Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Study review of biological, social and environmental factors associated with aggressive behavior

Abstracts

OBJECTIVES: To study the risk factors related to the development of aggressive behavior. METHOD: A search was carried out in two electronic databases, Medline and SciElo by retrospective studies, longitudinal and review that assessed risk factors for the development of aggressive behavior. RESULTS: There were selected 11 longitudinal studies (8 prospective and 3 case-control studies) and a cross sectional study that evaluated the risk factors and socio-biological related to aggressive behavior. Five studies have evaluated gene expression, five evaluated exposure to tobacco, alcohol and cocaine in the prenatal period, one evaluated the effect of early malnutrition on the development of aggressive behavior and one assessed the impact of child maltreatment. CONCLUSION: The main biological factors were: genetic (low expression of the monoamine oxidase gene and serotonin transporter gene, variations in transporter and dopamine receptor genes), exposure to substances during intrauterine development (tobacco, alcohol and cocaine) and nutrition (malnutrition). The main environmental factors were: child abuse, poverty, crime and antisocial behavior in childhood, while the highest level of evidence was related to early neglect. The interaction between biological and environmental factors can be catalyzed by a hostile environment, increasing the risk for the development of aggressive behavior.

Violence; Risk factors; Review; Aggression; Antisocial personality disorder


OBJETIVOS: Estudar os fatores de risco relacionados ao desenvolvimento do comportamento agressivo. MÉTODO: Foi realizada uma busca em duas bases de dados eletrônicas, Medline e SciElo, por estudos retrospectivos, longitudinais e de revisão que avaliaram fatores de risco para o desenvolvimento do comportamento agressivo. RESULTADOS: Foram selecionados 11 estudos longitudinais (8 prospectivos e 3 de casos-controle) e um transversal que avaliaram os fatores de risco biológicos e socioambientais relacionados ao comportamento agressivo. Cinco estudos avaliaram a expressão gênica, cinco a exposição ao tabaco, ao álcool e a cocaína no período pré-natal, um avaliou as implicações da desnutrição precoce no desenvolvimento do comportamento agressivo e um avaliou o impacto dos maus tratos na infância. CONCLUSÃO: os principais fatores biológicos encontrados foram: genéticos (baixa expressão do gene monoaminaoxidase e do gene transportador de serotonina, variações nos genes transportador e receptor de dopamina), exposição a substâncias durante o desenvolvimento intrauterino (tabaco, álcool e cocaína) e nutricionais (desnutrição infantil). os principais fatores socioambientais encontrados foram: maus tratos na infância, pobreza, criminalidade e comportamento antissocial na infância, sendo que o maior nível de evidência esteve relacionado à negligência precoce. A interação entre fatores biológicos e ambientais pode ser catalisada por um ambiente hostil aumentando os riscos para o desenvolvimento de comportamentos agressivos.

Violência; Fatores de risco; Revisão; Agressão; Transtorno de personalidade anti-social


ARTICLES

Study review of biological, social and environmental factors associated with aggressive behavior

Deise Daniela Mendes; Jair de Jesus Mari; Marina Singer; Gustavo Machado Barros; Andréa F. Mello

Department of Psychiatry, Universidade Federal de São Paulo (UniFeSP), São Paulo (SP), Brazil

Correspondence

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: To study the risk factors related to the development of aggressive behavior.

METHOD: A search was carried out in two electronic databases, Medline and SciElo by retrospective studies, longitudinal and review that assessed risk factors for the development of aggressive behavior.

RESULTS: There were selected 11 longitudinal studies (8 prospective and 3 case-control studies) and a cross sectional study that evaluated the risk factors and socio-biological related to aggressive behavior. Five studies have evaluated gene expression, five evaluated exposure to tobacco, alcohol and cocaine in the prenatal period, one evaluated the effect of early malnutrition on the development of aggressive behavior and one assessed the impact of child maltreatment.

CONCLUSION: The main biological factors were: genetic (low expression of the monoamine oxidase gene and serotonin transporter gene, variations in transporter and dopamine receptor genes), exposure to substances during intrauterine development (tobacco, alcohol and cocaine) and nutrition (malnutrition). The main environmental factors were: child abuse, poverty, crime and antisocial behavior in childhood, while the highest level of evidence was related to early neglect. The interaction between biological and environmental factors can be catalyzed by a hostile environment, increasing the risk for the development of aggressive behavior.

Descriptors: Violence; Risk factors; Review; Aggression; Antisocial personality disorder

Introduction

In its Resolution 49.25, adopted in 1961,1 the World Health Organization (WHO) declared violence a major public health problem. Violence rates, whose main victims are the youth, women and children, have been increasing. The WHO defines violence as "the intentional use of physical force or power, threatened or actual, against oneself, another person, or against a group or community, that either results in or has a high likelihood of resulting in injury, death, psychological harm, maldevelopment or deprivation".2 Aggressiveness, in turn, can be defined as the generation of a behavior that aims at causing physical or psychic harm to somebody else.3-5

In Brazil, external causes are the most usual cause of death, second only to cardiovascular diseases. The first external cause of death is homicide, followed by fatal traffic accidents. Young males are the most common fatal victims of violence in Brazil.6,7 Studies on the burden of a disease, which measures the number of years of life lost to premature death or disability, indicate that external causes represent 10.2% of a disease's total burden.8 Unfortunately, aside from homicides, other forms of violence are part of the day-to-day of our society: violence against one's partner, sexual abuse at childhood and adolescence, early involvement with alcohol and drugs, kidnappings, and violence against the elderly.9

In light of the current scenario, the study of risk factors for the development of an aggressive behavior, as well as prevention factors against it are of the utmost importance. The first research studies ever conducted in this field focused on social and environmental issues leading to aggressiveness. They pointed to social inequality, poverty and the environment as the main reasons for the display of an aggressive and criminal behavior.10,11 However, as current research in neurosciences uncovered the biological, i.e. the genetic and neurophysiological mechanisms implicated in aggressive behavior, it became evident that both social and environmental factors are not the only reasons explaining the development of aggressive and anti-social behavior. As a matter of fact, what happens is an interaction between the biological and socio-environmental factors that modulate violent behavior. The aim of this study, therefore, is to conduct a comprehensive review of the current literature on the main risk factors associated with aggressive behavior.

Method

The search for articles consisted in seeking cohort, control-case and review articles addressing the main risk factors associated with aggressive behavior pulled out from two electronic databases (Medline e SciELO).

Results

Twelve assessment studies on risk factors associated with aggressive behavior (Table 1) were selected.12-23 Of these, eight had adopted a prospective longitudinal study design,12,14,15,18,19,21-23 three were case-control studies16,17,20 and one was a transversal study.

Six studies related aggressive behavior to genic expression.12-17 Two demonstrated the genic expression of the serotonin transporter,13,14 one demonstrated the genic expression of the monoaminaoxidase (MAOA),12 another study demonstrated the genic expression of the transporter and the receptor of dopamine,15 and two studies presented the genic expression of the catechol-O-methyltransferase enzyme (COMT).16,17

Four studies related aggressive behavior to maternal exposure to psychoactive substances.18-21 Two other studies showed evidence that the use of tobacco during pregnancy constitutes an important risk factor,18,19 while one study demonstrated the role played by alcohol in this interaction.20 Exposure to cocaine was also the object of one of the studies.21 One study evaluated the effects of malnutrition during the first years of infancy22 and another evaluated the impacts of neglect and abuse at infancy.23 Finally, a review of the socio-environmental factors from a more qualitative perspective discussed how such factors interact with the development of aggressive behavior.

Next, we will discuss the factors contemplated in each of these studies.

Genetic factors

Genetic studies conducted with twins and adopted individuals have found genetic substrates underlying the development of aggressive, antisocial and violent behavior. Approximately 50% of the variance found in the anti-social phenotypes can be traced back to genetic factors.5,24 Genetic findings have implicated the genes associated to the expression of MAOA, as well as those involved in serotoninergic, noradrenergic and dopaminergic activities.

1. MAOA

Genetic studies have identified polymorphisms located at the promoter region of the gene involved in the MAOA genotype. The presence of four repetitions of alleles is related to the gene's high activity level i.e., to greater genetic expression while the presence of three repetitions is associated to a low activity level.25-27

In 2006, Widom et al. conducted a prospective study with 802 participants who had been subjected to abuse and neglect and were followed-up on until they reached adulthood.12 In an attempt to understand the development of aggressive behavior throughout one's lifetime, this group was compared to a control group which had not been subjected to aggression during childhood. The MAOA genotypes of these individuals were also compared. The population was divided according to ethnicity i.e., Caucasians and non-Caucasians. The conclusion was that, in the presence of an adverse environment at infancy, Caucasians (beta = 0.27, p < 0.01) with low-activity genotypes had a higher the risk of suffering from behavioral problems throughout their lifetime. In children who suffered abuse at infancy, however, a higher expression level of MAOA was associated to a lower frequency of violent and antisocial behavior at adulthood, although no statistical significance could be established. This association could not be demonstrated among non-Caucasians in whom the genetic polymorphism seems to be less correlated to the gene's expression levels. The low activity of the MAOA genotype was not predictive of violent and antisocial behavior in the absence of abuse at infancy. Thus, the authors concluded that the MAOA genotype has a moderating effect on the impact of neglect and abuse suffered at infancy on the development of violent and antisocial behavior at adulthood. Other studies are in agreement with this hypothesis.28,29

2. Serotonin

Beitchman et al. evaluated the association of polymorphisms in the transporter gene of serotonin and aggressive behavior at both infancy and adolescence.13 The sample was comprised of 82 individuals aged between 5 and 15 years who were genotyped for 5HTTLPR (n = 77) and 5HTT variable-number-tandem-repeat polymorphisms (n = 78). The presence of alleles with a low genic expression in the transcription control site in the serotonin transporter gene 5-HTTLPR (S/S, LG/S, Lg/Lg) was strongly associated to a risk twice as large of aggressiveness at infancy compared to individuals with high expression alleles (n = 77, p = 0.049, OR = 2.37, CI = 1.10-5.8). Similar results were shown by Haberstick et al., who conducted a prospective longitudinal study with a sample of 366 families with twin offspring that were followed- up on in a Longitudinal Twin Study (n = 1187).14 The allele -S 5HTTLPR was associated to high levels of aggressive behavior in school-aged children (p = 0.0779). Other studies have established the same association with aggressiveness at adulthood,30,31 thus leading to the conclusion that the presence of low expression alleles in adults is indeed associated with extreme violence.

3. Dopamine

Guo et al. conducted a study on the influence of the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1), which codifies the production of the dopamine transporter protein responsible for the uptake of dopamine in the synaptic cleft, and of the receptor gene (DRD2).15 Based on a population sample of approximately 2,500 adolescents and young adults from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, the authors concluded that the presence of 10 repetitions of 40 pairs of nitrogenous bases at region 3' doubles the risk of adolescents and young adults (p = 0.018) of becoming involved in violent and delinquent acts, while heterozygosis of DRD2 increases the risk by 20%, and homozygosis doubles the risk of the variation studied (p = 0.005).

Some of the studies investigated the relationship between the various polymorphisms in the dopamine receptor genes (DRD2 e DRD4) responsible for the production of receptors D2 and D4, respectively. Variations of the DRD2 gene as to the presence of alleles 1 or 2 and of seven repetitions in 48 pairs of nitrogenous bases in the DRD4 gene were investigated. The presence of allele 1 in the DRD2 gene proved to be associated with a higher risk of developing violent behavior in isolation32 and in association with a variant of the DRD4 gene.33,34 Individuals with isolated variations in the DRD4 gene did not present higher rates of violent behavior.33

4. COMT

Some of the studies investigated the presence of polymorphism in the COMT enzyme transcriptor gene. In a population of 353 individuals of which 180 suffered from schizophrenia and 173 were controls, Jones et al observed that patients who were homozygous for the polymorphism that determines the low activity level of the COMT gene presented significantly higher aggressiveness scores (OR = 2.07) when compared to heterozygous patients (OR = 0.54).16 While also studying a population of schizophrenic patients who had committed homicide (n = 30), compared to nonviolent schizophrenics (n = 62) and to a control group (n = 415) with regards to the presence of the valine or methionine alleles,17 Kotler et al. did not find a statistically significant difference between non-violent schizophrenics and the control group, although in the homicidal schizophrenics, the frequency of the low activity COMT genotype (p = 0.087) was higher. The presence of the methionine allele is related to the gene's low activity which, in turn, is associated with a higher risk for the development of aggressive and antisocial behavior. The authors believe that the gene's low activity is related to deficits in the prefrontal cortex, thus reducing the control of aggressive impulses.17

Complications in the prenatal period and at birth

There are known associations between complications experienced during the prenatal period and at birth and the development of behavioral problems at infancy. The main factors include maternal exposure to alcohol, tobacco and cocaine during pregnancy, maternal malnutrition and hypoxia at birth. Brennan et al. carried out a longitudinal prospective study with a sample of 2,127 sons whose mothers had smoked tobacco during the third gestational trimester and who, as adults, had been arrested for perpetrating violent and non-violent crimes.18 They concluded that intrauterine exposure to tobacco constitutes a marked risk factor for the development of an aggressive and antisocial behavior (p < 0.001). In a longitudinal prospective study comparing twins born to 467 mothers who had smoked during pregnancy and 898 twins born to mothers who had not smoked during pregnancy, Orlebeke et al.,19 observed that smoking during pregnancy is strongly associated to the externalization of problems such as opposition, aggressiveness and hyperactivity. Animal studies have related the intrauterine exposure to tobacco to damages in the noradrenergic system, reduced levels of dopamine and serotonin, reduced levels of glucose in the brain, and lesions to the basal ganglions and the cerebral and cerebellar cortexes.35,36

Fetal exposure to alcohol can damage several brain structures, including the corpus callosum. Violent behavior has been associated to lesions to the corpus callosum. In a case-control study, Roebuck et al. studied two groups of children where the first group had been heavily exposed to alcohol (n = 32) during the intrauterine period, while the second group had not been exposed to alcohol (n = 32).20 Children exposed to alcohol during pregnancy presented, more frequently, impairments in terms of cognition and psychosocial functioning such as loss of social abilities, increased hostility, impulsivity and emotional lability, as well as being more involved with delinquency (T-score = 76.28).

Delaney-Black et al. investigated the impact of fetal exposure to cocaine by associating this exposure to an increase in delinquency levels.21 A longitudinal prospective study with 201 children who had been exposed to cocaine and 270 who had not been exposed concluded that the first group presented more intense externalizing and delinquent behaviors (p = 0.018), with young boys being more vulnerable. Because cocaine affects monoaminergic systems, intrauterine exposure interferes with the development of these system's circuitry. These studies, however, present certain limitations in terms of the samples used in that these included confounding variables used such as family environment where there was very little stimulation, poverty, maternal nutrition and the use of other drugs.21,37

Intrauterine hypoxia or hypoxia at birth may damage various brain structures, including the hippocampus, which is involved in the control of aggressive behavior and appears to be the most sensitive. Complications at birth are now being associated with adult violent behavior when in combination with the presence of a hostile environment where there is, for example, early maternal rejection at infancy.35 Maternal malnutrition characterized by a poor intake of proteins, if experienced at the first and second trimesters, has been associated with adult antisocial behavior.35,38 Minor physical abnormalities at birth suggest poor neural development and may be associated with the delinquent and violent behavior seen in children and adolescents.39

Childhood malnutrition

Liu et al. carried out a longitudinal prospective study to evaluate the association between malnutrition experienced until the age of 3 and aggressive behavior at ages 8, 11 and 17 in a cohort of 1,795 children of which 353 had experienced malnourishment at age 3. A direct association between the degree of malnutrition and externalizing behavior (aggressiveness and hyperactivity, p = 0.04 at 8 and 17 years of age), as well as a reduced intelligence coefficient, was observed.22

Other findings

Other risk factors relating to aggressive behavior were identified during the selection process of the articles to be included in this review. Such studies were not selected and were, therefore, not listed in Table 1 either because their samples were not significant or because they were neither longitudinal nor statistically powerful studies. Although further studies are required, what follows next is a brief description of all the data collected which, in our opinion, seemed most relevant.

1. Neurological changes resulting in aggressive behavior

1) Dysfunctions of the frontal lobe

The frontal lobe is related to abilities such as making decisions, planning, proceeding with and giving continuance and coherence to one's acts, monitoring, making assessments, adjusting one's behavior according to internal and external drivers, modulating feelings and controlling an individual's behavior. It reaches full maturity at adolescence.40

The prefrontal region is associated with the control and regulation of emotions, reactions and impulses generated by the limbic system. Lesions to the prefrontal areas compromise the control of the subcortical areas, thus enhancing negative emotional reactions and violent behavior.

Neurophychopathological theories41 have established a relationship between aggressive behavior and damages to 1) the prefrontal areas and its connections with the brain's subcortical areas and 2) the temporal areas associated with limbic structures. This damage could result in deficits in the executive functions of attention, concentration, memory and finer mental processes, in the wrong interpreting of both stimuli and adverse events, and in an inability to regulate impulses.42,43

Patients with frontal lesions display poorer emotional control and are less able to judge the impact of their behavior. They have difficulty in establishing empathy and conducting critical assessments of their dysfunctional behaviors, which they tend repeat.41

Ventral and orbital prefrontal lesions are associated with disinhibition, increased impulsiveness and greater predisposition to engage in violent behavior.35 The earlier an individual suffers a lesion, the greater the impact on his or her internal controls. In both children and adolescents, traumatic lesions due to traumatic head injuries (TBIs), especially to the frontal regions are associated to increased aggressive behavior.35 Structural and functional neuroimagining studies found slower glucose metabolism and activity rates in the prefrontal area of aggressors, as well as reduced prefrontal gray matter in antisocial individuals in the range of 11% when compared to controls.44

2) Dysfunctions of the temporal lobe

Lesions to the medial temporal lobe where the structures of the limbic system are located are associated with intermittent impulse control disorders characterized by episodes of extreme unprovoked anger, as well as with rather more disorganized and less directed aggressive behaviors. There may be memory and intellectual deficits, auditory and visual hallucinations, receptive language impairments, aggressiveness and reduced impulse control.40

3) Changes in neurotransmitters

Impulsive aggressive behavior is associated with an imbalance between the various neurotransmitter systems of the prefrontal cortex, especially that of the serotoninergic system, which is responsible for the regulation of the dopaminergic system. 5-HT2 receptors inhibit dopaminergic activity. In the prefrontal areas and anterior cingulum, serotoninnergic transmission controls emotions and behavioral responses; reduced serotoninergic activity is mainly related to impulsive aggressive behavior.44,45 Antisocial individuals have low levels of serotonin metabolites, 5-HIAA and high levels of homovanillic acid, which is a metabolite of dopamine in the cerebral spinal fluid.44 Animal studies have demonstrated that increased dopaminergic transmission in both the prefrontal cortex and the nucleus accumbens result in increased aggressiveness. The blocking of dopaminergic activity in humans reduces their ability to identify facial expressions of anger and decrease impulsivity-driven aggressiveness. In conclusion, reduced serotoninergic activity and increased dopaminergic activity are interconnected and associated with a higher risk of violent behavior.

4) Hormonal

High levels of testosterone are associated with aggressive behavior.46,47 Women who have committed crimes tend to become more aggressive during their menstrual period. This can be attributed to the low estrogen levels that are typical of this period. High cortisol levels may be associated with persistent aggressive behavior in men.48

5) Nutritional

Nutritional deficiencies are risk factors for the development of aggressive behavior.49 Studies have shown that animals on a tryptophan-poor diet presented high rates of aggressive behavior.50 In children, zinc and/or iron deficiencies are also related to increased aggressive behavior51,52 since iron deficiency may reduce dopaminergic transmission.

6) Intoxications

Intoxications with metals such lead, copper and zinc predispose individuals to increased violent aggressive behavior. There is evidence, however, that an increase in the availability of calcium in the diet of intoxicated patients may have a positive effect,53 although this is still a preliminary result.

7) Gender

Aggressive behavior, especially of the more serious forms is more frequent in males. Some suggest that the reason why women do not manifest more aggressive forms of behavior is based on biological protection factors such as a larger corpus callosum, better interhemispheric communication, increased verbal ability and faster maturing of the frontal regions, thus promoting the development of cognitive and social abilities which, in turn, allow women to cope more easily with interpersonal problems. In addition to the biological gender differences, the fact that men are more exposed to environmental risk factors should be taken into account.54

Socio-environmental factors

In a longitudinal study, Farrington et al. identified the following key social predictors of violent and aggressive behavior: poverty, family history of criminality, precarious upbringing, failing at school, attention deficit, and hyperactivity and antisocial behavior during childhood.55

In Brazil, Schraiber et al. studied the main determining socio-environmental factors leading to aggressive behavior. They reached the conclusion that socio-economic inequalities, low pay, low family income, the lack of comprehensive public policies to meet the health, education, housing and security needs of the general population, the priority given to economic development to the detriment of social development, and the strong consumption appeal in contrast with the country's impoverishment are associated with increased risk for the extremely aggressive kind of behavior seen in the case of homicides.56 From a socio-environmental standpoint, more aggressive individuals have a deficit in processing social information i.e., their ability to process information, interpret and ponder the risks and benefits of his or her actions.57 According to the socio-environmental theories, violence and aggressiveness are acquired. The social learning theory believes that children acquire values and norms from the social group to which they belong, and that they do so through their own experiences and by witnessing their groups' social behavior. If a child is properly rewarded or punished for its behavior, if it is given positive examples by its parents and if it doesn't witness family conflicts, then the child will be able to develop the cognitions and social skills required for interpreting social data, thus making it possible for the child to behave in an appropriate and non-violent fashion.57,58 According to the social learning theory, the main risk factors for conduct disorder, aggressiveness, delinquency and crime are low levels of self-control and self-regulation and exposure to delinquent peers.59,60

Children that are either inconsistently punished or rewarded, who witness family conflicts or whose parent is involved in crime are unable to develop the skills required for coping with social problems. Parents are responsible for passing on the first batch of information that allows children to develop their social skills; children born to negligent parents that engage in erratic discipline, are in conflict with each other or commit crimes exhibit early criminal behavior which, in turn, can be attributed to the maintenance of such behavior during their lifetime.35,55

Forms of abuse at infancy such as maternal rejection, inter-parental violence, negligent parenting, repeated loss of the primary caregiver, severe or inconsistent discipline and sexual and physical abuse constitute risk factors for the development of violent behavior at infancy which, in turn, is predictive of offensive, aggressive and antisocial behavior in adults.61-63 For example, in a longitudinal prospective study (n = 1318) (LONGSCAN), Kotch et al. identified early neglect as the most important kind of abuse at infancy and associated it to the development of aggressive behavior (p < 0.01).23 Development theories that give special emphasis to the impact of early experiences such as the breaking of an infant's bond with his or her parental figure suggest that early forms of abuse suffered early in life may be stronger predictors of aggressive behavior than forms of abuse suffered later in life.23

Studies have shown that young people who have witness parental violence have a risk factor for getting involved in violent love relationships. Children exposed to violence use violence to solve their own conflicts. For example, victims of sexual abuse are twice as likely to experience at least one violent episode in their relationships.64,65

Teenagers who spend time with delinquent peers are more likely to behave in a delinquent fashion.60 Their low level of self-control and strong impulsiveness, which may be mediated by genetic and environmental factors make them feel attracted to these groups and rejected by pro-social groups due to their limited verbal skills and their narrow pool of solutions to which they can resort to solve their social problems. Teenagers who are poorly attached to their mothers or whose parents are absent, poorly engaged or emotionally disconnected or, still, whose parents are overly permissive get more frequently involved with delinquency, the use of alcohol and drugs and are more likely to socialize with delinquent peers.

According to the social disintegration theory, another key point to the understanding of violent behavior is the failure on the part of social and community institutions in providing the resources needed to ensure basic needs, social acknowledgment and physical integrity.65

The three levels of social integration are the socio-structural level, i.e. access to material and cultural goods, the institutional level, i.e. power, and the socio-emotional level.66 The more socially integrated the subject is, the easier it is for him or her to accept the social rules in place. The likelihood and intensity of violent behavior increase to the extent that the subject experiences fear of disintegration, which, in turn, decreases the subject's ability to control his or her violent behavior. Lack of social acknowledgement may be offset by violent actions because these help quieting feelings of weakness and enhance self-esteem. Violence is used as a means to restore justice or command respect and affirm one's identity.

The emotional stress experienced by the parents may cause the family to disintegrate, thus affecting the children's ability to socialize due to feelings of frustration and insecurity, as well as to tensions and conflicts. Children who grow up with a negative self-image are more likely to display aggressive tendencies. The presence of someone with whom the child may share his or her concerns may, however, serve as a useful model for breaking the cycle of violence.67

Interactions between the physical and biological environments

The development of social skills depends upon the interaction between biological development/maturation and environmental experiences. The acquisition of social cognitive skills is influenced by deficits in the frontal lobe and inter-hemispheric communication, socializing with both parents and peers and by verbal skills. Aggressive and antisocial individuals act on impulse, i.e. they do not think before they act, they have difficulties in abstracting and deficits in solving interpersonal cognitive problems, they are egotistical, show very little empathy and have difficulty in grasping what other people may be thinking or accepting the fact that other people may think differently.57,68 These cognitive deficits may place the individual in a socially disadvantageous position, making him or her even more susceptible to respond aggressively and to become involved with crime. Because the environment can eliminate certain behaviors while rewarding others, it is possible for individuals who are biologically vulnerable to aggressive and antisocial behavior to acquire, through learning, a more adaptive behavior placed in a more stimulating environment. Genetic factors interact with environmental factors to produce aggressive behavior. Genetic polymorphism alone does not suffice to explain the development of a certain behavior; however, it might make it more likely for certain behaviors to appear.69 As already mentioned, studies that have evaluated the impact of a polymorphism of the MAOA gene demonstrated that genetic influence on behavior is only seen in the presence of abuse at infancy.12,28

Ferguson developed an antisocial behavior model known as the catalyst model, according to which genetic factors and environmental effects such as exposure to family violence interact to produce a personality who is more inclined to behave extremely violently.69 This model also claims that human beings have a system located in the frontal lobes that controls their impulses, thus inhibiting the expression of aggressive behaviors. Because failures in this system are strongly influenced by genetic factors (50% to 90% inheritance), a hostile and stressful environment - especially with respect to family members or caregivers serves as a catalyst for aggressive behavior in predisposed individuals.5,60,69 Therefore, self-control and self-regulation are associated with genetic and social factors, i.e. socializing with parents, neighbors and schoolmates. Children who are raised in more favorable environments have more self-control. It can be concluded that biological and environmental factors act both independently and in association to produce antisocial and aggressive behavior.28 A risky environment might aggravate existing biological deficits while protective environments might attenuate these same deficits.

Conclusion

Several factors are involved in the development of aggressive behavior. Studies have provided evidence that biological and socio-environmental factors, as well as a combination of the two may be implicated in the development of aggressive, violent and antisocial behavior. According to the literature, more evidence has been gathered of the following biological risk factors: 1) genetic (low expression of the MAOA genotype in the serotonin transporter gene, polymorphism in the dopamine receptor genes i.e. DRD2 e DRD4 and in the dopamine transporter gene 2) complications experienced during the prenatal period (hypoxia, maternal malnutrition and exposure to alcohol and tobacco and 3) postnatal hypoxia, malnutrition at childhood and dysfunctions in the prefrontal cortex related to serotoninergic hypoactivity and dopaminergic hyperactivity resulting in the impairment of executive functions, ability to control oneself and inhibitory of the limbic system.

The socio-environmental factors that are most evident are related to the socio-environmental deficits present at childhood that compromise the development of cognitive skills which allow the child to identify and cope with social problems. The main impairment in terms of the learning process of social skills can be attributed to relationship problems with the child's parents. Children born to violent and negligent parents are unable to incorporate the values of their respective social groups. Moreover, it is worthwhile keeping in mind that these are not determining factors since there is always the possibility for the child of a violent parent to find in the person of another adult, at school or in other social network institutions the support it needs to find a way to insert itself other than through violence. It is also important to emphasize that this review did not search all databases, did not have the intention of identifying all the prospective studies available in the literature and, last but not least, did not attempt to aggregate results. Furthermore, with respect to the biological factors cited, this review assigned priority to quantitative studies while with regards to the socio-environmental factors, the main studies found were of a qualitative nature, except for the article that correlated early neglect in providing care and aggressive behavior. Nevertheless, we believe that the model that combines biological and socio-environmental factors is of a quantitative-qualitative nature. The study of aggressive behavior should not be based exclusively on one of these models because it is the combination of all the information that they provide that allows us to see this from a wider perspective, similarly to the study of the phenomenon of violence in our society.

The main conclusion of this review is that the interaction between biological and environmental factors may be catalyzed by a hostile environment, thus increasing the risk for the development of aggressive behaviors or, alternatively, that an environment favorable to one's development may attenuate unfavorable genetic characteristics, thus restraining the development of aggressiveness.69

Acknowledgements

JJM is a Level I researcher at the Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq). This study is part of a thematic project led by the São Paulo State Research Foundation (FAPESP, 2004/15039-0) and the Millennium Institute Mental Health and Violence of CNPq (420122/2005-2). AFM is a researcher pursuing her post-doctoral degree at UNIFESP. She is also a physician working for the Service and Research Program on Violence (PROVE), CNPq sponsorship process ID: 575114/2008-8 bid nº 33/2008 - Mental Health.

Click to enlarge

References

  • 1. World Health Organization. WHA 49.25 Prevention of violence: a public health priority. Forty-ninth Assembly May 20-25. Geneva: World Health Association; 1996.
  • 2. World Health Organization. World report on violence and health. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2002.
  • 3. Baron R, Richardson D. Human aggression New York: Plenum Press; 1994.
  • 4. Berkowitz N. Aggression: its causes, consequences and control New York: McGraw-Hill; 1993.
  • 5. Ferguson CJ, Beaver KM. Natural born killers: the genetic origins of extreme violence. Aggression Violent Behav 2009;14(5):286-94.
  • 6
    6. REDE Interagencial de informação para a saúde. Indicadores básicos para a saúde no Brasil: conceitos e aplicações 2a ed. Brasília: Organização Pan-americana da Saúde; 2008.
  • 7. Peres MFT. Violência e saúde no Brasil. In: Mello MF, Bressan RA, Andreoli SB, Mari JJ. Transtorno de estresse pós-traumático - diagnóstico e tratamento Barueri, São Paulo; 2006.
  • 8. Schramm JMA, Oliveira AF, Leite IC, Valente JG, Gadelha AM, Portela MC, Campos MR. Transição epidemiológica e o estudo da carga de doença no Brasil. Ciênc Saúde Coletiva 2004;9(4):897-908.
  • 9. Mari JJ, Mello MF, Figueira I. The impact of urban violence on mental health. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2008;30(3):183-4.
  • 10. Buss D, Shackelford T. Human aggression in evolutionary psychological perspective. Clin Psychol Rev. 1997;17(6):605-19.
  • 11. Okami P, Shackelford T. Human sex differences in sexual psychology and behavior. Ann Rev Sex Res. 2001;12:186-241.
  • 12. Widom CS, Brzustowicz LM. MAOA and the "cycle of violence:" chidhood abuse and neglect, MAOA genotype, and risk for violent and antisocial behavior. Biol Psychiatry 2006;60(7):684-9.
  • 13. Beitchman JH, Baldassarra L, Mik H, De Luca V, King N, Bender D, Ehtesham S, Kennedy JL. Serotonin transporter polymorphisms and persistent, pervasive childhood aggression. Am J Psychiatry 2006;163(6):1103-5.
  • 14. Haberstick BC, Smolen A, Hewitt JK. Family-based association test of the 5HTTLPR and aggressive behavior in a general population sample of children. Biol Psychiatry 2006;59(9):836-43.
  • 15. Guo G, Roettger ME, Shih JC. Contributions of the DAT1 and DRD2 genes to serious and violence delinquency among adolescents and young adults. Hum Genet. 2007;121(1):125-36.
  • 16. Jones G, Zammit S, Norton N, Hamshere MI, Jones SL, Milham C, Sanders RD, McCarthy GM, Jones LA, Cardno AG, Gray M, Murphy KC, Owen MJ. Aggressive behaviour in patients with schizophrenia is associated with catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype. Br J Psychiatry. 2001;179:351-5.
  • 17. Kotler M, Barak P, Cohen H, Averbuch IE, Grinshpoon A, Gritsenko I. Homicidal behavior in schizophrenia associated with a genetic polymorphism determining low COMT activity. Am J Med Genet 1999;88(6):628-33.
  • 18. Brennan PA, Grekin ER, Mednick SA. Maternal smoking during pregnancy and adult male criminal outcomes. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999;56(3):215-9.
  • 19. Orlebeke JF, Knol DL, Verhulst FC. Increase in child behavior problems resulting from maternal smoking during pregnancy. Arch Environ Health 1997;52(4):317-21.
  • 20. Roebuck TM, Mattson SN, Riley EP. Behavioral and psychosocial profiles of alcohol-exposed children. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1999;23(6):1070-6.
  • 21. Delaney-Black V, Covington C, Templin T, Ager J, Nordstrom-Klee B, Martier S, Leddick L, Czerwinski RH, Sokol RJ. Teacher-assessed behavior of children prenatally exposed to cocaine. Pediatrics 2000;106(4):782-91.
  • 22. Liu JH, Raine A, Venables P, Dalais C Mednick SA. Malnutrition at ages 3 years predisposes to externalizing behavior problems at ages 8, 11 and 17 years. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161(11):2005-13.
  • 23. Kotch JB, Lewis T, Hussey JM, English D, Thompson R, Litrownik AJ, Runyan DK, Bangdiwala SI, Margolis B, Dubowitz H. Importance of early neglect for childhood agression. Pediatrics 2008;121(4):725-31.
  • 24. Rowe DC, Stever C, Chase D, Sherman S, Abramowitz A, Waldman ID. Two dopamine receptor genes related to reports of childhood retrospective inattention and conduct disorder symptoms. Mol Psychiatry 2001;6(4):429-33.
  • 25. Deckert J, Catalano M, Syagailo YV, Bosi M, Okladnova O, Bella D, Nöthen MM, Maffei P, Franke P, Fritze J, Maier W, Propping P, Beckmann H, Bellodi L, Lesch KP. Excess of high activity monoamine oxidase A gene promotor alleles in female patients with panic disorder. Hum Mol Genet. 1999;8(4):621-4.
  • 26. Denney RM, Koch H, Craig IW. Association between monoamine oxidase A activity in human male skin fibroblasts and genotype of the MAOA promoter-associated variable number tandem repeat. Hum Genet 1999;105(6):542-51.
  • 27. Sabol SZ, Hu S, Hamer D. A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Hum Genet 1998;103(3):273-9.
  • 28. Caspy A, McClay J, Moffit TE, Mill J, Martin J, Craig IA. Role of genotype in cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science 2002;297(5582):851-4.
  • 29. Foley DL, Eaves LJ, Wormley B, Silberg JL, Maes HH, Kuhn J, Riley B. Childhood adversity, monoamine oxidase A genotype, and risk for conduct disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61(7):738-44.
  • 30. Retz W, Retz-Junginger P, Supprian T, Thome J, Rosler M. Association of serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism with violence: relation with personality disorders, impulsivity and childhood ADHD psychopathology. Behav Sci Law 2004;22(3):415-25.
  • 31. Liao DL, Hong CJ, Shih HL, Tsai SJ. Possible association between serotonin transporter promoter region polymorphism and extremely violent crime in Chinese males. Neuropsychobiology 2004;50():284-7.
  • 32. Eisenberg DT, MacKillop J, Modi M, Beauchemin J, Dang D, Lisman SA, Lum JK, Wilson DS. Examining impulsivity as an endophenotype using a behavioral approach: a DRD2 TaqI A and DRD4 48-bp VNTR association study. Behav Brain Funct 2007;3:2.
  • 33. Beaver KM, Wright JP, DeLisi M, Walsh A, Vaughn MG, Boisvert D, Vaske J. A gene x gene interaction between DRD2 and DRD4 is associated with conduct disorder and antissocial behavior in males. Behav Brain Funct 2007;3:30.
  • 34. Noble EP, Gottschalk LA, Fallon JH, Ritchie T, Wu JC. D2 dopamine polymorphism and brain regional glucose metabolism. Am J Med Genet 1997;74(2):162-6.
  • 35. Liu JH. Prenatal and perinatal complications as predispositions to externalizing behavior. J Prenatal Perinatal Psychol Health 2004;18(4):301-11.
  • 36. Raine A. Annotation: the role of prefrontal deficits low autonomic arousal and early health factors in the development of antissocial and aggressive behavior in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002;43(4):417-34.
  • 37. Singer LT, Arendt R, Minnes S, Farkas K, Salvator A, Kirchner HL, Kliegman R. Cognitive and motor outcomes of cocaine-exposed infants. JAMA. 2002;287(15):1952-60.
  • 38. Neugebauer HL, Hoek HW, Susser E. Prenatal exposure to wartime famine and development of antissocial personality disorder in early adulthood. JAMA 1999;282(5):455-62.
  • 39. Arseneault L, Tremblay RE, Boulerice B Seguin JR, Saucier JF. Minor physical anomalies and family adversity as risk factors for violent delinquency in adolescence. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157(6):917-23.
  • 40. Golden CJ, Jackson ML, Peterson-Rohne A, Gontkovsky ST. Neuropsychological correlates of violence and aggression: a review of the clinical literature. Aggression Violent Behav 1996;1(1):3-25.
  • 41. Jones H. Neuropsychology of violence. Forensic Reports 1984;5:221-33.
  • 42. Moffitt TE. The neuropsychological studies of juvenile delinquency: a critical review. In: Tonry M, Morris N, editors. Crime and Justice: a review of the literature Chicago: University of Chicago; 1990.
  • 43. Morgan AB, Lilienfeld SO. A meta-analytic review of the relation between antissocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clin Psychol Rev 2000;20(1):113-36.
  • 44. Seo D, Patrick CJ, Kennealy PJ. Role of serotonin and dopamine system interactions in the neurobiology of impulsive aggression and its comorbidity with other clinical disorders. Aggression Violent Behav 2008;13:382-95.
  • 45. Moore T, Scarpa A, Raine A. A meta-analysis of serotonin metabolite 5-HIAA and antissocial behavior. Aggressive Behav 2002;28:299-316.
  • 46. Book AS, Starzyk KB, Qunisey VL. The relationship between testosterone and aggression: a meta-analysis. Aggression Violent Behav 2001;6:579-99.
  • 47. Liu J, Wuerker A. Biosocial bases of aggressive and violent behavior-implications for nursing studies. Int J Nurs Stud. 2005; 42(2):229-41.
  • 48. McBurnett K, Lahey BB, Rathouz PJ, Loeber R. Low salivary cortisol and persistent aggression in boys referred for disruptive behavior. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57(1):38-43.
  • 49. Fishbein D. Biobehavioral perspectives in criminology Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning; 2001.
  • 50. Bjork M, Dougherty DM, Moeller FG, Cherek DR, Swann AC. The effects of tryptophan depletion and loading on laboratory aggression in men: time course and a food-restricted control. Psychopharmacology 1999;142(1):24-30.
  • 51. Watts DL. Trace elements and neuropsychological problems as reflected in tissue mineral analysis (TMA) patterns. J Orthomolecular Med 1990;5:159-66.
  • 52. Werbach M. Nutritional influences on aggressive behavior. J Orthomolecular Med 1995;7:45-51.
  • 53. Bodjen JD, Oleske JM, Louria DB. Lead poisoning-one approach to a problem that won't go away. Environ Health Perspect. 1997;105(12):1284-7.
  • 54. Harris MB. Aggression, gender and ethnicity. Aggression Violent Behav 1996;1(2):123-46.
  • 55. Farrington DP. Predictors, causes and correlates of male youth violence. In: Tonry M, Moore M, editors. Youth violence Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1998. p.421-75.
  • 56. Schraiber LB, D'Oliveira AF, Couto MT. Violência e saúde: estudos científicos recentes. Rev Saude Publica 2006;40(n Esp):112-20.
  • 57. Bennett S, Farrington DP, Huesmann R. Explaining gender differences in crime and violence: the importance of social cognitive skills. Aggression Violent Behav 2005;10:263-88.
  • 58. Miller N, Dollard J. Social learning and imitation New Haven, NJ: Yale University Press; 1941.
  • 59. Akers RL, Jensen GF. The empirical status of social learning theory of crime and deviance: the past, present and future. In: Cullen FT, Wright JP, Blevins KR, editors. Taking stock: the status of criminological theory New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers; 2006. v.15, p.37-76. (Advances in Criminological Theory Series ).
  • 60. Beaver KM, Shutt JE, Boutwell, BB, Ratchford M, Roberts K, Barnes JC. Genetic and environmental influences on levels of self-control and delinquent peer affiliation: results from a longitudinal sample of adolescent twins. Crim Justice Behav 2009;36:41-60.
  • 61. Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, Fletcher K. Young adult follow-up of hyperactive children: antissocial activities and drug use. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004;45(2):195-211.
  • 62. Deater-Deckard K, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS. Multiple risk factors in the development of externalizing behavior problems: group and individual differences. Dev Psychopathol. 1998;10(3):469-93.
  • 63. Reif A, Rösler M, Freitag CM, Schneider M, Eujen A, Kissling C, Wenzier D, Jacob CP, Retz-Junginger P, Thome J, Lesch KP, Retz W. Nature and nurture predispose to violent behavior: serotonergic genes and adverse childhood environment. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32(11):2375-83.
  • 64. Cyr M, Mc Duff P, Wright J. Prevalence and predictors of dating violence among adolescent female victims of child sexual abuse. J Interpers Violence. 2006;21(8):1000-17.
  • 65. Lewis SF, Fremouw W. Dating violence: a critical review of the literature. Clin Psychol Rev 2001;21(1):105-27.
  • 66. Anhut R, Heitmeyer W. Desintegration, Konflikt und Ethnisierung. Eine Problemanalyse und theoretische Rahmenkonzeption. In: Heitmeyer W, Anhut R, editors. Bedrohte Stadtgesellschaft. Gesellschaftliche Desintegrationprozesse and ethnisch-kulturelle Konfliktkonstelltionen Weinheim: Juventa; 2000. p.48.
  • 67. Heitmeyer W, Anhut R. Disintegration, recognition, and violence: a theorical perspective. New directions for youth development. 2008;119:25-37.
  • 68. Ross RR, Fabiano EA. Time to think: a cognitive model of delinquency prevention and offender rehabilitation Johnson City TN: Institute of Social Sciences and Arts; 1985.
  • 69. Ferguson CJ. An evolutionary approach to understanding violent antissocial behavior: diagnostic implications for duel-process etiology. J Forensic Psychol Practice 2008;8(4):321-43.
  • Correspondência:

    Deise Daniela Mendes
    Rua Botucatu, 431 - Vila Clementino
    04023-061 São Paulo, SP, Brasil
    Fone: (+55 11) 5082-2860
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      24 Nov 2009
    • Date of issue
      Oct 2009
    Associação Brasileira de Psiquiatria Rua Pedro de Toledo, 967 - casa 1, 04039-032 São Paulo SP Brazil, Tel.: +55 11 5081-6799, Fax: +55 11 3384-6799, Fax: +55 11 5579-6210 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: editorial@abp.org.br