Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Ideal physical type and body image satisfaction of regular walkers

Abstracts

The objective of the present study was to quantify the ideal physical type and to verify the body image satisfaction level of regular walkers. One hundred and eighty six individuals participated in this study as follows: 87 women (age = 28.70 ± 12.6 years, stature = 161.6 ± 6.2 cm, body mass = 58.9 ± 12.0 kg and fat = 25.7 ± 7.8 F%) and 98 men (age = 27.9 ± 12.9 years, stature = 177.2 ± 6.9 cm, body mass = 75.0 ± 12.3 kg and fat = 13.3 ± 6.1 F%). The individuals were asked to indicate which profile corresponded to his/her body and which profile they wanted to reach. Only 24% of women are satisfied. Profile 3 was pointed as ideal to be reached by 55% of women (profile 2 = 18%; and profile 4 = 21%). Profile 3, according to results of this study, corresponded to F% of 20.5 ± 0.9% (EPM) and to MBI of 20.0 ± 0.3 kg/m² (EPM). In relation to men, only 18% are satisfied. Profile 4 was pointed as ideal by 46% of men (profile 3 = 23%; and profile 5 = 19%). Profile 4 corresponds to F% of 9.8 ± 1.4% (EPM) and to BMI of 23.1 ± 0.4 kg/m² (EPM). There is an ideal physical type for both genders. No difference with body image satisfaction degree between genders was verified.

Body image; Body mass index; Fat percentage


O objetivo do presente estudo foi quantificar o tipo físico ideal e verificar o nível de insatisfação com a imagem corporal de praticantes de caminhada. Participaram do estudo 186 pessoas: 87 mulheres (idade = 28,70 ± 12,6 anos, estatura = 161,6 ± 6,2cm, massa corporal = 58,9 ± 12,0kg e gordura = 25,7 ± 7,8 G%) e 98 homens (idade = 27,9 ± 12,9 anos, estatura = 177,2 ± 6,9cm, massa corporal = 75,0 ± 12,3kg e gordura = 13,3 ± 6,1 G%). Solicitou-se que as pessoas indicassem qual silhueta correspondia ao seu corpo atualmente e qual gostariam de atingir. Apenas 24% das mulheres estão satisfeitas. A silhueta 3 foi apontada como ideal a ser atingido por 55% das mulheres (silhueta 2 = 18%; e 4 = 21%). A silhueta 3, de acordo com os resultados desse estudo, corresponde ao G% 20,5 ± 0,9% (EPM) e ao IMC de 20,0 ± 0,3kg/m² (EPM). Quanto aos homens, apenas 18% estão satisfeitos. A silhueta 4 foi apontada como ideal por 47% dos homens (silhueta 3 = 23%; e 5 = 19%). A silhueta 4 corresponde ao G% 9,8 ± 1,4% (EPM) e ao IMC de 23,1 ± 0,4kg/m² (EPM). Existe um tipo físico ideal para ambos os sexos. Não houve diferença entre o grau de insatisfação com a imagem corporal entre os sexos.

Imagem corporal; Índice de massa corporal; Percentual de gordura


El objetivo del presente estudio fué cuantificar el tipo físico ideal y verificar el nivel de insatisfacción con la imagen corporal de praticantes de caminata. Participaron del estudio 186 personas: 87 mujeres (edades = 28,70 ± 12,6 años, estatura = 161,6 ± 6,2 cm, masa corporal = 58,9 ± 12,0 kg y gordura = 25,7 ± 7,8 G%) y 98 hombres (edades = 27.9 ± 12,9 años, estatura = 177,2 ± 6,9 cm, masa corporal = 75,0 ± 12,3 kg y gordura = 13,3 ± 6,1 G%). Se solicitó que las personas indicasen cual era la silueta que correspondía a su cuerpo actualmente e cual guastarian de tener. Apenas 24% de las mujeres están satisfechas. La silueta 3 fué designada como ideal a ser presentada por el 55 % de las mujeres (silueta 2 = 18%; y 4 = 21%). La silueta 3, de acuerdo con los resultados de este estudio, corresponde al G% 20,5 ± 0,9% (EPM) y al IMC de 20,0 ± 0,3 kg/m² (EPM). En cuanto a los hombres, apenas el 18% están satisfechos. La silueta 4 fué apuntada como ideal por el 47% de los hombres (silueta 3 = 23%; y 5 = 19 %). La silueta 4 corresponde al G% 9,8 ± 1,4% (EPM) y al IMC de 23,1 ± 0,4 kg/m² (EPM). Existe un tipo físico ideal para ambos sexos. No hubo diferencia entre el grado de insatisfación con la imagen corporal entre los sexos.

Imagen corporal; Indice de masa corporal; Percentaje de Grasa


ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Ideal physical type and body image satisfaction of regular walkers

Vinicius Oliveira DamascenoI; Jorge Roberto Perrout LimaII; Jeferson Macedo ViannaII; Viviane Ribeiro Ávila ViannaIII; Jefferson Silva NovaesIV

ISalgado de Oliveira University Campus of Juiz de Fora, Estácio de Sá University Campus of Juiz de Fora, MS in Human Motricity Sciences/PROCIMH (UCB-RJ), Human Motricity Biosciences Laboratory (LABIMH/UCB-RJ)

IIFederal University of Juiz de Fora, Sports and Physical Education School, Motor Evaluation Laboratory, Campus of Martelos, Juiz de Fora MG

IIIEstácio de Sá University Campus of Juiz de Fora

IVFederal University of Rio de Janeiro, FUNADESP MS fellowship in Human Motricity Sciences/PROCIMH (UCB-RJ), Human Motricity Biosciences Laboratory (LABIMH/UCB-RJ)

Correspondence Correspondence to Vinicius Oliveira Damasceno Rua Quintino Bocaíuva, 655/403, Jardim Glória 36015-010 - Juiz de Fora, MG E-mail: viniciusdamasceno@acessa.com

ABSTRACT

The objective of the present study was to quantify the ideal physical type and to verify the body image satisfaction level of regular walkers. One hundred and eighty six individuals participated in this study as follows: 87 women (age = 28.70 ± 12.6 years, stature = 161.6 ± 6.2 cm, body mass = 58.9 ± 12.0 kg, and fat = 25.7 ± 7.8 F%), and 98 men (age = 27.9 ± 12.9 years, stature = 177.2 ± 6.9 cm, body mass = 75.0 ± 12.3 kg, and fat = 13.3 ± 6.1 F%). The individuals were asked to indicate which profile corresponded to his/her body and which profile they wanted to reach. Only 24% of women are satisfied. Profile 3 was pointed as ideal to be reached by 55% of women (profile 2 = 18%; and profile 4 = 21%). Profile 3, according to results of this study, corresponded to F% of 20.5 ± 0.9% (EPM) and to MBI of 20.0 ± 0.3 kg/m2 (EPM). In relation to men, only 18% are satisfied. Profile 4 was pointed as ideal by 47% of men (profile 3 = 23%; and profile 5 = 19%). Profile 4 corresponds to F% of 9.8 ± 1.4% (EPM) and to BMI of 23.1 ± 0.4 kg/m2 (EPM). There is an ideal physical type for both genders. No difference with body image satisfaction degree between genders was verified.

Key words: Body image. Body mass index. Fat percentage.

INTRODUCTION

Media, family circle and friends condition individuals to exercise themselves, to take care of their bodies, leading them to desires, habits, cares and dissatisfaction with their body visual appearance(1). Body image is a multidimensional construction that widely describes the inner representations of body structure and physical appearance in relation to ourselves and to others(2). The body image formation process may be influenced by gender, age and media as well as by the relations of body with cognitive processes such as beliefs, values and attitudes within a culture(1,3-5).

Some authors admit the existence of a strong cultural tendency in considering leanness as an ideal situation of social acceptance for women(6-10). There are also strong correlations between social pressure of being thin and body dissatisfaction among young adult women(6,11). According to the female leanness ideal tendency, Voracek and Fisher(12) presented the modifications on the anthropometrical standards of women who were photographed in the Playboy magazine during the last decades. Through descriptive study, a tendency of reduction on the body mass index (MBI) values and of increase on the waist/hip relation (WHI) was observed, demonstrating a body linearity tendency. On the other hand, among men, a stronger and larger body is considered as the ideal profile(13-17). Reinforcing this statement, Cohane and Pope Jr.(9) mention the study of McCreary and Sasse (2000), performed with 96 young individuals with average age of 18 years, who wanted to be heavier and stronger in relation to their actual bodies. In both cases, men and women aimed their attitudes in relation to their bodies towards fulfilling cultural pressures of society in which they belong to(19).

The tireless search for a better physical appearance of those who practice regular physical activities is a sociocultural phenomenon many times more significant than the own economic, affective or professional satisfaction(20). The dissatisfaction with the own body, or better, with the image one has about it, is maybe one of the main reasons people involve themselves into physical activity programs(21-24). It is possible that the body image satisfaction degree would influence the way how young adults perceive themselves in terms of the relation between body mass, fat percentage (F%) and stature. The body dissatisfaction is strongly related to the exposition of beautiful bodies by media and this fact has determined a compulsion for searching for the ideal anatomy in the last decades(25,26).

Although there are adequate BMI and F% values for the maintenance of a good health condition, the physical type idealized by individuals is culturally determined(24,27,29), and it seems that there is an ideal physical type that individuals who practice regular physical activity search to reach. Thus, the objectives of this work were, using the set of profiles of Stunkard et al.(29) (figure 1), to quantify the ideal physical type and to verify the body image satisfaction level of regular walkers.


METHOD

Subjects The sample was composed of 186 individuals (87 women and 98 men) who practiced regular walking in the Campus of the Federal University of Juiz de Fora, whose characteristics are described in table 1. In order to perform the measurements, an evaluation room was adapted next to the area used for the physical activity practice. The individuals invited to participate in this research were informed about the objectives and signed the consent term for researches involving human beings, according to Guidelines and Regulation Norms of Researches Involving Human Beings (Resolution 196 of October 10th, 1996) of the Health National Council. The research was previously approved by the Ethics Research Committee of the Federal University of Juiz de Fora.

As way to offer return to the participants of this research, a report containing BMI and F% with interpretation and explanation of results and their relation with health risk factors was delivered to them shortly after data collection.

Anthropometrical measurements The body mass measurement was performed, using a digital scale label Filizola® properly calibrated and with the Inmetro seal. Following, the stature measurement was performed by means of a stadiometer with accuracy of 1 mm fixed to the scale. The measurement of the following skinfolds were performed: thorax/pectoral; triceps, suprailliac, abdomen and thigh. A Lange® compass (USA) was used.

Body image measurements In order to verify the actual and ideal body image, the scale proposed by Stunkard et al.(29) (figure 1) was used. The set of profiles was shown to individuals and the following questions were asked: Which profile best represents your actual physical appearance? Which profile would you like to have? In order to verify body dissatisfaction, the difference between the actual profile (AP) and the ideal profile (IP) indicated by the individual was used. The appraiser was free of opinion in the selection of profiles.

Calculations and statistic treatment For the BMI calculation, the equation weight in kilograms divided by stature in meters, raised to the second power was used. The waist/hip relation was obtained through dividing the waist circumference by the hip circumference. The body density was estimated through the equation of Jackson and Pollock(30) and Jackson et al.(31), and, on conversion to F%, the equation of Siri(32) was used. The data were submitted to descriptive statistics. The BMI and F% data were grouped by AP. For the test of difference hypothesis between averages of F% and BMI between groups, the analysis of variance with one factor was performed, according to the Scheffé test (p < 0.05). For the study of the association between AP and the F% and BMI values, the Pearson correlation was performed. The differences between men and women dissatisfaction levels was tested with the chi-squared test (p < 0.05). The program Statistica® 6.0 for Windows® was used for calculation purposes.

RESULTS

Table 1 describes the average and the standard deviation of the sample's characteristics. Male BMI and F% were presented within normality range, according to classification of the World Health Organization(33) for BMI and Jackson and Pollock(30) and Jackson et al.(31) for F%. For women, the average values of BMI were presented within normality range and the F% was presented a little above normality range.

Table 2 presents the average values of F% and BMI by AP. In figure 2, the histograms demonstrate the sample distribution in relation to male and female AP and IP. One observes through the tendency identified by mode, that most women selected profile 4 as the AP and most men selected profile 5. With regard to the IP, the histogram indicates the tendency of women to select profile 3 (n = 49 or 55%) and men to select profile 4 (n = 46 or 47%). Profile 3 was indicated as ideal to be reached by 55% of women (profile 2 = 18%; and 4 = 21%). Profile 3, according to results from this study, corresponds to F% of 20.5 ± 0.9% (EPM) and to BMI of 20.0 ± 0.3 kg/m2 (EPM). In relation to men, only 18% are satisfied with their profiles. Profile 4 was indicated as the ideal by 47% of men (profile 3 = 23%; and 5 = 19 %). Profile 4 corresponds to F% of 9.8 ± 1.4% (EPM) to BMI of 23.1 ± 0.4 kg/m2 (EPM). No difference in the dissatisfaction degree with body image between men and women was verified (c2; p < 0.05).


Table 3 indicates the AP for men and women and the difference between AP and IP. One observes that the IP for male individuals who presented AP 1, 2, 3 and 5 is the profile 4, in other words, individuals who identified themselves with profiles lower than 4 wanted to climb up one or two positions in relation to the AP. Individuals who identified their profiles as higher than 5 wanted to step down one or two positions.

Women did not seem to want to climb positions in relation to the choice for the IP. Women who identified themselves with profiles 2 and 3 did not want to change profile. For those who identified themselves with profiles higher than 3, a tendency to step down two positions was observed. For example, women who identified themselves with profile 6 considered profile 4 as ideal.

Figure 3 presents the F% and AP of men and women. In relation to women, no significant differences between F% of profiles from 2 to 5 were observed. Only profiles 6 and 7 were significantly different from the other profiles in relation to F%. In relation to men, profiles 2, 3 and 4 presented no significant differences between each other as well as profiles 5, 6, 7 and 8. Only between profiles 2, 3 and 4 and profiles 5, 6, 7 and 8, significant differences in relation to F% were observed.


In figure 4, the BMI values in relation to AP of both genders were plotted. For the female group, profiles 2, 3 and 4 were not statistically different from each other. Profile 5 is significantly different from profiles 2, 6 and 7, but not significantly different from profiles 3 and 4. Profiles 6 and 7 are significantly different from the other profiles (p < 0.05). For male group, profiles 2, 3 and 4 presented no significant differences between each other, and the same occurred between profiles 5, 6, 7 and 8. It seems to exist two set of profiles: upper and lower profiles. Significant differences were found between these two groups of profiles in relation to the BMI values.


BMI and F% values were plotted in relation to AP for women (figure 5) and men (figure 6). For women, significant correlations were observed for BMI and F%, r = 0.75 and r = 0.70 respectively. For men, as for women, significant correlations were observed for BMI and F%, r = 0.79 and r = 0.60 respectively.



DISCUSSION

The objective of this study was to identify the ideal physical type and dissatisfaction with body image of a group of regular walkers. It was considered that, in the morphological evaluations performed by the physical activity programs, the adoption of the BMI and F% values recommended for good health conditions (25 kg/m2 and 15% for men and 25 kg/m2 and 25% for women) could lead to disagreement with bodies that our culture considers as ideal. The set of profiles proposed by Stunkard et al.(29) was used as instrument.

In relation to the instrument used, several limitations can be mentioned: profiles are two-dimensional and therefore they do not allow the representation of the individual as a whole, the distribution of his subcutaneous fat mass and other anthropometrical aspects relevant for the body image formation(34). Other limitation that could be pointed, especially in the AP identification, is the fact that the instrument used is subjective. Due to its simplicity, it could also neglect complex aspects such as the influence of family, friends, relatives, media and, in some cases, of some pathologies such as anorexia, bulimia and reverse anorexia, which are well-known intervenient variables in the body image formation process(1,12,35-41).

Despite the limitations presented, the strong correlations of MBI and F% with AP (r = 0.75 and r = 0.70, for women and r = 0.79 and r = 0.61 for men) demonstrate that, for the sample studied, the set of profiles was effective with regard to the AP identification. The group studied presented good perception of their body dimensions. Thus, the use of the average BMI and F% values grouped by AP is valid in the characterization of the ideal physical type.

The individuals who initiated a physical activity program, in most cases and somehow, search to modify the shape and proportions of their bodies. The amount of body fat is important factor for the health maintenance and in our culture, the low amount of body fat became a factor of undeniable aesthetic value(20,42). This exacerbated valorization of the low body fat levels exposed by media, the comparison between individuals from the same environment besides the opinion of the own family lead people to present high levels of dissatisfaction with the body appearance(1,42). Such dissatisfaction, in extreme cases, may lead to the adoption of self-destructive behaviors, such as the abuse of diets that may end up in bulimia and anorexia nervosa(43-46).

The BMI and F% determination for male and female profiles (table 4) allowed verifying the ideal physical type desired by the sample studied. For women, the profile 3 was considered as ideal, which corresponds to values of F% and BMI of 20.5 of F% and 20.0 kg/m2, respectively. Through values of BMI and F%, one observes that women would rather want thinner bodies with lower body volume than profiles recommended for good health conditions. Corroborating these findings, Cachelin et al.(47), also using the profiles of Stunkard(29), found that women tend to select smaller profiles than the actual ones. In literature, one can also find several studies reporting that the female ideal body standard or physical type is associated with low body weight and low fat percentage(6,7, 9,10,12,13,48,49).

Men, in turn, adopted profile 4 as ideal, whose values of F% and BMI are 9.8 F% and 23.1 kg/m2, respectively. One observes the male tendency of searching for a body with higher volume and lower amount of body fat. It was observed that the men who identified themselves with profile below 4 wanted to increase profile, reflecting the desire of presenting higher amount of body mass. These results corroborate findings of Pope Jr. et al.(15), who performed an experiment to identify the ideal physical type in relation to body fat and muscular mass of men in three different countries, Austria, the United States and France, and found a consensual desire of reducing fat percentage and increasing muscular mass. American and Austrian men considered 10.0% of fat percentage as ideal, while French men desired fat percentage around 13%. Concerning the muscular mass, the men of the three countries had a desire of increase around 14 kg.

In relation to body dissatisfaction, women presented dissatisfaction level similar to men, 76% and 82%, respectively. These findings are not in agreement with results found by Lokken et al.(50), Araújo and Araújo(51), Cachelin et al.(47) and Pingitore et al.(52), who verified higher body dissatisfaction level among women when compared to men. Loland(24) investigated 1,555 Norwegian males and females from different age ranges and physical fitness and verified that men are significantly more satisfied than women, regardless age and physical fitness levels.

The body image dissatisfaction leads people to search for physical activity programs(21-24,53). Women search for the leanness ideal, and men, on the other hand, search for being stronger and larger. A well-elaborated physical activity program may reduce body weight, as desired by women, or increase muscular mass, as desired by men. In fact, the practice of physical activities may lead individuals to reach body they consider as ideal. There are studies that report association between physical activity and high levels of body satisfaction(23,53,55). However, the practice of physical activity, in some cases, may result in negative outcomes causing women to be concerned with weight(56,57) and men to present the phenomenon known as muscle dysmorphia or reverse anorexia(58,59). Health promotion researchers should include the concern with possible negative effects of the practice of physical activity in their reports.

CONCLUSION

One may conclude that men want to have low fat percentage, stronger and larger bodies, with values of 9.8 F% and 23.1 kg/m2 of BMI, and women consider as ideal a leaner and thinner body, with values of 20.5 F% and 20.0 kg/m2 of body mass index. Once only a few individuals present bodies with such dimensions, most individuals studied are dissatisfied with their body image regardless gender, but no statistical differences between genders were observed.

REFERENCES

Received in 13/3/05. 2nd version received in 30/3/05. Approved in 20/4/05.

All the authors declared there is not any potential conflict of interests regarding this article.

  • 1. Blowers LC, Loxton NJ, Grady-Flesser MG, Occhipinti S, Dawe S. The relationship between sociocultural pressure to be thin and body dissatisfaction in preadolescent girls. Eating Behaviors 2003;4:229-44.
  • 2. Cash T, Pruzinsky T. Body Images. New York: Guilford Press, 1990.
  • 3. Fisher S. The evolution of psychological concepts about the body. In: Cash T, Pruzinsky T, editors. Body images. New York: Guilford Press, 1990;3-20.
  • 4. Ricciardelli LA, McCabe MP, Banfield S. Body image and body change methods in adolescent boys. Role of parents, friends, and the media. J Psychosom Res 2000; 49:189-97.
  • 5. Volkwein K, McConatha JT. Cultural contours of the body - The impact of age and fitness. In: Lidor R, Bar-Eli M, editors. Innovations in sport psychology: Linking theory and practice. Tel-Aviv: ISSP, 1997;744-6.
  • 6. Cattarin J, Williams R, Thomas CM, Thompson KJ. Body image, mood, and televised images of attractiveness: The role of social comparison. Br J Clin Psychol 2000;19:220-39.
  • 7. Garner DM, Garfinknel P, Schwartz D, Thompson M. The great American shape-up. Psychol Rep 1980;47:483-91.
  • 8. Harrison K, Cantor J. The relationship between media consumption and eating disorders. J Commun Disord 1997;47:40-67.
  • 9. Parham ES. Promoting body size acceptance in weight management counseling. J Am Diet Assoc 1999;99:920-25.
  • 10. Smolak L, Levine MP, Schermer F. Parental input and weight concerns among elementary school children. Int J Eat Disord 1999;25:263-71.
  • 11. Thompson JK, Coovert MD, Stormer S. Body image, social comparison, and eating disturbance: a covariance structure modeling investigation. Int J Eat Disord 1999;26:43-51.
  • 12. Voracek M, Fisher ML. Shapely centrefolds? Temporal change in body measures: trend analysis. BMJ 2002;325:1447-8.
  • 13. Cash T, Winstead B, Janda L. Cultural expectations thinness in women. Psychologic Today 1986;4:30-37.
  • 14. Jacobi L, Cash TF. In pursuit of the perfect appearance. Journal Applied Social Psychology 1994;24:379-69.
  • 15. Pope Jr HG, Gruber AJ, Mangweth B, Boreau B, DeCol C, Jouvent R, et al. Body image perception among men in three countries. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157: 1297-301.
  • 16. Strauss RS. Self-reported weight status and dieting in a cross-sectional sample of young adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1999;153:741-7.
  • 17. Thompson JK, Heinberg LJ, Altabe M, Tantleff-Dunn S. Exacting beauty: theory assessment and treatment of body image disturbance. American Psychological Association. Washington, DC, 1999.
  • 18. Cohane G, Pope Jr HG. Body image in boys: a review of the literature. Int J Eat Disord 2001;29:373-9.
  • 19. Sabino C. Os marombeiros: construção social de corpo e gênero em academias de musculação [Dissertação]. Rio de Janeiro UFRJ, Faculdade de Filosofia e Ciências Sociais, 2000.
  • 20. Novaes JS. Estética O corpo na Academia. Rio de Janeiro: Ed. Shape, 2001.
  • 21. Fox KR, Corbin CB. The physical self-perception profile: development and preliminary validation. Journal Sport Exercise Psychological 1989;11:408.
  • 22. Hart EA. Avaliando a imagem corporal. In: Medidas e avaliação em educação física e esportes de Barrow & McGee. São Paulo: Ed. Manole, 2003;457-88.
  • 23. Harter S. Competence as a dimension of self-evaluation: toward a comprehensive model of self-worth. In: Leahy R, editor. The development of the self. New York: Academic Press, 1985.
  • 24. Loland NW. The aging body: attitudes toward bodily appearance among physically active and inactive women and men of different ages. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity 2000;8:197-213.
  • 25. Labre MP. Adolescent boys and the muscular male body ideal. J Adolesc Health 2001;30:233-42.
  • 26. Nagel K, Jones K. Predisposition factors in anorexia nervosa. Adolescence 1992; 27:381-6.
  • 27. Kunkel M. Body weight perceptions, body mass index, and dieting practices of South Carolina adults. J Am Diet Assoc 1987;87:1217-8.
  • 28. Stice E, Shaw H. Adverse effects of the media portrayed thin-ideal on women and the linkages to bulimic symptomatology. Journal Social Clinical Psychology 1994;13:288-308.
  • 29. Stunkard AJ, Sorenson T, Schlusinger F. Use of the Danish Adoption Register for the study of obesity and thinness. In: Kety SS, Rowland LP, Sidman RL, Matthysse SW, editors. The genetics of neurological and psychiatric disorders. New York: Raven, 1983;115-20.
  • 30. Jackson AS, Pollock ML. Generalized equations for predicting body density of men. Br J Nutr 1978;40:497-504.
  • 31. Jackson AS, Pollock ML, Ward A. Generalized equations for predicting body density of women. Med Sci Sports Exerc 1980;12:175-82.
  • 32. Siri WE. Body composition from fluid spaces and density: analysis of methods 1961. Nutrition 1993;9:480-91.
  • 33
    World Health Organization, Obesity: preventing and managing the global epidemic. World Health Organization, Geneva, 1998.
  • 34. Kay S. The psychological and anthropometric in body image. In: Norton K, Olds T, editors. Anthropometrica. Sydney, Austrália: University of New South Wales Press, 1996;217-38.
  • 35. Benedikt R, Werthein EH, Love A. Eating attitudes and weight-loss attempts in female adolescents and their mothers. J Adolesc Health 1998;27:43-57.
  • 36. Botta R. Television images and adolescent girls' body image disturbance. J Commun Disord 1999;49:22-41.
  • 37. Dunkley TL, Wertheim EH, Paxton, SJ. Examination of a model of multiple sociocultural influences on adolescent girls' body dissatisfaction and dietary restraint. Adolescence 2001;36:265-79.
  • 38. Field AE, Camargo Jr, Taylor CB, Berkey CS, Colditz GA. Relation of peer and media influences to the development of purging behaviors among preadolescent and adolescent girls. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 1999;153:1184-89.
  • 39. Morant H. BMA demands more responsible media attitude on body image. BMJ 2000;320:1495.
  • 40. O'dea JA, Abraham S. Improving the body image, eating attitudes, and behaviors of young male and female adolescents: a new educational approach that focuses on self-esteem. Int J Eat Disord 2000;28:43-57.
  • 41. Novaes JS. Ginástica de academia do Rio de Janeiro [Dissertação]. Rio de Janeiro UFRJ, Faculdade de Educação Física, 1999.
  • 42. McLaren L, Gauvin L. Neighbourhood level versus individual level correlates of women's body dissatisfaction: toward a multilevel understanding of the role of affluence. J Epidemiol Community Health 2002;56:193-99.
  • 43. Delvin MJ, Zhu AJ. Body image in the balance (review). JAMA 2001;287:2159.
  • 44. Friedman M, Wilfley D, Pike KM, Striegel-Moore RH, Rodin J. The relationship between weight and psychological functioning among adolescent girls. Obes Res 1995;3:57-62.
  • 45. Garner DM, Olmsted MP, Polivy J, Garfinkel PE. Comparison between weight-preoccupied women and anorexia nervosa. Psychosom Med 1984;46:255-66.
  • 46. Nunes MA, Apolinário JC, Abuchaim ALG, Coutinho W. Transtornos alimentares e obesidade. Porto Alegre: Editora Artmed, 1998.
  • 47. Cachelin FM, Rebeck RM, Chung GH, Pelayo E. Does ethnicity influence body-size preference? A comparison of body image and body size. Obes Res 2002; 10:158-66.
  • 48. Parnell K, Sargent R, Thompson SH, Duhe SF, Valois RF, Kemper RC. Black and white adolescent females' perceptions of ideal body size. J Sch Health 1999; 66:112-8.
  • 49. Tovée MJ, Reinhardt S, Emery JL, Cornelissen PL. Optimum body-mass index and maximum sexual attractiveness. Lancet 1998;352:548.
  • 50. Lokken K, Ferraro FR, Kirchner T, Bowling M. Gender differences in body size dissatisfaction among individuals with low, medium, or high levels of body focus. J Gen Psychol 2003;130:305-10.
  • 51. Araújo DSMS, Araújo CGS. Autopercepção e insatisfação com peso corporal. Arq Bras Cardiol 2003; 80:235-42.
  • 52. Pingitore R, Spring B, Garfield D. Gender differences in body satisfaction. Obes Res 1997;5:402-9.
  • 53. Loland NW. Body image and physical activity: a survey among Norwegian men and women. International Journal Sport Psychological 1998;29:339-65.
  • 54. Loland NW. Body image, sports and exercise. Tidsskr Nor Laegeforen 2000; 120:2919-21.
  • 55. Williams P, Cash TF. Effects of a circuit weight training program on the body images of college students. Int J Eat Disord 2001;30:75-82.
  • 56. Davis C, Fox J. Excessive exercise and weight preoccupation in women Addict Behav 1993;18:201-11.
  • 57. Zabinski M, Calfas K, Gehrman CA, Wilfley DE, Sallis JF. Effects of a physical activity intervention on body image in university seniors: project GRAD. Ann Behav Med 2001;23:247-52.
  • 58. Leit RA, Gray JJ, Pope Jr HG. The media's representation of the ideal male body: a cause for muscle dysmorphia? Int J Eat Disord 2002;31:334-38.
  • 59. Pope Jr HG, Gruber AJ, Choi P, Olivardia R, Phillips KA. Muscle dysmorphia: an underrecognized form of body dysmorphic disorder. Psychosomatics 1997;38: 548-57.
  • Correspondence to
    Vinicius Oliveira Damasceno
    Rua Quintino Bocaíuva, 655/403, Jardim Glória
    36015-010 - Juiz de Fora, MG
    E-mail:
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      15 Sept 2005
    • Date of issue
      June 2005

    History

    • Received
      13 Mar 2005
    • Accepted
      20 Apr 2005
    Sociedade Brasileira de Medicina do Exercício e do Esporte Av. Brigadeiro Luís Antônio, 278, 6º and., 01318-901 São Paulo SP, Tel.: +55 11 3106-7544, Fax: +55 11 3106-8611 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: atharbme@uol.com.br