Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Predictors of low perceptual-motor skills in children at 4-5 years of age

Abstract

Objectives:

the purpose of this study was to evaluate the predictors of low perceptual-motor skills in children at preschool age.

Methods:

this is a cross-sectional study nested in a birth cohort involving mother-child pairs. The children’s perceptual-motor skills were assessed individually in their homes when they were 4-5 years old using the Pre-Literacy Skills and Knowledge Test (THCP®), a vali-dated Brazilian instrument. Logistic regression analysis was used to estimate the association between cognitive perceptual-motor skills and potential maternal and child risk factors.

Results:

of the 199 children included in the study, 53.8% were boys, 90.8% attended school, and 91.1% were enrolled in a public school. Among the children, 114 (57.3%), 41 (20.6%) and 44 (22.1%) had low, moderate and high perceptual-motor skills, respectively. Multivariate logistic regression analysis revealed greater odds of children at preschool age having low perceptual motor skills for boys (OR=2.10; CI95%= 1.14-3.88), children who did not attend school (OR=4.61; CI95%= 1.21-17.49), and those with a household income <5 minimum wages (MW) (OR=4.28; CI95%= 1.49-12.26).

Conclusions:

our study showed that male gender, not attending school and a monthly household income <5 MW were predictors of low perceptual-motor skills in children at 4-5 years of age.

Key words:
Perceptual-motor skill; Cognitive development; Cognitive skill; Pre-school age; Motor skill

Resumo

Objetivos:

avaliar os preditores da baixa habilidade percepto-motora em crianças em idade pré-escolar.

Métodos:

trata-se de um estudo transversal aninhado a uma coorte de nascimentos envol-vendo pares de mães-crianças. A habilidade percepto-motora das crianças foi avaliada indi-vidualmente em suas residências quando completaram 4-5 anos de idade, utilizando-se o Teste de Habilidades e Conhecimento Pré-Alfabetização (THCP®), um instrumento brasileiro validado. Análise de regressão logística foi utilizada para estimar a associação entre a baixa habilidade percepto-motora das crianças e potenciais fatores de risco materno-infantil.

Resultados:

das 199 crianças incluídas no estudo, 53,8% eram meninos, 90,8% frequen-tavam a escola, e 91,1% estudavam em escola pública. Entre as crianças, 114 (57,3%), 41 (20,6%) e 44 (22,1%) revelaram baixa, moderada e elevada habilidade percepto-motora, respectivamente. A análise de regressão logística ajustada revelou maior chance de crianças em idade pré-escolar apresentar baixa habilidade percepto-motora quando em meninos (OR=2,10; IC95%=1,14-3,88), crianças que não frequentavam a escola (OR=4,61; IC95%=1,21-17,49), e possuíam renda familiar <5 salários mínimos (SM) (OR=4,28; IC95%=1,49-12,26).

Conclusões:

nosso estudo revelou que sexo masculino, renda familiar <5 SM, e não frequentar a escola foram preditores significativos da baixa habilidade percepto-motora em crianças de 4-5 anos de idade.

Palavras-chave:
Habilidade percepto-motora; Desenvolvimento cognitivo; Idade pré-escolar; Habilidade motora

Introduction

Child cognitive development is complex, is influenced by multiple factors,11 Investigators M-EN. Early childhood cognitive develop-ment is affected by interactions among illness, diet, enteropathogens and the home environment: findings from the MAL-ED birth cohort study. BMJ Glob Health. 2018; 3 (4): e000752.and comprises different cognitive skills such as attention, memory, execu-tion/expression of information, and perceptual-motor skills.22 Schmitt JA, Benton D, Kallus KW. General methodological considerations for the assessment of nutritional influences on human cognitive functions. Eur J Nutr. 2005; 44 (8): 459-64.Motor skills comprises motor proficiency, which consists of a set of motor skills that describe an individual’s overall motor condition.33 Santos MO, Barbosa DG, Junior GJF, Silva RC, Pelegrini A, Felden EPG. Capacity of Objective Measures of Physical Activity to Predict Brazilian Children's Low Motor Proficiency. Percept Mot Skills. 2018; 125 (4): 669-81.Perceptual-motor skills refer to the recognition and interpretation of sensory stimuli, followed by a response in the form of movement22 Schmitt JA, Benton D, Kallus KW. General methodological considerations for the assessment of nutritional influences on human cognitive functions. Eur J Nutr. 2005; 44 (8): 459-64.and also a set of skills that involve coordination and sequencing of movements to achieve a particular goal.44 Liang J, Matheson BE, Kaye WH, Boutelle KN. Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. Int J Obes (Lond). 2014;38(4):494-506.

Motor development promotes and requires improvements in behavioral flexibility because new motor skills provide new opportunities for action but also require new solutions.55 Adolph KE, Hoch JE. Motor Development: Embodied, Embedded, Enculturated, and Enabling. Annu Rev Psychol. 2019;70:141-64.This area of functioning includes balance, fine and gross motor movements, speed, strength, agility, and manual dexterity.44 Liang J, Matheson BE, Kaye WH, Boutelle KN. Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. Int J Obes (Lond). 2014;38(4):494-506.In general, individuals with low perceptual-motor skills have low academic performance, writing problems, and difficulty in performing professions that require high perceptual-motor skills.66 Pearce A, Sawyer ACP, Chittleborough CR, Mittinty MN, Law C, Lynch JW. Do early life cognitive ability and self-regulation skills explain socio-economic inequalities in academic achievement? An effect decomposition analysis in UK and Australian cohorts. Soc Sci Med. 2016; 165: 108-18.

Perceptual-motor skills cannot be understood as independent processes and dissociated from the environment77 Herwig A. Linking perception and action by structure or process? Toward an integrative perspective. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015; 52: 105-16.since they are associated with several factors such as personality, weight status, feeding practice, physical development,88 Babic MJ, Morgan PJ, Plotnikoff RC, Lonsdale C, White RL, Lubans DR. Physical activity and physical self-concept in youth: systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2014;44(11):1589-601.cognitive, affective or motor disorders, socioeconomic characteristics, and motor stimulation in the family environment and at school.66 Pearce A, Sawyer ACP, Chittleborough CR, Mittinty MN, Law C, Lynch JW. Do early life cognitive ability and self-regulation skills explain socio-economic inequalities in academic achievement? An effect decomposition analysis in UK and Australian cohorts. Soc Sci Med. 2016; 165: 108-18.,99 Brinkman S, Gregory T, Harris J, Hart B, Blackmore S, Janus M. Associations Between the Early Development Instrument at Age 5, and Reading and Numeracy Skills at Ages 8, 10 and 12: a Prospective Linked Data Study. Child Ind Res. 2013; 6 (4): 695-708.

10 Ferreira L, Godinez I, Gabbard C, Vieira JLL, Cacola P. Motor development in school-age children is associated with the home environment including socioeconomic status. Child Care Health Dev. 2018; 44 (6): 801-6.

11 Ronfani L, Vecchi Brumatti L, Mariuz M, Tognin V, Bin M, Ferluga V, Knowles A, Montico M, Barbone F. The Complex Interaction between Home Environment, Socioeconomic Status, Maternal IQ and Early Child Neurocognitive Development: A Multivariate Analysis of Data Collected in a Newborn Cohort Study. PLoS One. 2015; 10 (5): e0127052.
-1212 Crampton A, Hall J. Unpacking socio-economic risks for reading and academic self-concept in primary school: Differential effects and the role of the preschool home learning environment. Br J Educ Psychol. 2017; 87 (3): 365-82.School is also crucial for the child’s cognitive development since the latter occurs during an important period of the child’s development.66 Pearce A, Sawyer ACP, Chittleborough CR, Mittinty MN, Law C, Lynch JW. Do early life cognitive ability and self-regulation skills explain socio-economic inequalities in academic achievement? An effect decomposition analysis in UK and Australian cohorts. Soc Sci Med. 2016; 165: 108-18.,99 Brinkman S, Gregory T, Harris J, Hart B, Blackmore S, Janus M. Associations Between the Early Development Instrument at Age 5, and Reading and Numeracy Skills at Ages 8, 10 and 12: a Prospective Linked Data Study. Child Ind Res. 2013; 6 (4): 695-708.Identifying the factors that affect perceptual-motor skills at the beginning of learning not only contributes to the improvement of the child’s performance in the classroom,99 Brinkman S, Gregory T, Harris J, Hart B, Blackmore S, Janus M. Associations Between the Early Development Instrument at Age 5, and Reading and Numeracy Skills at Ages 8, 10 and 12: a Prospective Linked Data Study. Child Ind Res. 2013; 6 (4): 695-708.but also to his/her professional performance as an adult.

Within this context, the aim of this study was to examine the association of biological, socioeconomic and demographic characteristics of mothers and their children with the child’s perceptual-motor skills four years after delivery. The results of this study will be useful to increase our understanding of children’s perceptual-motor skills at preschool age.

Methods

This is a cross-sectional study nested within the Predictors of Maternal and Infant Excess Body Weight - PREDI Study, a birth cohort study conducted at the Darcy Vargas Maternity Hospital in Joinville, the largest city in the State of Santa Catarina, Brazil. The PREDI Study was designed to examine the determinants and consequences of large birth size and child/maternal excess body weight in mothers and their children over the years.

The present study used data collected from adult women and their children at baseline (2012, when the children were born) and at 2 years and 4 years of follow-up (2014 and 2016, respectively). Details of the recruitment process at baseline have been described previously.1313 Sales WB, Silleno Junior JD, Kroll C, Mastroeni SSBS, Silva JC, Mastroeni MF. Influence of altered maternal lipid profile on the lipid profile of the newborn. Arch Endocrinol Metab. 2015; 59 (2): 123-8.,1414 Mastroeni MF, Czarnobay SA, Kroll C, Figueirêdo KBW, Mastroeni SSBS, silva JC, Khan MKA, Loehr S, Veugelers PJ. The Independent Importance of Pre-pregnancy Weight and Gestational Weight Gain for the Prevention of Large-for Gestational Age Brazilian Newborns. Matern Child Health J. 2017; 21 (4): 705-14.Subsequently, all women over the age of 18 years who gave birth to a full-term singleton (between 37 and 42 weeks of gestation) were invited to participate in the study with their newborns in January-February 2012 (baseline). The baseline exclusion criteria included pre-eclampsia, presence of an infectious contagious disease (AIDS, hepatitis, syphilis, and toxoplasmosis), birth defects, and plans of giving up the newborn for adoption immediately after delivery. Of the 529 eligible pairs (mothers and infants), 58 did not meet the study criteria and 36 were not considered for other reasons, totaling 435 mother-infant pairs who participated in the baseline assessment in 2012 (Figure 1).

Figure 1
Flow chart of participants through the recruitment process, Joinville, Brazil.

A trained health professional collected the data at baseline and all follow-ups, which included anthropometric assessment and demographic, biological and socioeconomic information. In all follow-ups, data were collected individually in a private room of the family’s home using a previously tested structured questionnaire.

Baseline, 2012

We used only the newborn’s gender from the baseline period, which was obtained from the hospital records in 2012 when the cohort study was started.

First follow-up, 2013-2014

The children’s weight was measured with a pediatric digital portable scale (Model BY20, Beurer®) to the nearest 10 g. Length was measured with a pediatric anthropometric ruler (Model Wood, WCS®) to the nearest 0.1 cm. The children’s nutritional status was classified into two categories based on the 2006 World Health Organization (WHO) body mass index (BMI)-for-age growth standards: <85thpercentile, >85thand <97thpercentile, and >97thpercentile.1515 WHO. (World Health Organization). WHO Child Growth Standards: Length/height-for-age, weight-for-age, weight-for-length, weight-for-height and body mass index-for-age: Methods and development. WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group. Geneva; 2006.All anthropometric measurements were performed in duplicate in the first and second assessment and the mean of the two measurements was used for analysis.

Duration of breast-feeding (in months) was selfreported by the mothers. Breast-feeding was classified according to the WHO indicators for assessing infant and young child feeding practices.1616 WHO (World Health Organization). Indicators for Assessing Infant and Young Child Feeding Practices: part 1: definitions: conclusions of a consensus meeting held 6-8 November 2007 in Washington, DC, USA. Geneva; 2008.For the present study, all non-exclusive breast-feeding categories were combined into a single non-exclusive breast-feeding category.1616 WHO (World Health Organization). Indicators for Assessing Infant and Young Child Feeding Practices: part 1: definitions: conclusions of a consensus meeting held 6-8 November 2007 in Washington, DC, USA. Geneva; 2008.,1717 Mastroeni MF, Mastroeni SSBS, Czarnobay SA, Ekwaru JP, Loehr SA, Veugelers PJ. Breast-feeding duration for the prevention of excess body weight of mother-child pairs concurrently: a 2-year cohort study. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20(14):2537-48.

Second follow-up, 2016-2017

In the 2nd follow-up, the child’s perceptual-motor skills were collected individually using the Teste de Habilidades e Conhecimento Pré-Alfabetização (Pre-Literacy Skills and Knowledge Test, THCP® in the Portuguese acronym), a validated Brazilian instrument.1818 Silva RS, Flores-Mendonza C, Telles M. Teste de Habilidades e Conhecimentos Pré-Alfabetização (THCP). V.1. 1 ed. ed. São Paulo: Vetor; 2012.In addition to perceptual-motor skills, the test evaluates the most important cognitive and motor skills of pre-literacy development in children aged 4 to 7 years, including memory, language skills, quantitative thinking, and focused attention.1818 Silva RS, Flores-Mendonza C, Telles M. Teste de Habilidades e Conhecimentos Pré-Alfabetização (THCP). V.1. 1 ed. ed. São Paulo: Vetor; 2012. For the present study, only the children’s perceptualmotor skills were considered in the data analysis. These skills include the ability 1) to perform fine and precise finger movements in response to a visual stimulus; 2) to discriminate stimuli that are perceived visually, and 3) to perceive positions in space. The test was applied by a psychologist in a private room of the participant’s home, usually in the presence of the mother. Prior to application of the test, the mother was informed of how the test would be conducted and the importance of discretion during its execution. After the material was shown to the child, once the child became receptive, the researcher started the test. The average time of test application was 25 min. The answer to each question was converted into a score according to the child’s age. The cognitive perceptual-motor score was calculated considering age and the number of correct answers obtained by the child throughout the test. The maximum score to be achieved was 30 points.

The children’s perceptual-motor performance was classified into three categories according to age and the total score obtained: low (0-11), moderate (12-15), and high (>16) for children aged 4 years; low (0-16), moderate (17-19), and high (>20) for children aged >5 years. For the purpose of the present study, moderate and high scores were combined into a single category.

The data were analyzed using the IBM SPSS Statistics 22.0 software package. The x22 Schmitt JA, Benton D, Kallus KW. General methodological considerations for the assessment of nutritional influences on human cognitive functions. Eur J Nutr. 2005; 44 (8): 459-64.test was used to compare the frequency of categorical variables according to the child’s cognitive perceptual-motor skills (low and moderate/high). Maternal age (<30, 30-40, and >40 years old) and monthly household income (<3, 3-5, and >5 minimum wages -MW) were classified according to the criteria of the Brazilian Institute of Geography and Statistics.1919 IBGE (Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística). Pesquisa Nacional por Amostra de Domicílios. 2010. [Accessed April 2017]. http://www.ibge.gov.br/home/esta-tistica/populacao/trabalhoerendimento/pnad98/saude/analis e.shtm.
http://www.ibge.gov.br/home/esta-tistica...
Maternal education (<9 years for women who completed primary school; 9-12 years for those who completed high school; >12 years for those who started/finished undergraduate courses) was classified according to the 1996 Education Law (Lei das Diretrizes e Bases da Educação Nacional).2020 Brasil. Presidência da República. Casa Civil. Subchefia para Assuntos Jurídicos. Lei nº 9.394, de 20 de dezembro de 1996. Estabelece as Diretrizes e Bases da Educação Nacional. Brasília. 1996. [Accessed April 2017]. Avaible from: http://www.planalto.gov.br/ccivil_03/Leis/L9394.htm.
http://www.planalto.gov.br/ccivil_03/Lei...

The Mann-Whitney U test was used to compare the median and interquartile range (IQR) of variables with non-normal distribution (perceptual-motor score, child’s BMI-for-age, duration of breastfeeding, mother’s age, mother’s education, and monthly household income).

Logistic regression was used to estimate the association between the categories of the outcome “cognitive perceptual-motor skills (lower and moderate/high)” and the independent variables (gender, duration of breast-feeding, feeding practice, child’s BMI, attending school, type of school, child stimulus, mother’s age, marital status, mother’s education, and monthly household income). Odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) were also calculated. In unadjusted analysis (Model 1), the crude effects of each risk factor were estimated. We used a hierarchical three-level approach (distal, intermediate, and proximal) to fit the adjusted models considering only variables withp<0.20 in the unadjusted analysis (Model 1). The distal variables were introduced first (mother’s age, mother’s education, and monthly household income), followed by the intermediate (gender) and proximal (attending school and type of school) variables (Model 2). In the second adjusted model (Model 3), “type of school” was not included in the analysis since students who did not attended school were not classified regarding the type of school. The MW in 2014 was US$ 306.00.

In this study, the presence of child stimulus was defined when the mother reported practicing any activity that contributed to the child’s learning, such as reading different types of books, storytelling, use of educational games, activities involving music, and other related activities. The variance inflation factor test with commonly applied cut-off values revealed little collinearity among the independent variables. Ap<0.05 was considered statistically significant in all analyses.

All procedures developed in the study were in accordance with the ethical standards involving human participants. The Research Ethics Committee of the University of Joinville Region approved this study (Protocol Nº. 107/2011).

Results

Of the 199 children included in the study, 114 (57.3%), 41 (20.6%) and 44 (22.1%) had low, moderate and high perceptual-motor skills, respectively. Table 1 shows the categorical variables of the children and mothers from 2012 to 2016 according to perceptual-motor skills at 4-5 years of age. The proportion of children with significantly lower perceptual-motor skills was higher among boys (63.6%), children not attending school (83.3%) and those studying at public schools (56.4%) compared to girls, children attending school and those studying at private schools, respectively. Regarding maternal characteristics, the proportion of children with low perceptual-motor skills was significantly higher among mothers who studied less than 12 years (70.3%) and whose monthly household income was <5 MW (60.2%) compared to those studying 12 or more years and earning 5 or more MW.

Table 1
Characteristics (categorical variables) of the study participants according to the child's perceptual-motor skills at 4-5 years of age. PREDI Study, 2014-17.

The determinants of low perceptual-motor skills are described in Table 2. Unadjusted analysis (Model 1) showed that boys and children not attending school had significantly greater odds of lower perceptual-motor skills at 4-5 years of age than girls (OR = 1.74, CI95%= 0.99-3.07) and those attending school (OR = 4.23, CI95%= 1.18-15.11). Regarding maternal characteristics, the odds of children having low perceptual-motor skills at 4-5 years of age were higher for mothers who studied less than 9 years and those earning <5 MW when compared to those who studied >9 years and those earning >5 MW (OR = 2.11, CI95%= 1.04-4.27; OR = 4.29, CI95%= 1.6111.43, for mother’s education and monthly household income, respectively). After adjustment considering only variables withp<0.20 from the unadjusted analysis (Model 1), male gender and monthly household income <5 MW were associated with higher odds of having low perceptual-motor skills at 4-5 years of age compared to female gender and monthly household income >5 MW (Model 2: OR = 2.11, CI95%= 1.12-3.97; OR = 5.10, CI95%= 1.58-16.45, respectively).

Table 2
Determinants of low cognitive perceptual-motor skills in Brazilian children at 4-5 years of age. PREDI Study, 2014-17.

Model 3 was adjusted considering only variables withp<0.20 from the unadjusted analysis, except for “type of school”. Male gender and not attending school continued to be independent predictors of low perceptual-motor skills (OR = 2.10, CI95%= 1.143.88; OR = 4.61, CI95%= 1.21-17.49, respectively). Regarding maternal characteristics, monthly household income <5 MW continued to be strongly associated with low perceptual-motor skills in children at preschool age (OR = 4.28, CI95%= 1.49- 12.26).

Discussion

The present study showed that gender, not attending school and monthly household income were associated with increased odds of children having low perceptual-motor skills at 4-5 years of age. Additionally, we observed a positive association between mother’s education and child’s perceptual-motor skills, but without statistical significance after adjusting for important confounders.

Some authors studied how children and adults perceive their movement skills (object control and locomotor) and how their perceptual skills are linked to the environment and concluded that children of both sexes were generally accurate in perceiving their movement competence.2121 Liong GH, Ridgers ND, Barnett LM. Associations between skill perceptions and young children's actual fundamental movement skills. Percept Mot Skills. 2015; 120 (2): 591-603.,2222 Croft JL, Bertram JEA. Affordance Boundaries Are Defined by Dynamic Capabilities of Parkour Athletes in Dropping from Various Heights. Front Psychol. 2017; 8: 1571.There seems to be a gender difference in favor of girls for almost all motor skill tasks of preschoolers.2323 Matarma T, Lagstrom H, Loyttyniemi E, Koski P. Motor Skills of 5-Year-Old Children: Gender Differences and Activity and Family Correlates. Percept Mot Skills. 2020; 127(2):367-85.Girls tend to develop more fine motor skills that request better concentration such as painting, drawing, modeling objects, and playing dolls, while boys tend to perform more gross motor activities such as sports, climbing trees, and running.33 Santos MO, Barbosa DG, Junior GJF, Silva RC, Pelegrini A, Felden EPG. Capacity of Objective Measures of Physical Activity to Predict Brazilian Children's Low Motor Proficiency. Percept Mot Skills. 2018; 125 (4): 669-81.Girls develop the areas of the brain responsible for motor development one year earlier than boys.2424 Larson JC, Mostofsky SH, Goldberg MC, Cutting LE, Denckla MB, Mahone EM. Effects of gender and age on motor exam in typically developing children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007; 32 (1): 543-62.In other words, greater control of performing repetitive and standardized movements tends to occur earlier in preschool girls compared to boys of the same age group.2424 Larson JC, Mostofsky SH, Goldberg MC, Cutting LE, Denckla MB, Mahone EM. Effects of gender and age on motor exam in typically developing children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007; 32 (1): 543-62.

Environmental and cultural factors may be one of the causes for the difference in motor skill strengths between boys and girls.2323 Matarma T, Lagstrom H, Loyttyniemi E, Koski P. Motor Skills of 5-Year-Old Children: Gender Differences and Activity and Family Correlates. Percept Mot Skills. 2020; 127(2):367-85.However, this difference in perceptual-motor development between boys and girls tends to disappear in adolescence.2424 Larson JC, Mostofsky SH, Goldberg MC, Cutting LE, Denckla MB, Mahone EM. Effects of gender and age on motor exam in typically developing children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007; 32 (1): 543-62.These effects are in agreement with our findings showing better perceptual-motor skills in girls.

The stimulation of cognitive functions at preschool age is extremely important and the effective participation of parents and relatives is necessary to continue promoting activities that stimulate the child’s cognitive development. Children show better cognitive development when their mothers have more years of education and higher IQ test scores compared to mothers with lower education level,2525 Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Kleinman KP, et al. Infant feeding and childhood cognition at ages 3 and 7 years: Effects of breastfeeding duration and exclusivity. JAMA Pediatrics. 2013; 167 (9): 836-44.,2626 Gibbs BG, Forste R. Breastfeeding, parenting, and early cognitive development. J Pediatrics. 2014; 164 (3): 487-93.even when they are born preterm.2727 Crane JR, Naylor PJ, Cook R, Temple VA. Do Perceptions of Competence Mediate The Relationship Between Fundamental Motor Skill Proficiency and Physical Activity Levels of Children in Kindergarten? J Phys Act Health. 2015;12(7):954-61.,2828 Oberer N, Gashaj V, Roebers CM. Motor skills in kinder-garten: Internal structure, cognitive correlates and relation-ships to background variables. Hum Mov Sci. 2017; 52: 170-80.In the present study, children born to mothers with a higher education level exhibited better perceptual-motor skills in unadjusted analysis, but not in the adjusted model. Mothers with higher education level are likely to stimulate their children and to become more engaged in their children’s school activities.

The socioeconomic status is also an important condition that influences the child’s cognitive development. Children whose parents had higher education showed better body coordination,2323 Matarma T, Lagstrom H, Loyttyniemi E, Koski P. Motor Skills of 5-Year-Old Children: Gender Differences and Activity and Family Correlates. Percept Mot Skills. 2020; 127(2):367-85.which agrees with our results. We demonstrated that the proportion of children with low perceptual-motor skills was significantly higher among mothers who reported a monthly household income <5 MW compared to those who earned five or more MW. In general, low-income families cannot afford materials that help stimulate their children’s cognitive development, such as books, educational games, toys, drawings, and paintings, nor do they have adequate space to play with their children. Most parents of low-income families work all day, leaving the child with relatives such as siblings, grandparents, or uncles/aunts who are also engaged in other household activities. It is also important to emphasize that low-income families generally do not have the culture of investing in their children’s learning because they did not receive such encouragement as a child. Both reading and writing are associated with the development of perceptual-motor skills.1010 Ferreira L, Godinez I, Gabbard C, Vieira JLL, Cacola P. Motor development in school-age children is associated with the home environment including socioeconomic status. Child Care Health Dev. 2018; 44 (6): 801-6.,2424 Larson JC, Mostofsky SH, Goldberg MC, Cutting LE, Denckla MB, Mahone EM. Effects of gender and age on motor exam in typically developing children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007; 32 (1): 543-62.

Another important finding of the present study was the association between not attending school and increased odds of having low perceptual-motor skills, which is probably due to the lack of stimuli. Children who attend school participate in various activities that stimulate different cognitive skills, including perceptual-motor skills. Some authors have highlighted the role of the school in the improvement of children’s cognitive development.2727 Crane JR, Naylor PJ, Cook R, Temple VA. Do Perceptions of Competence Mediate The Relationship Between Fundamental Motor Skill Proficiency and Physical Activity Levels of Children in Kindergarten? J Phys Act Health. 2015;12(7):954-61.,2828 Oberer N, Gashaj V, Roebers CM. Motor skills in kinder-garten: Internal structure, cognitive correlates and relation-ships to background variables. Hum Mov Sci. 2017; 52: 170-80.Children who do not attend school or are not stimulated by their parents at home are more likely to have low perceptual-motor skills. School educational activities at the beginning of literacy are critical for children to improve different cognitive skills and will certainly influence their school performance over the years.

Most studies evaluating the relationship between breast-feeding and cognitive functions in children and adolescents have used general assessment tests to evaluate cognitive development, such as the IQ test. In general, these tests are strongly influenced by biological factors such as physical state, mood, and motivation,22 Schmitt JA, Benton D, Kallus KW. General methodological considerations for the assessment of nutritional influences on human cognitive functions. Eur J Nutr. 2005; 44 (8): 459-64.,2929 Horta BL, Loret de Mola C, Victora CG. Breastfeeding and intelligence: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Paediatr. 2015; 104 (467): 14-9. and are conducted when the child has already started elementary school.1010 Ferreira L, Godinez I, Gabbard C, Vieira JLL, Cacola P. Motor development in school-age children is associated with the home environment including socioeconomic status. Child Care Health Dev. 2018; 44 (6): 801-6.,3030 Larson K, Russ SA, Nelson BB, Olson LM, Halfon N. Cognitive ability at kindergarten entry and socioeconomic status. Pediatrics. 2015; 135 (2): e440-8.We used a specific test validated exclusively for Brazilian children to evaluate the child’s perceptual-motor skills, which may reduce the influence of factors associated with tests developed in other populations.

The strengths of the current study should be mentioned. The data obtained in this study are primary data, a fact providing opportunities for future research in this field. Furthermore, this is a longitudinal study in which the mother-child pair was evaluated at the same time by the same team, ensuring the quality of the information. The adjustment for several important confounding factors is also an important strength of the study. Finally, the test used to evaluate the child’s perceptual-motor skills was developed in Brazil and adaptation to our population was therefore not necessary.

This study also has some limitations. First, the lack of studies and the different study designs made it difficult to compare the data. Second, since data collection took place in the family homes, children may have been influenced by the presence of the parents or distractions in the home environment. Third, some children were not motivated to respond

References to the instrument until the end of the study, a fact that may have influenced the test results and led to lower final scores. Motivation is important for any psychology test as it maintains interest and improves the response performance. Fourth, although the Mann-Whitney U test showed no significant difference in maternal education years, birthweight or child’s BMI-for-age between mothers/children enrolled at baseline and those who were not enrolled in the follow-ups, possible bias resulting from losses to follow-up cannot be disregarded. Finally, the relatively high prevalence of children with low perceptual-motor skills (57.3%) who were from families with low income (monthly household income <5 MW in 83.5%) in our study suggests them to be a high-risk group.

In conclusion, our study showed that gender, not attending school and monthly household income are important factors that have an impact on perceptual-motor skills at the beginning of literacy. From an educational and public health perspective, we believe that strategies aimed at enhancing the child’s perceptual-motor skills may help improve cognitive performance in adolescence and adulthood.

References

  • 1
    Investigators M-EN. Early childhood cognitive develop-ment is affected by interactions among illness, diet, enteropathogens and the home environment: findings from the MAL-ED birth cohort study. BMJ Glob Health. 2018; 3 (4): e000752.
  • 2
    Schmitt JA, Benton D, Kallus KW. General methodological considerations for the assessment of nutritional influences on human cognitive functions. Eur J Nutr. 2005; 44 (8): 459-64.
  • 3
    Santos MO, Barbosa DG, Junior GJF, Silva RC, Pelegrini A, Felden EPG. Capacity of Objective Measures of Physical Activity to Predict Brazilian Children's Low Motor Proficiency. Percept Mot Skills. 2018; 125 (4): 669-81.
  • 4
    Liang J, Matheson BE, Kaye WH, Boutelle KN. Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. Int J Obes (Lond). 2014;38(4):494-506.
  • 5
    Adolph KE, Hoch JE. Motor Development: Embodied, Embedded, Enculturated, and Enabling. Annu Rev Psychol. 2019;70:141-64.
  • 6
    Pearce A, Sawyer ACP, Chittleborough CR, Mittinty MN, Law C, Lynch JW. Do early life cognitive ability and self-regulation skills explain socio-economic inequalities in academic achievement? An effect decomposition analysis in UK and Australian cohorts. Soc Sci Med. 2016; 165: 108-18.
  • 7
    Herwig A. Linking perception and action by structure or process? Toward an integrative perspective. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015; 52: 105-16.
  • 8
    Babic MJ, Morgan PJ, Plotnikoff RC, Lonsdale C, White RL, Lubans DR. Physical activity and physical self-concept in youth: systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2014;44(11):1589-601.
  • 9
    Brinkman S, Gregory T, Harris J, Hart B, Blackmore S, Janus M. Associations Between the Early Development Instrument at Age 5, and Reading and Numeracy Skills at Ages 8, 10 and 12: a Prospective Linked Data Study. Child Ind Res. 2013; 6 (4): 695-708.
  • 10
    Ferreira L, Godinez I, Gabbard C, Vieira JLL, Cacola P. Motor development in school-age children is associated with the home environment including socioeconomic status. Child Care Health Dev. 2018; 44 (6): 801-6.
  • 11
    Ronfani L, Vecchi Brumatti L, Mariuz M, Tognin V, Bin M, Ferluga V, Knowles A, Montico M, Barbone F. The Complex Interaction between Home Environment, Socioeconomic Status, Maternal IQ and Early Child Neurocognitive Development: A Multivariate Analysis of Data Collected in a Newborn Cohort Study. PLoS One. 2015; 10 (5): e0127052.
  • 12
    Crampton A, Hall J. Unpacking socio-economic risks for reading and academic self-concept in primary school: Differential effects and the role of the preschool home learning environment. Br J Educ Psychol. 2017; 87 (3): 365-82.
  • 13
    Sales WB, Silleno Junior JD, Kroll C, Mastroeni SSBS, Silva JC, Mastroeni MF. Influence of altered maternal lipid profile on the lipid profile of the newborn. Arch Endocrinol Metab. 2015; 59 (2): 123-8.
  • 14
    Mastroeni MF, Czarnobay SA, Kroll C, Figueirêdo KBW, Mastroeni SSBS, silva JC, Khan MKA, Loehr S, Veugelers PJ. The Independent Importance of Pre-pregnancy Weight and Gestational Weight Gain for the Prevention of Large-for Gestational Age Brazilian Newborns. Matern Child Health J. 2017; 21 (4): 705-14.
  • 15
    WHO. (World Health Organization). WHO Child Growth Standards: Length/height-for-age, weight-for-age, weight-for-length, weight-for-height and body mass index-for-age: Methods and development. WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group. Geneva; 2006.
  • 16
    WHO (World Health Organization). Indicators for Assessing Infant and Young Child Feeding Practices: part 1: definitions: conclusions of a consensus meeting held 6-8 November 2007 in Washington, DC, USA. Geneva; 2008.
  • 17
    Mastroeni MF, Mastroeni SSBS, Czarnobay SA, Ekwaru JP, Loehr SA, Veugelers PJ. Breast-feeding duration for the prevention of excess body weight of mother-child pairs concurrently: a 2-year cohort study. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20(14):2537-48.
  • 18
    Silva RS, Flores-Mendonza C, Telles M. Teste de Habilidades e Conhecimentos Pré-Alfabetização (THCP). V.1. 1 ed. ed. São Paulo: Vetor; 2012.
  • 19
    IBGE (Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística). Pesquisa Nacional por Amostra de Domicílios. 2010. [Accessed April 2017]. http://www.ibge.gov.br/home/esta-tistica/populacao/trabalhoerendimento/pnad98/saude/analis e.shtm
    » http://www.ibge.gov.br/home/esta-tistica/populacao/trabalhoerendimento/pnad98/saude/analis e.shtm
  • 20
    Brasil. Presidência da República. Casa Civil. Subchefia para Assuntos Jurídicos. Lei nº 9.394, de 20 de dezembro de 1996. Estabelece as Diretrizes e Bases da Educação Nacional. Brasília. 1996. [Accessed April 2017]. Avaible from: http://www.planalto.gov.br/ccivil_03/Leis/L9394.htm
    » http://www.planalto.gov.br/ccivil_03/Leis/L9394.htm
  • 21
    Liong GH, Ridgers ND, Barnett LM. Associations between skill perceptions and young children's actual fundamental movement skills. Percept Mot Skills. 2015; 120 (2): 591-603.
  • 22
    Croft JL, Bertram JEA. Affordance Boundaries Are Defined by Dynamic Capabilities of Parkour Athletes in Dropping from Various Heights. Front Psychol. 2017; 8: 1571.
  • 23
    Matarma T, Lagstrom H, Loyttyniemi E, Koski P. Motor Skills of 5-Year-Old Children: Gender Differences and Activity and Family Correlates. Percept Mot Skills. 2020; 127(2):367-85.
  • 24
    Larson JC, Mostofsky SH, Goldberg MC, Cutting LE, Denckla MB, Mahone EM. Effects of gender and age on motor exam in typically developing children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007; 32 (1): 543-62.
  • 25
    Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Kleinman KP, et al. Infant feeding and childhood cognition at ages 3 and 7 years: Effects of breastfeeding duration and exclusivity. JAMA Pediatrics. 2013; 167 (9): 836-44.
  • 26
    Gibbs BG, Forste R. Breastfeeding, parenting, and early cognitive development. J Pediatrics. 2014; 164 (3): 487-93.
  • 27
    Crane JR, Naylor PJ, Cook R, Temple VA. Do Perceptions of Competence Mediate The Relationship Between Fundamental Motor Skill Proficiency and Physical Activity Levels of Children in Kindergarten? J Phys Act Health. 2015;12(7):954-61.
  • 28
    Oberer N, Gashaj V, Roebers CM. Motor skills in kinder-garten: Internal structure, cognitive correlates and relation-ships to background variables. Hum Mov Sci. 2017; 52: 170-80.
  • 29
    Horta BL, Loret de Mola C, Victora CG. Breastfeeding and intelligence: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Paediatr. 2015; 104 (467): 14-9.
  • 30
    Larson K, Russ SA, Nelson BB, Olson LM, Halfon N. Cognitive ability at kindergarten entry and socioeconomic status. Pediatrics. 2015; 135 (2): e440-8.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    30 Oct 2020
  • Date of issue
    Jul-Sep 2020

History

  • Received
    04 Feb 2019
  • Reviewed
    25 May 2020
  • Accepted
    26 June 2020
Instituto de Medicina Integral Prof. Fernando Figueira Rua dos Coelhos, 300. Boa Vista, 50070-550 Recife PE Brasil, Tel./Fax: +55 81 2122-4141 - Recife - PR - Brazil
E-mail: revista@imip.org.br