Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Defensive repertoire of Xenodon dorbignyi (Serpentes, Dipsadidae)

Repertório defensivo de Xenodon dorbignyi (Serpentes, Dipsadidae)

Abstracts

The ability of a species to defend itself against a predator is directly correlated with its survivorship. Thus, prey/predator interaction mechanisms are important elements of the natural history of species. In this study, we examined the defensive repertoire of the South-American hognose snake (Xenodon dorbignyi) through simulations of predator attacks in the field. Nine defensive displays were observed. The most frequently observed displays were erratic movements, body flattening, head triangulation and tail display. No differences were detected in the defensive strategies shown by males and females, regardless of their reproductive state. Our findings suggest that X. dorbignyi has the ability to evaluate the level of threat imposed by the aggressor, with cryptic behavior, body flattening and locomotor escape as the primary defensive strategies, with other displays used as secondary responses to a predator attack. Our results support the hypothesis that X. dorbignyi is a mimic of both Micrurus and Bothrops.

defensive behavior; predation; snake; display


A capacidade de uma espécie em defender-se da ação de um predador esta diretamente relacionada à sua sobrevivência. Nesse sentido, os mecanismos de interação presa/predador são elementos importantes da história natural das espécies. Neste estudo foi avaliado o repertório defensivo da cobra-nariguda (Xenodon dorbignyi), a partir da simulação de ataques predatórios na natureza. Foram observadas nove unidades comportamentais defensivas, sendo os movimentos erráticos, achatamento do corpo, triangulação da cabeça e display caudal os comportamentos mais freqüentemente exibidos. Não foram detectadas diferenças na estratégia defensiva entre os sexos ou entre indivíduos em diferentes estados reprodutivos. Os resultados sugerem que X. dorbignyi possui a capacidade de avaliar o nível de ameaça imposto pelo agressor, sendo o comportamento críptico, o achatamento do corpo e a fuga as estratégias defensivas primárias, com os demais displays representando respostas secundárias ao ataque predatório. Os resultados reforçam a hipótese de que X. dorbignyi represente simultaneamente um mímico de Micrurus e Bothrops.

comportamento defensivo; predação; serpente; display


ARTICLES

Defensive repertoire of Xenodon dorbignyi (Serpentes, Dipsadidae)

Repertório defensivo de Xenodon dorbignyi (Serpentes, Dipsadidae)

Alexandro Marques TozettiI,* * Corresponding Author: Alexandro Marques Tozetti, e-mail: mtozetti@uol.com.br ; Roberto Baptista de OliveiraII; Glaucia Maria Funk PontesII

ILaboratório de Ecologia de Vertebrados Terrestres, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande - UFRG, Av. Itália, Km 8, CEP 96201-900, Rio Grande, RS, Brazil

IILaboratório de Herpetologia, Museu de Ciências e Tecnologia da PUCRS, Av. Ipiranga, 6681, CEP 90619-900, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil

ABSTRACT

The ability of a species to defend itself against a predator is directly correlated with its survivorship. Thus, prey/predator interaction mechanisms are important elements of the natural history of species. In this study, we examined the defensive repertoire of the South-American hognose snake (Xenodon dorbignyi) through simulations of predator attacks in the field. Nine defensive displays were observed. The most frequently observed displays were erratic movements, body flattening, head triangulation and tail display. No differences were detected in the defensive strategies shown by males and females, regardless of their reproductive state. Our findings suggest that X. dorbignyi has the ability to evaluate the level of threat imposed by the aggressor, with cryptic behavior, body flattening and locomotor escape as the primary defensive strategies, with other displays used as secondary responses to a predator attack. Our results support the hypothesis that X. dorbignyi is a mimic of both Micrurus and Bothrops.

Keywords: defensive behavior, predation, snake, display.

RESUMO

A capacidade de uma espécie em defender-se da ação de um predador esta diretamente relacionada à sua sobrevivência. Nesse sentido, os mecanismos de interação presa/predador são elementos importantes da história natural das espécies. Neste estudo foi avaliado o repertório defensivo da cobra-nariguda (Xenodon dorbignyi), a partir da simulação de ataques predatórios na natureza. Foram observadas nove unidades comportamentais defensivas, sendo os movimentos erráticos, achatamento do corpo, triangulação da cabeça e display caudal os comportamentos mais freqüentemente exibidos. Não foram detectadas diferenças na estratégia defensiva entre os sexos ou entre indivíduos em diferentes estados reprodutivos. Os resultados sugerem que X. dorbignyi possui a capacidade de avaliar o nível de ameaça imposto pelo agressor, sendo o comportamento críptico, o achatamento do corpo e a fuga as estratégias defensivas primárias, com os demais displays representando respostas secundárias ao ataque predatório. Os resultados reforçam a hipótese de que X. dorbignyi represente simultaneamente um mímico de Micrurus e Bothrops.

Palavras-chave: comportamento defensivo, predação, serpente, display.

Introduction

Predators play an important role in the population dynamics of their prey, affecting even demographic aspects of these populations (e.g., Sinclair & Arcese 1995, Lima 1998). The ability of a species to defend itself from a predator is intrinsically associated to its survivorship, increasing its capacity to exploit the environment as well as to obtain resources (Lima & Dill 1990, Downes 2001). Thus, antipredator mechanisms are important factors in the evolutionary processes of many animals (see Vermeij 1982, Lima & Dill 1990). In general, prey/predator interaction mechanisms are key elements of the natural history of species, and their understanding contributes to the development of evolutionary biology and ecological studies (Greene 1988).

Reptiles exhibit extremely varied antipredator mechanisms, including cryptic coloration and/or behavior, mimicry, aposematism, in addition to various ways of intimidating aggressors (Pough et al. 2004). Snakes are interesting models for studies on defense mechanism, as they have very elaborated behaviors and are potential preys of several animals, such as mammals, birds, other reptiles and invertebrates (Greene 1988). Because of their ectotherm physiology, snakes have their capacity of performing defensive behaviors influenced by environmental factors, such as level of solar exposure and air temperature, that directly affect their body temperature and metabolic levels (Duvall et al. 1985, Brodie & Russel 1999, Shine et al. 2000). Another important environmental element associated to antipredator strategies is habitat heterogeneity, which provides different possibilities for shelter (e.g. holes, burrows, vegetation; Shine et al. 2000). Shepard (2007) shows that the probability of attack to lizards models is higher in more open microhabitats.

The defensive repertoire of the South-American hognose snake (Xenodon dorbignyi) makes it an interesting subject for studies on defense mechanisms against predators. Xenodon dorbignyi is a small semi-fossorial colubrid snake of the tribe Xenodontini (Cadle 1984) usually associated to open environments (e.g. grasslands) that occur in southern Brazil, Paraguay, Uruguay, and Argentina (Lema 1994, Oliveira 2005). The genus Lystrophis has been recently synonymized with Xenodon, according to results of a revised phylogeny and classification of Neotropical xenodontines (Zaher et al. 2009). Despite the relatively wide distribution of this species, field studies are scarce (Oliveira et al. 2001). In the present study, the defensive behavior of individuals from a population of X. dorbignyi was examined based on environmental and sexual variables through simulations of predator attacks in natural environment.

Material and Methods

Fieldwork was carried out in an area of coastal sand dunes (sampled area of approximately 333 ha; 30º 21' S and 50º 17" W; sea level), in the municipality of Pinhal, northern coast of Rio Grande do Sul State, Brazil. The area is characterized by coastal dunes with sparse vegetation consisted of shrubs and plants associated to a system of temporary lagoons (Waechter 1985). The climate is humid subtropical, according to the classification of Köppen (Vieira & Rangel 1998), with average annual temperature of 20 ºC and uniform average annual precipitation of 1322.9 mm (Hasenack & Ferraro 1989). Individuals of Xenodon dorbignyi were found by non-systematic visual search between November and December of 2007, a period that includes the breeding season (Oliveira 2005). Searches were conducted by walking slowly while examining as many microhabitats as possible in the study area. Searches were carried out between between 7:00 and 12:00 AM and between 4:00 and 6:00 PM including the daily peak of activity of this species (Oliveira 2005). Immediately after each encounter, individuals were subjected to a stimulatory session of predator attacks. The simulation consisted of five repeated movements of the experimenter's hand towards the snake's head. The snake was confronted with the experimenter's open hand at a constant speed (Figure 1) so that the time interval between the first and the fifth approach was not longer than 20 seconds. Despite this simple scheme, studies show that the approach of the human hand elicits the same types of response observed in a real predator attack on snakes and frogs (Herzog & Bailey 1987, Shine et al. 2000, Gibbons & Dorcas 2002, Toledo et al. 2005).


All defensive displays shown by individuals were classified according to Greene (1988). After each session of stimuli, individuals were measured (size and body mass) and ventral scales were permanently tattooed (Di-Bernardo et al. 2007). The sex of individuals was determined by everting the hemipenis and/or use of cloacal probing. Females were palpated to assess their reproductive status (e.g. presence of eggs of large follicles; Fitch 1987). Each snake was tested only once immediately after being found, and was released after the test.

The behavior of snakes, from the moment they were found until the examination and release was divided into three categories (treatments): 1 - response during first approach (defensive displays observed during the interval between the encounter of the animal and the beginning of the simulation); 2 - reaction during the session of stimuli (defensive displays observed during the simulation) and; 3 - reaction during handling (defensive displays observed during the manipulation process for marking and measurements). For each one of the three categories, the occurrence of each defensive display was recorded, rather than the number of times of each defensive display. For example, if an individual stroke once or several times during only one category (e.g. hand approach), only one record was made for this display; if the individual stroke once or several times in the three categories, three records were made, one for each category. Thus, it was possible to quantitatively compare displays of different natures without having to evaluate frequency (e.g. number of strikes) or the duration of the display (e.g. strike, body flatten) in the same session. Only displays recorded in at least 30% of the observations were considered.

Because of the predominance of exposed soil with little or no vegetation, snakes were visually located from an average distance of two meters, allowing the observation of the behavior of the animal before the effects of the disturbance caused by the approach of the experimenter. Only active animals (exposed) on the surface of the substrate were tested. The existence of correlations between the displays observed and substrate and air temperature was examined with the Spearman Rank Order Correlation tests, rs. Comparisons between the obtained and expected values for each one of the displays were conducted with the chi-square test. Comparisons between treatments were conducted with G test (Fowler et al. 1998).

The defensive repertoire of X. dorbignyi was compared to those of other species of the tribe Xenodontini. The comparison with species of the same tribe is justified, because despite the scarcity of phylogenetic studies on neotropical snakes, there are indications that species of South American Xenodontini form a monophyletic group (Vidal et al. 2000; Zaher et al. 2009). The comparison with other species was performed using the data reported by Marques et al. (2004), Carreira et al. (2005), Marques et al. (2005) and Sawaya et al. (2008). These studies were chosen because the procedures used to record defensive behaviors of snakes were similar to those of the present study. We compiled species data in a presence/absence matrix of each defensive display and perform a comparison among species with a cluster analysis (Sneath & Sokal 1973), using the Euclidian distance and the grouping method "weighted pair-group average" (WPGA; Sneath & Sokal 1973), which sets different weights for groups of different sizes (variation on the number of displays observed in each species). We adopted the species nomenclature of Bérnils (2009). For all analyses, differences were considered significant when p < 0.05 (Zar 1999).

Results

We analysed a total of 31 adult individuals of Xenodon dorbignyi, including 15 males (average body size = 396.5 mm ± 102.3 mm; range = 138.0 mm - 494.0 mm; average body mass = 29.4 g ± 12.0 g; range = 8.45 g - 51.6 g) and 16 females (average body size = 396.6 mm ± 54.7 mm; range = 265.0 mm - 484.0 mm; average body mass = 34.1 g ± 11.1 g; range = 10.8 g - 56.4 g), of which six were gravid. Nine behavioral displays were characterized: (1) body flattening (dorsoventral body compression), (2) cloacal discharge (contents of the cloaca are expelled, including feces and urine), (3) erratic movements (body thrashing), (4) false strike (strike with closed mouth), (5) head triangulation, (6) hide head (the head is hidden under one or more parts of the body), (7) locomotor escape, (8) tail display (coiled tail is elevated; Figure 1) and (9) immobility (not evaluated during the handling).

A significant variation in the number of records of each behavioral display was observed (χ2 = 68.0; df = 8; p < 0.001). The most frequently observed displays were erratic movements (21.0% of records), flattened body (18.8% of records), head triangulation (16.1% of records) and tail display (14.0% of records; Table 1). A small number of records of false strikes (2.7% of records) and hiding the head (2.0% of records) was observed. Cloacal discharge was recorded exclusively during handling and false strikes never occurred during the first approach (Table 1).

The frequency of each defensive display varied significantly between first approach and the simulation of the predator attack (G = 25.150; df = 14; p < 0.05). When analyzed separately, the frequency of each display observed during the first approach did not vary significantly (χ2 = 8.2; df = 5; p = 0.14; Figure 2). However, this variation was significant during the simulation of the predator attack (hand approach; χ2 = 41.2; df = 7; p < 0 .001; Figure 2), in which the most frequent displays were body flattening (22.0% of records), tail display (21.0% of records), erratic movements (20.0% of records), and head triangulation (16.0% of records; Figure 2). In 2.0% of the records, animals hid their heads during hand approach, and remained motionless in only 1.0% of records. The frequency of each defensive display varied significantly during snake handling (χ2 = 37.5; df = 5; p < 0.001; Figure 3). When handled, the most frequent displays were cloacal discharge (35.0% of records), erratic movements (35.0% of records) and head triangulation (21.0% of records).



When analyzed separately, no significant differences were found in the number of records of each display in gravid females (χ2 = 11.0; df = 8; p = 0.20; Table 1). However, differences were significant for non-gravid females (χ2 = 25.7; df = 8; p < 0.001) and males (χ2 = 38.9; df = 8; p < 0.001; Table 1). The most frequently observed displays were erratic movements (20.4% of records of non-gravid females and 21.6% of records of males), body flattening (18.5% of records of non- gravid females and 20.6% of records of males), head triangulation (18.5% of records of non- gravid females and 15.5% of records of males), followed by tail display in non-gravid females (18.5% of records) and locomotor escape in males (14.4% of records; Table 1). A significant positive correlation was observed between the number of displays and substrate temperature (rs = 0.36; p = 0.047; N = 30). However, no significant correlations were found between the number of displays observed and body temperature (rs = 0.16; p = 0.38; N = 30) or air temperature (rs = 0.34; P = 0.065; N = 30).

The comparison with other species of the tribe Xenodontini revealed two large clusters, one grouping most of the species of Xenodon compared and another group consisted mostly of species of the genus Liophis (Figure 4). Among the species of Xenodon compared, X. nattereri was most similar to X. dorbignyi, which was also similar to L. aesculapii.


Discussion

Tested individuals of Xenodon dorbignyi exhibited an elaborated defensive repertoire comprised of a diverse set of defensive displays. Xenodon dorbignyi actively forages on the substrate surface (Oliveira et al. 2001, Oliveira 2005), is diurnal and associated with open habitats (e.g. fields and sand dunes), favoring the action of visually oriented predators (e.g. birds of prey and/or mammals; Oliveira et al. 2004). This hypothesis is reinforced by the fact that the most frequent displays observed in the tested individuals (body flattening, tail display, and head triangulation) are associated with defense against visually oriented predators. Body flattening and head triangulation, for example, may create the illusion that X. dorbignyi is larger than it is, inducing the predator to misjudge its capacity to subdue its prey. Erratic movements might be a way to demonstrate physical vigor, which would require a considerable energetic cost to the predator. This tactic may be especially important against predators with limited capacity to manipulate snakes, due to small size or inexperience (Sazima & Martins 1990). In addition, erratic movements can reduce the precision of the predator attack as most attacks (e.g. pecking) are directed to the snake's head (see Buasso et al. 2006). Thus, erratic movements might be more efficient to protect the head than simply hide it under the body, which had few records. Also, by hiding the head, the ability of the snake to show other displays, such as false strikes or head triangulation, is limited.

Although diversified, the repertoire of displays of X. dorbignyi is not completely efficient, as indicated by the frequent presence of carcasses of X. dorbignyi near nests of burrowing-owls (Athene cunicularia) in the study area. However, Oliveira et al. (2004) suggest that smaller individuals of X. dorbignyi may be preferentially predated by A. cunicularia, supporting the importance of antipredator displays that create the illusion that the snake is larger than it is. In this case, showing these displays might be extremely important during the breeding season, a period of extended daily activity on the surface (e.g. males searching for females; Oliveira 2005, Wilgers & Horne 2007).

Apparently, a useful defense strategy of X. dorbignyi is to take advantage of its cryptic coloration or move far from the opponent. The more diverse kinds of displays were observed as secondary responses when a predator attack is imminent. This hypothesis is supported by the high number of animals that remained motionless or flattened the body during the first approach, compared to low frequency of these displays during hand approach. Both these behaviors do not increase visual cues to the predators. In other hand, during the first approach, a high number of animals have tried locomotor escape, avoiding proximity with predators. In general, snakes showed more displays during the hand approach or handling than when first approached. Thus, disruptive coloration and cryptic behaviors associated with semi-fossorial habits may limit the detection by predators.These results suggest that X. dorbignyi is capable of evaluating the level of threat imposed by the aggressor. Strikes, for example, are responses restricted to situations of extreme danger, as they were never observed during the first approaches. In addition, cloacal discharges were recorded only during handling, possibly as an extreme defense mechanism, when the snake tries to discourage its ingestion by the predator.

In some snakes, gravid females show a more aggressive behavior than males or non-gravid females (Shine 1993). This, however, was not observed for X. dorbignyi. Instead, the lower number of displays by gravid females revealed a lower tendency to exhibit defensive displays compared to males or non-gravid females. The presence of eggs may prevent the snake from showing some displays, such as body flattening and tail displays. However, it is possible that embryos of some females were not detected during handling, making the comparison between gravid and non-gravid females difficult. Males, on the other hand, had a higher tendency to flee, which might be associated to a lower mobility of females in advanced phases of vitellogenesis.

As metabolic rates of ectotherms are dependent on environmental temperature, so is their capacity to react to stimuli (Goode & Durvall 1989, Keogh & Deserto 1994). Thus, during colder days, cryptic behavior (immobility) might be more frequent than other displays (see Shine et al. 2000). In this study, no significant correlations were found between the number of displays recorded and body or air temperature. A positive correlation found between the number of displays shown by animals and substrate temperature would reflect highs metabolic rates at the warmer hours of the day.

The general aspect of individuals of X. dorbignyi of the study area is similar to that of Bothrops, as reported by Yanosky & Chani (1988). These authors suggest that X. dorbignyi is a mimic of Bothrops, not only due to its visual similarity, but also due to behavioral convergence (head triangulation and false strikes). These authors also suggest that X. dorbignyi is a mimic of Micrurus coral snakes, because of its caudal display, exhibiting red ventral scales, which, if effective, should reduce the risk of predation due to avoidance of snakes with coral-like pattern (Sazima & Abe 1991, Marques 2002, Buasso et al. 2006). Similarly, Xenodon nattereri, in southeastern Brazil, exhibits a color pattern similar to that of sympatric and syntopic Bothrops itapetiningae, and when handled, strikes (Sawaya et al. 2008). A similar behavior is observed in Xenodon neuwiedii, a supposed mimic of Bothrops jararaca (F. Barbo, personal communication).

If the mimicry hypothesis is valid, the defensive repertoire of X. dorbignyi should be associated to those of species that are also mimics of venomous snakes, regardless of phylogenetic factors (see Marques 2001). The cluster analysis supports this hypothesis, as X. dorbignyi was grouped with Liophis aesculapii (mimic species of the coral snake; Marques & Puorto 1991), closer than with other Xenodon species. Although a speculation, it is plausible that X. dorbignyi might be a mimic of Micrurus and Bothrops simultaneously. It should be pointed out that despite significant sampling and the systematic studies conducted in recent years, there are no records of Micrurus in the coastline of the southernmost region of Brazil (Oliveira 2005, Borges-Martins et al. 2007). However, historic factors might play an important role in the selection of elements of the defensive repertoire, and discussions based only on current predators have a limited scope (Greene 1988). It is possible that the complex repertoire of X. dorbignyi was shaped through diverse selective processes by the action of different types of predators or environments with different levels of exposure of snakes.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Fausto Barbo and Hebert Ferrarezi for information about Xenodontini, Cristiano Nogueira, Frederico França and Daniel Loebmann (Obelix) for critically reading earlier versions of the manuscript.

Recebido em 08/01/09

Versão reformulada recebida em 20/07/09

Publicado em 08/09/09

  • BÉRNILS, R.S. 2009. Brazilian reptiles: list of species. Sociedade Brasileira de Herpetologia, Belo Horizonte. http://www.sbherpetologia.org.br/ (último acesso em 30/07/2009).
  • BORGES-MARTINS, M., ALVES, M.L.M., ARAUJO, M.L., OLIVEIRA, R.B. & ANÉS, A.C. 2007. Répteis. In Biodiversidade: regiões da Lagoa do Casamento e dos Butiazais de Tapes, Planície Costeira do Rio Grande do Sul (F.G. Becker, R.A. Ramos & L.A. Moura, orgs.). Ministério do Meio Ambiente, Secretaria de Biodiversidade e Florestas, Brasília, p. 292-315. (v. 25).
  • BRODIE III, E.D. & RUSSEL, N.H. 1999. The consistency of individual differences in behaviour: temperature effects on antipredator behaviour in garter snakes. Anim. Behav. 57(2):445-451.
  • BUASSO, C.M., LEYNAUD, G.C. & CRUZ, F.B. 2006. Predation on snakes of Argentina: effects of coloration and ring pattern on coral and false coral snakes. Stud. Neotrop. Fauna E. 4(3):183-188.
  • CADLE, J.E. 1984. Molecular systematics of xenodontine colubrid snakes: I. South American xenodontines. Herpetologica. 40:8-20.
  • CARREIRA, S., MENEGHEL, M. & ACHAVAL, F. 2005. Reptiles de Uruguay. Facultad de Ciencias, Univ. de La República, Montevideo.
  • DI-BERNARDO, M., BORGES-MARTINS, M., OLIVEIRA, R.B. & PONTES, G.M.F. 2007. Taxocenoses de serpentes de regiões temperadas do Brasil. In Herpetologia no Brasil II (L.B. Nascimento & M.E. Oliveira, eds.). Sociedade Brasileira de Herpetologia, Belo Horizonte, p. 222-263.
  • DOWNES, S. 2001.Trading heat and food for safety: costs of predator avoidance in a lizard. Ecology. 82(10):2870-2881.
  • DUVALL, D., KING, M.B. & GUTZWEILER, K.J. 1985. Behavioral ecology and ethology of the prairie rattlesnake. Natl. Geogr. Res. 1:80-111.
  • FITCH, H.S. 1987. Collecting and life history techniques. In Snakes: ecology and evolutionary biology (R.A. Seigel, J.T. Collins & S.S. Novak, eds.). McMillan Publishing Company, New York, p. 143-164.
  • FOWLER, J., COHEN, L. & JARVIS, P. 1998. Practical Statistics for Field Biology. John Wiley and Sons, Chichester.
  • GIBBONS, J.W & DORCAS, M.E. 2002. Defensive Behavior of Cottonmouths (Agkistrodon piscivorus) toward Humans. Copeia. (1):195-198.
  • GOODE, M.J. & DURVALL, D. 1989. Body Temperature and Defensive Behaviour of Free-ranging Prairie Rattlesnakes, Crotallus viridis viridis Ani. Behav. 32(2):360-362.
  • GREENE, H.W. 1988. Antipredator mechanisms in reptiles. In Biology of the reptilian (C. Gans & R.B. Huey, eds.). Alan R. Liss, New York, p. 1-152. (v. 16).
  • HASENACK, H. & FERRARO, L.W. 1989. Considerações sobre o clima da região de Tramandaí, RS. Pesquisas. 22: 53-70.
  • HERZOG, H.A & BAILEY, B.B. 1987. Development of antipredator responses in snakes. II. Effects of recent feeding on defensive behaviors of juvenile garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis). J. Comp. Physiol. 101(4):387-389.
  • KEOGH, J.S. & DESERTO, F.P. 1994. Temperature dependent defensive behavior in three species of north american colubrid snakes. J. Herpetol. 28(2):258-261.
  • LEMA, T. 1994. Lista comentada dos répteis ocorrentes no Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil. Comunicações do Museu de Ciências da PUCRS, Rio Grande do Sul, p. 41-150. (Série Zoologia, v. 7).
  • LIMA, S.L. 1998. Nonlethal effects in the ecology of predator-prey interactions. Bioscience. 48(1):25-34.
  • LIMA, S.L. & DILL, L.M. 1990. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can. J. Zoolog. 68(4):619-640.
  • MARQUES, O.A.V. 2001. Tail display of the false coral snake Simophis rhinostoma (Colubridae). Amph. Rept. 20(1):127-129.
  • MARQUES, O.A.V. 2002. Natural history of the coral snake Micrurus decoratus (Elapidae) from the Atlantic Forest in southeast Brazil, with comments on possible mimicry. Amph. Rept. 23(2):228-232.
  • MARQUES, O.A.V., ETEROVIC, A. & SAZIMA, I. 2004. Snakes of the Brazilian Atlantic Forest: an illustrated field guide for the Serra do Mar range. Holos, Ribeirão Preto.
  • MARQUES, O.A.V., ETEROVIC, A., STRUSSMANN, C. & SAZIMA, I. 2005. Serpentes do Pantanal: guia ilustrado. Holos, Ribeirão Preto.
  • MARQUES, O.A.V. & PUORTO, G. 1991. Padrões cromáticos, distribuição e mimetismo de Erythrolamprus aesculapii (Serpentes, Colubridae). Memo. Inst. Butantan. 53(1):127-134.
  • OLIVEIRA, R.B. 2005. História Natural da Comunidade de Serpentes de uma Região de Dunas do Litoral Norte do Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil. Tese de Doutorado, Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul, Rio Grande do Sul.
  • OLIVEIRA, R.B., DI-BERNARDO, M., PONTES, G.M.F., MACIEL, A.P. & KRAUSE, L. 2001. Dieta e comportamento alimentar da cobra nariguda, Lystrophis dorbignyi (Duméril, Bibron & Duméril, 1854), no Litoral Norte do Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil. Cuadernos de Herpetologia. 14(2): 117-122.
  • OLIVEIRA, R.B., PONTES, G.F., SOLÉ, M., DI-BERNARDO, M. & BORGES-MARTINS, M. 2004. Lystrophis dorbignyi (nariguda) and Helicops infrataeniatus (cobra-d'agua). Herpetol. Rev. 35(1):70.
  • POUGH, F.H., ANDREWS, R.H., CADLE, J.E, CRUMP, M.L., SAVITZKY, A.H. & WELLS, K.D. 2004. Herpetology. 3 ed. Pearson Pretince Hall, Upper Saddle River.
  • SAZIMA, I. & ABE, A.S. 1991. Habits of five Brazilian snakes with coral-snake pattern, including a summary of defensive tactics. Stud. Neotrop. Fauna E. 26(3):159-164.
  • SAZIMA, I. & MARTINS, M. 1990. Presas grandes e serpentes jovens: quando os olhos são maiores que a boca. Memo. Inst. Butantan. 52(3):73-79.
  • SAWAYA, R.J., MARQUES, O.A.V. & MARTINS, M. 2008. Composição e história natural das serpentes de Cerrado de Itirapina, São Paulo, sudeste do Brasil. Biota Neotrop. 8(2): http://www.biotaneotropica.org.br/v8n2/pt/abstract?article+bn01308022008 ISSN 1676-0603 (último acesso em 22/09/2009).
  • SHEPARD, D.B. 2007. Habitat but not body shape affects predator attack frequency on lizard models in the brazilian cerrado. Herpetologica. 63(2):193-202.
  • SHINE, R. 1993. Sexual dimorphism in snakes. In Snakes: ecology and behavior (R.A. Seigel, J.T. Collins & S.S Novak, eds.). McGraw-Hill, New York, p. 49-86.
  • SHINE, R., OLSSON, M.M., LEMASTER, M.P., MOORE, I.T. & MASON, R.T. 2000. Effects of sex, body size, temperature and location on the antipredator tactics of free-ranging gartersnakes (Thamnophis sirtalis, Colubridae). Behav. Ecol. 11(3):239-245.
  • SINCLAIR, A.R.E. & ARCESE, P. 1995. Population consequences of predation-sensitive foraging: the Serengeti Wildebeest. Ecology.76(3): 882-891.
  • SNEATH, P.H.A. & SOKAL, R.R. 1973. Numerical taxonomy. Freeman & Co., San Francisco.
  • TOLEDO, L.F., TOZETTI, A.M. & ZINA, J. 2005. Leptodactylus labyrinthicus (Pepper Frog): defensive repertoire. Herpetol. Bull. 90:29-31.
  • VERMEIJ, G.J. 1982. Unsuccessful predation and evolution. Am. Nat. 120(6):701-720.
  • VIDAL, N., SHANNON, G.K., WONG, A. & HEDGES, S.B. 2000. Phylogenetic relationships of Xenodontine snakes inferred from 12S and 16S ribosomal RNA sequences. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 14(3):389-402.
  • VIEIRA, E.F. & RANGEL, S.R.S. 1998. Planície Costeira do Rio Grande do Sul: geografia física, vegetação e dinâmica sócio-demográfica. Editora Sagra, Porto Alegre.
  • YANOSKY, A.A. & CHANI, J.M. 1988. Possible Dual Mimicry of Bothrops and Micrurus by the Colubrid, Lystrophis dorbignyi J. Herpetol. 22(2):222-224.
  • WILGERS, D.J. & HORNE, E.A. 2007. Spatial variation in predation attempts on artificial snakes in a fire-disturbed tallgrass prairie. Southwest. Nat. 52(2):263-270.
  • WAECHTER, J.L. 1985. Aspectos ecológicos da vegetação de restinga no Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil. Comunicações do Museu de Ciências da PUCRS, Rio Grande do Sul, p. 49-68. (Série Zoologia, v. 33).
  • ZAHER, H., GRAZZIOTIN, F.G., CADLE, J.E., MURPHY, R.W., MOURA-LEITE, J.C. & BONATTO, S.L. 2009. Molecular phylogeny of advanced snakes (Serpentes, Caenophidia) with an emphasis on South American Xenodontines: a revised classification and descriptions of new taxa. Pap. Avulsos Zool. 49(11):115-153.
  • ZAR, J.H. 1999. Biostatistical analysis. 2 ed. Prentice-Hall, Upper Saddle River.
  • *
    Corresponding Author: Alexandro Marques Tozetti, e-mail:
  • Publication Dates

    • Publication in this collection
      26 Jan 2010
    • Date of issue
      Sept 2009

    History

    • Accepted
      08 Sept 2009
    • Reviewed
      20 July 2009
    • Received
      08 Jan 2009
    Instituto Virtual da Biodiversidade | BIOTA - FAPESP Departamento de Biologia Vegetal - Instituto de Biologia, UNICAMP CP 6109, 13083-970 - Campinas/SP, Tel.: (+55 19) 3521-6166, Fax: (+55 19) 3521-6168 - Campinas - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: contato@biotaneotropica.org.br