Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Prevalence of risk factors for stuttering among boys: analytical cross-sectional study

Prevalência dos fatores de risco para gagueira entre meninos: estudo transversal analítico

Abstracts

CONTEXT AND OBJECTIVE:

There have been few studies on the risk factors for subgroups of stuttering. The aim of this study was to characterize the risk factors for developmental familial stuttering among boys who stutter and who do not stutter, such as disfluency types, associated quality and communication factors, emotional and physical stress, familial attitudes and personal reactions.

DESIGN AND SETTING:

Analytical cross-sectional study with a control group, performed at the Fluency Studies Laboratory of the Department of Speech and Hearing Disorders of a public university.

METHODS:

The parents of 40 age-matched stuttering and non-stuttering boys took part in this study. The participants were divided into two groups: stuttering children (SC) and non-stuttering children (NSC), with ages between 6 years 0 months and 11 years 11 months. Initially, all of the participants underwent a fluency assessment and then data were gathered using the Protocol for the Risk of Developmental Stuttering.

RESULTS:

There were no differences in the physical stress distribution factors and personal reactions between the groups. Inappropriate familial attitudes were presented by 95% of the SC and 30% of the NSC. Four risk factors analyzed were not shown by the NSC, namely stuttering-like disfluency, quality factors, physical stress and emotional stresses.

CONCLUSIONS:

The findings suggest that the presence of stuttering-like disfluency, associated quality and communication factors, emotional stress and inappropriate family attitudes are important risk factors for familial developmental stuttering among boys.

Stuttering; Risk factors; Speech disorders; Genetics; Speech, language and hearing sciences


CONTEXTO E OBJETIVO:

Há poucos estudos sobre os fatores de risco para os subgrupos de gagueira. O objetivo deste estudo foi caracterizar os fatores de risco para a gagueira desenvolvimental familial em meninos que gaguejam e que não gaguejam como tipologia das disfluências, fatores qualitativos e comunicativos associados, estresse físico e emocional, atitude familiar e reação pessoal.

TIPO DE ESTUDO E LOCAL:

Estudo transversal analítico com um grupo controle, realizado no Laboratório de Estudo da Fluência, que faz parte do Departamento de Fonoaudiologia de uma universidade pública.

MÉTODOS:

Pais de 40 meninos com e sem gagueira pareados por idade fizeram parte do estudo. Os participantes foram divididos em dois grupos: crianças com gagueira e crianças sem gagueira, com idades entre 6 anos 0 meses e 11 anos 11 meses Inicialmente todos os participantes foram submetidos a uma avaliação de fluência e depois os dados foram coletados por meio do Protocolo de Risco para a Gagueira do Desenvolvimento.

RESULTADOS:

Não foi observada diferença nos fatores de estresse físicos e reações pessoais entre os grupos. As atitudes inadequadas familiares foram apresentadas por 95% das crianças com gagueira e 30% das crianças sem gagueira. Quatro fatores de risco analisados não ocorreram nas crianças sem gagueira, a saber, disfluências gagas, fatores qualitativos, estresse físico e emocional.

CONCLUSÕES:

Os achados sugerem que a presença de disfluências gagas, fatores qualitativos e comunicativos associados, estresse emocional e atitude familiar inadequada são fatores de risco importantes para a gagueira desenvolvimental familial em meninos.

Gagueira; Fatores de risco; Distúrbios da fala; Genética; Fonoaudiologia


INTRODUCTION

Stuttering is a multifactorial and complex disorder that results from the influence of many factors, which include genetic predisposition, motor speech skills, linguistic skills and cognitive, emotional and environmental factors.11. Smith A, Kelly E. Stuttering: A dynamic, multifactorial model. In: Curlee RF, Siegel GM. Nature and treatment of stuttering: new directions. Allyn & Bacon; 1997. p. 204-17. It is know that the spectrum of risk factors for stuttering is wide and heterogeneous.22. Ajdacic-Gross V, Vetter S, Müller M, et al. Risk factors for stuttering: a secondary analysis of a large data base. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010;260(4):279-86. Stuttering typically begins during the preschool years, which suggests that many important factors act during this developmental process.33. Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early childhood stuttering: for clinicians by clinicians. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2005.

There is a clinically important reason for identifying stuttering among preschoolers. It has been shown that young stuttering children respond well to direct intervention, thereby helping to prevent the disorder from developing into a more intractable chronic form.44. Bothe AK, Davidow JH, Bramlett RE, Ingham RJ. Stuttering treatment research 1970-2005: I. Systematic review incorporating trial quality assessment of behavioral, cognitive, and related approaches. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2006;15(4):321-41. , 55. Einarsdóttir J, Ingham RJ. The effect of stuttering measurement training on judging stuttering occurrence in preschool children who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2008;33(3):167-79. Therefore, early diagnosis and intervention are important and, for this reason, the risk factors for persistent developmental stuttering need to be investigated.66. Dworzynski K, Remington A, Rijsdijk F, Howell P, Plomin R. Genetic etiology in cases of recovered and persistent stuttering in an unselected, longitudinal sample of young twins. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2007;16(2):169-78.

7. Brosch S, Winkler S. Diagnostik und Therapie bei stotternden Kindern [Diagnostics and therapy of stuttering children]. Laryngorhinootologie. 2008;87(7):511-8; quiz 519-23.
- 88. Oliveira CMC, Souza HA, Santos AC, Cunha D, Giacheti CM. Fatores de risco na gagueira desenvolvimental familial e isolada [Risk factors in the familial and sporadic developmental stuttering]. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13(2):205-13.

A wide range of possible risk factors has been proposed in the literature, including age; gender; type and manner of onset; duration of the disfluency; type of disfluency; associated communicative and qualitative factors; physical and emotional stress; family history of stuttering; personal, familial and social reaction; and family attitudes.99. Andrade CRF. Gagueira infantil: risco, diagnóstico e programas terapêuticos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006.

Gender is an important risk factor for stuttering, given that stuttering is more prevalent among males.33. Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early childhood stuttering: for clinicians by clinicians. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2005. , 1010. Ambrose NG, Cox NJ, Yairi E. The genetic basis of persistence and recovery in stuttering. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1997;40(3):567-80. , 1111. Felsenfeld S, Kirk KM, Zhu G, et al. A study of the genetic and environmental etiology of stuttering in a selected twin sample. Behav Genet. 2000;30(5):359-66. This risk among boys is higher when any communication disorder is present, independent of the family history.88. Oliveira CMC, Souza HA, Santos AC, Cunha D, Giacheti CM. Fatores de risco na gagueira desenvolvimental familial e isolada [Risk factors in the familial and sporadic developmental stuttering]. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13(2):205-13.

Associated qualitative factors, such as body and facial muscle tension, rapid speech rate, uncoordinated breathing and/or vocal intensity and vocal frequency variation may be present together with the disfluency seen among children who stutter.1212. Yairi E, Ambrose NG, Niermann R. The early months of stuttering: a developmental study. J Speech Hear Res. 1993;36(3):521-8.

Regarding physical stress, some authors1111. Felsenfeld S, Kirk KM, Zhu G, et al. A study of the genetic and environmental etiology of stuttering in a selected twin sample. Behav Genet. 2000;30(5):359-66. , 1313. Poulos MG, Webster WG. Family history as a basis for subgrouping people who stutter. J Speech Hear Res. 1991;34(1):5-10. have reported that the origin of sporadic stuttering (without a family history of stuttering) may be found in perinatal or childhood physical trauma that perhaps caused some brain dysfunction. Some examples of the physical stress that occurs just prior to the onset of stuttering have included such conditions as respiratory problems, surgery or illness requiring hospitalization, asthma requiring medical treatment, and acute illness.33. Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early childhood stuttering: for clinicians by clinicians. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2005.

In the same way, emotional stress can contribute towards the beginning of stuttering, such as divorcing of parents, moving, death of a beloved pet, birthdays, family vacations or excessive sibling rivalry.33. Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early childhood stuttering: for clinicians by clinicians. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2005.

Other communicative disorders can occur together with persistent developmental stuttering, like phonological or myofunctional disorders. Moreover, some studies have shown higher scores in vocabulary tests associated with stuttering beginning in young children.33. Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early childhood stuttering: for clinicians by clinicians. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2005. , 1414. Reilly S, Onslow M, Packman A, et al. Predicting stuttering onset by the age of 3 years: a prospective, community cohort study. Pediatrics. 2009;123(1):270-7.

The disfluency of stuttering consists of repetition of sounds or syllables and blockage or prolongation of sounds.1515. Sawyer J, Yairi E. The effect of sample size on the assessment of stuttering severity. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2006;15(1):36-44. , 1616. Suresh R, Ambrose N, Roe C, et al. New complexities in the genetics of stuttering: significant sex-specific linkage signals. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;78(4):554-63.

Several studies over past decades have shown that genetic factors are involved in transmission of susceptibility to stuttering.1717. Kang C, Riazuddin S, Mundorff J, et al. Mutations in the lysosomal enzyme-targeting pathway and persistent stuttering. N Engl J Med. 2010;362(8):677-85. , 1818. Drayna D, Kang C. Genetic approaches to understanding the causes of stuttering. J Neurodev Disord. 2011;3(4):374-80. A positive family history may play an important role in the diagnosis process, since it increases the risk of persistent developmental stuttering.33. Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early childhood stuttering: for clinicians by clinicians. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2005. , 66. Dworzynski K, Remington A, Rijsdijk F, Howell P, Plomin R. Genetic etiology in cases of recovered and persistent stuttering in an unselected, longitudinal sample of young twins. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2007;16(2):169-78.

Family attitudes and inappropriate behavior regarding childhood disfluency can also have an influence through increasing the disruptions exhibited by such children.1919. Oliveira CM, Araujo FG, Vocurca MC. Atitudes verbais e não verbais de pais de crianças com queixa de gagueira [Familiar counseling and its effects on childhood stuttering]. Revista da Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia. 2000;5(7):3-10.

20. Millard SK, Nicholas A, Cook FM. Is parent-child interaction therapy effective in reducing stuttering? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):636-50.
- 2121. Dehqan A, Bakhtiar M, Panahi SS, Ashayeri H. Relationship between stuttering severity in children and their mothers speaking rate. Sao Paulo Med J. 2008;126(1):29-33. Another important point to be considered in this risk factor analysis is the personal reaction. People who are perfectionists or anxious, shy and insecure are more likely to suffer from persistent stuttering when it is associated with other risk factors.99. Andrade CRF. Gagueira infantil: risco, diagnóstico e programas terapêuticos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006.

There have been few studies on the risk factors for subgroups of stuttering. Thus, it is very important to identify risk factors for stuttering and make comparisons with controls in order to check which are the primary risk factors for the disorder. Knowledge of these factors is important in adopting preventive measures and more appropriate treatment for stuttering.

OBJECTIVE

The aim of this study was to characterize the risk factors for developmental familial stuttering among boys who stutter and who do not stutter, such as the type of disfluency, associated quality and communication factors, emotional and physical stress factors, family attitudes and personal reactions.

METHODS

This was an analytical cross-sectional study with a control group that was conducted at the Fluency Studies Laboratory of the Department of the Speech and Hearing Disorders of a public university clinic. This study included boys who stuttered with ages between 6 years 0 months and 11 years 11 months. The present study used a non-probabilistic convenience sample that originally comprised 45 stutterers who were seen at the Fluency Studies Laboratory between April 2012 and September 2012, of whom 36 were children between 6 years 0 months and 11 years 11 months of age, but 9 were girls. Thus, the group was initially composed of 27 boys who stuttered. However, only 20 of them met the inclusion criteria.

The procedure for selecting the 20 boys who did not stutter consisted of recruitment among the students at a public school who met the inclusion criteria. In this manner, the study sample was composed of 40 boys aged 6 years 0 months and 11 years 11 months, of whom 20 were stutterers and 20 were non-stutterers.

The protocol for this study was approved by the institution's Ethics Committee (no 0396/2011), and the adults responsible for these children signed a consent statement.

Subjects

Forty age-matched stuttering and non-stuttering children and their parents took part in this study. The participants who stuttered were invited when attending the Fluency Studies Laboratory, which is part of the speech clinic at a public university. Fluent control participants were recruited through contacting students at a public school in Marília, Brazil, the city where the study was developed.

The participants were divided into two groups: stuttering children (SC) and non-stuttering children (NSC), in order to investigate whether there were any differences in relation to any of the risk factors for persistent developmental stuttering that were analyzed.

The SC group was composed of 20 boys with ages between 6 years 0 months and 11 years 11 months, with a diagnosis of stuttering. The NSC group was composed of 20 boys who were age-matched with the SC.

The inclusion criteria for the two groups were as follows: age between 6 years 0 months and 11 years 11 months, male gender and speakers of Brazilian Portuguese, without any other associated communication, neurological, hearing, visual or cognitive shortfall.

The inclusion criteria for the SC group were as follows: stuttering disorders reported by both parents; developmental stuttering present before 10 years of age; minimum duration of disfluency of 12 months, without remission (persistent); demonstration of stuttering in at least 3% of the syllables in the speech sample obtained by the examiner;2222. Yairi E, Ambrose N. A longitudinal study of stuttering in children: a preliminary report. J Speech Hear Res. 1992;35(4):755-60.

23. Bloodstein O. A handbook on stuttering. Chicago: National Easter Seal Society; 1995.
- 2424. Yairi E, Ambrose N, Cox N. Genetics of stuttering: a critical review. J Speech Hear Res. 1996;39(4):771-84. score of 11 points or more (i.e. severity equivalent to at least "mild") on the Stuttering Severity Instrument 3 (SSI-3).2525. Riley GD. Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults. Austin: Pro Ed; 1994.

The selection criterion for the NSC were as follows: no personal or family history of stuttering and/or cluttering; presentation of not more than two instances of stuttering-like disfluency per 100 syllables of conversational speech; and a total overall score of 10 points or lower (i.e. a severity equivalent of less than "mild") on the Stuttering Severity Instrument 3 (SSI-3).2525. Riley GD. Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults. Austin: Pro Ed; 1994.

Procedures

Initially, all of the boys underwent a fluency assessment to separate them into the two groups (SC and NSC) and then data were gathered using the Protocol for the Risk of Developmental Stuttering (PRGD).2626. Andrade CRF. Perfil da fluência da fala: parâmetros comparativo diferenciado por idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e idosos. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2006.

Speech samples were obtained in situations of spontaneous speech. Each speech sample was audiotaped and contained at least 200 fluent syllables. The samples were transcribed literally.2626. Andrade CRF. Perfil da fluência da fala: parâmetros comparativo diferenciado por idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e idosos. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2006. The types of disfluency were analyzed to differentiate between other disfluencies (OD) and stuttering-like disfluencies (SLD), in accordance with the Fluency Profile Assessment,2626. Andrade CRF. Perfil da fluência da fala: parâmetros comparativo diferenciado por idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e idosos. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2006. and thus to calculate the percentage of stuttered syllables.

The stuttering severity was determined by means of an international instrument (the Stuttering Severity Instrument, SSI-3).2525. Riley GD. Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults. Austin: Pro Ed; 1994. This test assesses the frequency and duration of speech disruptions, and also the presence of physical concomitants associated with these disruptions. Based on these parameters, the stuttering severity index was determined as very mild, mild, moderate, severe or very severe.

Any histories of important risk factors for persistent developmental stuttering, like age, type of disfluency, associated communicative and qualitative factors, physical and emotional stress, family history, family attitudes and personal reactions (Protocol for the Risk of Developmental Stuttering, PRGD)99. Andrade CRF. Gagueira infantil: risco, diagnóstico e programas terapêuticos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006. were elicited from all the participants' parents (SC and NSC groups).

Statistical analysis

The results were expressed as percentages of the presence of each risk factor, analyzed for the two groups. The P values were calculated by means of the chi-square test. Descriptive values below 5% (P value < 0.05) were considered statistically significant. The data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the So cial Sciences (SPSS) 20.0 software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, USA).

RESULTS

With regard to the purpose of this study, the data obtained are presented in table form. Table 1 shows the individuals' ages, the total number of stuttering-like disfluencies (SLDs), the percentage of stuttered syllables (%ss) and the total score of the Stuttering Severity Instrument (SSI-3).2525. Riley GD. Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults. Austin: Pro Ed; 1994.

Table 1.
Means and standard deviations (SD) of age and speech fluency results of the stuttering children (SC, n = 20) versus non-stuttering children (NSC, n = 20)

Table 2 describes the risk factors for persistent developmental stuttering among the SC and NSC. There were no differences in the distribution of physical stress or personal reactions between the groups. Inappropriate family attitudes were shown by 95% of the SC and 30% of the NSC. Four risk factors that were analyzed were not shown by the NSC, i.e. stuttering-like disfluency, quality factors (rapid speech rate, uncoordinated breathing or associated stress), communication factors (phonological or myofunctional disorders) and emotional stress (parental death, parental divorce or familial or parental disease or illness).

Table 2.
Prevalence of risk factors among stuttering and non-stuttering children, with P-values

DISCUSSION

Several studies have shown that, in cases of stuttering, the earlier an intervention is instituted, the more favorable the outcome is.44. Bothe AK, Davidow JH, Bramlett RE, Ingham RJ. Stuttering treatment research 1970-2005: I. Systematic review incorporating trial quality assessment of behavioral, cognitive, and related approaches. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2006;15(4):321-41. , 55. Einarsdóttir J, Ingham RJ. The effect of stuttering measurement training on judging stuttering occurrence in preschool children who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2008;33(3):167-79. Therefore, studying the risk factors for this disorder is very important for clinical practice and for improving the knowledge of developmental stuttering. Our study showed the importance of studying risk factors for developmental stuttering in Brazil.88. Oliveira CMC, Souza HA, Santos AC, Cunha D, Giacheti CM. Fatores de risco na gagueira desenvolvimental familial e isolada [Risk factors in the familial and sporadic developmental stuttering]. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13(2):205-13. , 2727. Oliveira CMC, Souza HA, Santos AC, Cunha DS. Análise dos fatores de risco para gagueira em crianças disfluentes sem recorrência familial [Analysis of the risk factors for stuttering in disfluent children without familial recurrence]. Revista CEFAC. 2012;14(6):1028-35. , 2828. Oliveira CMC, Cunha DS, Santos AC. Fatores de risco para gagueira em crianças disfluentes com recorrência familial [Risk factors for stuttering in disfluent children with familial recurrence]. Audiol Commun Res. 2013;18(1):43-9.

The present study on the risk factors for persistent developmental stuttering forms an important part of the process of diagnosing fluency disorders. These data, together with fluency assessment data will lead to precise diagnosis and support for the therapeutic process.

In this study, some significant factors were associated with familial persistent developmental stuttering among boys, such as stuttering-like disfluency (SLD) and quality, communication and emotional stress factors, as well as family attitudes. This suggests that occurrences of these factors are associated with a higher risk of stuttering for this subgroup. Furthermore, the results showed that there were no differences between the groups (SC versus NSC) with regard to physical stress factors and personal reactions.

Regarding stuttering-like disfluencies (SLDs; i.e. sound-syllable repetition, prolongation and blockage), our study confirmed that observable speech disruption is a central feature of this disorder.2929. Yaruss JS. Assessing quality of life in stuttering treatment outcomes research. J Fluency Disord. 2010;35(3):190-202. Thus, this is considered to be a chronic disorder that involves involuntary disruptions in fluent speech.3030. Tran Y, Blumgart E, Craig A. Subjective distress associated with chronic stuttering. J Fluency Disord. 2011;36(1):17-26. Developmental stuttering presents as a chronic disruption of an individual's ability to produce smooth, effortless and forward-moving speech.3131. de Andrade CR, Cervone LM, Sasso FC. Relationship between the stuttering severity index and speech rate. Sao Paulo Med J. 2003;121(2):81-4. In other words, the principal manifestation of the stuttering, regardless of gender, is stuttering-like disfluencies (SLDs). Therefore, this is not specifically a characteristic of the subgroup of boys.

All the children who stuttered presented at least one of the quality factors, while all the children who did not stutter did not present these, according to their parents' reports. A previous study correlated the associated quality factors, such as body and facial muscle tension, rapid speech rate, uncoordinated breathing and/or vocal intensity and vocal frequency variation, with disfluencies among SC.99. Andrade CRF. Gagueira infantil: risco, diagnóstico e programas terapêuticos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006. In the present study, we confirmed that disfluencies among SC can be followed by quality factors.

The majority of our children who stuttered presented some communication factors, and all the children who did not stutter did not present these, according to their parents' reports. These results are in accordance with another study that reported that children who stuttered frequently presented other associated communication factors such as phonological or myofunctional disorders.99. Andrade CRF. Gagueira infantil: risco, diagnóstico e programas terapêuticos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006.

The present study confirmed the findings previously published regarding speakers of Brazilian Portuguese, in relation to emotional stress factors. This previous study also found a significant relationship between persistent developmental stuttering among boys and emotional stress factors.2828. Oliveira CMC, Cunha DS, Santos AC. Fatores de risco para gagueira em crianças disfluentes com recorrência familial [Risk factors for stuttering in disfluent children with familial recurrence]. Audiol Commun Res. 2013;18(1):43-9. Our study presents evidence that stuttering is associated with multiple factors, like many other authors have previously shown.11. Smith A, Kelly E. Stuttering: A dynamic, multifactorial model. In: Curlee RF, Siegel GM. Nature and treatment of stuttering: new directions. Allyn & Bacon; 1997. p. 204-17. , 22. Ajdacic-Gross V, Vetter S, Müller M, et al. Risk factors for stuttering: a secondary analysis of a large data base. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010;260(4):279-86.

In our study, we found that family attitudes were inappropriate in the cases of 95% of the children who stuttered and 30% of the controls. Therefore, this finding confirmed that the prevalence of inappropriate family attitudes among children who stuttered was higher than among the controls. In another cross-sectional study carried out in São Paulo, Brazil, it was found that the highest risk factor for worse stuttering was the quality of the parents' behavior, with significant differences.99. Andrade CRF. Gagueira infantil: risco, diagnóstico e programas terapêuticos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006. Like in the present study, other authors have also demonstrated that among the families of children who stuttered, inappropriate family attitudes were commonly found.99. Andrade CRF. Gagueira infantil: risco, diagnóstico e programas terapêuticos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006. , 1919. Oliveira CM, Araujo FG, Vocurca MC. Atitudes verbais e não verbais de pais de crianças com queixa de gagueira [Familiar counseling and its effects on childhood stuttering]. Revista da Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia. 2000;5(7):3-10. For example, faster speaking rates among mothers were associated with greater stuttering severity in their children.2121. Dehqan A, Bakhtiar M, Panahi SS, Ashayeri H. Relationship between stuttering severity in children and their mothers speaking rate. Sao Paulo Med J. 2008;126(1):29-33.

We also found that in this subgroup of persistent developmental stuttering, there was no difference between the SC and NSC in relation to physical stress factors. This finding is similar to that of another study on Brazilian children with familial stuttering, which showed no relationship between their stuttering and physical stress factors.2828. Oliveira CMC, Cunha DS, Santos AC. Fatores de risco para gagueira em crianças disfluentes com recorrência familial [Risk factors for stuttering in disfluent children with familial recurrence]. Audiol Commun Res. 2013;18(1):43-9. This result is also in line with the findings of Poulos and Webster,1313. Poulos MG, Webster WG. Family history as a basis for subgrouping people who stutter. J Speech Hear Res. 1991;34(1):5-10. in which physical stress factors were correlated with the subgroup of sporadic developmental stuttering, i.e. individuals without any positive family history of stuttering.

With regard to personal reactions, there was no significant difference between SC and NSC. This finding suggests, like in other studies, that personal reactions should not be considered to be a causal factor for development of stuttering.3232. Iverach L, O'Brian S, Jones M, et al. The five factor model of personality applied to adults who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2010;43(2):120-32. , 3333. Bloodstein O, Bernstein Ratner N. A handbook on stuttering. 6th ed. Clifton Park: Thomson/Delmar Learning; 2008. Thus, when the population consists of children, these reactions are not always clear. Although there are many studies on personality and temperament among people who stutter,3434. Alm PA, Risberg J. Stuttering in adults: the acoustic startle response, temperamental traits, and biological factors. J Commun Disord. 2007;40(1):1-41.

35. Anderson JD, Pellowski MW, Conture EG, Kelly EM. Temperamental characteristics of young children who stutter. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(5):1221-33.

36. Schwenk KA, Conture EG, Walden TA. Reaction to background stimulation of preschool children who do and do not stutter. J Commun Disord. 2007;40(2):129-41.
- 3737. Seery CH, Watkins RV, Mangelsdorf SC, Shigeto A. Subtyping stuttering II: contributions from language and temperament. J Fluency Disord. 2007;32(3):197-217. there is still no consensus on whether stuttering indeed has any significant impact on people's personalities. One reason for the contradictory results obtained in this field is the lack of use of standardized questionnaires.3838. Bleek B, Montag C, Faber J, Reuter M. Investigating personality in stuttering: results of a case control study using the NEO-FFI. J Common Disord. 2011;44(2):218-22. These results corroborate other studies and will be an essential tool in designing and implementing future clinical protocols for diagnosing childhood stuttering in order to determine the risk of this disorder that children present.

In order to critically evaluate this study, it is necessary to consider the limitations inherent to the research design and analysis of the study. Firstly, the method of parental reporting that was used to study the risk factors has certain limitations. Although the parents' answers were restricted in an attempt to gather unambiguous responses, the reporting remains the result of the parents' perceptions, sensitivity, memory and interpretation, thereby adding some variance to the data. The presence of the researcher during the face-to-face interviews may have influenced the information given by the parents, since they may have provided answers that they thought the researcher wished to hear. Secondly, all the participants were students at a public school and thus were of lower socioeconomic level. Lastly, the findings from this study are subtle and based on a subgroup of developmental stuttering, i.e. familial persistent developmental stuttering among boys, and hence the ability to generalize these results to the wider population of children who stutter may be limited.

These limitations, as well as the findings from the current study, suggest important areas for future research. For example, replication of this type of study among girls who stutter might elicit additional data with regard to gender differences among children who stutter. Furthermore, because the participants were children at a public school, it was not possible to explore different sociocultural factors. Therefore, another suggestion for future research is to study the risk factors in two groups: one composed of children from public schools and another of children from private schools.

CONCLUSION

The findings suggest that the presence of stuttering-like disfluencies, quality and communication factors, emotional stress factors and inappropriate family attitudes are important risk factors for familial developmental stuttering among boys. Identifying these risk factors might make it possible to define the cases that require intervention, and thus, to provide earlier therapy.

Acknowledgement: The authors would like to thank all the participants, and the director of the public school that the non-stuttering boys attended, along with the parents who took part in this study

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Smith A, Kelly E. Stuttering: A dynamic, multifactorial model. In: Curlee RF, Siegel GM. Nature and treatment of stuttering: new directions. Allyn & Bacon; 1997. p. 204-17.
  • 2
    Ajdacic-Gross V, Vetter S, Müller M, et al. Risk factors for stuttering: a secondary analysis of a large data base. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010;260(4):279-86.
  • 3
    Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early childhood stuttering: for clinicians by clinicians. Austin: Pro-Ed; 2005.
  • 4
    Bothe AK, Davidow JH, Bramlett RE, Ingham RJ. Stuttering treatment research 1970-2005: I. Systematic review incorporating trial quality assessment of behavioral, cognitive, and related approaches. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2006;15(4):321-41.
  • 5
    Einarsdóttir J, Ingham RJ. The effect of stuttering measurement training on judging stuttering occurrence in preschool children who stutter. J Fluency Disord. 2008;33(3):167-79.
  • 6
    Dworzynski K, Remington A, Rijsdijk F, Howell P, Plomin R. Genetic etiology in cases of recovered and persistent stuttering in an unselected, longitudinal sample of young twins. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2007;16(2):169-78.
  • 7
    Brosch S, Winkler S. Diagnostik und Therapie bei stotternden Kindern [Diagnostics and therapy of stuttering children]. Laryngorhinootologie. 2008;87(7):511-8; quiz 519-23.
  • 8
    Oliveira CMC, Souza HA, Santos AC, Cunha D, Giacheti CM. Fatores de risco na gagueira desenvolvimental familial e isolada [Risk factors in the familial and sporadic developmental stuttering]. Rev CEFAC. 2011;13(2):205-13.
  • 9
    Andrade CRF. Gagueira infantil: risco, diagnóstico e programas terapêuticos. Barueri: Pró Fono; 2006.
  • 10
    Ambrose NG, Cox NJ, Yairi E. The genetic basis of persistence and recovery in stuttering. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1997;40(3):567-80.
  • 11
    Felsenfeld S, Kirk KM, Zhu G, et al. A study of the genetic and environmental etiology of stuttering in a selected twin sample. Behav Genet. 2000;30(5):359-66.
  • 12
    Yairi E, Ambrose NG, Niermann R. The early months of stuttering: a developmental study. J Speech Hear Res. 1993;36(3):521-8.
  • 13
    Poulos MG, Webster WG. Family history as a basis for subgrouping people who stutter. J Speech Hear Res. 1991;34(1):5-10.
  • 14
    Reilly S, Onslow M, Packman A, et al. Predicting stuttering onset by the age of 3 years: a prospective, community cohort study. Pediatrics. 2009;123(1):270-7.
  • 15
    Sawyer J, Yairi E. The effect of sample size on the assessment of stuttering severity. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2006;15(1):36-44.
  • 16
    Suresh R, Ambrose N, Roe C, et al. New complexities in the genetics of stuttering: significant sex-specific linkage signals. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;78(4):554-63.
  • 17
    Kang C, Riazuddin S, Mundorff J, et al. Mutations in the lysosomal enzyme-targeting pathway and persistent stuttering. N Engl J Med. 2010;362(8):677-85.
  • 18
    Drayna D, Kang C. Genetic approaches to understanding the causes of stuttering. J Neurodev Disord. 2011;3(4):374-80.
  • 19
    Oliveira CM, Araujo FG, Vocurca MC. Atitudes verbais e não verbais de pais de crianças com queixa de gagueira [Familiar counseling and its effects on childhood stuttering]. Revista da Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia. 2000;5(7):3-10.
  • 20
    Millard SK, Nicholas A, Cook FM. Is parent-child interaction therapy effective in reducing stuttering? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2008;51(3):636-50.
  • 21
    Dehqan A, Bakhtiar M, Panahi SS, Ashayeri H. Relationship between stuttering severity in children and their mothers speaking rate. Sao Paulo Med J. 2008;126(1):29-33.
  • 22
    Yairi E, Ambrose N. A longitudinal study of stuttering in children: a preliminary report. J Speech Hear Res. 1992;35(4):755-60.
  • 23
    Bloodstein O. A handbook on stuttering. Chicago: National Easter Seal Society; 1995.
  • 24
    Yairi E, Ambrose N, Cox N. Genetics of stuttering: a critical review. J Speech Hear Res. 1996;39(4):771-84.
  • 25
    Riley GD. Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults. Austin: Pro Ed; 1994.
  • 26
    Andrade CRF. Perfil da fluência da fala: parâmetros comparativo diferenciado por idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e idosos. Barueri: Pró-Fono; 2006.
  • 27
    Oliveira CMC, Souza HA, Santos AC, Cunha DS. Análise dos fatores de risco para gagueira em crianças disfluentes sem recorrência familial [Analysis of the risk factors for stuttering in disfluent children without familial recurrence]. Revista CEFAC. 2012;14(6):1028-35.
  • 28
    Oliveira CMC, Cunha DS, Santos AC. Fatores de risco para gagueira em crianças disfluentes com recorrência familial [Risk factors for stuttering in disfluent children with familial recurrence]. Audiol Commun Res. 2013;18(1):43-9.
  • 29
    Yaruss JS. Assessing quality of life in stuttering treatment outcomes research. J Fluency Disord. 2010;35(3):190-202.
  • 30
    Tran Y, Blumgart E, Craig A. Subjective distress associated with chronic stuttering. J Fluency Disord. 2011;36(1):17-26.
  • 31
    de Andrade CR, Cervone LM, Sasso FC. Relationship between the stuttering severity index and speech rate. Sao Paulo Med J. 2003;121(2):81-4.
  • 32
    Iverach L, O'Brian S, Jones M, et al. The five factor model of personality applied to adults who stutter. J Commun Disord. 2010;43(2):120-32.
  • 33
    Bloodstein O, Bernstein Ratner N. A handbook on stuttering. 6th ed. Clifton Park: Thomson/Delmar Learning; 2008.
  • 34
    Alm PA, Risberg J. Stuttering in adults: the acoustic startle response, temperamental traits, and biological factors. J Commun Disord. 2007;40(1):1-41.
  • 35
    Anderson JD, Pellowski MW, Conture EG, Kelly EM. Temperamental characteristics of young children who stutter. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2003;46(5):1221-33.
  • 36
    Schwenk KA, Conture EG, Walden TA. Reaction to background stimulation of preschool children who do and do not stutter. J Commun Disord. 2007;40(2):129-41.
  • 37
    Seery CH, Watkins RV, Mangelsdorf SC, Shigeto A. Subtyping stuttering II: contributions from language and temperament. J Fluency Disord. 2007;32(3):197-217.
  • 38
    Bleek B, Montag C, Faber J, Reuter M. Investigating personality in stuttering: results of a case control study using the NEO-FFI. J Common Disord. 2011;44(2):218-22.
  • Sources of funding: This research was supported by Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo (Fapesp, no. 2012/00540-2)

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    14 Apr 2014
  • Date of issue
    2014

History

  • Received
    22 Oct 2012
  • Reviewed
    03 July 2013
  • Accepted
    16 July 2013
Associação Paulista de Medicina - APM APM / Publicações Científicas, Av. Brigadeiro Luís Antonio, 278 - 7º and., 01318-901 São Paulo SP - Brazil, Tel.: +55 11 3188-4310 / 3188-4311, Fax: +55 11 3188-4255 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistas@apm.org.br