Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Relationship between the Brazilian version of the Montreal-Toulouse language assessment battery and education, age and reading and writing characteristics. A cross-sectional study

Relação entre a bateria Montreal-Toulouse de avaliação da linguagem, versão brasileira, e escolaridade, idade e hábitos de leitura e escrita. Um estudo transversal

Abstracts

CONTEXT AND OBJECTIVE:

There is growing concern about understanding how sociodemographic variables may interfere with cognitive functioning, especially with regard to language. This study aimed to investigate the relationship between performance in the Brazilian version of the Montreal-Toulouse language assessment battery (MTL-BR) and education, age and frequency of reading and writing habits (FRWH).

DESIGN AND SETTING:

Cross-sectional study conducted in university and work environments in Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil.

METHOD:

The MTL-BR was administered to a group of 233 healthy adults, aged 19 to 75 years (mean = 45.04, standard deviation, SD = 15.47), with at least five years of formal education (mean = 11.47, SD = 4.77).

RESULTS:

A stepwise multiple linear regression model showed that, for most tasks, the number of years of education, age and FRWH were better predictors of performance when analyzed together rather than separately. In separate analysis, education was the best predictor of performance in language tasks, especially those involving reading and writing abilities.

CONCLUSION:

The results suggested that the number of years of education, age and FRWH seem to influence performance in the MTL-BR, especially education. These data are important for making diagnoses of greater precision among patients suffering from brain injuries, with the aim of avoiding false positives.

Language tests; Educational status; Age factors; Aphasia; Adult


CONTEXTO E OBJETIVO:

Há uma preocupação crescente com o entendimento de como as variáveis sociodemográficas podem interferir no funcionamento cognitivo, especialmente na linguagem. Este estudo teve como objetivo investigar a relação entre o desempenho na Bateria Montreal-Toulouse de Avaliação da Linguagem, em sua versão brasileira (MTL-BR), e a escolaridade, a idade e a frequência de hábitos de leitura e escrita (FHLE).

TIPO DE ESTUDO E LOCAL:

Estudo transversal conduzido em ambientes universitários e de trabalho do Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil.

MÉTODOS:

A Bateria MTL-BR foi administrada em um grupo de 233 adultos saudáveis, de 19 a 75 anos de idade (M = 45,04, desvio padrão, DP = 15,47), com no mínimo cinco anos de estudo formal (M = 11,47, DP = 4,77).

RESULTADOS:

O modelo de regressão linear múltipla (stepwise) mostrou que, para a maioria das tarefas, os anos de escolaridade, de idade e FHLE são melhores preditores de desempenho quando analisados em conjunto do que separadamente. Quando analisada isoladamente, a escolaridade foi o melhor preditor para o desempenho nas tarefas linguísticas, principalmente nas tarefas que envolvem habilidades de leitura e escrita.

CONCLUSÃO:

Os resultados mostraram que os anos de escolaridade, idade e FHLE parecem influenciar o desempenho na MTL-BR, principalmente a escolaridade. Esses dados são relevantes para a realização de diagnóstico mais preciso de pacientes que sofreram lesão cerebral a fim de evitar falsos positivos.

Testes de linguagem; Escolaridade; Fatores etários; Afasia; Adulto


INTRODUCTION

Interpretations on the findings from neuropsychological assessments of language tend to have significant bias because of difficulty in distinguishing between cognitive effects due to brain damage and biological and sociocultural traits, in patients examined. 11. Lecours AR, Mehler J, Parente MA, et al. Illiteracy and brain damage-1. Aphasia testing in culturally contrasted populations (control subjects) . Neuropsychologia. 1987;25(1B):231-45. , 22. Pineda DA, Mejía SE, Rosselli M, et al. Variabilidad en la prueba de Boston para el diagnóstico de las afasias en adultos laboralmente activos [Variability of the Boston test for the diagnosis of aphasia in active working adults]. Rev Neurol. 1998;26(154):962-70. Thus, there is growing concern about understanding how age, gender, race, education and socioeconomic status, among other factors, may interfere with cognitive functioning.33. Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solis F, Rosselli M, Gómez C. Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2000;15(6):495-513.

4. Brucki SMD, Mansur LL, Carthery-Goulart MT, Nitrini R. Educação formal, alfabetismo em saúde e mini-exame do estado mental [Formal education, health literacy and Mini-Mental State Examination]. Dement Neuropsychol. 2011;5(1):26-30.

5. Freitas S, Simões MR, Alves L, Santana I. Montreal Cognitive Assessment: influence of sociodemographic and health variables. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2012;27(2):165-75.
- 66. Pineda DA, Rosselli M, Ardila A, et al. The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination-Spanish version: the influence of demographic variables. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):802-14.

Among the abovementioned factors, education level and age are the ones highlighted in the literature as the core influences on cognition. Overall, old age and low education level have been correlated with decreased performance.77. Ganguli M, Snitz BE, Lee CW, et al. Age and education effects and norms on a cognitive test battery from a population-based cohort: the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team. Aging Ment Health. 2010;14(1):100-7.

8. Ortiz KZ, Costa FP. Aplicação do teste M1-Alpha em sujeitos normais com baixa escolaridade: estudo piloto [M1-Alpha test in normal subjects with low educational level: a pilot study]. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(3):220-6.

9. Soares EC, Ortiz KZ. Influence of schooling on language abilities of adults without linguistic disorders. Sao Paulo Med J. 2009;127(3):134-9.

10. Snitz BE, Unverzagt FW, Chang CC, et al. Effects of age, gender, education and race on two tests of language ability in community-based older adults. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(6):1051-62.
- 1111. Tsapkini K, Vlahou CH, Potagas C. Adaptation and validation of standardized aphasia tests in different languages: Lessons from the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination - Short Form in Greek. Behav Neurol. 2010;22(3-4):111-9. However, this relationship is not always linear, given that there may be interactions between these factors, such as in naming and verbal fluency tasks, in which the effect of age is canceled when individuals are highly educated.1212. Kempler D, Teng EL, Dick M, Taussig IM, Davis DS. The effects of age, education, and ethnicity on verbal fluency. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998;4(6):531-8.

13. Neils J, Baris JM, Carter C, et al. Effects of age, education, and living environment on Boston Naming Test performance. J Speech Hear Res. 1995;38(5):1143-9.
- 1414. Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MP, Van Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. Normative data for the Animal, Profession and Letter M Naming verbal fluency tests for Dutch speaking participants and the effect of age, education, and sex. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12(1):80-9. In addition, although education is considered to be the cultural factor that has the greatest influence on cognition, there are limitations in analyses on this variable. Education level is generally based on the number of years of study, which addresses differences in the duration and not the quality of education.

One alternative that has been suggested is to use reading level1515. Manly J, Jacobs D, Touradji P, Small SA, Stern Y. Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2002;8(3):341-8. as a predictor of cognitive function. Since participants who have higher frequency of reading and writing habits (FRWH) are more proficient in these skills and therefore score better in cognitive and language tests,66. Pineda DA, Rosselli M, Ardila A, et al. The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination-Spanish version: the influence of demographic variables. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):802-14. , 1010. Snitz BE, Unverzagt FW, Chang CC, et al. Effects of age, gender, education and race on two tests of language ability in community-based older adults. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(6):1051-62. , 1616. Dotson VM, Kitner-Triolo MH, Evans MK, Zonderman AB. Effects of race and socioeconomic status on the relative influence of education and literacy on cognitive functioning. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(4):580-9.

17. Lachman ME, Agrigoroaei S, Murphy C, Tun PA. Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(1):4-10.
- 1818. Manly JJ, Byrd DA, Touradji P, Stern Y. Acculturation, reading level, and neuropsychological test performance among African American elders. Appl Neuropsychol. 2004;11(1):37-46. reading performance has been regarded as equally or more important than education level. It is also worth noting that the practice of writing can influence reading comprehension: people who read more write better and vice versa.1919. Cunha NB, Santos AAA. Relação entre a compreensão da leitura e a produção escrita em universitários [Reading comprehension and writing production among undergraduates]. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2006;19(2):237-45. , 2020. Stotsky S. Research on reading/writing relationships: a synthesis and suggested directions. Language Arts. 1983;60(5):627-43. Available from: http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/41961512?uid=3737664&uid=2129&uid=2&uid=70&uid=4&sid=21103557699791. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/41...

The effects of age and education level on healthy samples have been investigated in Brazilian research studies using the two main language assessment batteries: Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE)2121. Goodglass H, Kaplan E, Barresi B. The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2001. and Montreal-Toulouse Language Assessment Battery.2222. Nespoulous JL, Lecours AR, Lafond D. MT 86 - Protocole Montréal-Toulouse d'examen linguistique de l'aphasie. Isbergues: Ortho Edition; 1986. These studies have aimed to demonstrate the strong impact of these two factors on tasks involving oral and written comprehension, reading aloud, spelling, naming, written naming, reading numbers and copying. The results have revealed that older individuals with lower education levels are the ones who underperform. However, no studies investigating the independent effects of age, education and FRWH and their interactions in aphasia batteries have been conducted in Brazilian populations.88. Ortiz KZ, Costa FP. Aplicação do teste M1-Alpha em sujeitos normais com baixa escolaridade: estudo piloto [M1-Alpha test in normal subjects with low educational level: a pilot study]. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(3):220-6. , 99. Soares EC, Ortiz KZ. Influence of schooling on language abilities of adults without linguistic disorders. Sao Paulo Med J. 2009;127(3):134-9. , 2323. Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8.

Several tools designed to assess language among patients with aphasia have been described in the international literature. The BDAE,2121. Goodglass H, Kaplan E, Barresi B. The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2001. Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT)2424. Huber W, Poeck K, Willmes K. The Aachen Aphasia Test. Adv Neurol. 1984;42:291-303. and Western Aphasia Battery (WAB)2525. Kertesz A. Western Aphasia Battery test manual. New York: Grune & Stratton; 1982. are the ones that stand out. Although not as widely used, another battery of Francophone origin that is used for language assessment is the Montreal-Toulouse Language (MTL) Assessment Battery.2222. Nespoulous JL, Lecours AR, Lafond D. MT 86 - Protocole Montréal-Toulouse d'examen linguistique de l'aphasie. Isbergues: Ortho Edition; 1986. The Brazilian Portuguese adaptation of this battery (MTL-BR)2626. Parente MAMP, Ortiz KZ, Soares ECS, et al. Bateria Montreal-Toulouse de Avaliação da Linguagem - Bateria MTL-Brasil. São Paulo: Vetor Editora [in press]. has been seen to present interesting advantages with regard to evaluation and clinical interpretation of the different components of oral and written language. In particular, this adapted version has made it possible to analyze dissociations between different inputs and outputs, and different levels of complexity (word, sentence and discourse).2626. Parente MAMP, Ortiz KZ, Soares ECS, et al. Bateria Montreal-Toulouse de Avaliação da Linguagem - Bateria MTL-Brasil. São Paulo: Vetor Editora [in press]. The psychometric measurements were verified in a previous study2727. Pagliarin KC, Ortiz KZ, Parente MA, et al. Montreal-Toulouse language assessment battery for aphasia: validity and reliability evidence. NeuroRehabilitation. 2014;34(3):463-71. that found evidence of validity (Cronbach's alpha 0.79-0.90) and reliability (mean test-retest score of 0.52) for the battery. Nevertheless, there is still a lack of research on healthy participants, with the aim of comprehending the effect of each sociocultural and/or biological factor on linguistic and language-related abilities.

OBJECTIVE

In this context, this study aimed to investigate the relationship between the Montreal-Toulouse language assessment battery and education, age and FRWH, using a sample of neurologically healthy adults.

METHODS

Participants

The study included 233 neurologically healthy adults from southern Brazil: 151 females and 82 males. Their ages ranged from 19 to 75 (mean = 45.04; standard deviation, SD = 15.47) and education level ranged from 5 to 23 years of formal schooling (mean = 11.47; SD = 4.77).

The exclusion criteria included: a) impaired vision and/or hearing that was not corrected by means of visual and/or hearing aids; b) signs suggestive of neurological/psychiatric conditions; c) signs of moderate to severe depression (scores above 19 points), as measured using the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II);2828. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual do Inventário de Depressão de Beck - BDI-II. Gorenstein C, Yuan-Pang W, Argimon IIL, Werlang BSG [adaptação brasileira]. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2012. and d) signs of cognitive decline, as measured using the clock-drawing test2929. Juby A, Tench S, Baker V. The value of clock drawing in identifying executive cognitive dysfunction in people with a normal Mini-Mental State Examination score. CMAJ. 2002;167(8):859-64. in association with the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE),3030. Chaves ML, Izquierdo I. Differential diagnosis between dementia and depression: a study of efficiency increment. Acta Neurol Scan. 1992;85(6):378-82. with scores below 22 points for individuals with 5 to 8 years of schooling and below 24 points for more than 8 years.3131. Kochhann R, Varela JS, Lisboa CSM, Chaves MLF. The mini mental state examination review of cutoff points adjusted for schooling in a large Southern Brazilian sample [Mini exame do estado mental: revisão de pontos de corte ajustados para a escolaridade em uma grande amostra do sul do Brasil]. Dement Neuropsychol. 2010;4(1):35-41. In addition, participants who had a history of alcoholism and/or current or previous abuse of illicit drugs or benzodiazepines and antipsychotics, except for atypical neuroleptics (data collected through a questionnaire on the sociocultural aspects of health),3232. Fonseca RP, Zimmermann N, Oliveira CR, et al. Métodos em avaliação neuropsicológica: pressupostos gerais, neurocognitivos, neuropsicolinguísticos e psicométricos no uso e desenvolvimento de instrumentos. In: Fukushima SS. Métodos em psicobiologia, neurociêcias e comportamento. São Paulo: ANPEPP; 2012. were not included in the study.

Materials and procedure

The participants were recruited from university environments and work centers (convenience sample). After receiving clarifications about the study, all participants signed a consent form and participated as unpaid volunteers. It is important to state that all ethical procedures were respected, with a guarantee that participation in the study would be voluntary. The study was conducted under approval by the research ethics committee of a higher-education institution (Pontifical Catholic University of Rio Grande do Sul; under approval no. 04908/09).

The FRWH was evaluated using an inventory that included questions about reading habits (magazines, newspapers, books and other materials) and writing habits (text messages, letters and other materials), and the frequency of each activity was scored as follows: 4 points for every day; 3 for several days a week; 2 for once a week; 1 for rarely; and 0 for never, with a maximum frequency score of 28 points.3333. Pawlowski J, Remor E, Parente MAMP, et al. The influence of reading and writing habits associated with education on the neuropsychological performance of Brazilian adults. Reading and Writing. 2012;25(9):2275-89. Available from: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2Fs11145-012-9357-8. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007...
In this sample, the 14-point band was regarded as the median. Scores higher and lower than 14 were thus denominated high FRWH or low FRWH, respectively.

Finally, the participants were evaluated using the MTL-BR battery.2626. Parente MAMP, Ortiz KZ, Soares ECS, et al. Bateria Montreal-Toulouse de Avaliação da Linguagem - Bateria MTL-Brasil. São Paulo: Vetor Editora [in press]. This tool enables characterization of the subjects' oral and written language expression and comprehension behavior. The tasks used for this study were:

  1. Directed interview: Includes 13 open-ended questions to analyze speech and auditory comprehension. However, only comprehension is scored, with a maximum score of 26 points: 13 items with maximum score of two points each.

  2. Automatic speech: Assesses the ability to evoke automatisms such as numbers, days of the week and the birthday song. This task involves formal factors (occurrences of phonemic errors), which are scored with a maximum of six points, and content factors (occurrences of omissions), also with a maximum score of six points.

  3. Auditory comprehension: Measures the ability to identify images that represent words and phrases from auditory input. The task consists of a total of 19 items, five words (plates with six stimuli comprising one target and five distracters: one phonological, one semantic, one visual and two neutral) and 14 sentences (both simple and complex). The maximum score is five points for words and 14 points for phrases, with one point for each correct answer.

  4. Oral narrative task: Evaluates the ability to tell a story from visual inputs. The task consists of describing a picture depicting a bank robbery. The narrative is analyzed for the number of words produced and the information units (IU) produced (microstructure): bank, robbery, thieves, guard, car, running, waiting, calling, people and money. Each word gets one IU point. Furthermore, three major elements of the scene (macrostructure) are scored: the robbery itself (main scene), someone waiting for the thieves and someone telling the police. The maximum score for the components of the microstructure (IU) is 10 points and for the macrostructure (main elements of the scene), three points.

  5. Written comprehension: Assesses the ability to identify the input from visual images corresponding to words and written sentences. The task consists of a total of 13 items, five words (plates with six stimuli comprising one target and five distracters: one orthographic, one semantic, one visual and two neutral) and eight phrases (both simple and complex). The maximum score is five points for words and eight points for phrases, with one point for each correct answer.

  6. Sentence copying: Assesses the ability to recognize and reproduce letters. The task consists of a sentence made of eight words. The maximum score is eight points, with one point for each word spelled correctly. Verbatim or servile copying is not considered to be correct.

  7. Dictation: Assesses the individual's ability to understand the auditory stimulus and search the corresponding written representation. The task consists of nine words (regular, irregular, foreign words and non-words) and two sentences. The maximum score is 22 points, with one point for each word written correctly, including phrases.

  8. Repetition: Measures the individual's ability to reproduce auditory stimulus orally, following verbal models. The task consists of 11 words (regular, irregular and non-words) and three sentences. The maximum scores are 11 and 22 points for words and phrases respectively, with one point for each word produced correctly.

  9. Reading: Assesses the ability to recognize letters and produce the appropriate phonological sounds corresponding to 12 words (regular, irregular, foreign words and non-words) and three target sentences. The maximum scores are 12 and 21 points for words and phrases respectively, with one point for each correct answer.

  10. Semantic verbal fluency: Evaluates spontaneous production of words in the category "animals" within a time period of 90 seconds. Each word correctly selected from this class is equivalent to one point, ignoring repetitions, morphological derivatives of the same word and other words that do not match the requested category.

  11. Naming: Measures the ability to identify and name pictures that refer to nouns and verbs, from a visual input. Fifteen pictures are presented (12 nouns and three actions), placed on individual boards. The maximum score is 30 points, comprising 15 items with a maximum score of two points each.

  12. Nonverbal praxis: Assesses the ability to produce isolated gestures and movement sequences involving the face and tongue, requested by the evaluator through verbal instructions. The task consists of a total of six items with maximum scores of four points each, giving a maximum total of 24 points.

  13. Object manipulation: Assesses the ability to understand simple and complex commands. The individual is instructed to perform six commands given by the clinician, using physical objects (key, comb, cup, pen and paper). The complexity of orders increases gradually. The maximum score is 16 points.

  14. Phonological verbal fluency: Evaluates spontaneous production of words that start with the letter M within a time period of 90 seconds. Each correct word equals one point, ignoring repetitions, morphological derivatives of the same word and proper names.

  15. Body part recognition and left-right orientation: Assesses the individual's ability to identify parts of the body and their laterality. The maximum score is eight points, of which four points are given for each body part (limbs) and the other four are given for the right-left orientation.

  16. Written naming: Assesses the ability to identify and indicate 15 figures referring to 12 nouns and three actions (verbs), from visual input. The examiner presents each of the figures on different boards. The maximum score is 30 points, comprising 15 items with a maximum score of two points each.

  17. Oral text comprehension: Assesses the ability to understand auditory input from a text read by the clinician. The individual must answer six questions orally or in writing after listening to the text (three open-ended and three closed-ended questions). The maximum score is nine points: a maximum of 2 points for each of the three open-ended questions and one point for each of the closed-ended questions.

  18. Number dictation: Assesses the ability to understand the auditory stimulus and write down the corresponding number. The task consists of six numbers. Each number written correctly gets one point, with a maximum score of six points.

  19. Reading of numbers: Assesses the ability to recognize numerical and visual stimuli and reproduce them orally. The maximum score is six points: one point for each number read correctly.

  20. Written narrative: Involves an individual's ability to write a story from visual input. The task consists of a picture depicting a robbery at a bakery. We analyzed the number of words produced and information units (IU) produced (microstructure): bakery, robbery, robbers, guard, car, running, waiting, calling and gun (one point for each word). In addition, three major elements of the scene (macrostructure) are scored: the robbery itself (main scene), someone waiting for the bandits and someone telling the police. The maximum score is 10 points for IU and three points for elements of the scene.

  21. Written text comprehension: Evaluates the ability to decode and interpret linguistic signs in a written text. The task consists of six questions (three open-ended and three closed-ended) that can be answered orally or in writing after reading. The maximum score is nine points: a maximum score of two points each for the three open-ended questions and one point each for the closed-ended questions.

  22. Calculation: Evaluates the ability to perform the numerical operations of addition, subtraction, multiplication and division, as well as mental and written mathematical problems. The maximum score is 12 points: one point for each correctly solved calculation (four mental and four written) and two math problems.

The participants were evaluated in a single 90-minute session, by properly trained and qualified healthcare professionals who had completed or were in the process of completing additional training in language neuropsychology. The MTL-BR score was determined by two reviewers, and the final score was established by consensus.

Statistical analysis

The data were analyzed using SPSS 17.0. Initially, Pearson correlation analysis was used to investigate the relationship between age, education and FRWH and the scores from the MTL-BR tasks. Next, multiple linear regression analysis using the stepwise method was performed in order to identify which variables provided the best explanatory models. A significance level of P ≤ 0.05 was applied.

RESULTS

The scores from 17 out of the 22 tasks were significantly correlated with education, while the scores from nine subtests (oral narrative task, written comprehension, dictation, reading, semantic and phonological verbal fluency, written naming, written narrative and calculation) achieved moderate positive correlations. Ten tasks were negatively correlated with age (directed interview, auditory comprehension, oral narrative task total number of words, written comprehension, repetition, semantic verbal fluency, naming, written naming, written narrative total number of words and oral text comprehension). Performance in the oral narrative task, dictation, reading, semantic and phonological verbal fluency, written naming, written narrative and calculation presented significant moderate positive correlation with FRWH. The highest correlation was found between the written narrative task (total number of written words) and education variables, followed by the association between semantic verbal fluency and education (Table 1).

Table 1.
Correlations between education level, age and frequency of reading and writing habits (FRWH) and the tasks of the Montreal-Toulouse language assessment battery, Brazilian version (MTL-BR)

Table 2 displays the results from the regression analysis on the predictive model for performance in language tasks, considering education, age and FRWH as predictors. As the results show, education was a significant predictor (P < 0.01) for most tasks, with explanatory power ranging from 6% to 28%. Interestingly, it was also the only significant predictive factor in seven subtests: automatisms (form), oral narrative task (total number of IU), reading, number dictation, reading of numbers, written narrative (total number of scenes) and written text comprehension. The FRWH was a significant predictor only for automatisms (content), while age was the only predictor for the guided interview.

Table 2.
Results from multiple regression analysis on the tasks of the Montreal-Toulouse language assessment battery, Brazilian version (MTL-BR), considering the study variables and the total number of participants

In general, among the tests in which education was combined with other variables, the variable FRWH contributed 2-3% to the explanatory model. In contrast, age contributed 3-6% to the explanatory power when combined with education in some tasks.

With regard to the explanatory models for each dependent variable, education level and FRWH jointly explained 32% of the variance of performance in the semantic verbal fluency and written narrative task (total number of words) and 30% of the variance relating to dictation. Education and age explained 25% of the scores for written naming. These three variables combined showed the greatest explanatory power (33%) for the semantic verbal fluency task. These findings corroborate the results presented in Table 1 (significant correlations).

DISCUSSION

The results from this study revealed that the MTL-BR is mainly influenced by education, such that higher education level was correlated with better performance. Likewise, other studies have shown the importance of the number of years of formal education in language tasks, with findings similar to those observed in the present study.11. Lecours AR, Mehler J, Parente MA, et al. Illiteracy and brain damage-1. Aphasia testing in culturally contrasted populations (control subjects) . Neuropsychologia. 1987;25(1B):231-45. , 66. Pineda DA, Rosselli M, Ardila A, et al. The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination-Spanish version: the influence of demographic variables. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):802-14. , 88. Ortiz KZ, Costa FP. Aplicação do teste M1-Alpha em sujeitos normais com baixa escolaridade: estudo piloto [M1-Alpha test in normal subjects with low educational level: a pilot study]. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(3):220-6.

9. Soares EC, Ortiz KZ. Influence of schooling on language abilities of adults without linguistic disorders. Sao Paulo Med J. 2009;127(3):134-9.
- 1010. Snitz BE, Unverzagt FW, Chang CC, et al. Effects of age, gender, education and race on two tests of language ability in community-based older adults. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(6):1051-62. , 3333. Pawlowski J, Remor E, Parente MAMP, et al. The influence of reading and writing habits associated with education on the neuropsychological performance of Brazilian adults. Reading and Writing. 2012;25(9):2275-89. Available from: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2Fs11145-012-9357-8. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007...

As seen in the results, in this study there was a moderate positive correlation with education in tasks that involve dictation, written naming, written comprehension, auditory comprehension and written narrative. These tasks require reading-writing abilities that are developed over the course of education. However, education was not a predictor of the tasks relating to the guided interview, repetition, automatic speech (content) and oral narrative task (total number of words) (Table 2). Previous research has shown that tasks involving graphic stimuli (written comprehension, dictation, reading, written naming, number dictation, reading of numbers, written narrative and written text comprehension) tend to be more sensitive to the influence of education.66. Pineda DA, Rosselli M, Ardila A, et al. The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination-Spanish version: the influence of demographic variables. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):802-14. , 88. Ortiz KZ, Costa FP. Aplicação do teste M1-Alpha em sujeitos normais com baixa escolaridade: estudo piloto [M1-Alpha test in normal subjects with low educational level: a pilot study]. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(3):220-6. , 1111. Tsapkini K, Vlahou CH, Potagas C. Adaptation and validation of standardized aphasia tests in different languages: Lessons from the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination - Short Form in Greek. Behav Neurol. 2010;22(3-4):111-9.

The importance, albeit weaker, of FRWH in linguistic performance deserves to be highlighted, given that it increases the predictive power of the education variable. Regularly practicing reading and writing can compensate for low education levels in linguistic performance,1717. Lachman ME, Agrigoroaei S, Murphy C, Tun PA. Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(1):4-10. , 3333. Pawlowski J, Remor E, Parente MAMP, et al. The influence of reading and writing habits associated with education on the neuropsychological performance of Brazilian adults. Reading and Writing. 2012;25(9):2275-89. Available from: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2Fs11145-012-9357-8. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007...
and the quality of what is read is one of the biggest predictors of cognitive performance.1616. Dotson VM, Kitner-Triolo MH, Evans MK, Zonderman AB. Effects of race and socioeconomic status on the relative influence of education and literacy on cognitive functioning. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(4):580-9. , 3434. Manly JJ, Jacobs DM, Sano M, et al. Effect of literacy on neuropsychological test performance in nondemented, education-matched elders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1999;5(3):191-202. Despite the relevance of FRWH in this study, although this variable had significant positive correlations with various tasks of the MTL-BR (Table 1), it did not show strong explanatory power in comparison with a model composed only by education (Table 2). The main hypothesis for this finding relates to the information obtained from FRWH, since this was restricted to measurement of the frequency of the material that was read and produced, but not the quality, level of complexity or resulting potential for neurocognitive stimulation of this material. However, Pawlowski et al.3333. Pawlowski J, Remor E, Parente MAMP, et al. The influence of reading and writing habits associated with education on the neuropsychological performance of Brazilian adults. Reading and Writing. 2012;25(9):2275-89. Available from: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2Fs11145-012-9357-8. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007...
used this same questionnaire to verify the influence of FRWH combined with education level on performance in neuropsychological tasks among adults and noted that frequent practice of reading and writing among individuals with low education promoted an improvement in cognitive performance, as well as in language tasks.

The greater explanatory power found in joint education-FRWH models is based on the combination of these two sociocultural variables: the higher the education level and the frequency of reading and writing habits are, the better the scores in language tasks will be. On the other hand, people with low education levels are less motivated to perform tasks that require reading and writing in their day-to-day lives,1717. Lachman ME, Agrigoroaei S, Murphy C, Tun PA. Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(1):4-10. , 3535. Kliegel M, Zimprich D, Rott C. Life-long intellectual activities mediate the predictive effect of early education on cognitive impairment in centenarians: a retrospective study. Aging Ment Health. 2004;8(5):430-7. which can hamper understanding in some subtests involving increasing complexity and may cause lower performance.

It has been shown that not only education level, but also reading scores are predictors in the verbal fluency test, naming task and comprehension.1010. Snitz BE, Unverzagt FW, Chang CC, et al. Effects of age, gender, education and race on two tests of language ability in community-based older adults. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(6):1051-62. , 1717. Lachman ME, Agrigoroaei S, Murphy C, Tun PA. Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(1):4-10. , 1818. Manly JJ, Byrd DA, Touradji P, Stern Y. Acculturation, reading level, and neuropsychological test performance among African American elders. Appl Neuropsychol. 2004;11(1):37-46. This is mainly because anyone who reads and writes more often possesses a richer vocabulary, and makes use of attention, mnemonic and executive skills. People who tend to read less may present cognitive decline and worse outcomes in reading tasks, especially the elderly.

The habits of reading and writing are also considered to be one of the factors that contribute towards formation of a cognitive reserve, thereby preventing the effects of aging on cognition.3636. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(10):2015-28. The findings from this study are in agreement with results relating to the effects of age, in a study on performance in the subtests of oral narrative, repetition, verbal fluency, auditory comprehension (sentences), written narrative, reading, written text and sentence comprehension, in which older individuals had lower scores than younger subjects did.3737. Labos E, Del Rio M, Zabala K. Perfil de desempeño linguístico en el adulto mayor. Revista Argentina de Neuropsicología. 2009;13:1-13. Available from: http://www.academia.edu/7564486/Perfil_de_desempeno_linguistico_en_el_adulto_mayor. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
http://www.academia.edu/7564486/Perfil_d...
Like in other studies with similar tasks and batteries,11. Lecours AR, Mehler J, Parente MA, et al. Illiteracy and brain damage-1. Aphasia testing in culturally contrasted populations (control subjects) . Neuropsychologia. 1987;25(1B):231-45. , 66. Pineda DA, Rosselli M, Ardila A, et al. The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination-Spanish version: the influence of demographic variables. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):802-14. , 1010. Snitz BE, Unverzagt FW, Chang CC, et al. Effects of age, gender, education and race on two tests of language ability in community-based older adults. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(6):1051-62. age had little effect on task performance. Despite the decline in task performance found among older age groups, it has been shown that in narrative tasks the elderly have better scores and higher production of words and sentences to express pictorial information,3838. James LE, Burke DM, Austin A, Hulme E. Production and perception of "verbosity" in younger and older adults. Psychol Aging. 1998;13(3):355-67. , 3939. Juncos-Rabadán O, Pereiro AX, Rodríguez MS. Narrative speech in aging: quantity, information content, and cohesion. Brain Lang. 2005;95(3):423-34. thus corroborating the findings in our study. This is because the elderly tend to have more repetitions, although these are not necessarily off-topic.4040. Korolija N. Coherence-inducing strategies in conversations amongst the aged. Journal of Pragmatics. 2000;32(4):425-62. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0378216699000582. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/art...
In speech, it is possible that repetitions are intentional, produced in order to emphasize certain information,4141. Arbuckle TY, Nohara-LeClair M, Pushkar D. Effect of off-target verbosity on communication efficiency in a referential communication task. Psychol Aging. 2000;15(1):65-77. or even to make time for organization of thought.4242. Roberts B, Kisner K. Temporal cycles in speech production. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2000;15(2):129-57. Available from: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/016909600386075#.VA9NGfmtksY. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.10...
Moreover, they may be due to the changes in communication style that occur naturally as a result of aging.

During the guided interview, age was the only predictor, contributing 3% to performance. In conversations with autobiographical topics, elderly people may have more personal observations, with frequent remembrance of the past. Moreover, with the aging process, narratives tend to have a complex plot, with more episodes and better management of resources, so as to maintain the interest of communicative interaction by making linguistic and extra-linguistic adjustments.4343. Brandão L, Parente MAMP. Os estudos de linguagem do idoso neste último século. Estudos Interdisciplinares sobre o Envelhecimento. 2001;3:37-53. Available from: file:///C:/Users/Dell/Downloads/4668-14935-1-PB.pdf. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9). It is important to pay special attention to the heterogeneity of communicative performance, which is greater in the elderly population.4444. Duong A, Giroux F, Tardif A, Ska B. The heterogeneity of picture-supported narratives in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Lang. 2005;93(2):173-84. , 4545. Wittig F. Estructura narrativa en el discurso oral de adultos mayores [Narrative structure in oral discourse by elder speakers]. Rev Signos. 2004;37(56):91-101.

Some limitations of this study should be taken into consideration in order to make better use of its findings. The fact that illiterate or functionally illiterate patients were not included may have contributed towards the predictive outcome of models, with a maximum of 28% for the education level variable. Since the effect of education is not linear in cognitive tests (mainly because of lower education levels), a difference of one or two years of schooling interferes with performance.66. Pineda DA, Rosselli M, Ardila A, et al. The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination-Spanish version: the influence of demographic variables. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):802-14. In addition, it should be pointed out that both education level and FRWH were used as quantitative measurements, thus not covering specific measurements of education quality or the quality of the material read and/or written.1818. Manly JJ, Byrd DA, Touradji P, Stern Y. Acculturation, reading level, and neuropsychological test performance among African American elders. Appl Neuropsychol. 2004;11(1):37-46. Thus, additional studies are needed in order to analyze these factors, so as to better understand the influence of these variables on language performance.

These data are important with regard to making correct diagnoses among patients suffering from neurological injuries, because of the potential for avoiding false positives. In addition, the variables studied are important because they establish normative data for clinical populations, especially aphasic individuals.

CONCLUSION

These results show the influence of sociodemographic variables, especially education and its association with FRWH, among the MTL-BR tasks. Examiners should take these variables into consideration when evaluating language performance, especially in clinical populations.

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Lecours AR, Mehler J, Parente MA, et al. Illiteracy and brain damage-1. Aphasia testing in culturally contrasted populations (control subjects) . Neuropsychologia. 1987;25(1B):231-45.
  • 2
    Pineda DA, Mejía SE, Rosselli M, et al. Variabilidad en la prueba de Boston para el diagnóstico de las afasias en adultos laboralmente activos [Variability of the Boston test for the diagnosis of aphasia in active working adults]. Rev Neurol. 1998;26(154):962-70.
  • 3
    Ardila A, Ostrosky-Solis F, Rosselli M, Gómez C. Age-related cognitive decline during normal aging: the complex effect of education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2000;15(6):495-513.
  • 4
    Brucki SMD, Mansur LL, Carthery-Goulart MT, Nitrini R. Educação formal, alfabetismo em saúde e mini-exame do estado mental [Formal education, health literacy and Mini-Mental State Examination]. Dement Neuropsychol. 2011;5(1):26-30.
  • 5
    Freitas S, Simões MR, Alves L, Santana I. Montreal Cognitive Assessment: influence of sociodemographic and health variables. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2012;27(2):165-75.
  • 6
    Pineda DA, Rosselli M, Ardila A, et al. The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination-Spanish version: the influence of demographic variables. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2000;6(7):802-14.
  • 7
    Ganguli M, Snitz BE, Lee CW, et al. Age and education effects and norms on a cognitive test battery from a population-based cohort: the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team. Aging Ment Health. 2010;14(1):100-7.
  • 8
    Ortiz KZ, Costa FP. Aplicação do teste M1-Alpha em sujeitos normais com baixa escolaridade: estudo piloto [M1-Alpha test in normal subjects with low educational level: a pilot study]. J Soc Bras Fonoaudiol. 2011;23(3):220-6.
  • 9
    Soares EC, Ortiz KZ. Influence of schooling on language abilities of adults without linguistic disorders. Sao Paulo Med J. 2009;127(3):134-9.
  • 10
    Snitz BE, Unverzagt FW, Chang CC, et al. Effects of age, gender, education and race on two tests of language ability in community-based older adults. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009;21(6):1051-62.
  • 11
    Tsapkini K, Vlahou CH, Potagas C. Adaptation and validation of standardized aphasia tests in different languages: Lessons from the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination - Short Form in Greek. Behav Neurol. 2010;22(3-4):111-9.
  • 12
    Kempler D, Teng EL, Dick M, Taussig IM, Davis DS. The effects of age, education, and ethnicity on verbal fluency. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1998;4(6):531-8.
  • 13
    Neils J, Baris JM, Carter C, et al. Effects of age, education, and living environment on Boston Naming Test performance. J Speech Hear Res. 1995;38(5):1143-9.
  • 14
    Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MP, Van Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. Normative data for the Animal, Profession and Letter M Naming verbal fluency tests for Dutch speaking participants and the effect of age, education, and sex. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006;12(1):80-9.
  • 15
    Manly J, Jacobs D, Touradji P, Small SA, Stern Y. Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2002;8(3):341-8.
  • 16
    Dotson VM, Kitner-Triolo MH, Evans MK, Zonderman AB. Effects of race and socioeconomic status on the relative influence of education and literacy on cognitive functioning. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(4):580-9.
  • 17
    Lachman ME, Agrigoroaei S, Murphy C, Tun PA. Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010;18(1):4-10.
  • 18
    Manly JJ, Byrd DA, Touradji P, Stern Y. Acculturation, reading level, and neuropsychological test performance among African American elders. Appl Neuropsychol. 2004;11(1):37-46.
  • 19
    Cunha NB, Santos AAA. Relação entre a compreensão da leitura e a produção escrita em universitários [Reading comprehension and writing production among undergraduates]. Psicol Reflex Crit. 2006;19(2):237-45.
  • 20
    Stotsky S. Research on reading/writing relationships: a synthesis and suggested directions. Language Arts. 1983;60(5):627-43. Available from: http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/41961512?uid=3737664&uid=2129&uid=2&uid=70&uid=4&sid=21103557699791. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
    » http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/41961512?uid=3737664&uid=2129&uid=2&uid=70&uid=4&sid=21103557699791
  • 21
    Goodglass H, Kaplan E, Barresi B. The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins; 2001.
  • 22
    Nespoulous JL, Lecours AR, Lafond D. MT 86 - Protocole Montréal-Toulouse d'examen linguistique de l'aphasie. Isbergues: Ortho Edition; 1986.
  • 23
    Radanovic M, Mansur LL, Scaff M. Normative data for the Brazilian population in the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination: influence of schooling. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2004;37(11):1731-8.
  • 24
    Huber W, Poeck K, Willmes K. The Aachen Aphasia Test. Adv Neurol. 1984;42:291-303.
  • 25
    Kertesz A. Western Aphasia Battery test manual. New York: Grune & Stratton; 1982.
  • 26
    Parente MAMP, Ortiz KZ, Soares ECS, et al. Bateria Montreal-Toulouse de Avaliação da Linguagem - Bateria MTL-Brasil. São Paulo: Vetor Editora [in press].
  • 27
    Pagliarin KC, Ortiz KZ, Parente MA, et al. Montreal-Toulouse language assessment battery for aphasia: validity and reliability evidence. NeuroRehabilitation. 2014;34(3):463-71.
  • 28
    Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual do Inventário de Depressão de Beck - BDI-II. Gorenstein C, Yuan-Pang W, Argimon IIL, Werlang BSG [adaptação brasileira]. São Paulo: Casa do Psicólogo; 2012.
  • 29
    Juby A, Tench S, Baker V. The value of clock drawing in identifying executive cognitive dysfunction in people with a normal Mini-Mental State Examination score. CMAJ. 2002;167(8):859-64.
  • 30
    Chaves ML, Izquierdo I. Differential diagnosis between dementia and depression: a study of efficiency increment. Acta Neurol Scan. 1992;85(6):378-82.
  • 31
    Kochhann R, Varela JS, Lisboa CSM, Chaves MLF. The mini mental state examination review of cutoff points adjusted for schooling in a large Southern Brazilian sample [Mini exame do estado mental: revisão de pontos de corte ajustados para a escolaridade em uma grande amostra do sul do Brasil]. Dement Neuropsychol. 2010;4(1):35-41.
  • 32
    Fonseca RP, Zimmermann N, Oliveira CR, et al. Métodos em avaliação neuropsicológica: pressupostos gerais, neurocognitivos, neuropsicolinguísticos e psicométricos no uso e desenvolvimento de instrumentos. In: Fukushima SS. Métodos em psicobiologia, neurociêcias e comportamento. São Paulo: ANPEPP; 2012.
  • 33
    Pawlowski J, Remor E, Parente MAMP, et al. The influence of reading and writing habits associated with education on the neuropsychological performance of Brazilian adults. Reading and Writing. 2012;25(9):2275-89. Available from: http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2Fs11145-012-9357-8. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
    » http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2Fs11145-012-9357-8
  • 34
    Manly JJ, Jacobs DM, Sano M, et al. Effect of literacy on neuropsychological test performance in nondemented, education-matched elders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1999;5(3):191-202.
  • 35
    Kliegel M, Zimprich D, Rott C. Life-long intellectual activities mediate the predictive effect of early education on cognitive impairment in centenarians: a retrospective study. Aging Ment Health. 2004;8(5):430-7.
  • 36
    Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(10):2015-28.
  • 37
    Labos E, Del Rio M, Zabala K. Perfil de desempeño linguístico en el adulto mayor. Revista Argentina de Neuropsicología. 2009;13:1-13. Available from: http://www.academia.edu/7564486/Perfil_de_desempeno_linguistico_en_el_adulto_mayor. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
    » http://www.academia.edu/7564486/Perfil_de_desempeno_linguistico_en_el_adulto_mayor
  • 38
    James LE, Burke DM, Austin A, Hulme E. Production and perception of "verbosity" in younger and older adults. Psychol Aging. 1998;13(3):355-67.
  • 39
    Juncos-Rabadán O, Pereiro AX, Rodríguez MS. Narrative speech in aging: quantity, information content, and cohesion. Brain Lang. 2005;95(3):423-34.
  • 40
    Korolija N. Coherence-inducing strategies in conversations amongst the aged. Journal of Pragmatics. 2000;32(4):425-62. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0378216699000582. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
    » http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0378216699000582
  • 41
    Arbuckle TY, Nohara-LeClair M, Pushkar D. Effect of off-target verbosity on communication efficiency in a referential communication task. Psychol Aging. 2000;15(1):65-77.
  • 42
    Roberts B, Kisner K. Temporal cycles in speech production. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2000;15(2):129-57. Available from: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/016909600386075#.VA9NGfmtksY. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
    » http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/016909600386075#.VA9NGfmtksY
  • 43
    Brandão L, Parente MAMP. Os estudos de linguagem do idoso neste último século. Estudos Interdisciplinares sobre o Envelhecimento. 2001;3:37-53. Available from: file:///C:/Users/Dell/Downloads/4668-14935-1-PB.pdf. Accessed in 2014 (Sep 9).
  • 44
    Duong A, Giroux F, Tardif A, Ska B. The heterogeneity of picture-supported narratives in Alzheimer's disease. Brain Lang. 2005;93(2):173-84.
  • 45
    Wittig F. Estructura narrativa en el discurso oral de adultos mayores [Narrative structure in oral discourse by elder speakers]. Rev Signos. 2004;37(56):91-101.
  • Sources of funding: Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES), grant number: 12247/12-0

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    17 Mar 2015
  • Date of issue
    July-Aug 2015

History

  • Received
    20 Feb 2014
  • Reviewed
    15 Sept 2015
  • Accepted
    16 Oct 2014
Associação Paulista de Medicina - APM APM / Publicações Científicas, Av. Brigadeiro Luís Antonio, 278 - 7º and., 01318-901 São Paulo SP - Brazil, Tel.: +55 11 3188-4310 / 3188-4311, Fax: +55 11 3188-4255 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revistas@apm.org.br