Writing the name; Writing the alphabet in sequence; Copying of forms; Dictation of words and pseudo words; Dictation of figures and Dictation of numbers.

The individual version1515. Capellini SA, Smythe I, Silva C. Protocolo de avaliação das habilidades cognitivo-linguísticas. Ribeirão Preto: Booktoy, 2017. of the Assessment Protocol is composed of subtests Alphabet Recognition in sequence; Alphabet Recognition in random order; Reading of words; Non-word reading; Rhyme; Alliteration; Syllabic segmentation; Hearing discrimination; Repetition of words; Repetition of nonwords; Inverted numbers; Speed Naming of Figures and Speed Naming of Numbers.

The results of the subtests of the collective and individual version were obtained by assigning one (1) point to each correct answer of the student, and the application of the procedures that comprehend the evaluation protocol was performed in two sessions, with duration of 50 minutes each.

The phonological intervention involved the following tasks: Alphabet recognition and identification of the sound of letters in sequence; Alphabet recognition and identification of the sound of letters, in a random order; Identification and production of rhyme using words and phrases; Identification and manipulation of words; Identification and manipulation of syllables; Segmentation and Analysis of syllables; Identification of phonemes; Segmentation of phonemes; Replacement and Analysis of phonemes; Identification and Discrimination of phonemes with texts and phrases.

Ten sessions of cumulative character were performed, with an average duration of 40 minutes each, for the development of phonological intervention. The results of the activities were obtained by means of point for correct answers, that is, score achieved by correct answers.

The results of this study were submitted to statistical analysis for greater reliability. The quantitative variables were presented by mean and standard deviation. The test used for normality of the data was the Shapiro-Wilk test. For comparison of pre and post testing between the groups, the Wilcoxon test was used, by adopting a p<0,05 significance level, and using the Stata version 11.0 as the statistical program.

Results

The data were grouped by abilities, such as reading, writing, phonological awareness, auditory processing, visual processing and processing speed, aiming at facilitating the analysis of the results and the comparison between the groups studied.

The results of the reading ability (Table 1) show a statistically significant difference for word reading for the GIV, GV and GVI groups, when reading pseudo words for the GV group and in words read correctly in 1 (one) minute for the GIII group.

Table 1:
Comparison of the pre and post testing of students of GI, GII, GIII, GIV, GV and GVI in the reading ability

In Table 2, regarding the writing ability, there was a statistically significant difference for writing the alphabet in the GIV, GV and GVI groups, dictation of pseudo words for GIV and total dictation for GIII and GIV.

Table 2:
Comparison of the pre and post testing of students of GI, GII, GIII, GIV, GV and GVI in the writing ability

In the phonological awareness ability, a statistically significant difference was found in the comparison of GI, GII, GIII, GIV, GV and GVI, at the moment of pre and post testing, for the alliteration variable for GVI, and rhyme, for GII.

Table 3:
Comparison of the pre and post testing of students of GI, GII, GIII, GIV, GV and GVI in the phonological awareness skills

In Table 4, for auditory processing ability, there was a statistically significant difference for GVI in auditory discrimination, GI in word repetition, GII in repetition of nonwords and GIII in inverted numbers.

Table 4:
Comparison of the pre and post testing of students of GI, GII, GIII, GIV, GV and GVI in the auditory processing ability

The performance of students in visual processing ability (Table 5) show a statistically significant difference for visual memory of forms, for GIV. For the speed processing ability, there was a significance for GI, GIII and GV in speed naming of figures, GIII in speed naming of numbers in first naming and GIV and GV in speed naming of numbers in the second naming.

Table 5:
Comparison of the pre and post testing performance of students of GI, GII, GIII, GIV, GV and GVI in the visual processing and processing speed abilities

Discussion

The results of this study allowed the comparison of the performance of students in public and private education, in relation to the cognitive-linguistic performance test, in order to verify the performance in pre and post testing, after exposure to a phonological intervention program.

Thus, it can be seen, in Table 1, that there was a decrease in the means of performance of the GIV, GV and GVI students, attending the private education, for the words reading test (LP). For the GV group there was a decrease in the mean performance of the pseudo word reading test (LPs). The decrease of the means of performance for the analysis of the LP and LPs variables is favorable, since this variable is analyzed by the time spent reading words and pseudo words, thus, the decrease of the means of performance indicates that the students demanded less time for the decoding, performing the test more quickly and effectively. The results suggest that GV and GVI students were influenced by the intervention program with phonological abilities, since the students were exposed to intervention with predictive abilities to acquire reading. However, the performance of GIV suggests an improvement in its performance due to the influence of schooling, because it was not submitted to the intervention, but because it is part of the private educational context that develops skills to acquire writing from early grades33. Novaes CB, Mishima F, Santos PL. Treinamento breve de consciência fonológica: impacto sobre a alfabetização. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2013;30(93):189-200.,1414. Silva C, Capellini SA. Efficacy of phonological intervention program in students at risk for dyslexia. Rev. CEFAC. 2015;17(6):1827-37..

The performance of students who undergo phonological intervention reflects the expansion of lexical memory, which favors the correct decoding of words in a shorter time. Being that the best performance for the reading of pseudo words can be supported directly in the development of the phonological awareness, allowing the access and use of the phonological route by these students99. Nicolielo-Carrilho AP, Hage SRV. Metacognitive reading strategies of children with learning disabilities. CoDAS. 2017;29(3):e20160091.,1616. Silva C, Capellini SA. Comparison of performance in metalinguistic tasks among students with and without risk of dyslexia. J. Hum. Growth Dev. 2017;27(2):198-205..

In relation to the students of the GIII group, belonging to the public school, there was an increase in the average performance in the test of words read correctly in one minute (Cor1m), indicating that the initial difficulty in the decoding of symbols was supplied for the execution of this activity. Indicating the influence of the intervention of phonological basis, which allowed the students with learning difficulties to improve their performance in decoding real words55. Neefa NE, Müller B, Liebig J, Schaadt G, Grigutsch M, Gunter TC et al. Dyslexia risk gene relates to representation of sound in the auditory brainstem. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;24:63-71.,1717. Silva C, Capellini SA. Desempenho de escolares com e sem transtorno de aprendizagem em leitura, escrita, consciência fonológica, velocidade de processamento e memória de trabalho fonológica. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2013;30(91):3-11..

The improvement in performance in reading ability tests is also reflected in the tests proposed by the processing speed ability, presented in Table 5. Since, the students in the public education, the GI and GIII groups showed a decrease in performance averages, depending on the execution time, for the rapid naming of figure test (NRF); as well as, GIII decreases its performance in rapid naming of numbers in the first naming (NRN1). Indicating that the students demanded less time to access the information requested, performing the test faster and more accurately.

For students with learning difficulties it is suggested that the best performance was the result of the ability to decode the presented information quickly and accurately, an ability developed along with improved access to lexical memory for visual input and image recognition, and immediate to numbers. In relation to students of public education who were not submitted to intervention, we suggest the development of the ability to decode stimuli resulting from the learning process provided by teaching school, due to exposure to new concepts and stimuli, in addition to maturational cognitive development1818. Machado AC, Capellini SA. Tutoring in reading and writing based on the RTI - response to intervention model in children with developmental dyslexia. Rev. CEFAC. 2014;16(4):1161-67..

However, for GIV and GV students in private education, the improvement in performance was characterized by a decrease of the means in the rapid naming of numbers test in second naming (NRN2), suggesting that exposure to continuous stimuli did not generate overload memory. Data that this indicates that the capacity of processing, storage and recovery of students without learning difficulties develops together with school learning, regardless of whether or not submitted to intervention1919. Oliveira AM, Germano GD, Capellini SA. Students' performance in letter identification and lexical process tests. Rev. CEFAC. 2016;18(5):1121-32..

In the ability of phonological awareness, belonging to Table 3, students of GVI presented superior performance in the alliteration test (Alit) and the students of GII for the rhyme test. The results suggest that the students with learning difficulties of the private education suffered the joint influence of the phonological intervention, associated to the literacy process offered in educational scope. As for students of public education, is also verified the influence of phonological intervention on the perception of sonorous segments worked directly with the task of letter/sound relationship, identification and production of rhymes, and belonging to the literacy process, identified in the speed of processing continuous stimuli44. Pinheiro L, Correa J, Mousinho R. A eficácia de estratégias de remediação fonoaudiológica na avaliação das dificuldades de aprendizagem. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2012;29(89):215-25.,2020. Silva C, Cunha VLO, Pinheiro FH, Capellini SA. Nomeação rápida, leitura e compreensão em escolares com dificuldades de aprendizagem. J. Soc. Bras. Fonoaudiol. 2012;24(4):355-60..

In relation to auditory processing ability (Table 4), there was an increase in the mean performance of GVI students, for the sound discrimination test (DS). For students in public education, there was an increase in the means for the repetition of words test (RepP) for GI, repetition of non-words (RepNP) for GII and inverted numbers (NInv) for GIII1616. Silva C, Capellini SA. Comparison of performance in metalinguistic tasks among students with and without risk of dyslexia. J. Hum. Growth Dev. 2017;27(2):198-205..

The systematic learning of phonological skills through phonological intervention has potentiated such abilities, allowing the development of auditory discrimination, that is, the development of these abilities occurred in parallel and joint. Thus, the improvement in the sound discrimination reflects in the phonological awareness, generating a reciprocal performance between the involved abilities, causing an increase of the means of performance of the GVI students1313. Männel C, Schaadt G, Illner FK, van der Meer E, Friederici AD. Phonological abilities in literacy-impaired children: brain potentials reveal deficient phoneme discrimination, but intact prosodic processing. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;23:14-25.,1919. Oliveira AM, Germano GD, Capellini SA. Students' performance in letter identification and lexical process tests. Rev. CEFAC. 2016;18(5):1121-32..

For the words repetition test (RepP) there was an increase in the mean performance for the students of the GI, and this group did not undergo intervention. Based on the results, it is possible to deduce that the improvement in performance was due to the fact that the students did not present difficulties and did not require the direct access of the phonological route to perform the proposed test. This fact makes it possible to storage, retrieval and access the information through lexical memory, preserved in this specific population1616. Silva C, Capellini SA. Comparison of performance in metalinguistic tasks among students with and without risk of dyslexia. J. Hum. Growth Dev. 2017;27(2):198-205.,2121. Furnes B, Samuelsson S. Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling: results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learn Individ Differ. 2011;21(1):85-95..

In relation to the non-words repetition test (RepNP), there was an increase in the mean for GII students, indicating that there was an effective access to the phonological route, which allowed to process, store and retrieve the requested information. Such performance may come from the explicit learning of phonological awareness, provided by the intervention program. In the inverted numbers test (NInv) the GIII students had averages higher performance, comparing the pre and post testing, suggesting the expansion of the mechanisms of retention of information by the immediate memory of work2020. Silva C, Cunha VLO, Pinheiro FH, Capellini SA. Nomeação rápida, leitura e compreensão em escolares com dificuldades de aprendizagem. J. Soc. Bras. Fonoaudiol. 2012;24(4):355-60.

21. Furnes B, Samuelsson S. Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling: results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learn Individ Differ. 2011;21(1):85-95.

22. Salles JF, Paulla FV. Compreensão da leitura textual e sua relação com as funções executivas. Educ Rev. 2016;62:53-67.
-2323. Porta ME, Carrada MA, Ison MS. Phonological awareness intervention and attention efficiency in children at risk: evidence of effectiveness on visual attention. CoDAS. 2016;28(3):314-8..

In the writing ability, presented in Table 2, there was an increase in the mean of the performance in the comparison of the GIV, GV and GVI students, belonging to the private education, in the alphabet writing test (EAlf). This result can be justified by the improvement in the retention of information related to long term memory, reflecting the phonological intervention and the maturational development generated by literacy, both for students with learning difficulties and for students with academic performance expected. As well, the superior performance of GIV students in the pseudo words dictation test (DitPP) also signals the learning of phonological awareness, directly interfering in the use of the phonological route in a systematic form2222. Salles JF, Paulla FV. Compreensão da leitura textual e sua relação com as funções executivas. Educ Rev. 2016;62:53-67.,2424. Santos MJ, Barrera SD. Impacto do treino em habilidades de consciência fonológica na escrita de pré-escolares. Psicologia Escolar e Educacional. 2017;21(1):93-102..

The students of the public education in the GIII group, with learning difficulties, obtained a superior performance in the comparison of the means of pre and post testing in relation to the total dictation test (DitT). This result indicates that the development of phonological awareness provides support for the development of reading, such as for coding. Since the writing process derives from the effective performance of decoding, that is, the formation and access to lexical memory act as a positive factor for writing1111. Toll AWM, Van Luit JEW. Accelerating the early numeracy development of kindergartners with limited working memory skills through remedial education. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(2):745-55.,2323. Porta ME, Carrada MA, Ison MS. Phonological awareness intervention and attention efficiency in children at risk: evidence of effectiveness on visual attention. CoDAS. 2016;28(3):314-8.

24. Santos MJ, Barrera SD. Impacto do treino em habilidades de consciência fonológica na escrita de pré-escolares. Psicologia Escolar e Educacional. 2017;21(1):93-102.
-2525. Rosal AGC, Cordeiro AAA, Silva ACF, Silva RL, Queiroga BAM. Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid serial naming for initial learning of writing. Rev. CEFAC. 2016;18(1):74-85..

The results of this study aim to identify the benefits of phonological intervention in both the public and private educational environments, as well as to identify gains for students with learning difficulties in both educational contexts. However, it is necessary to expand the sample of interest, as well as the verification of the real educational influence on the performance of the students who were not submitted to the intervention, in public and private education, to verify if there is a consistency in the increase of averages for specific skills.

Conclusion

The results of the comparison of the students' performance of the analyzed groups allowed us to conclude that private school students presented a superior performance after intervention in Reading, Writing, Phonological Awareness, Visual Processing and Processing Speed skills, acquiring a significant performance in the integrality of the analyzed subtests. In relation to public school students, a superior performance in specific tests of phonological awareness, reading of correct words in one minute, total dictation, auditory processing and processing speed, was observed. These findings allow the conclusion that both student groups presented advances in distinct metalinguistic aspects, when stimulated with phonological based teaching associated to the schooling process.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Tabile AF, Jacometo MCD. Fatores influenciadores no processo de aprendizagem: um estudo de caso. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2017;34(103):75-86.
  • 2
    Marques ACTL, Marandino M. Alfabetização científica, criança e espaços de educação não formal: diálogos possíveis. Educação e Pesquisa. 2018;44: e170831.
  • 3
    Novaes CB, Mishima F, Santos PL. Treinamento breve de consciência fonológica: impacto sobre a alfabetização. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2013;30(93):189-200.
  • 4
    Pinheiro L, Correa J, Mousinho R. A eficácia de estratégias de remediação fonoaudiológica na avaliação das dificuldades de aprendizagem. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2012;29(89):215-25.
  • 5
    Neefa NE, Müller B, Liebig J, Schaadt G, Grigutsch M, Gunter TC et al. Dyslexia risk gene relates to representation of sound in the auditory brainstem. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;24:63-71.
  • 6
    Michelino SM, Cardoso AD, Silva PB, Macedo EC. Desempenho em testes psicopedagógicos e neuropsicológicos de crianças e adolescentes com dislexia do desenvolvimento e dificuldade de aprendizagem. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2017;34(104):111-25.
  • 7
    Lonigan CJ, Purpura DJ, Wilson SB, Walker PM, Clancy-Menchetti J. Evaluating the components of an emergent literacy intervention for preschool children at risk for reading difficulties. J Exp Child Psychol. 2013;114(1):111-30.
  • 8
    Silva NSM, Crennite PAP. Performance of children at risk for reading difficulties submitted to an intervention program. CoDAS. 2016;28(5):517-25.
  • 9
    Nicolielo-Carrilho AP, Hage SRV. Metacognitive reading strategies of children with learning disabilities. CoDAS. 2017;29(3):e20160091.
  • 10
    Fukuda MTM, Capellini SA. Programa de intervenção fonológica associado à correspondência grafema-fonema em escolares de risco para a dislexia. Psicol Refl Crít. 2012;4(25):783-90.
  • 11
    Toll AWM, Van Luit JEW. Accelerating the early numeracy development of kindergartners with limited working memory skills through remedial education. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(2):745-55.
  • 12
    Lawton T. Improving dorsal stream function in dyslexics by training figure/ground motion discrimination improves attention, reading fluency, and working memory. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:397.
  • 13
    Männel C, Schaadt G, Illner FK, van der Meer E, Friederici AD. Phonological abilities in literacy-impaired children: brain potentials reveal deficient phoneme discrimination, but intact prosodic processing. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2017;23:14-25.
  • 14
    Silva C, Capellini SA. Efficacy of phonological intervention program in students at risk for dyslexia. Rev. CEFAC. 2015;17(6):1827-37.
  • 15
    Capellini SA, Smythe I, Silva C. Protocolo de avaliação das habilidades cognitivo-linguísticas. Ribeirão Preto: Booktoy, 2017.
  • 16
    Silva C, Capellini SA. Comparison of performance in metalinguistic tasks among students with and without risk of dyslexia. J. Hum. Growth Dev. 2017;27(2):198-205.
  • 17
    Silva C, Capellini SA. Desempenho de escolares com e sem transtorno de aprendizagem em leitura, escrita, consciência fonológica, velocidade de processamento e memória de trabalho fonológica. Rev. Psicopedagogia. 2013;30(91):3-11.
  • 18
    Machado AC, Capellini SA. Tutoring in reading and writing based on the RTI - response to intervention model in children with developmental dyslexia. Rev. CEFAC. 2014;16(4):1161-67.
  • 19
    Oliveira AM, Germano GD, Capellini SA. Students' performance in letter identification and lexical process tests. Rev. CEFAC. 2016;18(5):1121-32.
  • 20
    Silva C, Cunha VLO, Pinheiro FH, Capellini SA. Nomeação rápida, leitura e compreensão em escolares com dificuldades de aprendizagem. J. Soc. Bras. Fonoaudiol. 2012;24(4):355-60.
  • 21
    Furnes B, Samuelsson S. Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling: results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learn Individ Differ. 2011;21(1):85-95.
  • 22
    Salles JF, Paulla FV. Compreensão da leitura textual e sua relação com as funções executivas. Educ Rev. 2016;62:53-67.
  • 23
    Porta ME, Carrada MA, Ison MS. Phonological awareness intervention and attention efficiency in children at risk: evidence of effectiveness on visual attention. CoDAS. 2016;28(3):314-8.
  • 24
    Santos MJ, Barrera SD. Impacto do treino em habilidades de consciência fonológica na escrita de pré-escolares. Psicologia Escolar e Educacional. 2017;21(1):93-102.
  • 25
    Rosal AGC, Cordeiro AAA, Silva ACF, Silva RL, Queiroga BAM. Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid serial naming for initial learning of writing. Rev. CEFAC. 2016;18(1):74-85.
  • Research carried out in the Health Institute of Nova Friburgo by the Department of Specific Training in Speech and Hearing Therapy - FEF, Federal Fluminense University - UFF - Nova Friburgo, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    08 Apr 2019
  • Date of issue
    2019

History

  • Received
    24 Oct 2018
  • Accepted
    04 Feb 2019
ABRAMO Associação Brasileira de Motricidade Orofacial Rua Uruguaiana, 516, Cep 13026-001 Campinas SP Brasil, Tel.: +55 19 3254-0342 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistacefac@cefac.br