Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Neuropsychological dysfunction in adults with early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder: the search for a cognitive endophenotype

Abstract

Objective:

Evidence suggests that early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is an etiologically distinct subtype of OCD. The objective of the present work was to search for neurocognitive endophenotypes of early-onset OCD based on assessments of attention, memory, and executive function in patients with the disorder and their unaffected siblings.

Methods:

We compared the performance of 40 adult patients with early-onset OCD, 40 of their unaffected siblings, and 40 unrelated healthy controls on a neuropsychological battery designed for this study. We searched for associations among test performance, demographic variables (age, sex and years of education) and clinical symptoms of early-onset OCD.

Results:

Patients performed significantly worse than healthy controls on the Tower of Hanoi, and the Stroop and Wisconsin tests, indicating impairments in planning, mental flexibility and inhibitory control. The performance of the unaffected first-degree siblings of patients with early-onset OCD on the Stroop and Wisconsin tests also differed from that of healthy controls. Symptom severity in early-onset OCD was strongly correlated with performance on the Tower of Hanoi.

Conclusions:

Our findings support the existence of specific executive function deficits in patients with early-onset OCD. Relatives presented an intermediate phenotype between patients and controls, suggesting that executive functions such as mental flexibility and response inhibition may be considered candidate endophenotypes of early-onset OCD.

Executive functions; endophenotype; mental flexibility; response inhibition; obsessive-compulsive disorder


Introduction

Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is a highly heterogeneous condition, whose etiology and pathogenesis are still poorly understood. Dysfunctions in orbitofrontal-striatal circuits have been hypothesized to play a crucial role in the pathophysiology of OCD.11. Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1986;9:357-81.,22. Maia TV, Cooney RE, Peterson BS. The neural bases of obsessive-compulsive disorder in children and adults. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20:1251-83. This condition is associated with significant neurocognitive impairment and concomitant functional disability. Neural network abnormalities in both pediatric and adult OCD have been associated with deficits in a variety of cognitive domains, including visuospatial processing abilities, cognitive flexibility, set-shifting and various executive functions.33. Kuelz AK, Hohagen F, Voderholzer U. Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a critical review. Biol Psychol. 2004;65:185-236.

4. Cavedini P, Riboldi G, D'Annucci A, Belotti P, Cisima M, Bellodi L. Decision-making heterogeneity in obsessive-compulsive disorder: ventromedial prefrontal cortex function predicts different treatment outcomes. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:205-11.

5. Olley A, Malhi G, Sachdev P. Memory and executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a selective review. J Affect Disord. 2007;104:15-23.

6. Bohne A, Savage CR, Deckersbach T, Keuthen NJ, Jenike MA, Tuschen-Caffier B, et al. Visuospatial abilities, memory, and executive functioning in trichotillomania and obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2005;27:385-99.
-77. Huyser C, Veltman DJ, Wolters LH, de Haan E, Boer F. Functional magnetic resonance imaging during planning before and after cognitive-behavioral therapy in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:1238-48. The primary neuropsychological impairment in OCD lies in executive functions such as working memory, cognitive flexibility, and response inhibition.55. Olley A, Malhi G, Sachdev P. Memory and executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a selective review. J Affect Disord. 2007;104:15-23.,88. Brock LL, Brock CD, Thiedke CC. Executive function and medical non-adherence: a different perspective. Int J Psychiatry Med. 2011;42:105-15.,99. Diniz JB, Miguel EC, de Oliveira AR, Reimer AE, Brandão ML, de Mathis MA, et al. Outlining new frontiers for the comprehension of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a review of its relationship with fear and anxiety. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2012;34:S81-91. These deficits have been hypothesized to mediate the relationship between brain dysfunction and clinical symptomatology in patients with OCD.1010. Chamberlain SR, Blackwell AD, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:399-419. This observation is supported by studies which show a direct relationship between anterior cingulate (ACC) activity and cognitive control, since the conflict-monitoring processes in the ACC appear to be involved in the engagement of this cognitive ability.1111. Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW 3rd, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science. 2004;303:1023-6. The core symptoms of OCD are persistent, obsessive thoughts accompanied by an inability to inhibit the compulsive repetition of behaviors or mental acts. The high demands on conflict-monitoring observed in OCD may lead to ACC dysfunction, resulting in impaired cognitive control. Two meta-analyses have found that the presence of executive impairments in patients with OCD is mainly determined by the integrity of orbitofrontal-striatal circuitry, which includes the orbitofrontal cortex, the striatum and the ACC.1212. Chamberlain SR, Blackwell AD, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:399-419.,1313. Menzies L, Chamberlain SR, Laird AR, Thelen SM, Sahakian BJ, Bullmore ET. Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder: the orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:525-49. However, it has also been hypothesized that impaired planning capacity in both pediatric77. Huyser C, Veltman DJ, Wolters LH, de Haan E, Boer F. Functional magnetic resonance imaging during planning before and after cognitive-behavioral therapy in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:1238-48. and adult OCD1414. van den Heuvel OA, Veltman DJ, Groenewegen HJ, Cath DC, van Balkom AJ, van Hartskamp J, et al. Frontal-striatal dysfunction during planning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:301-9. is linked to dysfunctions in dorsolateral-striatal circuitries implicated in the neuropathology of OCD, whereas deficits in inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility may be more closely associated with orbitofrontal-striatal pathways.1515. Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, Suckling J, Fineberg NA, del Campo N, et al. Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science. 2008;321:421-2.

Patients with OCD show a bimodal distribution in the age of symptom onset, with an early-onset group centered at 11.1±4.1 years, and a late-onset group centered at 23.5±11.1 years.1616. Geller DA. Obsessive-compulsive and spectrum disorders in children and adolescents. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2006;29:353-70.,1717. Salum GA, DeSousa DA, do Rosario MC, Pine DS, Manfro GG. Pediatric anxiety disorders: from neuroscience to evidence-based clinical practice. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2013;35:S03-21. Early-onset OCD has been proposed to be an etiologically distinct subtype of the disorder,1818. Geller DA, Biederman J, Faraone S, Agranat A, Cradock K, Hagermoser L, et al. Developmental aspects of obsessive compulsive disorder: findings in children, adolescents, and adults. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2001;189:471-7.

19. Rosario-Campos MC, Leckman JF, Mercadante MT, Shavitt RG, Prado HS, Sada P, et al. Adults with early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1899-903.
-2020. Wang X, Cui D, Wang Z, Fan Q, Xu H, Qiu J, et al. Cross-sectional comparison of the clinical characteristics of adults with early-onset and late-onset obsessive compulsive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2012;136:498-504. associated with greater symptom severity,2020. Wang X, Cui D, Wang Z, Fan Q, Xu H, Qiu J, et al. Cross-sectional comparison of the clinical characteristics of adults with early-onset and late-onset obsessive compulsive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2012;136:498-504.,2121. Fontenelle LF, Mendlowicz MV, Marques C, Versiani M. Early- and late-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder in adult patients: an exploratory clinical and therapeutic study. J Psychiatr Res. 2003;37:127-33. a higher prevalence of tic-related disorders among patients and their first-degree relatives,2222. Millet B, Kochman F, Gallarda T, Krebs MO, Demonfaucon F, Barrot I, et al. Phenomenological and comorbid features associated in obsessive-compulsive disorder: influence of age of onset. J Affect Disord. 2004;79:241-6.,2323. Chabane N, Delorme R, Millet B, Mouren MC, Leboyer M, Pauls D. Early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder: a subgroup with a specific clinical and familial pattern? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46:881-7. a more familial form of the condition,2424. do Rosario-Campos MC, Leckman JF, Curi M, Quatrano S, Katsovitch L, Miguel EC, et al. A family study of early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;136B:92-7.,2525. Nestadt G, Lan T, Samuels J, Riddle M, Bienvenu OJ 3rd, Liang KY, et al. Complex segregation analysis provides compelling evidence for a major gene underlying obsessive-compulsive disorder and for heterogeneity by sex. Am J Hum Genet. 2000;67:1611-6.and a greater prevalence of psychiatry disorders in first-degree relatives2626. Hanna GL, Himle JA, Curtis GC, Gillespie BW. A family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder with pediatric probands. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;134B:13-9. as compared to late-onset OCD. Tourette syndrome and pervasive developmental disorder are also more likely to co-occur with early-onset OCD, suggesting that neurodevelopmental abnormalities of ventral prefrontal-striatal circuits may contribute to the etiology of both disorders.2727. Huyser C, Veltman DJ, de Haan E, Boer F. Paediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder, a neurodevelopmental disorder? Evidence from neuroimaging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:818-30. However, it is important to note that, in the present article, “early-onset” was operationalized to mean “onset before age 13” rather than pediatric OCD.

Endophenotypes have been defined as measurable components unseen by the unaided eye located on the continuum between disease and the distal genotype, and which can therefore be found in the unaffected family members of patients with particular disorders at a higher rate than in the general population.2828. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:636-45. Endophenotypes are associated with the illness, heritable, and primarily state-independent. With regards to OCD, neuropsychological impairments in executive functioning similar to those displayed by patients with the condition have also been identified in their unaffected first-degree relatives.2929. Menzies L, Achard S, Chamberlain SR, Fineberg N, Chen CH, del Campo N, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain. 2007;130:3223-36.

30. Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, Blackwell AD, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, et al. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:335-8.
-3131. Viswanath B, Janardhan Reddy YC, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T, Chandrashekar CR. Cognitive endophenotypes in OCD: a study of unaffected siblings of probands with familial OCD. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009;33:610-5.Neurocognitive impairments in abilities such as inhibitory control,1515. Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, Suckling J, Fineberg NA, del Campo N, et al. Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science. 2008;321:421-2.,3232. Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, Blackwell AD, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, et al. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:335-8.set-shifting,1515. Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, Suckling J, Fineberg NA, del Campo N, et al. Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science. 2008;321:421-2. cognitive flexibility,3030. Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, Blackwell AD, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, et al. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:335-8.,3333. Viswanath B, Janardhan Reddy YC, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T, Chandrashekar CR. Cognitive endophenotypes in OCD: a study of unaffected siblings of probands with familial OCD. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009;33:610-5. planning3434. Cavedini P, Zorzi C, Piccinni M, Cavallini MC, Bellodi L. Executive dysfunctions in obsessive-compulsive patients and unaffected relatives: searching for a new intermediate phenotype. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:1178-84. and decision making3333. Viswanath B, Janardhan Reddy YC, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T, Chandrashekar CR. Cognitive endophenotypes in OCD: a study of unaffected siblings of probands with familial OCD. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009;33:610-5. have all been suggested as potential endophenotype markers of OCD. Inhibitory and cognitive flexibility deficits have been found to be mediated by abnormalities in orbitofrontal-striatal circuitry in patients with OCD and their first-degree relatives.1515. Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, Suckling J, Fineberg NA, del Campo N, et al. Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science. 2008;321:421-2. There is also strong evidence to suggest that familial factors may influence the structural variation of orbitofrontal-striatal brain systems, which are known to be related to inhibitory control deficits.3030. Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, Blackwell AD, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, et al. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:335-8. Moreover, a longitudinal study has found that executive function deficits in childhood were strongly associated with OCD in adulthood,3535. Grisham JR, Anderson TM, Poulton R, Moffitt TE, Andrews G. Childhood neuropsychological deficits associated with adult obsessive-compulsive disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195:138-41. providing further evidence of executive dysfunction as a defining characteristic of OCD. In fact, studies suggest that executive dysfunction may be a trait-like marker of OCD, which has lead researchers to posit such deficits as potential neuropsychological endophenotypes associated with the genetics of the disease.3636. Rao NP, Reddy YC, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T, Chandrashekar CR. Are neuropsychological deficits trait markers in OCD? Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2008;32:1574-9. The identification of endophenotypes improves the chances of early detection and diagnosis and is therefore particularly important in the case of clinically heterogeneous conditions which vary in comorbidity and age of onset, as is the case of OCD.

Neuropsychological impairments in early-onset OCD have not been widely studied, and it is therefore unclear whether previous research on OCD pathophysiology, which typically combines both early- and late-onset cases, is applicable to this particular form of the condition. Additionally, few studies have examined whether unaffected relatives of patients with early-onset OCD show any disease characteristics which could be considered an endophenotypes for this subtype of disease.

To determine whether the neurocognitive deficits documented in previous OCD research are also present in early-onset cases, we administered a battery of tests to both adult patients with early-onset OCD and healthy unrelated controls so as to examine the following neurocognitive domains: attention, memory, and executive function. To investigate whether these neurocognitive deficits have a familial component, as has been found in other psychiatric disorders, the same battery of tests was also administered to unaffected first-degree siblings of patients with early-onset OCD. Additionally, we evaluated whether any of the neurocognitive deficits observed were related to clinical characteristics such as symptom severity, age, and duration of illness, as well as other features associated with impaired neurocognitive function.

Methods

Subjects

The sample consisted of 40 patients with early-onset OCD (onset age < 13), 40 healthy first-degree siblings, and 40 healthy controls. Recruitment and neuropsychological assessment were performed between May 2012 and June 2013. All subjects were Han Chinese in origin, and aged between 18 and 40 years. Patients with were recruited by local advertisement. Screening procedures included a telephone assessment conducted by two trained psychologist (JZ and QY). Of the 81 subjects who applied for participation, 15 were excluded during telephone screening for having an age of onset greater than age 14, 14 were excluded for not having siblings, three were excluded due to a history of head injury, four due to a comorbid diagnosis of major depressive disorder (MDD) and one due to a history of drug abuse identified during the psychiatric assessment. Of the 44 remaining patients in the sample, four discontinued participation due to an inability to remain without medication during the wash-out period. Control subjects were recruited from staff and students of the South West University. These participants reported no family history of OCD and no previous or current psychiatric disorders in first- and second- degree relatives. Each subject received 100 Chinese Yuan for participation. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the South West University, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Two experienced psychiatrists (NL and JW) evaluated patients with OCD using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders, Clinician Version (SCID-CV).3737. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59:22-33;quiz 4-57. Subjects had no other Axis I comorbidities. All patients underwent a 4-week washout period from medication and psychotherapy before the study. At the start of the study, the same two psychiatrists assessed patients using the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS), the 14-item Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HARS), and the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS). The presence and severity of tics were determined via interview and using the Yale Global Tic Severity Scale (YGTSS).3838. Leckman JF, Riddle MA, Hardin MT, Ort SI, Swartz KL, Stevenson J, et al. The Yale Global Tic Severity Scale: initial testing of a clinician-rated scale of tic severity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28:566-73.

Any siblings of OCD patients and healthy controls with a prior psychiatric diagnosis were excluded from the sample, as were participants with a history of intellectual disability, neurological illness, brain injury or trauma, or drug or alcohol abuse.

Neuropsychological measurement

The following neuropsychological tests were administered to all participants during one session, which lasted approximately 1 hour and 30 minutes:

Attention tests

The digit span test (forward and backward) from the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) III was used to evaluate attention span. The Trail Making Test (TMT) (times needed to complete task A and B) was used to evaluate attention (part A) and attention and psychomotor speed (part B).

Memory tests

The Logical Memory (LM) subtest of the WMS III assesses the immediate (LM1) and 30-minute delayed recall (LM2) of two stories told by a psychologist, while the Visual Reproduction (VR) test measures the number of details in two geometric figures which patients can recall both immediately (VR1) and after a delay (VR2).

Executive function tests

The Stroop Color Naming and Color/Word Interference test measures selective attention and response inhibition.3939. Zalonis I, Christidi F, Bonakis A, Kararizou E, Triantafyllou NI, Paraskevas G, et al. The stroop effect in Greek healthy population: normative data for the Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;24:81-8.In the interference task, participants are incongruently colored words and asked to name the color in which the word is printed, while inhibiting the automatic tendency to simply read the word. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) was used to assess cognitive flexibility and set-shifting abilities.4040. Sherer M, Nick TG, Millis SR, Novack TA. Use of the WCST and the WCST-64 in the assessment of traumatic brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2003;25:512-20. The total number of perseverative errors, non-perseverative errors, and categories completed were scored according to the test manual. The Tower of Hanoi (TOH) test measures planning and decision-making abilities.4141. Welsh MC, Revilla V, Strongin D, Kepler M. Towers of Hanoi and London: is the nonshared variance due to differences in task administration? Percept Mot Skills. 2000;90:562-72. Subjects must solve problems with the fewest possible steps as quickly as possible while following certain rules.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS version 17.0 for Windows. The sociodemographic characteristics of OCD patients, siblings and healthy controls were compared using chi-square analyses. Between-group comparisons of continuous variables were performed using univariate analysis of variance (one-way ANOVA). Significant ANOVAs were followed by Fisher's least significant difference (LSD) post-hoc test. The cognitive performance of OCD patients with and without tics was compared using Student's t-test.

Pearson correlations were performed to analyze the relationship between performance on the three executive function tests and the severity of OCD symptoms (Y-BOCS scores) or clinical characteristics which may influence cognitive performance (duration of illness, HARS scores, and HDRS scores). Differences were considered significant if p < 0.05.

Results

Demographic characteristics

The demographic characteristics of the three study groups are shown in Table 1. Twenty-six patients (65%) had tics. The distribution of YGTSS scores (range: 0-50) in the OCD group was as follows: 14 patients obtained a score of 0 (n=14), 16 obtained scores between 1 and 9 (n=16), and 10 scored between 10 and 19. Three of the OCD patients without tics had such symptoms during adolescence. Patients with and without tics did not differ on any neuropsychological test. All patients were exclusively right-handed. Additionally, OCD patients, their siblings and healthy controls did not differ in terms of age (F = 1.91, df = 2, 117, p = 0.152), gender (χ2 = 0.46, df = 2, p = 0.79) or years of education (F = 1.095, df = 2, 117, p = 0.339).

Table 1
Demographic and clinical characteristics of the three participant groups

Neuropsychological measures

Table 2 shows the neuropsychological test results of patients, healthy siblings and controls. A one-way ANOVA revealed significant between-group differences on the Stroop test and the WCST. Group differences on the Stroop test were observed on the Stroop Color Naming (F = 26.57, df = 2, 117, p < 0.001) and Color/Word Interference conditions (F = 26.16, df = 2, 117, p < 0.001). Significant group differences were also observed in the number of categories (F = 9.64, df = 2, 117, p < 0.001), perseverative errors (F = 7.39, df = 2,117, p = 0.001) and non-perseverative errors (F = 11.76, df = 2, 117, p < 0.001) on the WCST. Groups differed significantly on the TOH (F = 3.576, df = 2, 117, p = 0.031), which evaluates planning and decision-making abilities. However, no significant effects were identified on memory tests or the TMT.

Post-hoc comparisons revealed that patients with OCD and their first-degree siblings showed significantly poorer response inhibition on the Stroop test than controls, and made significantly more category, perseverative and non-perseverative errors on the WCST. However, patients and their siblings did not differ from each other on any of the tests administered. Lastly, patients with OCD showed worse performance than controls on the TOH (p = 0.026).

Table 2
Neurocognitive test scores for patients, siblings and controls

Correlation between neuropsychological performance and clinical symptoms

Pearson correlation analysis showed a strong relationship between TOH performance and Y-BOCS scores in adults with early-onset OCD (r = -0.548, p = 0.009, two-tailed). No significant associations were identified between Y-BOCS scores and performance on the WCST or Stroop test.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate neuropsychological performance in adults with early-onset OCD, their unaffected first-degree siblings and unrelated healthy controls with no family history of OCD. Our study revealed that both patients with OCD and their healthy relatives performed significantly worse than controls on the WCST and Stroop test. To our knowledge, this is the first study to report executive dysfunction in the unaffected relatives of patients with early-onset OCD. We suggest that executive dysfunction, particularly in the areas of mental flexibility and response inhibition, may be common to both adults with early-onset OCD and their unaffected siblings. Furthermore, our findings suggested that the severity of early-onset OCD symptoms is associated with impairments in planning ability.

The patients with early-onset OCD in the present sample showed poorer cognitive flexibility than controls, as indicated by their tendency to make more perseverative and non-perseverative errors and complete fewer categories on the WCST. These findings corroborate reports of impaired cognitive flexibility in pediatric OCD. Studies have found, for instance, that pediatric patients with OCD were slower to complete a set-shifting task4242. Britton JC, Stewart SE, Killgore WD, Rosso IM, Price LM, Gold AL, et al. Amygdala activation in response to facial expressions in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2010;27:643-51. than healthy controls, in addition to and making significantly more errors and completing fewer categories on the WCST than healthy participants of the same age.4343. Shin MS, Choi H, Kim H, Hwang JW, Kim BN, Cho SC. A study of neuropsychological deficit in children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2008;23:512-20. These results, together with the present study, suggest that early-onset OCD is associated with impairments in mental set-shifting and non-verbal abstract concept formation. fMRI studies have also found that set-shifting deficits in pediatric OCD were accompanied by lower frontal-striatal activation than that observed in healthy controls.4444. Britton JC, Rauch SL, Rosso IM, Killgore WD, Price LM, Ragan J, et al. Cognitive Inflexibility and Frontal-Cortical Activation in Pediatric Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:944-53.

Whether late-onset OCD is also associated with deficits in cognitive flexibility is still unclear, since previous studies of this cognitive process tend to evaluate mixed patient populations containing individuals with both early- and late-onset disease. Not surprisingly, these studies have yielded inconsistent results. Some have reported impaired mental set-shifting in OCD patients relative to healthy controls,4545. Bannon S, Gonsalvez CJ, Croft RJ, Boyce PM. Executive functions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: state or trait deficits? Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40:1031-8.,4646. Lawrence NS, Wooderson S, Mataix-Cols D, David R, Speckens A, Phillips ML. Decision making and set shifting impairments are associated with distinct symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology. 2006;20:409-19. while others have identified no such impairments.4747. Moritz S, Birkner C, Kloss M, Jacobsen D, Fricke S, Böthern A, et al. Impact of comorbid depressive symptoms on neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2001;110:653-7.,4848. Moritz S, Birkner C, Kloss M, Jahn H, Hand I, Haasen C, et al. Executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder, unipolar depression, and schizophrenia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2002;17:477-83.

These results suggest that, as in the case of pediatric OCD, adults with the early onset form of the disorder may also have deficits in cognitive flexibility. Some executive impairments may be state independent in early onset OCD. However, these findings should be interpreted with caution since all patients in the present study were medicated and most presented with symptom recurrence after the wash-out period. Therefore, cognitive performance in our sample may have been worse than that reported in other similar studies, and differences between subjects may have been more evident.

Adults with early-onset OCD also showed significantly lower response inhibition on the Stroop Color Naming Test and Word/Color Interference Test than healthy controls. This is consistent with previous reports suggesting that children with OCD show impaired performance in oculomotor tests of response inhibition.4949. Rosenberg DR, Averbach DH, O'Hearn KM, Seymour AB, Birmaher B, Sweeney JA. Oculomotor response inhibition abnormalities in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54:831-8.,5050. Rosenberg DR, Dick EL, O'Hearn KM, Sweeney JA. Response-inhibition deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: an indicator of dysfunction in frontostriatal circuits. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 1997;22:29-38.

Impairments in the inhibitory control of patients with pediatric OCD have also been detected in a multi-source interference task,5151. Liu Y, Gehring WJ, Weissman DH, Taylor SF, Fitzgerald KD. Trial-by-trial adjustments of cognitive control following errors and response conflict are altered in pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder. Front Psychiatry. 2012;3:41. during which patients failed to exhibit either post-error slowing or post-conflict adaptation of cognitive control. A similar loss of interference control has been reported in studies involving mixed samples of adults with early- and late-onset disease.2929. Menzies L, Achard S, Chamberlain SR, Fineberg N, Chen CH, del Campo N, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain. 2007;130:3223-36.,5252. Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Blackwell AD, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1282-4. The inability to inhibit repetitive thoughts or behaviors may explain the repetitive intrusive thoughts and compulsive behaviors which characterize OCD according to the DSM-IV. This impairment in response inhibition may lead patients with early-onset OCD to be less successful than those with late-onset disease at controlling intrusive thoughts (obsessions) and behaviors.

In addition to impairments in cognitive flexibility and response inhibition, our adult patients with early-onset OCD performed poorly on the TOH, suggesting impairments in planning and decision-making abilities. These findings are consistent with previous studies. An fMRI study found that pediatric patients with OCD performed a planning task significantly more slowly than healthy controls, albeit with similar accuracy, indicating impairments in planning ability.77. Huyser C, Veltman DJ, Wolters LH, de Haan E, Boer F. Functional magnetic resonance imaging during planning before and after cognitive-behavioral therapy in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:1238-48. Patients with OCD may be slower at such tasks because they spend more time checking for errors and generating alternative responses when such an error is found.5353. Veale DM, Sahakian BJ, Owen AM, Marks IM. Specific cognitive deficits in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med. 1996;26:1261-9. Our findings showed that individuals with early-onset OCD were slower than healthy controls even in the simpler problems on the TOH, suggesting significant difficulty facing novel situations.

Perhaps the most striking contribution of the present study was the finding that unaffected first-degree relatives of adult patients with early-onset OCD share many of the executive function deficits observed in the patients themselves. First-degree relatives performed significantly worse than healthy controls on the Stroop test and the WCST, suggesting that impaired cognitive flexibility and response inhibition may be trait markers for early-onset OCD which contribute to, rather than result from, the clinical symptoms of OCD. We also observed a strong negative correlation between performance on the TOH and symptom severity in adults with early-onset OCD. First-degree siblings did not appear to share all the executive function deficits of patients with early-onset OCD. This suggests that inhibitory control and flexibility, but not planning, may be considered part of the endophenotype of early-onset OCD. Neuroanatomical findings suggest that dysfunctions in the orbitofrontal-striatal circuitry, which are also directly associated with executive functions, may contribute to the pathophysiology of OCD.5454. Coetzer BR. Obsessive-compulsive disorder following brain injury: a review. Int J Psychiatr Med. 2004;34:363-77. Deficits in cognitive flexibility and response inhibition have also been linked to alterations in orbitofrontal-striatal circuitry in pediatric OCD.4444. Britton JC, Rauch SL, Rosso IM, Killgore WD, Price LM, Ragan J, et al. Cognitive Inflexibility and Frontal-Cortical Activation in Pediatric Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:944-53.

Previous studies of unaffected first-degree relatives of adult patients with OCD have reported cognitive deficits in various domains, such as planning,5555. Delorme R, Gousse V, Roy I, Trandafir A, Mathieu F, Mouren-Siméoni MC, et al. Shared executive dysfunctions in unaffected relatives of patients with autism and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2007;22:32-8. response inhibition, set-shifting,3030. Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, Blackwell AD, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, et al. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:335-8. and decision-making.3131. Viswanath B, Janardhan Reddy YC, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T, Chandrashekar CR. Cognitive endophenotypes in OCD: a study of unaffected siblings of probands with familial OCD. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009;33:610-5. While it is reassuring that our findings in early-onset OCD were similar to those obtained in earlier studies, comparisons between these findings should be made with caution, since earlier studies involved mixed samples of patients with early- and late-onset OCD. The success of our approach suggests that future studies can and should analyze the two subtypes of the disorder separately. Nevertheless, our results, together with those of previous studies, suggest that specific executive traits can be used as endophenotypes to dissect the genetic complexity of early-onset OCD.

The present study was the first to demonstrate familial resemblance in the performance of cognitive flexibility and response inhibition tests, but not in other measures of executive function such as Digit Span Backward, TOH, and TMT-Part B, which rely on abilities such as working memory, planning and executive control. This could be related to the fact that current data do not support the involvement of working memory and planning impairments in the endophenotype of adult early-onset OCD, since these abilities rely on dorsolateral frontal loops which are not as closely associated with the etiology of OCD. Additionally, we could not rule out all moderator variables known to influence cognition functions, such as chronicity of the disease, medication effects and symptom relapse after wash-out. In summary, our findings suggested that impairments in executive functions such as cognitive flexibility and response inhibition may be endophenotypic markers of early-onset OCD. Future studies should examine larger samples of patients and relatives, and perhaps combine neuropsychological assessments with imaging and genetic approaches, in order to elucidate the pathophysiology of early-onset OCD and its relationship to the late-onset form of the condition.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by a grant (# DHA120235) awarded to XY by the Ministry of Education of the People's Republic of China.

References

  • 1
    Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1986;9:357-81.
  • 2
    Maia TV, Cooney RE, Peterson BS. The neural bases of obsessive-compulsive disorder in children and adults. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20:1251-83.
  • 3
    Kuelz AK, Hohagen F, Voderholzer U. Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a critical review. Biol Psychol. 2004;65:185-236.
  • 4
    Cavedini P, Riboldi G, D'Annucci A, Belotti P, Cisima M, Bellodi L. Decision-making heterogeneity in obsessive-compulsive disorder: ventromedial prefrontal cortex function predicts different treatment outcomes. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:205-11.
  • 5
    Olley A, Malhi G, Sachdev P. Memory and executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a selective review. J Affect Disord. 2007;104:15-23.
  • 6
    Bohne A, Savage CR, Deckersbach T, Keuthen NJ, Jenike MA, Tuschen-Caffier B, et al. Visuospatial abilities, memory, and executive functioning in trichotillomania and obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2005;27:385-99.
  • 7
    Huyser C, Veltman DJ, Wolters LH, de Haan E, Boer F. Functional magnetic resonance imaging during planning before and after cognitive-behavioral therapy in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:1238-48.
  • 8
    Brock LL, Brock CD, Thiedke CC. Executive function and medical non-adherence: a different perspective. Int J Psychiatry Med. 2011;42:105-15.
  • 9
    Diniz JB, Miguel EC, de Oliveira AR, Reimer AE, Brandão ML, de Mathis MA, et al. Outlining new frontiers for the comprehension of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a review of its relationship with fear and anxiety. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2012;34:S81-91.
  • 10
    Chamberlain SR, Blackwell AD, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:399-419.
  • 11
    Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW 3rd, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science. 2004;303:1023-6.
  • 12
    Chamberlain SR, Blackwell AD, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:399-419.
  • 13
    Menzies L, Chamberlain SR, Laird AR, Thelen SM, Sahakian BJ, Bullmore ET. Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder: the orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:525-49.
  • 14
    van den Heuvel OA, Veltman DJ, Groenewegen HJ, Cath DC, van Balkom AJ, van Hartskamp J, et al. Frontal-striatal dysfunction during planning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:301-9.
  • 15
    Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, Suckling J, Fineberg NA, del Campo N, et al. Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science. 2008;321:421-2.
  • 16
    Geller DA. Obsessive-compulsive and spectrum disorders in children and adolescents. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2006;29:353-70.
  • 17
    Salum GA, DeSousa DA, do Rosario MC, Pine DS, Manfro GG. Pediatric anxiety disorders: from neuroscience to evidence-based clinical practice. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2013;35:S03-21.
  • 18
    Geller DA, Biederman J, Faraone S, Agranat A, Cradock K, Hagermoser L, et al. Developmental aspects of obsessive compulsive disorder: findings in children, adolescents, and adults. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2001;189:471-7.
  • 19
    Rosario-Campos MC, Leckman JF, Mercadante MT, Shavitt RG, Prado HS, Sada P, et al. Adults with early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1899-903.
  • 20
    Wang X, Cui D, Wang Z, Fan Q, Xu H, Qiu J, et al. Cross-sectional comparison of the clinical characteristics of adults with early-onset and late-onset obsessive compulsive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2012;136:498-504.
  • 21
    Fontenelle LF, Mendlowicz MV, Marques C, Versiani M. Early- and late-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder in adult patients: an exploratory clinical and therapeutic study. J Psychiatr Res. 2003;37:127-33.
  • 22
    Millet B, Kochman F, Gallarda T, Krebs MO, Demonfaucon F, Barrot I, et al. Phenomenological and comorbid features associated in obsessive-compulsive disorder: influence of age of onset. J Affect Disord. 2004;79:241-6.
  • 23
    Chabane N, Delorme R, Millet B, Mouren MC, Leboyer M, Pauls D. Early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder: a subgroup with a specific clinical and familial pattern? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46:881-7.
  • 24
    do Rosario-Campos MC, Leckman JF, Curi M, Quatrano S, Katsovitch L, Miguel EC, et al. A family study of early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;136B:92-7.
  • 25
    Nestadt G, Lan T, Samuels J, Riddle M, Bienvenu OJ 3rd, Liang KY, et al. Complex segregation analysis provides compelling evidence for a major gene underlying obsessive-compulsive disorder and for heterogeneity by sex. Am J Hum Genet. 2000;67:1611-6.
  • 26
    Hanna GL, Himle JA, Curtis GC, Gillespie BW. A family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder with pediatric probands. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;134B:13-9.
  • 27
    Huyser C, Veltman DJ, de Haan E, Boer F. Paediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder, a neurodevelopmental disorder? Evidence from neuroimaging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:818-30.
  • 28
    Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:636-45.
  • 29
    Menzies L, Achard S, Chamberlain SR, Fineberg N, Chen CH, del Campo N, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain. 2007;130:3223-36.
  • 30
    Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, Blackwell AD, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, et al. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:335-8.
  • 31
    Viswanath B, Janardhan Reddy YC, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T, Chandrashekar CR. Cognitive endophenotypes in OCD: a study of unaffected siblings of probands with familial OCD. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009;33:610-5.
  • 32
    Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, Blackwell AD, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, et al. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:335-8.
  • 33
    Viswanath B, Janardhan Reddy YC, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T, Chandrashekar CR. Cognitive endophenotypes in OCD: a study of unaffected siblings of probands with familial OCD. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009;33:610-5.
  • 34
    Cavedini P, Zorzi C, Piccinni M, Cavallini MC, Bellodi L. Executive dysfunctions in obsessive-compulsive patients and unaffected relatives: searching for a new intermediate phenotype. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:1178-84.
  • 35
    Grisham JR, Anderson TM, Poulton R, Moffitt TE, Andrews G. Childhood neuropsychological deficits associated with adult obsessive-compulsive disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195:138-41.
  • 36
    Rao NP, Reddy YC, Kumar KJ, Kandavel T, Chandrashekar CR. Are neuropsychological deficits trait markers in OCD? Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2008;32:1574-9.
  • 37
    Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59:22-33;quiz 4-57.
  • 38
    Leckman JF, Riddle MA, Hardin MT, Ort SI, Swartz KL, Stevenson J, et al. The Yale Global Tic Severity Scale: initial testing of a clinician-rated scale of tic severity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28:566-73.
  • 39
    Zalonis I, Christidi F, Bonakis A, Kararizou E, Triantafyllou NI, Paraskevas G, et al. The stroop effect in Greek healthy population: normative data for the Stroop Neuropsychological Screening Test. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;24:81-8.
  • 40
    Sherer M, Nick TG, Millis SR, Novack TA. Use of the WCST and the WCST-64 in the assessment of traumatic brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2003;25:512-20.
  • 41
    Welsh MC, Revilla V, Strongin D, Kepler M. Towers of Hanoi and London: is the nonshared variance due to differences in task administration? Percept Mot Skills. 2000;90:562-72.
  • 42
    Britton JC, Stewart SE, Killgore WD, Rosso IM, Price LM, Gold AL, et al. Amygdala activation in response to facial expressions in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2010;27:643-51.
  • 43
    Shin MS, Choi H, Kim H, Hwang JW, Kim BN, Cho SC. A study of neuropsychological deficit in children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2008;23:512-20.
  • 44
    Britton JC, Rauch SL, Rosso IM, Killgore WD, Price LM, Ragan J, et al. Cognitive Inflexibility and Frontal-Cortical Activation in Pediatric Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:944-53.
  • 45
    Bannon S, Gonsalvez CJ, Croft RJ, Boyce PM. Executive functions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: state or trait deficits? Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40:1031-8.
  • 46
    Lawrence NS, Wooderson S, Mataix-Cols D, David R, Speckens A, Phillips ML. Decision making and set shifting impairments are associated with distinct symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology. 2006;20:409-19.
  • 47
    Moritz S, Birkner C, Kloss M, Jacobsen D, Fricke S, Böthern A, et al. Impact of comorbid depressive symptoms on neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2001;110:653-7.
  • 48
    Moritz S, Birkner C, Kloss M, Jahn H, Hand I, Haasen C, et al. Executive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder, unipolar depression, and schizophrenia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2002;17:477-83.
  • 49
    Rosenberg DR, Averbach DH, O'Hearn KM, Seymour AB, Birmaher B, Sweeney JA. Oculomotor response inhibition abnormalities in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54:831-8.
  • 50
    Rosenberg DR, Dick EL, O'Hearn KM, Sweeney JA. Response-inhibition deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: an indicator of dysfunction in frontostriatal circuits. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 1997;22:29-38.
  • 51
    Liu Y, Gehring WJ, Weissman DH, Taylor SF, Fitzgerald KD. Trial-by-trial adjustments of cognitive control following errors and response conflict are altered in pediatric obsessive compulsive disorder. Front Psychiatry. 2012;3:41.
  • 52
    Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Blackwell AD, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1282-4.
  • 53
    Veale DM, Sahakian BJ, Owen AM, Marks IM. Specific cognitive deficits in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Med. 1996;26:1261-9.
  • 54
    Coetzer BR. Obsessive-compulsive disorder following brain injury: a review. Int J Psychiatr Med. 2004;34:363-77.
  • 55
    Delorme R, Gousse V, Roy I, Trandafir A, Mathieu F, Mouren-Siméoni MC, et al. Shared executive dysfunctions in unaffected relatives of patients with autism and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Psychiatry. 2007;22:32-8.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    24 Mar 2015
  • Date of issue
    Apr-Jun 2015

History

  • Received
    24 July 2014
  • Accepted
    14 Oct 2014
Associação Brasileira de Psiquiatria Rua Pedro de Toledo, 967 - casa 1, 04039-032 São Paulo SP Brazil, Tel.: +55 11 5081-6799, Fax: +55 11 3384-6799, Fax: +55 11 5579-6210 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: editorial@abp.org.br