Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

"Feedback" após boas versus más tentativas melhora a aprendizagem motora em crianças

Feedback after good versus poor trials enhances motor learning in children

Resumos

O presente estudo investigou se a aprendizagem motora de crianças pode ser beneficiada pelo "feedback" (conhecimento de resultados -CR) fornecido após tentativas relativamente boas de prática, ao invés de após tentativas ruins. A tarefa requeriu que os participantes arremessassem saquinhos de feijão em um alvo circular fixo, posicionado no chão, a uma distância de 3 m. Vinte e oito crianças do ensino fundamental (idade média: 10,6 anos) participaram deste experimento. A fase de prática consistiu de 10 blocos de seis tentativas. Após cada bloco de tentativas, um grupo (KR "good") recebeu CR relacionado aos três arremessos mais precisos, enquanto ao outro grupo (KR "poor") foi fornecido CR relacionado aos três arremessos menos precisos. Os participantes não foram informados sobre as tentativas nas quais o "feedback" seria fornecido. Imediatamente após a fase de prática, os participantes preencheram o questionário de motivação intrínseca. Um dia após a fase de prática, foi conduzido um teste de retenção composto por 10 tentativas, sem CR. Os resultados demonstraram que a aprendizagem foi melhorada através do fornecimento de CR após as boas tentativas de prática. Ainda, os resultados do questionário revelaram que a motivação intrínseca dos aprendizes foi aumentada pelo "feedback" positivo. Os presentes achados adicionam evidências de que os efeitos motivacionais do "feedback" possuem um impacto direto sobre a aprendizagem.

Motivação; Conhecimento de resultados; Arremesso; IMI


The present study investigated whether children would benefit from feedback (knowledge of results, KR) provided after relatively good as opposed to poor trials. The task required participants to throw beanbags at a circular target that was placed on the floor at a distance of 3 m. Twenty-eight elementary school children (mean age: 10.6 years) participated in this experiment. The practice phase consisted of 10 blocks of 6 trials. After each 6-trial block, one group (KR good) received KR on the 3 most accurate tosses, whereas another group (KR poor) was given KR on the 3 least accurate ones. Participants were not informed about the trials on which they were provided feedback. Immediately following the practice phase, participants completed the intrinsic motivation inventory. One day after the practice phase, a retention test consisting of 10 trials without KR was conducted. The results demonstrated that learning was enhanced by providing KR after good trials during practice. Furthermore, the questionnaire results revealed that learners' intrinsic motivation was increased by positive feedback. The present findings add to the accumulating evidence that the motivational effects of feedback have a direct impact on learning.

Motivation; Knowledge of results; Throwing; IMI


ARTIGOS ORIGINAIS - PEDAGOGIA E COMPORTAMENTO

Feedback after good versus poor trials enhances motor learning in children

"Feedback" após boas versus más tentativas melhora a aprendizagem motora em crianças

Esmaeel SaemiI; Gabriele WulfII; Ahmad Ghotbi VarzanehI; Mehdi ZarghamiI

IShahid Chamran University of Ahvaz - Iran

IIUniversity of Nevada, Las Vegas - USA

Endereço Endereço para correspondência: Gabriele Wulf Department of Kinesiology and Nutrition Sciences University of Nevada 4505 Maryland Parkway Las Vegas - NV - 89154-3034 - USA e-mail: gabriele.wulf@univ.edu

ABSTRACT

The present study investigated whether children would benefit from feedback (knowledge of results, KR) provided after relatively good as opposed to poor trials. The task required participants to throw beanbags at a circular target that was placed on the floor at a distance of 3 m. Twenty-eight elementary school children (mean age: 10.6 years) participated in this experiment. The practice phase consisted of 10 blocks of 6 trials. After each 6-trial block, one group (KR good) received KR on the 3 most accurate tosses, whereas another group (KR poor) was given KR on the 3 least accurate ones. Participants were not informed about the trials on which they were provided feedback. Immediately following the practice phase, participants completed the intrinsic motivation inventory. One day after the practice phase, a retention test consisting of 10 trials without KR was conducted. The results demonstrated that learning was enhanced by providing KR after good trials during practice. Furthermore, the questionnaire results revealed that learners' intrinsic motivation was increased by positive feedback. The present findings add to the accumulating evidence that the motivational effects of feedback have a direct impact on learning.

Uniterms: Motivation; Knowledge of results; Throwing; IMI.

RESUMO

O presente estudo investigou se a aprendizagem motora de crianças pode ser beneficiada pelo "feedback" (conhecimento de resultados -CR) fornecido após tentativas relativamente boas de prática, ao invés de após tentativas ruins. A tarefa requeriu que os participantes arremessassem saquinhos de feijão em um alvo circular fixo, posicionado no chão, a uma distância de 3 m. Vinte e oito crianças do ensino fundamental (idade média: 10,6 anos) participaram deste experimento. A fase de prática consistiu de 10 blocos de seis tentativas. Após cada bloco de tentativas, um grupo (KR "good") recebeu CR relacionado aos três arremessos mais precisos, enquanto ao outro grupo (KR "poor") foi fornecido CR relacionado aos três arremessos menos precisos. Os participantes não foram informados sobre as tentativas nas quais o "feedback" seria fornecido. Imediatamente após a fase de prática, os participantes preencheram o questionário de motivação intrínseca. Um dia após a fase de prática, foi conduzido um teste de retenção composto por 10 tentativas, sem CR. Os resultados demonstraram que a aprendizagem foi melhorada através do fornecimento de CR após as boas tentativas de prática. Ainda, os resultados do questionário revelaram que a motivação intrínseca dos aprendizes foi aumentada pelo "feedback" positivo. Os presentes achados adicionam evidências de que os efeitos motivacionais do "feedback" possuem um impacto direto sobre a aprendizagem.

Unitermos: Motivação; Conhecimento de resultados; Arremesso; IMI.

Introduction

Augmented feedback (knowledge of result, knowledge of performance) has long been considered to be one of the most important variables for motor skill learning (e.g., Schmidt & Lee, 2005). Yet, the views of how exactly feedback influences learning have changed over time. According to early accounts of feedback, learning was not believed to occur in its absence (e.g., Bilodeau & Bilodeau, 1958; Bilodeau, Bilodeau & Schumsky, 1959). In the 1980s, this view changed with the review and re-appraisal of the feedback literature by Salmoni, Schmidt, and Walter (1984). According to the guidance hypothesis proposed by Salmoni, Schmidt, and Walter (1984), frequent feedback guides the performer toward the goal movement. However, if feedback is provided too frequently, learners tend to become dependent to the augmented information, and their performance tends to be relatively inconsistent due to constant corrections of even small errors. The result is in degraded learning compared to when feedback is provided less frequently feedback. While numerous studies have provided support for the guidance hypothesis (for reviews, see Schmidt, 1991; Swinnen, 1996), there are also findings that are inconsistent with the guidance idea (e.g., Swinnen, Lee, Verschueren, Serrien, & Bogaerds, 1997; Wulf, McConnel, Gärtner, & Schwarz, 2002; Wulf, Shea, & Matschiner, 1998; for a review, see Wulf & Shea, 2004).

In recent years, another effective feedback manipulation has emerged that cannot be accounted for by the guidance explanation. Studies investigating the effects of self-controlled KR or KP have demonstrated that giving learners the opportunity to decide when to receive feedback enhanced learning compared to not having this opportunity (e.g., Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2002, 2005; Chiviacowsky, Wulf, Laroque de Medeiros, Kaefer, & Tani, 2008; Janelle, Barba, Frehlich, Tennant, & Cauraugh, 1997; Janelle, Kim, & Singer, 1995; Wulf, Clauss, Shea, & Whitacre, 2001). An interesting finding that emerged from studies in which questionnaires where used to determine when or why learners with self-control asked for KR (Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2002; Chiviacowsky, Wulf, Wally, & Borges, 2009) was that learners preferred to receive KR after they thought they had a relatively successful trial, but not when they thought their performance was relatively poor.

In a follow-up study, Chiviacowsky and Wulf (2007) provided learners with KR after "good" or "poor" trials. Young adults (average age: 21.1 years) practiced a throwing task with their non-dominant arm, with participants being assigned to either a group that received KR on the three best (i.e., most accurate) tosses after each 6-trial block, or a group that received KR after the three poorest tosses. The results showed that participants who received KR after good trials demonstrated more effective learning than those who were provided KR after relatively poor trials. Chiviacowsky et al. (2009) replicated these findings with older adults (average age 65.9 years). Various possible reasons have been suggested for the learning benefits of "positive" KR. Chiviacowsky and Wulf (2007) hypothesized that KR after good trials may be more motivating for learners than negative feedback, which, in turn, could lead to more effective learning. Badami, VaezMousavi, Wulf, and Namazizadeh (2011) directly examined the effects of KR after good versus poor trials on intrinsic motivation. Their results confirmed the notion that KR after good trials enhanced learners' intrinsic motivation. In addition, learners who received KR after good trials demonstrated greater self-confidence and more effective arousal management in another study (Badami, VaezMousavi, Namazizadeh, & Wulf, in press). These findings appear to be inconsistent with the guidance hypothesis, according to which feedback should be more effective after poor trials or large errors. Moreover, these recent results high-light the need to consider social-cognitive-affective influences on motor learning, rather than simply the informational role of practice variables (Lewthwaite & Wulf, 2010a). Traditional views of feedback with their emphasis on the informational properties of feedback are not able to adequately explain newer findings that demonstrate motivational influences of feedback on motor learning.

An interesting question - from both theoretical and practical perspectives - is whether the findings from studies with adults showing more effective learning with KR after good rather than poor trials (Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 200; Chiviacowsky et al., 2009) would generalize to learning in children. Children have been shown to have limited information-processing capabilities compared to those of adults (e.g., Badan, Hauert, & Mounoud, 2000; Chi, 1977; Connolly, 1970, 1977; Lambert & Bard, 2005; Pollock & Lee, 1997; Sullivan, Kantak, & Burtner, 2008). For example, children are less effective in attending to and interpreting intrinsic feedback, and have greater difficulty with the detection and estimation of movement errors (for a review, see Sullivan, Kantak, & Burtner, 2008). Thus, one may assume that children might need more error information (i.e., KR on poor trials). Yet, if the benefits of KR on good trials are mainly motivational in nature - that is, due to enhanced intrinsic motivation, self-confidence, and arousal management (Badami et al., 2011, in press) - one would expect to see the same learning advantages in children that have been found for adults.

Therefore, one purpose of the present study was to examine whether feedback after good trials would benefit motor learning in children, relative to feedback after poor trials. Chiviacowsky et al. (2008), who examined the effects of self-controlled feedback in 10-year-old children, found that participants asked for feedback more frequently after relatively good rather than poor trials. However, the learning effectiveness of KR after good versus poor trials has not been directly assessed yet. Also, the influence of these two types of KR on children's' intrinsic motivation has not yet been examined. Thus, a second purpose of the present study was to address this issue. Ten-year old children practiced a beanbag-throwing task, similar to the tasks used by Chiviacowsky and colleagues (Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2007; Chiviacowsky et al., 2009), while being provided KR on either the 3 most accurate or inaccurate throws, respectively, after each block of 6 trials. Learning was assessed by a delayed retention test without KR. We examined the effects of feedback on children's intrinsic motivation by using the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (IMI) (see BADAMI et al., 2011).

Methods

Participants

Twenty-eight children (mean age = 10.61 years, SD = 0.88) participated in this experiment. They where recruited from four classes in a local elementary school. Informed consent was obtained from the elementary school and the parents, and assent was obtained from the students. Participants had no prior experience with the experimental task and were not aware of our specifi c study purpose.

Apparatus and task

The apparatus, task, and procedure were similar to those used in previous studies (Badami et al., 2011; Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2007; Chiviacowsky et al., 2008, 2009). The task required participants to toss beanbags to a target placed on the floor, using their non-dominant arm. The target was circular, had a radius of 10 cm, and was placed at a distance of 3 m from the participant. Concentric circles with radii of 20, 30, 40, 50, 60, 70, 80, 90, and 100 cm were drawn around the target. These served as zones to assess the accuracy of the throws. If the beanbag landed on the target, 100 points were awarded. If it landed in one of the other zones, or outside the circles, 90, 80, 70, 60, 50, 40, 30, 20, 10, or 0 points, respectively, were recorded. If the ball landed on a line separating two zones, the participant was awarded the higher score. Also, the target was divided into four quadrants for the provision of KR (see the FIGURE 1). All testing took place during normal physical education class periods.


To measure intrinsic motivation, the children responded to questions on the IMI, which was translated into the Farsi (see Badami et al., 2011). The IMI assesses participants' subjective experience related to a target activity. It consists of 6 subscales, including items related to interest/enjoyment, perceived competence, effort/importance, value/usefulness, pressure and tension, and perceived choice while performing a given activity (e.g., McAuley, Duncan, & Tammen, 1989; Plant & Ryan, 1985; Ryan, Mims, & Koestner, 1983). For the present study, the nine items of the IMI related to the interest/enjoyment, perceived competence, and effort/importance subscales were adapted to measure the participants' intrinsic motivation (as in Badami et al., 2011) (see TABLE 1). Response choices ranged from 7 (strongly agree) to 1 (strongly disagree). The IMI for assessing intrinsic motivation in children has also been used in previous studies (e.g. Goudas, Dermitzaki, & Bagiatis, 2000; Baric, Erpic, & Babic, 2002; Koka & Hein, 2003). Internal consistency of each subscale was calculated using Cronbach's α statistic. They were high: interest/enjoyment (0.88), perceived competence (0.81), and effort/importance (0.8).

Procedure

Participants were randomly assigned to the "KR good" group and "KR poor" group (14 participants per group). All participants were informed about the task goal, and they were instructed to toss the beanbags overhand, while standing with both feet on the ground. After each block of 6 trials, participants in the KR good group received KR on the 3 best (i.e., most accurate) tosses in that block, whereas those in the KR poor group received KR on the three poorest tosses. Participants in both groups were informed that, at the end of each block of 6 trials, they would receive KR on the 3 of those trials. However, they did not know for which trials they would receive KR. KR was provided in terms of the direction and the extent of the deviation from the target (Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2007; Chiviacowsky et al., 2009). Specifically, it consisted of the trial number and the respective score, as well as directional information. Participants were allowed to look at the target before each 6-trial block. During the experimental phase (practice, retention) participants were required to wear opaque swimming goggles to prevent them from viewing the outcome. To control the timing of the trials and KR presentation, a digital timer was used. Participants had 6 s to complete each trial. KR was written on a board and presented for 15 s. Thus, the inter-trial interval was about 21 s after every 6th trial. All participants performed 60 practice trials. At the end of practice phase, participants in both groups completed the IMI. A retention test consisting of 10 trials without KR was conducted 24 hours after the practice phase.

Data analysis

Accuracy scores for the practice phase were averaged across blocks of 6 trials and analyzed in a 2 (group: KR good versus KR poor) x 10 (6-trial blocks) analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures on the last factor. The retention test scores were averaged across all 10 trials and analyzed in a one-way ANOVA. Independent t-tests were used to investigate the effects of KR after good trials versus poor trials on participants' overall intrinsic motivation as well as the 3 subscales of the IMI (interest/enjoyment, perceived competence, and effort/importance).

Results

Throwing accuracy

Practice

Accuracy scores during practice fluctuated somewhat, with the KR good group tending to show a somewhat greater increase across blocks compared to the KR poor group (see the FIGURE 2, left). The main effect of block, F (9, 234) = 1.96, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.07, was significant. Post-hoc tests (LSD) on the Block effect indicated that accuracy scores were significantly higher on Block 8 relative to Blocks 3, 5, 6, 7, and 10. In addition, Blocks 4 and 9 had higher scores than Block 7; and scores on Block 9 were higher than those on Block 5 (ps < 0.05). Furthermore, the Group main effect was significant, with F (1, 26) = 8.88, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.26, indicating that the KR good group generally outperformed the KR poor group across practice. The interaction of group and block was not signifi cant F (9, 234) = 0.99, p > 0.05.


Retention

On the no-KR retention test, performed one day after practice phase, the KR good group again demonstrated higher accuracy scores than KR poor group. The main effect of group was significant, F (1, 30) = 13.39, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.309 (see FIGURE 2, right).

Motivation

The average intrinsic motivation scores can be seen in TABLE 2. Receiving KR on good trials resulted in significantly higher perceived competence scores than KR on poor trials, t (26) = 5.18, p < 0.001. In contrast, the KR poor group had higher scores on effort/importance than the KR good group, t (26) = -2.68, p < 0.05. There were no significant differences between groups in terms of interest/enjoyment, t (26) = 1.45, p > 0.05. Overall, intrinsic motivation was significantly higher in the KR good group than in the KR poor group, t (26) = 3.58, p < 0.001.

Discussion

Recent studies have demonstrated the learning effectiveness of providing KR after good as opposed to poor trials for learning in adults (Badami et al., 2011; Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2007; Chiviacowsky et al., 2009). In the present study, we asked whether children would show similar learning benefits when receiving KR after good trials. In addition, we attempted to replicate previous findings (Badami et al., 2011) demonstrating enhanced intrinsic motivation as a function of "positive" KR in adults.

The present findings demonstrated that children's motivation was indeed enhanced when they were given KR on trials with relatively small rather than larger errors. Thus, even though participants were not told on which trials they would receive KR, the type of KR impacted their intrinsic motivation. In particular, KR on good trials enhanced learner's perceived competence. The feeling of competence is an integral aspect of motivation, and motivation generally increases with the feeling of competence. In fact, competence is considered to be a fundamental psychological need (along with autonomy and social relatedness) (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 2000, 2008). Optimal functioning and learning in a broad range of domains appears to depend on the satisfaction of those basic needs.

Interestingly, participants who received KR on poor trials seemed to increase their effort as a function of the feedback. Their ratings of their effort exerted and importance of doing well on the task were significantly higher than those of participants in the KR good group. Yet, despite their stated effort, they were outperformed by learners who received more positive feedback. That is, the increased intrinsic motivation resulting from increased in perceived competence produced more effective learning in the KR good condition.

These findings are in line with those of previous studies showing increased intrinsic motivation (Badami et al., 2011) and enhanced motor learning (Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2007; Chiviacowsky et al., 2009) in adults. In fact, there is accumulating evidence that the motivational role of feedback in the learning process is more important than previously thought. In the motor learning literature, feedback is assumed to have two roles in facilitating the learning process. Most importantly, feedback is assumed to have an informational role, providing the learner with knowledge about refinements of the movement plan and its execution (e.g., Schmidt & Lee, 2005). A secondary role of feedback is its motivational role - thought to enhance task interest and encourage continued effort, persistence, and attention to goal accomplishment (e.g., Schmidt & Lee, 2005; Schmidt & Wrisberg, 2008). Yet, the direct impact of the motivational properties of feedback on learning has only become evident more recently.

For example, self-controlled feedback satisfies individuals' need to act autonomously (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 2000) - which is presumably one reason for its beneficial effects on learning (e.g., Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2002, 2005; Janelle et al., 1997; Patterson & Carter, 2010). Moreover, it allows learners to request feedback after good trials (Chiviacowsky & Wulf, 2002; Chiviacowsky et al., 2008), thus enhancing participants' feeling of competence. To be intrinsically motivated, individuals need to perceive themselves as both autonomous and competent. Learners' perceived competence - and subsequent performance or learning - can also be increased by social-comparative feedback indicating that they are performing better than the "norm" or their peers (Hutchinson, Sherman, Martinovic, & Tenenbaum, 2008; Lewthwaite & Wulf, 2010b; Wulf, Chiviacowsky, & Lewthwaite, 2010). For instance, in a recent study (Lewthwaite & Wulf, 2010b) that examined effect of positive versus negative social-comparative information on the learning of balance task, learners were led to believe their performance was either above or below average. Participants who believed they were more skilled (or competent) than others demonstrated more effective learning of the task than participants who assumed their performance was below average, or control participants who were not given normative feedback. Interestingly, the two latter groups showed similar learning. This suggests that it is the positive information and its affective consequences that promote learning. Learners who believe their performance is worse than that of others, or who are unsure about how their performance compares to that of others (control conditions) may also adopt a more self-related focus of attention (Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2010). Concerns about performance have been known to increase conscious effort to control actions in attempts to improve performance (e.g., Baumeister, 1984). The greater effort expressed by KR poor group participants in the present study is in line with the notion. Yet, an increase in conscious control attempts is usually detrimental to performance and learning (e.g., Baumeister, 1984; Wulf, 2007; Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2010).

Overall, the findings of the present study add to the converging evidence that the motivational consequences of feedback directly impact the learning of motor skills. To our knowledge, they are the first to demonstrate the beneficial effects of KR after good trials on learning in children. Moreover, the present results show that these effects are mediated by an increase in intrinsic motivation, and in particular learners' perceived competence. Clearly, the motivational role of feedback - and of practice conditions, in general - needs to be given more attention in future studies on motor learning. The present findings also have implications for practical and instructional settings, in which instructors tend to give feedback when they assume the child needs it most to avoid errors, and to guide the child to to the correct movement pattern. It now appears that another, and perhaps more fruitful approach may be to reserve feedback for good performances, to highlight aspects of the skill that are performed correctly, or to focus on the child's improvement.

Recebido para publicação: 07/03/2011

Revisado: 05/08/2011

Aceito: 29/08/2011

  • BADAMI, R.; VAEZMOUSAVI, M.; NAMAZIZADEH, M.; WULF, G. Feedback after good versus poor trials: Differential effects on self-confidence and activation. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Washington, in press.
  • BADAMI, R.;VAEZMOUSAVI, M.; WULF, G.; NAMAZIZADEH, M. Feedback after good trials enhances intrinsic motivation. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Washington, v.82, p.360-4, 2011.
  • BADAN, M.; HAUERT, C.A.; MOUNOUD, P. Sequential pointing in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, New York, v.75, p.43-69, 2000.
  • BARIC, R.; ERPIC, C.S.; BABIC, V. Intrinsic motivation and goal orientation in track-and-field children. Kinesiology, Zagreb, v.34, p.50-60, 2002.
  • BAUMEISTER, R.F. Choking under pressure: self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Washington, v.46, p.610-20, 1984.
  • BILODEAU, E.A.; BILODEAU, I.M. Variable frequency of knowledge of results and the learning of a simple skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Washington, v.55, p.379-83, 1958.
  • BILODEAU, E.A.; BILODEAU, I.M.; SCHUMSKY, D.A. Some effects of introducing and withdrawing knowledge of results early and late in practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Washington, v.58, p.142-4, 1959.
  • CHI, M.T.H. Age differences in memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, New York, v.23, p.266-81, 1977.
  • CHIVIACOWSKY S.; WULF, G. Feedback after good trials enhances learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Washington, v.78, p.40-7, 2007.
  • _______. Self-controlled feedback: does it enhance learning because performers get feedback when they need it? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Washington, v.73, p.408-15, 2002.
  • _______. Self-controlled feedback is effective if it is based on the learner's performance. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Washington, v.76, p.42-8, 2005.
  • CHIVIACOWSKY, S.; WULF, G.; LAROQUE DE MEDEIROS, F.; KAEFER, A.; TANI, G. (2008). Learning benefits of self-controlled knowledge of results in 10-year-old children. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Washington, v.79, p.405-10, 2008.
  • CHIVIACOWSKY, S.; WULF, G.; WALLY, R.; BORGES, T. KR after good trials enhances learning in old adults. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Washington, v.80, p.663-8, 2009.
  • CONNOLLY, K. Mechanisms of motor skill development. London: Academic Press, 1970.
  • _______. The nature of motor skill development. Journal of Human Movement Studies, London, v.3, p.128-43, 1977.
  • DECI, E.L.; RYAN, R.M.. Self-determination theory: a macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Canadian Psychology, Winnipeg, v.49, p.182-5, 2008.
  • _______. The "what" and "why" of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, Mahwah, v.11, p.227-68, 2000.
  • GOUDAS, M.; DERMITZAKI, I.; BAGIATIS, K. Predictor of students' intrinsic motivation in school physical education, Europen Journal of Psychology of Education, New York, v.15, p.271-80, 2000.
  • HUTCHINSON, J.C.; SHERMAN, T.; MARTINOVIC, N.; TENENBAUM, G. The effect of manipulated self-efficacy on perceived and sustained effort. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, Philadelphia, v.20, p.457-72, 2008.
  • JANELLE, C.M.; BARBA, D.A.; FREHLICH, S.G.; TENNANT, L.K.; CAURAUGH, J.H. (1997). Maximizing performance effectiveness through videotape reply and a self-controlled learning environment. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Washington, v.68, p.269-79, 1997.
  • JANELLE, C.M.; KIM, J.; SINGER, R.N. Subject-controlled performance feedback and learning of a closed motor skill. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Missoula, v.81, p.627-34, 1995.
  • KOKA, A.; HEIN, V. Perceptions of teachers' feedback and learning environment as predictors of intrinsic motivation in physical education. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, Amsterdam, v.4, p.333-46, 2003.
  • LAMBERT, J.; BARD, C. Acquisition of visuomanual skills and improvement of information processing capacities in 6- to 10-year old children performing a 2D pointing task. Neuroscience Letters, Limerick, v.377, p.1-6, 2005.
  • LEWTHWAITE, R.; WULF, G. Grand challenge for movement science and sport psychology: embracing the social-cognitive-affective-motor nature of motor behavior. Frontiers in Movement Science and Sport Psychology, Lausanne, v . 1, p..42, 2010a. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00042.
  • _______. Social-comparative feedback affects motor skill learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, London, v.63, p.738-49, 2010b.
  • McAULEY, E.; DUNCAN, T.; TAMMEN, V.V. (1989). Psychometric properties of the Intrinsic Motivation Inventory in a competitive sport setting: A confirmatory factor analysis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Washington, v.60, p.48-58, 1989.
  • PATTERSON, J.; CARTER, M. Learner-regulated knowledge of results during the acquisition of multiple timing goals. Human Movement Science, Amsterdam, v.29, p.214-27, 2010.
  • PLANT, R.W.; RYAN, R.M. Intrinsic motivation and the effects of self-consciousness, self-awareness, and ego-involvement: an investigation of internally-controlling styles. Journal of Personality, Malden, v.53, p.435-49, 1985.
  • POLLOCK, B.J.; LEE, T.D. Dissociated contextual interference effects in children and adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Missoula, v.84, p.851-8, 1997.
  • RYAN, R.M.; MIMS, V.; KOESTNER, R. Relation of reward contingency and interpersonal context to intrinsic motivation: a review and test using cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Washington, v.45, p.736-50, 1983.
  • SALMONI, A.W.; SCHMIDT, R.A.; WALTER, C.B. Knowledge of results and motor learning: a review and critical reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, Washington, v.95, p.355-86, 1984.
  • SCHMIDT, R.A. Frequent augmented feedback can degrade learning: evidence and interpretations. In: REQUIN, J.; STELMACH, G.E. (Eds.). Tutorials in motor neuroscience Netherlands: Kluwer Academic, 1991. p.59-75.
  • SCHMIDT, R.A.; LEE, T.D. Motor control and learning: a behavioral emphasis. 4th ed. Champaign: Human Kinetics, 2005.
  • SCHMIDT, R.A.; WRISBERG, C.A. Motor learning and performance 4th ed. Champaign: Human Kinetics, 2008.
  • SULLIVAN, K.J.; KANTAK, S.S.; BURTNER, P.A. Motor learning in children: feedback effects on skill acquisition. Physical Therapy, Alexandria, v.88, p.720-32, 2008.
  • SWINNEN, S.P. Information feedback for motor skill learning: a review. In: ZELAZNIK, H.N. (Ed.). Advances in motor learning and control. Champaign: Human Kinetics, 1996. p.37-66
  • SWINNEN, S.P.; LEE, T.D.; VERSCHUEREN, S.; SERRIEN, D.J.; BOGAERDS, H. (1997). Interlimb coordination: Learning and transfer under different feedback conditions. Human Movement Science, Amsterdam, v.16, p.749-85, 1997.
  • WULF, G. Attention and motor skill learning Champaign: Human Kinetics, 2007.
  • WULF, G.; CHIVIACOWSKY, S.; LEWTHWAITE, R. Normative feedback effects on learning a timing task. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Washington, v.81, p.425-31, 2010.
  • WULF, G.; CLAUSS, A.; SHEA, C.H.; WHITACRE, C. Benefits of self-control in dyad practice. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, Washington, v.72, p.299-303, 2001.
  • WULF, G.; LEWTHWAITE, R. Effortless motor learning? An external focus of attention enhances movement effectiveness and efficiency. In: BRUYA, B. (Ed.). Effortless attention: a new perspective in attention and action. Cambridge: MIT, 2010. p.75-101.
  • WULF, G.; McCONNEL, N.; GARTNER, M.; SCHWARZ, A. Feedback and attentional focus: enhancing the learning of sport skills through external-focus feedback. Journal of Motor Behavior, Washington, v.34, p.171-82, 2002.
  • WULF, G.; SHEA, C.H. Understanding the role of augmented feedback: the good, the bad, and the ugly. In: WILLIAMS, A.M.; HODGES, N.J. (Eds.). Skill acquisition in sport: research, theory and practice. London: Routledge, 2004. p.121-44.
  • WULF, G.; SHEA, C.H.; MATSCHINER, S. Frequent feedback enhances complex motor skill learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, Washington, v.30, p.180-92, 1998.
  • Endereço para correspondência:

    Gabriele Wulf
    Department of Kinesiology and Nutrition Sciences University of Nevada
    4505 Maryland Parkway
    Las Vegas - NV - 89154-3034 - USA
    e-mail:
  • Datas de Publicação

    • Publicação nesta coleção
      24 Jan 2012
    • Data do Fascículo
      Dez 2011

    Histórico

    • Revisado
      05 Ago 2011
    • Recebido
      07 Mar 2011
    • Aceito
      29 Ago 2011
    Escola de Educação Física e Esporte da Universidade de São Paulo Av. Prof. Mello Moraes, 65, 05508-030 São Paulo SP/Brasil, Tel./Fax: (55 11) 3091 3147 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
    E-mail: reveefe@usp.br