1 Introduction
The captive environment differs from the wild with respect to what animals experience. For instance, unlike wild animals, those housed in zoos are subjected to contact with unfamiliar humans. Public exposure can be one of the determining factors in the expression of stereotypic pacing (e.g., Carlstead, 1996; Davey, 2007; Mason et al., 2007; Morgan and Tromborg, 2007), the repetitive and apparently functionless locomotor behavior displayed by captive animals (Mason et al., 2007). Davey (2007) suggested that compared to small animals, large animals such as big cats are less prone to the negative effects of visitors. Therefore, one might expect varying results, as it seems to be the case. Increased pacing rates have been associated with the presence of large crowds in the vicinity of the enclosures for Indian leopards, Panthera pardus (Pocock, 1927) (Mallapur and Chellam, 2002). In contrast, Sellinger and Ha (2005) and Quirke et al. (2012) did not find positive associations for jaguars, Panthera onca (Linnaeus, 1758) and cheetahs, Acinonyx jubatus (Schreber, 1775), respectively. Overall, researchers concentrate on primates and a few other groups of species (Davey, 2007), but the effect of the visitor number on the display of pacing by large felids remains poorly addressed.
Increasing interest is emerging regarding the evaluation of currently used enrichment techniques that are alleged to reduce stereotypic behaviors (Swaisgood and Shepherdson, 2005, 2006; Shyne, 2006; Wells, 2009). For example, in a meta-analysis encompassing 54 studies Shyne (2006) reported that zoo animals exhibit less often stereotypic behaviors after the provision of an enrichment condition. While there is little doubt about the effectiveness of environmental enrichment on reducing stereotypic and abnormal behavior, the remaining challenge has been to determine suitable enrichment models for a given species. Different models have been introduced to felid enclosures, varying from novel food items or olfactory stimuli (Mellen and Shepherdson, 1997; McPhee, 2006; Bashaw et al., 2003; Wells and Egli, 2004; Skibiel et al., 2007; Quirke and O’Riordan, 2011; Resende et al., 2011) to objects or physical toys (Moreira et al., 2007). Until recently the provisioning of novel scents as enrichment alternatives for felids had been overlooked when compared to food items (Skibiel et al., 2007). However, adding odors has been shown to be successful in minimizing the time devoted to pacing (Skibiel et al., 2007; Quirke and O’Riordan, 2011; Resende et al., 2011).
Jaguars are the largest extant felid in the Americas (Nowell and Jackson, 1996) about which hardly any information regarding pacing is available. Sellinger and Ha (2005) found that jaguars increased pacing behavior in response to low-level noise, but not to visitor numbers. Three spices (cinnamon, chilli powder, and cumin) and four enrichment models (toy, food, sound, and scent) have been shown to reduce pacing in jaguars (Skibiel et al., 2007; Castillo-Guevara et al., 2012). Unfortunately, the separate effect of each spice or model was not tested by the researchers. The number of jaguars a given zoo is able to shelter is normally small, which precludes researchers from obtaining large sample sizes. However, zoos provide the opportunity to carrying out individual-based approaches. Here, we address the following questions regarding three captive jaguars: 1) Does exposure to an increasing number of visitors lead to a rise in the display of pacing? and 2) Does pacing expression reduce after the provisioning of spices? We predicted that pacing expression in jaguars would increase in accordance with visitor number. We also hypothesized that jaguars would respond positively to the provisioning of each spice by devoting less time to pacing.
2 Material and Methods
2.1 Subjects and housing
We tested all jaguars available at Parque Ecológico de São Carlos (PESC) in São Carlos, SP, Brazil: two adult males and one adult female. One of the males (B) was housed singly while the other male (T) and the female (A) were housed as a pair. Born at PESC, the males B and T were 5 and 11 years-old, respectively, at the start of the study. The female A was brought to the zoo after spending some time in an institution that shelters wild animals rescued from illegal trade. Therefore, her birthplace and age were unknown. The jaguars were 70 kg (both B and T) and 50 kg (A) in body size.
The area encompassed by each enclosure, one adjacent to the other, was 128 m2. These enclosures, separated by a concrete wall, were both partially covered by wooden sheds for shelter. A glass barrier covered the front part of each enclosure, allowing a full view of the inner side. The floor contained bare ground and grass interspersed with rocks and tree trunks. A few palm trees and shrubs provided the animals with shelter from sunlight and a hiding place from public view, respectively. The jaguars were fed red meat three days a week, and Wistar rat, Rattus norvegicus (Berkenhout, 1796) and chicken meat once and four times a month, respectively.
2.2 Experimental protocol
In the month prior to the start of the study, the jaguars were habituated to the presence of the observers. We collected behavioral data in two phases: 1) prior to the addition of the enrichment (baseline) from October 2006 to March 2007 and 2) following the addition of the enrichment from September 2007 to May 2008. Sampling was carried out on Saturdays from 0900 h to 1600 h. Although we recognize that gathering data after the removal of the enrichment is important to evaluate to what extent the stimuli improved the animals’ behavior (Bashaw et al., 2003), time and logistical constraints prevented us from doing so. Sampling was always performed under on-exhibit conditions, i.e., when the jaguars were exposed to visitors. In both phases we recorded behavioral data (see below) and the total number of visitors in the surroundings of the enclosures during the timespan of each sample period.
We used a focal animal sampling method (Altmann, 1974). The order in which the animals were sampled was randomized. Each focal individual was continuously observed during sample periods lasting two or three hours. We carried out 11 sample periods of 2 h and 6 periods of 3 h per animal in both phases. We thus collected 40 h of focal data per individual in each phase. No individual was sampled twice in a single day. In each sample period, we recorded the animal’s identity, and with a cronometer, the extension of time (minutes) the animal engaged in pacing. Before starting data collection in phase 2, 50 mg of powdered cinnamon (Cinnamomum zeylanicum Blume.) or black pepper (Piper nigrum L.) was sprinkled over the tree trunks in the form of a scent trail. We selected these spices because they are readily purchasable and easily provided. One type of spice was provided on a Friday afternoon and left in the enclosure until the next Friday, when it was added again. We finished the cinnamon trials and after one week-interval, during which the zoo personnel cleaned the cages, we started the black pepper trials. This protocol adheres to the guidelines for the use of animals in research of the International Society for Applied Ethology.
2.3 Data analysis
Although using each sample period gathered for the same animal as a replication might be regarded as a pseudoreplication, we believe that the seven days that elapsed between two consecutive sampling periods lend a minimal level of independence to the observations. We used linear regression to test whether the log10-transformed number of visitors determined the time jaguars devoted to pacing. Data were transformed to minimize the discrepancy between very dissimilar values. The test of the effectiveness of olfactory enrichment involved a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for each animal. We compared the mean time devoted to pacing by the same jaguar in the baseline and after the provisioning of each spice separately. Raw data were tested for homogeneity of variance using Bartlett’s test (Zar, 1999) and transformed to their square root when necessary. When variance heterogeneity persisted, we tested the nontransformed data using the Kruskal-Wallis test. The difference between the effects of adding cinnamon or black pepper was tested by Tukey test when the previous test had been ANOVA or Newman-Keuls test when the previous test had been Kruskal-Wallis. The confidence level was set at 95% (α = 0.05) for all tests.
3 Results
The number of visitors in the vicinity of the enclosures in phase 1 varied from 0-177, 0-231, and 0-131 during the sampling periods of B, T and A, respectively. The amount of time that all jaguars engaged in pacing increased significantly with the number of visitors (R2 = 0.444; n = 17; P = 0.003 for B; R2 = 0.605; n = 17; P = 0.000 for T and R2 = 0.724; n = 17; P = 0.000 for A; Figure 1). B, T and A devoted respectively 42.47 ± 21.94 min (mean ± sd), 41.58 ± 31.81 min and 1.94 ± 3.38 min to pacing in the baseline. In phase 2 the number of visitors near the enclosures of B, T and A varied from 0-164, 2-197 and 0-182, respectively. Following the provisioning of the scents, pacing display by B (F = 16.87; df = 2; P = 0.00) and T (Kruskall-Wallis comparison KW = 10.54; P = 0.005) decreased significantly (Figure 2), a fall corresponding to around 73% and 89% in B and T, respectively. Cinnamon and black pepper showed similar effects on pacing expression by B (Tukey comparison MS = 2.22; df = 26; P = 0.273) and T (Newmann-Keuls comparison MS = 632.060; P = 0.608). In contrast, the difference in time devoted to pacing by A between the two phases was not significant (F = 1.88; df = 2; P = 0.172).
4 Discussion
The amount of time that jaguars dedicated to pacing was highly related to the number of visitors around their cages. This is in contrast to Sellinger and Ha’s (2005) finding of a lack of association between visitor density and the frequency of pacing by an adult pair of jaguars. However, low-level noise (quiet talking) led to an increase of pacing behavior by the female (Sellinger and Ha, 2005). Despite the paucity of data, our results combined with those of Sellinger and Ha (2005), which also used an individual-based approach, suggest that the effect of public exposure on jaguars’ stereotypic pacing may operate on an individual-basis, being ultimately determined by the sensitivity of the enclosure’s occupant to one or more environmental stimuli. Although this argument may seem speculative, taking it as truth, one could ask why all the jaguars at PESC are sensitive to the increase in visitor numbers. Perhaps the shrubs planted inside both enclosures, which supposedly provide jaguars with hiding places, are ineffective. Alternatively, the reduced size of the enclosure could be operating in consort with public exposure, thereby enhancing pacing expression. Although some of the current available data lack statistical treatment, evidences suggest that confinement in enclosures leading to restricted movement may be harmful for felids (Bashaw et al., 2007; Moreira et al., 2007; Szokalski et al., 2012). For example, tigers housed in small enclosures paced more frequently than those in larger enclosures (Bashaw et al., 2007). The smallest enclosure (149 m2) in the investigation by Bashaw et al. (2007) is slightly larger than those in our study site (128 m2). On the other hand, the enclosure of the jaguar pair in Sellinger and Ha’s (2005) study is smaller (88 m2) than ours, but these authors did not detect an effect of visitor number on pacing expression. We cannot disregard that the reduced “territory” of the jaguars we studied may influence the expression of pacing. However, enclosures vary in their structural complexity, and arrangements within enclosures may compensate for restriction of available space. Indeed, controlling for structural diversity when comparing animals from different zoos housed in enclosures of approximately similar size is difficult, and is therefore hardly ever done.
We found that the amount of time the male jaguars dedicated to pacing diminished after providing the spices. Both the cinnamon and black pepper trails were equally effective in reducing pacing expression. This suggests that these spices are suitable for the male jaguars, despite the difference between B and T regarding age (5 × 11 years-old) and housing conditions (isolated x paired). The female increased pacing in accordance with visitor density, but her mean pacing time did not differ in the two phases. Actually, A displayed short pacing bouts in the baseline phase, a pattern that was not observed in the males. Two prevailing circumstances in the baseline period may have caused that. First, despite our care towards preventing bias, we noted reduced visitor density around the enclosure of the pair in the sampling periods of A. The reason for this difference in visitor number is unknown, as the sampling of all animals was carried out at similar periods of the day. Second, the repetitive attempts of T to copulate with A may have led to an increase in inactivity on her part. Meanwhile, how far these conditions are associated to her unresponsiveness to the spice provisioning is an open question. Perhaps a study with female jaguars housed as singletons and as pairs should be conducted to test whether male behavior play a role in female low level or lack of pacing. Our data agree with previous studies reporting the addition of spices and a subsequent decrease in pacing displayed by felids (Wells and Egli, 2004; Skibiel et al., 2007; Resende et al., 2011). However, unlike our study, these investigators report their outcomes for all animals combined, which prevents uncovering the possible influence of individual attributes.
Although apparently multiple factors besides visitor number may underlie the display of pacing by jaguars, it is highly recommended that the husbandry staff in our study site implement strategies to counteract the visitor effect. For example, the replacement of the current glass barrier by a one-way viewing glass barrier should be considered. This measure should be followed by the use of effective wording on signs appealing to visitors not to knock on the glass barrier or not to make excessive noise.
Our results must be viewed with caution because of the reduced number of animals and total behavioral sampling of each animal. The results also have a limited scope because we do not have additional behavioral data rather than stereotypic pacing display. Moreover, we cannot evaluate whether other behaviors are affected by public exposure, as is the case with pumas, Puma concolor (Linnaeus, 1771): instead of affecting locomotion behavior, the high number of visitors and the noise they make increased the inactivity of an adult female and her 8-year-old daughter during the morning periods (Maia et al., 2012). Nevertheless, we found interesting outputs that add novelty to a poorly studied felid species. Visual contact with an increasing number of visitors led to extended pacing display in the studied jaguars. Despite differences between male jaguars regarding age and presence of a companion, either cinnamon or black pepper were suitable to reduce the expression of pacing in them. The unresponsiveness of the female jaguar to spice provisioning was likely associated to her prior reduced engagement in pacing. Despite the aforementioned constraints, our findings point out to the validity of investing efforts towards evaluating the effectiveness of novel scents through individual-based approaches. They may be useful in revealing how far olfactory stimuli are suitable to minimizing stereotypies irrespective of divergences in particular traits. Further research should test whether the usefulness reported in this case study can be generalized to other circumstances.