Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Cognition and environment are predictors of infants' motor development over time

ABSTRACT

We conducted a longitudinal investigation on the relationships among motor and cognitive development, biological aspects, maternal practices, parental knowledge, and family environments of infants. Forty-nine infants aged between 3 and 16 months participated in the study. They were evaluated through the Alberta Infant Motor Scale and the Cognitive Scale of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. Their parents answered a questionnaire about biological factors, the Daily Activities of Infant Scale, affordances for motor development (Baby Scale) in the home environment, and the Brazilian version of the Knowledge of Infant Development Inventory. We conducted evaluations in schools for 4 months. Generalized estimating equations, Bonferroni correction, and Spearman's rank correlation coefficient were used. Significant associations were found in the (1) univariate analysis between motor and cognitive development and environmental factors (education level, income, toy availability, physical space, parental practices and knowledge, breastfeeding duration, and school frequency); (2) multivariate analyses between motor development and income, and between father's age and physical space at home. Motor and cognitive developments were concluded to depend on each other, and environmental factors were shown to be more significant in the associations rather than the biological ones, stressing the importance of home, of parental care, and of the experiences children go through along the first years of their lives.

Keywords:
Motor Skills; Cognition; Environment; Child development

RESUMO

Investigou-se longitudinalmente relações entre desenvolvimento motor e cognitivo, aspectos biológicos, práticas maternas, conhecimento parental e ambiente familiar de bebês. Participaram do estudo 49 bebês (3-16 meses) avaliados com a Alberta Infant Motor Scale e a Escala Mental da Bayley Scale of Infant Development. Os pais responderam o questionário sobre fatores biológicos, Daily Activities of Infant Scale, o Affordances no Ambiente Domiciliar para o Desenvolvimento Motor - Escala Bebê, e o Inventário sobre Conhecimento do Desenvolvimento Infantil. Avaliações foram conduzidas nas escolas ao longo de 4 meses. Foram utilizadas Equações de Estimativa Generalizada, teste de Bonferroni e coeficiente de correlação de Spearman. Observaram-se associações significativas na (1) análise univariada entre desenvolvimento motor e cognitivo e fatores ambientais (escolaridade, renda, disponibilidade de brinquedos, espaço físico, práticas e conhecimento parental, tempo de aleitamento e frequência na escola); (2) multivariada entre o desenvolvimento motor e renda, idade do pai e espaço físico da residência. Concluiu-se que os desenvolvimentos motores e cognitivos se mostraram interdependentes e fatores ambientais se mostraram mais significativos nas associações em detrimento dos biológicos, reforçando-se a importância do lar, do cuidado dos pais e das experiências que a criança vivencia ao longo dos primeiros anos de vida.

Descritores:
Destreza Motora; Cognição; Ambiente; Desenvolvimento Infantil

RESUMEN

Se investigó longitudinalmente relaciones entre desarrollo motor y cognitivo, aspectos biológicos, prácticas maternas, conocimiento parental y ambiente familiar de bebés. Participaron del estudio 49 bebés (3-16 meses) evaluados con la Alberta Infant Motor Scale y la Escala Mental da Bayley Scale of Infant Development. Los padres respondieron al cuestionario sobre factores biológicos Daily Activities of Infant Scale, el Affordance en el Ambiente Domiciliario para el Desarrollo Motor - Escala Bebé, y el Inventario sobre Conocimiento del Desarrollo Infantil. Se condujeron evaluaciones en las escuelas a lo largo de 4 meses. Se utilizaron Ecuaciones de Estimativa Generalizada, prueba de Bonferroni y coeficiente de correlación de Spearman. Se observó asociaciones significativas en la (1) análisis univariada entre desarrollo motor y cognitivo y factores ambientales (escolaridad, renta, disponibilidad de juguetes, espacio físico, prácticas y conocimiento parental, tiempo de amamantamiento y frecuencia en la escuela); (2) multivariada entre el desarrollo motor y renta, edad del padre y espacio físico de la residencia. Se concluye que los desarrollos motores y cognitivos se mostraron interdependientes y factores ambientales se mostraron más significativos en las asociaciones debido a los factores biológicos, se reforzando la idea de la importancia del hogar, del cuidado de los padres y de las experiencias que el niño vive a lo largo de sus primeros años de vida.

Palabras clave:
Destreza Motora; Cognición; Ambiente; Desarrollo Infantil

INTRODUCTION

Development challenges in the first years of live foretell future disorders11. Formiga CKMR, Cezar MEN, Linhares MBM. Avaliação longitudinal do desenvolvimento motor e da habilidade de sentar em crianças nascidas prematuras. Fisioter Pesq. 2010; 17: 102-7.. In this phase, the quick brain development22. Eickman SH, Maciel MAS, Lira PIC, Lima MC. Fatores associados ao desenvolvimento mental e motor de crianças de quatro creches públicas de Recife, Brasil. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2009; 27: 282-8. guides behavioral acquisitions22. Eickman SH, Maciel MAS, Lira PIC, Lima MC. Fatores associados ao desenvolvimento mental e motor de crianças de quatro creches públicas de Recife, Brasil. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2009; 27: 282-8.), (33. Haydari A, Askari P, Nezhad MZ. Relationship between affordances in the home environment and motor development in children aged 18-42 months. J Social Scien. 2009; 5: 319-28.. Biological factors11. Formiga CKMR, Cezar MEN, Linhares MBM. Avaliação longitudinal do desenvolvimento motor e da habilidade de sentar em crianças nascidas prematuras. Fisioter Pesq. 2010; 17: 102-7.), (22. Eickman SH, Maciel MAS, Lira PIC, Lima MC. Fatores associados ao desenvolvimento mental e motor de crianças de quatro creches públicas de Recife, Brasil. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2009; 27: 282-8.), (55. Saccani R, Valentini NC. Análise do desenvolvimento motor de crianças de zero a 18 meses de idade: representatividade dos itens da por faixa etária e postura. Rev Bra Cresc Desenv Hum. 2010; 20: 711-22.), (66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71., environments44. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5.), (55. Saccani R, Valentini NC. Análise do desenvolvimento motor de crianças de zero a 18 meses de idade: representatividade dos itens da por faixa etária e postura. Rev Bra Cresc Desenv Hum. 2010; 20: 711-22.), (66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71.; socioeconomic family status66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71., parental practices, and home structure conditions44. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5. may influence development paths44. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5.), (66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71.), (77. Koutra K, Chatzi L, Roumeliotaki T, Vassilaki M, Giannakoupolou E, Bastos C, Koutis A, Kogevinas M. Sociodemographic determinants of infant neurodevelopment at 18 months of age: Mother-Child Cohort (Rhea Study) in Crete, Greece. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 48-59.), (88. Miquelote AF, Santos DCC, Caçola PM, Montebelo MI, Gabbard C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 329-34.), (99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203.. Associations have been reported between child development and environmental factors, and these sometimes play a stronger role than the very biological vulnerability of children, which suggests the environment is capable of modulating the risks children are exposed to77. Koutra K, Chatzi L, Roumeliotaki T, Vassilaki M, Giannakoupolou E, Bastos C, Koutis A, Kogevinas M. Sociodemographic determinants of infant neurodevelopment at 18 months of age: Mother-Child Cohort (Rhea Study) in Crete, Greece. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 48-59..

Environmental factors lead to behavioral differences, and challenging environments have been shown to be positive in the acquisitions44. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5.), (66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71.), (1010. Almeida, CS, Valentini, NC. Contexto dos berçários e um programa de intervenção no desenvolvimento de bebés. Motricidade. 2013; 9: 22-32.. Factors posing risks or providing protection - other than economic aspects, physical housing aspects, and toy availability - need to be identified. Specifically, parental practices and knowledge have received little attention from Brazilian researchers99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203.), (1010. Almeida, CS, Valentini, NC. Contexto dos berçários e um programa de intervenção no desenvolvimento de bebés. Motricidade. 2013; 9: 22-32.), (1111. Eickman SH, Lira PIC, Lima MC, Coutinho SB, Teixeira MLPD, Ashworth A. Breastfeeding and mental and motor development at 12 months in a law-income population in northeast Brazil. Pediatr Perinatal Epidemiol. 2007; 21: 129-37., due to their limited tools99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203. and difficulty getting parents to follow their children's routines. The related literature is observed to predominantly have studies focusing on risk factors, rather than investigating protection factors1212. Dipietro JA. Baby and the brain: advances in child development. Ann Rev Public Health. 2000; 21: 455-71., which play roles to improve the quality of life of children. A prevalence of investigations focusing on just one aspect or constraint is also observed to exist, in which researchers fail to investigate how different factors, whether biological or environmental, are combined to interfere in an individual's development path1313. Pereira KRG, Valentini NC, Saccani R. Brazilian infant's motor and cognitive development: longitudinal influence of risk factors. Int Pediatr. no prelo, 2016..

Moreover, although cross-sectional designs help understand posture acquisition by infants1111. Eickman SH, Lira PIC, Lima MC, Coutinho SB, Teixeira MLPD, Ashworth A. Breastfeeding and mental and motor development at 12 months in a law-income population in northeast Brazil. Pediatr Perinatal Epidemiol. 2007; 21: 129-37.), (1414. Son SH, Morrisson FJ. The Nature and impact of changes in home learning environment on development of language and academic skills in preschool children. Dev Psychol. 2010; 46: 1103-18., longitudinal studies are limited. Brazil has few cross-sectional studies focusing on infants' posture acquisition and its influencing factors11. Formiga CKMR, Cezar MEN, Linhares MBM. Avaliação longitudinal do desenvolvimento motor e da habilidade de sentar em crianças nascidas prematuras. Fisioter Pesq. 2010; 17: 102-7.), (88. Miquelote AF, Santos DCC, Caçola PM, Montebelo MI, Gabbard C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 329-34.. Thus, the objective of this study was to conduct a longitudinal investigation on the associations among motor and cognitive development, biological aspects, maternal practices, parental knowledge, and family environments of infants. In this study, we introduce the assumption that, throughout time, the strength of biological and environmental factors is modified in a way that provides a different way to foretell infants' motor and cognitive development.

METHODOLOGY

Design and subjects

This is a cross-sectional, descriptive, observational study. The research has been approved by the ethics committees of two universities (numbers 0596081 2.6.0000.5341 and 2008018). All parents signed consent forms.

Forty-nine infants participated in the study, of which 55.1% were boys and 24.5% were preterm. The corrected ages of the babies ranged from 3 to 16 months at the three evaluation times (T), whose averages were 8 (T1), 10 (T2), and 12 (T3) months. The inclusion criteria were: being an infant attending a children's school and being aged between 0 and 18 months. The exclusion criteria comprised: having bone, muscle, and joint alterations, having neurological diseases, having acute diseases, and having underwent interventions.

Tools

A questionnaire was used, containing biological factors of infants (Apgar score at minute 5); prematurity; weight; length, and head circumference at birth; commitment to neonatal ICUs, age of parents, cohabiting, caregiver's duties, and time of exclusive breastfeeding.

Motor development was evaluated using the Alberta Infant Motor Scale (AIMS)1515. Piper MC, Darrah J. Motor assessment of the developing infant. Philadelphia WB: Saunders Company; 1994., an instrument that has been validated1616. Valentini NC, Saccani R. Brazilian Validation of the Alberta Infant Motor Scale. Phys Ther. 2012; 92: 440-7. and regulated1717. Saccani R, Valentini NC. Reference curves for the Brazilian Alberta infant motor scale: percentiles for clinical description and follow-up over time. J Pediatria (Rio J). 2012; 88: 40-7. in Brazil. Individual evaluations with minimum handling, in prone (21 items), supine (9), sitting (12) and standing (16) positions being performed for 20 minutes each. We analyzed assumed postures, anti-gravity motion, and weight bearing. A percentile was used, which describes motor development in three categories: delayed (below 5), suspected (between 5 and 25), and normal (above 25)1515. Piper MC, Darrah J. Motor assessment of the developing infant. Philadelphia WB: Saunders Company; 1994..

Cognitive development was evaluated through the Mental Scale of Bayley Scale of Infant Development - second edition1818. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1993., which was individually applied through different tasks, each of which specific to a certain development stage. Gross scores are linked to corrected ages and converted into the Mental Development Index (MDI) - a variable used in the study and presented as Bayley MDI - it describes cognitive performance in four categories: expedited (above 119), within normal limits (below 85 and 114), slightly delayed (between 70 and 84), and significantly delayed (below 69)1818. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1993..

The Brazilian version1919. Caçola PM, Gabbard C, Montebelo MIL, Santos DCC. The new affordances in the home environment for motor development - infant scale (AHEMD-IS): Versions in English and Portuguese Languages. Braz J Phys Ther. 2015; 19(6): 507-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/bjpt-rbf.2014.0112
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/bjpt-rbf.2014....
of Affordances in the Home Environment for Motor Development - Infant Scale (AHEMD-IS) was used to investigate opportunities for learning at home44. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5.. AHEMD is organized in dimensions of characteristics regarding socioeconomic status, family, home physical space, toys, and daily activities44. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5.), (1919. Caçola PM, Gabbard C, Montebelo MIL, Santos DCC. The new affordances in the home environment for motor development - infant scale (AHEMD-IS): Versions in English and Portuguese Languages. Braz J Phys Ther. 2015; 19(6): 507-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/bjpt-rbf.2014.0112
http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/bjpt-rbf.2014....
.

Maternal practices were investigated using and adapted version of Daily Activities of Infant Scale (DAIS), which evaluates opportunities for posture control and exploration of movements made available by the caregiver during different tasks in an infant's routine. The responses are arranged in an ordered scale with various opportunities for development2020. Bartlett DJ, Fanning JK, Miller L. Development of the Daily Activities of Infant Scale: a measure supporting early motor development. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2008; 50: 613-7.. The sum of the scores make up the total score of DAIS.

The version adapted to Brazil of the Knowledge of Infant Development Inventory (KIDI), Inventário de Conhecimento sobre o Desenvolvimento Infantil2121. Ribas RC, Seidl de Moura ML, Gomes AAN, Soares ID. Adaptação brasileira do inventário de conhecimento sobre o desenvolvimento infantil de David Macphee [Resumo]. In: Anais do III Congresso Brasileiro de Psicologia do Desenvolvimento. Niterói, RJ, 2000., was used to evaluate the caregivers' knowledge of child development; 20 questions regard to specific periods when skills are acquired. The score was obtained by dividing the number of correctly answered questions by the total number of answered questions, and it ranged from 0 (little) to 1 (much knowledge).

Procedures

The motor and cognitive evaluations of infants were conducted at the schools whose representatives signed consent forms. Consent forms were sent to all parents. The remaining instruments were sent to the families; the measurements were performed at three moments in a 4-month period, with a 2-month interval between each evaluation.

Analysis of data

The statistical analysis was conducted through SPSS software (version 20.0). Measures of central tendency and variability were described. Generalized estimating equations were used with Bonferroni correction for motor and cognitive development throughout time; Spearman's rank correlation coefficient was used for the relationship between cognitive and motor scores (values of r<0.30 = weak; 0.30 < values < 0.60 = moderate; > 0.60 = strong). To associate independents variables with the motor percentiles, simple linear regression was used (for each factor of a subject and their environment at each time). To enable the investigation of a higher number of factors accounting for development and how they behave together, the variables that had p≤0.25 were used in the multivariate linear regression2222. Cohen J, Cohen P. Applied multiple regression / correlation analysis for the behavorial sciences. Second edition. Hilsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, 1983.. The ones that had statistical significance were the only ones kept in the model. Significance level: p≤0.05.

RESULTS

The subjects' characteristics are shown in Table 1, with the distribution of frequencies by age and in the categories of motor and cognitive development at each evaluation time. This sample is observed to comprise infants from children's schools whose ages ranged between 2 and 16 months, most of whom were categorized as having normal motor and cognitive development at the three evaluation moments. A higher prevalence of delays and suspected delays was observed in motor development.

Table 1
Distribution of frequencies by age and in the categories of motor and cognitive development at the three evaluation moments

Family income of the evaluated infants ranged from R$ 400.00 to R$ 8000.00 per month (M ± SD = R$ 2,267.55 ± 1735.48). Regarding the education level of their parents, high school was the most prevalent one (44.9% females; 46.9% males). The detailed description of the biological variables of study subjects with averages and dispersion is shown in Table 2.

Table 2
Biological characteristics of subjects

The motor percentile was found to increase (p=0.033) and a significant difference was found between M1 and M2 (p1-2=0.035; p2-3=1.00; p1-3=0.347), which characterizes variability and development plateau (Figure 1).

Figure 1
Motor development (AIMS percentile) over the months; *p ≤ 0.05: significant difference between moments

The correlation between motor and cognitive development was significant and moderate at M1 (rho1=0.496; p1<0.001) and M2 (rho2=0.520; p2<0.001); and strong at M3 (rho3=0.634; p3<0.001). Concerning the simple regression, a significant association was observed to exist between motor skills and cognition, with high beta values. In the multivariate one, the association remained in the model at M2 and M3. The increases in the motor repertoire were related to more sophisticated cognitive behavior.

In the regressions, significant associations were observed between motor development and residential factors. In the univariate one, regardless of the moment, the significant association between motor development and cognition, education levels of parents, income, toy availability, physical space at home, maternal practices, parental knowledge, breastfeeding duration, and school frequency time. Table 3 shows the univariate regression results with the weights of biological and environmental factors having motor development as an outcome variable.

Table 3
Simple linear regression (univariate) at the three evaluation moments with biological and environmental variables having motor development as outcome

In the multivariate regression (Table 4), all factors investigated in this study were included in the data analysis process in order to verify how they are associated with motor development over time. Significant associations were observed with cognition (M2 and M3), income (M1, M2, and M3), father's age (M1 and M2), physical space at home (M1 and M2), and maternal practices (M3). Table 4 shows the values of Beta, p, and adjusted R2. Beta indicated that the weight of each variable in the model, being corrected for the range of variation in the studied factor, was changed at the three evaluation times. The variables that remained in the model accounted for a great deal of variability in motor skills (values of adjusted R2 close to or above 0.5). The factors with the highest weights in the model (higher Beta values) were income and cognition.

Table 4
Jultivariate linear regression at the three evaluation moments with motor development as the outcome

DISCUSSION

Motor development

Throughout the three evaluation moments, changes in the values of performance percentiles were observed along the evaluations (M1, M2, and M3). At the second and third evaluation moments, the children were found to have lower motor percentiles, which consequently shows a lower number of acquisitions at this age. The motor and cognitive normality levels were observed to decrease with age, but the delays were found to increase (M3). Studies have described variations in motor percentiles and categorization55. Saccani R, Valentini NC. Análise do desenvolvimento motor de crianças de zero a 18 meses de idade: representatividade dos itens da por faixa etária e postura. Rev Bra Cresc Desenv Hum. 2010; 20: 711-22.), (88. Miquelote AF, Santos DCC, Caçola PM, Montebelo MI, Gabbard C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 329-34.. For example, a recent study reported fluctuations in the motor development categories of 32 Brazilian infants over time: the average or above average category was observed in 68% of the 9-month old infants - six months later, this frequency rose to 94%88. Miquelote AF, Santos DCC, Caçola PM, Montebelo MI, Gabbard C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 329-34..

Also considering the percentiles, a similar study with Brazilian infants, albeit with a larger sample (n=561) found higher percentile values at the first three months and after 13 months, and these children were found to have less suspected or real motor delays55. Saccani R, Valentini NC. Análise do desenvolvimento motor de crianças de zero a 18 meses de idade: representatividade dos itens da por faixa etária e postura. Rev Bra Cresc Desenv Hum. 2010; 20: 711-22.. These results corroborate the ones from this study (higher prevalence of delays at the third evaluation moment) and show that the percentile changes may represent individuality or stability periods in acquisitions2323. Darrah, Hodge, Magill-Evans, et.al. Stability of serial assessment of motor and communication abilities in typically developing infants: implications for screening. Early Hum Dev. 2003; 72: 97-110.), (2424. Edwards SL, Sarwark JF. Infant and Child Development. Clin Orthopaedics Related Res. 2005; 434: 33-9.. The stable periods are not necessarily related to stagnant development. The children were not found to have changed motor patterns, but they may be developing parameters to acquire them. One example is the development of crawling. A child is firstly shown to be capable of standing on all fours, and then of swinging while in the position. This movement will cause their upper and lower limbs to be strengthened in order to be able to move in such way.

Associations and predictors of motor development:

Generally speaking, the factors that were found to be the most associated with the development of studied babies were the environmental ones, rather than individual aspects. Such possibility was already reported in a previous review study77. Koutra K, Chatzi L, Roumeliotaki T, Vassilaki M, Giannakoupolou E, Bastos C, Koutis A, Kogevinas M. Sociodemographic determinants of infant neurodevelopment at 18 months of age: Mother-Child Cohort (Rhea Study) in Crete, Greece. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 48-59.), (1313. Pereira KRG, Valentini NC, Saccani R. Brazilian infant's motor and cognitive development: longitudinal influence of risk factors. Int Pediatr. no prelo, 2016.), (2525. Voos MC, Moura MCS, Caromano FA, Hasue RH. A influência do ambiente no desenvolvimento motor: revisão de literatura. Temas Desenvolv. 2013; 19(106): 154-8.. Biological factors, despite being frequently mentioned as risks to child development77. Koutra K, Chatzi L, Roumeliotaki T, Vassilaki M, Giannakoupolou E, Bastos C, Koutis A, Kogevinas M. Sociodemographic determinants of infant neurodevelopment at 18 months of age: Mother-Child Cohort (Rhea Study) in Crete, Greece. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 48-59.), (1111. Eickman SH, Lira PIC, Lima MC, Coutinho SB, Teixeira MLPD, Ashworth A. Breastfeeding and mental and motor development at 12 months in a law-income population in northeast Brazil. Pediatr Perinatal Epidemiol. 2007; 21: 129-37.), (2626. Pin TW, Darrer T, Eldridge B, Galea MP. Motor development from 4 to 8 months corrected age in infants born at or less than 29 weeks' gestation. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009; 51: 739-45.), (2727. Kolobe THA. Childrearing practices and developmental expectations for mexican-american mothers and the developmental status of their infants. Phys Ther, 2004; 88: 439-53., were not found to influence the regression models in this study.

The relationship between motor skills and cognition was confirmed in the correlation tests and regressions used, similarly to a previous study2727. Kolobe THA. Childrearing practices and developmental expectations for mexican-american mothers and the developmental status of their infants. Phys Ther, 2004; 88: 439-53., which reinforces the interaction of these processes and the predictive ability of cognitive development over the motor one88. Miquelote AF, Santos DCC, Caçola PM, Montebelo MI, Gabbard C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 329-34.), (2828. Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 2000; 71: 44-56.. A possible explanation is the coactivation of certain brain areas during these tasks. Cognition mainly activates the prefrontal cortex, which is co-activated in a motor action. In the same direction, a motor action activates the cerebellum, which is also co-activated in a cognitive task2828. Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 2000; 71: 44-56.. That is, although the nervous system has predetermined primary functional areas, these same areas can be activated in different situations2828. Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 2000; 71: 44-56..

Factors at home were found to be strong and significant in the associations, remaining in the final multivariate regression models and accounting for a great deal of the development variability. Similar results previously reported with Brazilian infants99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203.), (1313. Pereira KRG, Valentini NC, Saccani R. Brazilian infant's motor and cognitive development: longitudinal influence of risk factors. Int Pediatr. no prelo, 2016., as well as in a literature review2525. Voos MC, Moura MCS, Caromano FA, Hasue RH. A influência do ambiente no desenvolvimento motor: revisão de literatura. Temas Desenvolv. 2013; 19(106): 154-8., reinforce the importance of the environment experienced by a child, and this may minimize biological risk factors.

Income was found to have a significant association in the simple regression, and it was the only factor observed to remain in the multivariate model at the three moments, besides having the highest beta value and being configured as the variable that accounted for the motor percentile the most comprehensively. Associations between motor skills and income were described in a Brazilian study99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203.. A higher incidence of delays was observed in families of low socioeconomic statuses66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71., as they are more exposed to risk factors and are more prone to being affected by pre-established risks. These families have higher availability of physical space, toys, and time for interaction with their infants44. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5.), (66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71. which are factors that account for these results to a great extent.

Despite not remaining in the multivariate model, the education levels of parents were not found to be associated with motor percentiles in the univariate analysis at the three moments. Lower education levels of parents77. Koutra K, Chatzi L, Roumeliotaki T, Vassilaki M, Giannakoupolou E, Bastos C, Koutis A, Kogevinas M. Sociodemographic determinants of infant neurodevelopment at 18 months of age: Mother-Child Cohort (Rhea Study) in Crete, Greece. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 48-59.), (88. Miquelote AF, Santos DCC, Caçola PM, Montebelo MI, Gabbard C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 329-34.), (99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203., mainly the mothers66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71.), (2929. Abbott AL, Bartlett DJ. Infant motor development and equipment use in the home. Child Care Health Dev. 2001; 27: 295-306. negatively affect child development. Mother's education level is reported as a variable with a persistent impact on child development, and it influences the child care99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203., the organization of the environment1414. Son SH, Morrisson FJ. The Nature and impact of changes in home learning environment on development of language and academic skills in preschool children. Dev Psychol. 2010; 46: 1103-18., and available motor opportunities99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203..

In the multivariate model, father's age was found to have significance at the first and second moments. Studies suggest inferior development of teenage mother's children3030. Sartori N, Saccani R, Valentini NC. Comparação do desenvolvimento motor de lactentes de mães adolescentes e adultas. Fisioter Pesq. 2010; 12: 306-11., which is similar to this study, with the poorest performance being the one of children of younger parents. The restricted opportunities of interaction between parents and their children, which negatively impact child development88. Miquelote AF, Santos DCC, Caçola PM, Montebelo MI, Gabbard C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 329-34.), (99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203., may be due to parents' heavy workloads. The age of the infants may have also contributed, as young fathers may demonstrate lack of knowledge and security in caring and stimulating small infants.

Physical space at home was found to be associated with motor percentiles in univariate and multivariate models. Their homes and their surrounding areas are the first environments experienced by children; available space is a factor of protection to motor development99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203., it is fundamental in the mediation of walking33. Haydari A, Askari P, Nezhad MZ. Relationship between affordances in the home environment and motor development in children aged 18-42 months. J Social Scien. 2009; 5: 319-28.), (99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203., and it influences exploratory behavior66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71., mainly in the age range of the infants analyzed in this this study, who are mostly in the period where they acquire the skills of crawling and walking, between 6 and 14 months.

Available toys were found to be associated with motor development in the simple regression, and this relationship had already been reported by previous studies, with association values inferior to the one observed in this study44. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5.), (99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203.. Using appropriate toys stimulates new and varied motor actions and problem-solving44. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5.), (99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203., helps eye-hand coordination3131. Bober SJ, Humphry R, Carswell HC, Core AJ. Toddler's persistence in the emerging occupations of functional play and self-feeding. Am J OccupTher. 2001; 55: 369-76., thus developing manipulative skills. Toy availability predicts cognitive and motor behavior of infants33. Haydari A, Askari P, Nezhad MZ. Relationship between affordances in the home environment and motor development in children aged 18-42 months. J Social Scien. 2009; 5: 319-28.), (44. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5., a relationship that was confirmed by this study.

Maternal practices were found to be associated with motor skills in both regression models. At the third moment, the mothers who provided longer exposure to independent positions favored the development of their children; the mothers who carried their infants longer minimized their opportunities for development. Studies suggest that Brazilian mothers carry their infants very often and offer little opportunity for children to sit3232. Santos DCC, Gabbard C, Gonçalves VMG. Motor development during the first 6 months: the case of brazilian infants. Inf Child Dev. 2000; 9: 161-6. or lie facing down3333. Lopes VB, Lima CD, Tudella E. Motor aquisition rate in brazillian infants. Inf Child Dev. 2009; 18: 122-32. unsupported on the floor Positions that demand higher muscle strength against the action of gravity2323. Darrah, Hodge, Magill-Evans, et.al. Stability of serial assessment of motor and communication abilities in typically developing infants: implications for screening. Early Hum Dev. 2003; 72: 97-110. should and must be exercised precociously. Caregivers avoid such positions reporting discomfort from the children and fear of death by suffocation, which leads to development delays55. Saccani R, Valentini NC. Análise do desenvolvimento motor de crianças de zero a 18 meses de idade: representatividade dos itens da por faixa etária e postura. Rev Bra Cresc Desenv Hum. 2010; 20: 711-22.), (99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203.), (2323. Darrah, Hodge, Magill-Evans, et.al. Stability of serial assessment of motor and communication abilities in typically developing infants: implications for screening. Early Hum Dev. 2003; 72: 97-110.), (3434. Formiga CKM, Pedrazzani ES, Tudella E. Desenvolvimento motor de lactentes pré-termo participantes de um programa de intervenção fisioterapêutica precoce. Braz J PhysTher. 2004; 8: 239-45., a factor that may explain these results.

The motor percentile was found to be associated with the knowledge on child development at all moments; parents and caregivers who precisely reported their children's abilities were observed to be capable of adapting the environment in a way to enrich available stimuli, a result that is similar to the one of a previous study66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71.. In order to understand the needs from their children, they need to have time to interact with them88. Miquelote AF, Santos DCC, Caçola PM, Montebelo MI, Gabbard C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 329-34.), (99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203.. Infant parents are interested in receiving information that may mediate the development of their children3232. Santos DCC, Gabbard C, Gonçalves VMG. Motor development during the first 6 months: the case of brazilian infants. Inf Child Dev. 2000; 9: 161-6.; they do not always have access to it. Parental education programs which instruct parents on activities that are proper to their children's skills88. Miquelote AF, Santos DCC, Caçola PM, Montebelo MI, Gabbard C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 329-34.), (99. Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203., which moderate the use of equipment3232. Santos DCC, Gabbard C, Gonçalves VMG. Motor development during the first 6 months: the case of brazilian infants. Inf Child Dev. 2000; 9: 161-6., and which allow children to be on the floor for longer3232. Santos DCC, Gabbard C, Gonçalves VMG. Motor development during the first 6 months: the case of brazilian infants. Inf Child Dev. 2000; 9: 161-6. are currently required.

Breastfeeding was found to be associated with motor development in the univariate model. Higher motor scores are observed in babies who are breastfed for longer66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71., which is similar to this study. Higher electroencephalographic activity frequencies3333. Lopes VB, Lima CD, Tudella E. Motor aquisition rate in brazillian infants. Inf Child Dev. 2009; 18: 122-32. and cognitive1111. Eickman SH, Lira PIC, Lima MC, Coutinho SB, Teixeira MLPD, Ashworth A. Breastfeeding and mental and motor development at 12 months in a law-income population in northeast Brazil. Pediatr Perinatal Epidemiol. 2007; 21: 129-37. and motor development66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71. superiority are observed in breastfed infants. Nutrition is fundamental for development, and it modulates processes associated with the maturation of brain structure and activity; they interfere in the child development path3535. Jing H, Gilchrist JM, Badger TM, Pivik RT. A longitudinal study of differences in electroencephalographic activity among breastfed, milk formula-fed, and soy formula-fed infants during the first year of life. Early Hum Dev. 2010; 86: 119-25.. Establishing the bond between infants and their mothers and the physical contact between them stimulates motor activity and mediates the remaining abilities of individuals66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71.. Before this evidence, a minimum six-month breastfeeding period is suggested in order to prevent undesirable delay conditions66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71. and promote development.

School frequency time was found to be associated with the motor percentile in the simple regression at the third moment, with higher scores for babies who have attended school for longer, and that is similar to previous studies44. Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5.), (66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71.. This is summed to daycare time - the quality of the context that is offered to infants. A study suggests that infants from schools with contexts that were more appropriate to development were found to have higher development scores3636. Almeida CS, Valentini NC. Contexto dos berçários e um programa de intervenção no desenvolvimento de bebés. Motricidade. 2013; 9: 22-32.. Children's schools may provide experiencing tasks that are more varied than the ones at home, and that stimulates the motor skills66. Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71., as long as the environment is properly organized3636. Almeida CS, Valentini NC. Contexto dos berçários e um programa de intervenção no desenvolvimento de bebés. Motricidade. 2013; 9: 22-32.. In this study, the infants seem to be finding conditions that are proper to development at schools.

Having in mind that this study observed a higher association between environmental factors and child development, a need is suggested, as a practical outcome, to map children who have increased risks of delays and to train their fathers to create opportunities for development. Some actions may potentialize development paths, such as teaching parents how to carry and position infants, making various toys and materials available, and adapting home environments.

CONCLUSION

Environmental factors and cognition accounted for most of the variability in motor development, rather than biological factors, with a special mention to family income, space at home, maternal practices, and father's age. This is a trend that has already been observed in the literature, which suggests that an environment that has plenty of stimuli can minimize the effects from biological vulnerability. Also, environments with restricted opportunities may potentialize risks of development delays.

REFERÊNCIAS

  • 1
    Formiga CKMR, Cezar MEN, Linhares MBM. Avaliação longitudinal do desenvolvimento motor e da habilidade de sentar em crianças nascidas prematuras. Fisioter Pesq. 2010; 17: 102-7.
  • 2
    Eickman SH, Maciel MAS, Lira PIC, Lima MC. Fatores associados ao desenvolvimento mental e motor de crianças de quatro creches públicas de Recife, Brasil. Rev Paul Pediatr. 2009; 27: 282-8.
  • 3
    Haydari A, Askari P, Nezhad MZ. Relationship between affordances in the home environment and motor development in children aged 18-42 months. J Social Scien. 2009; 5: 319-28.
  • 4
    Caçola P, Gabbard C, Santos DCC, Batistela ACT. Development of the Affordances in the home environment for motor development: infant scale. Pediatr Int. 2011; 53: 820-5.
  • 5
    Saccani R, Valentini NC. Análise do desenvolvimento motor de crianças de zero a 18 meses de idade: representatividade dos itens da por faixa etária e postura. Rev Bra Cresc Desenv Hum. 2010; 20: 711-22.
  • 6
    Zajonz R, Muller AB, Valentini NC. A influência de fatores ambientais no desempenho motor e social de crianças da periferia de Porto Alegre. Rev Educ Fís UEM. 2008; 19: 159-71.
  • 7
    Koutra K, Chatzi L, Roumeliotaki T, Vassilaki M, Giannakoupolou E, Bastos C, Koutis A, Kogevinas M. Sociodemographic determinants of infant neurodevelopment at 18 months of age: Mother-Child Cohort (Rhea Study) in Crete, Greece. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 48-59.
  • 8
    Miquelote AF, Santos DCC, Caçola PM, Montebelo MI, Gabbard C. Effect of the home environment on motor and cognitive behavior of infants. Infant Behav Dev. 2012; 35: 329-34.
  • 9
    Saccani R, Pereira KRG, Muller AB, Valentini NC, Gabbard C. Influence of biological factors and affordances in the home on infant motor development. Pediatr Int. 2013; 55: 197-203.
  • 10
    Almeida, CS, Valentini, NC. Contexto dos berçários e um programa de intervenção no desenvolvimento de bebés. Motricidade. 2013; 9: 22-32.
  • 11
    Eickman SH, Lira PIC, Lima MC, Coutinho SB, Teixeira MLPD, Ashworth A. Breastfeeding and mental and motor development at 12 months in a law-income population in northeast Brazil. Pediatr Perinatal Epidemiol. 2007; 21: 129-37.
  • 12
    Dipietro JA. Baby and the brain: advances in child development. Ann Rev Public Health. 2000; 21: 455-71.
  • 13
    Pereira KRG, Valentini NC, Saccani R. Brazilian infant's motor and cognitive development: longitudinal influence of risk factors. Int Pediatr. no prelo, 2016.
  • 14
    Son SH, Morrisson FJ. The Nature and impact of changes in home learning environment on development of language and academic skills in preschool children. Dev Psychol. 2010; 46: 1103-18.
  • 15
    Piper MC, Darrah J. Motor assessment of the developing infant. Philadelphia WB: Saunders Company; 1994.
  • 16
    Valentini NC, Saccani R. Brazilian Validation of the Alberta Infant Motor Scale. Phys Ther. 2012; 92: 440-7.
  • 17
    Saccani R, Valentini NC. Reference curves for the Brazilian Alberta infant motor scale: percentiles for clinical description and follow-up over time. J Pediatria (Rio J). 2012; 88: 40-7.
  • 18
    Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1993.
  • 19
    Caçola PM, Gabbard C, Montebelo MIL, Santos DCC. The new affordances in the home environment for motor development - infant scale (AHEMD-IS): Versions in English and Portuguese Languages. Braz J Phys Ther. 2015; 19(6): 507-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/bjpt-rbf.2014.0112
    » http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/bjpt-rbf.2014.0112
  • 20
    Bartlett DJ, Fanning JK, Miller L. Development of the Daily Activities of Infant Scale: a measure supporting early motor development. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2008; 50: 613-7.
  • 21
    Ribas RC, Seidl de Moura ML, Gomes AAN, Soares ID. Adaptação brasileira do inventário de conhecimento sobre o desenvolvimento infantil de David Macphee [Resumo]. In: Anais do III Congresso Brasileiro de Psicologia do Desenvolvimento. Niterói, RJ, 2000.
  • 22
    Cohen J, Cohen P. Applied multiple regression / correlation analysis for the behavorial sciences. Second edition. Hilsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, 1983.
  • 23
    Darrah, Hodge, Magill-Evans, et.al. Stability of serial assessment of motor and communication abilities in typically developing infants: implications for screening. Early Hum Dev. 2003; 72: 97-110.
  • 24
    Edwards SL, Sarwark JF. Infant and Child Development. Clin Orthopaedics Related Res. 2005; 434: 33-9.
  • 25
    Voos MC, Moura MCS, Caromano FA, Hasue RH. A influência do ambiente no desenvolvimento motor: revisão de literatura. Temas Desenvolv. 2013; 19(106): 154-8.
  • 26
    Pin TW, Darrer T, Eldridge B, Galea MP. Motor development from 4 to 8 months corrected age in infants born at or less than 29 weeks' gestation. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009; 51: 739-45.
  • 27
    Kolobe THA. Childrearing practices and developmental expectations for mexican-american mothers and the developmental status of their infants. Phys Ther, 2004; 88: 439-53.
  • 28
    Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 2000; 71: 44-56.
  • 29
    Abbott AL, Bartlett DJ. Infant motor development and equipment use in the home. Child Care Health Dev. 2001; 27: 295-306.
  • 30
    Sartori N, Saccani R, Valentini NC. Comparação do desenvolvimento motor de lactentes de mães adolescentes e adultas. Fisioter Pesq. 2010; 12: 306-11.
  • 31
    Bober SJ, Humphry R, Carswell HC, Core AJ. Toddler's persistence in the emerging occupations of functional play and self-feeding. Am J OccupTher. 2001; 55: 369-76.
  • 32
    Santos DCC, Gabbard C, Gonçalves VMG. Motor development during the first 6 months: the case of brazilian infants. Inf Child Dev. 2000; 9: 161-6.
  • 33
    Lopes VB, Lima CD, Tudella E. Motor aquisition rate in brazillian infants. Inf Child Dev. 2009; 18: 122-32.
  • 34
    Formiga CKM, Pedrazzani ES, Tudella E. Desenvolvimento motor de lactentes pré-termo participantes de um programa de intervenção fisioterapêutica precoce. Braz J PhysTher. 2004; 8: 239-45.
  • 35
    Jing H, Gilchrist JM, Badger TM, Pivik RT. A longitudinal study of differences in electroencephalographic activity among breastfed, milk formula-fed, and soy formula-fed infants during the first year of life. Early Hum Dev. 2010; 86: 119-25.
  • 36
    Almeida CS, Valentini NC. Contexto dos berçários e um programa de intervenção no desenvolvimento de bebés. Motricidade. 2013; 9: 22-32.
  • 5
    Study developed on Research Laboratory of Exercise (LAPEX) from Physical Education Department of the Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul - Porto Alegre (RS), Brazil.
  • Financing source: Coordination for higher Education Staff Development (CAPES) and Brazilian National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq)
  • 8
    Approved by the Research Ethics Committees of Federal University of Rio Grande do Sul (process number: 2008018) and University of Caxias do Sul (process number: 0596081 2.6.0000.5341).

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Jan-Mar 2016

History

  • Received
    Mar 2015
  • Accepted
    Mar 2016
Universidade de São Paulo Rua Ovídio Pires de Campos, 225 2° andar. , 05403-010 São Paulo SP / Brasil, Tel: 55 11 2661-7703, Fax 55 11 3743-7462 - São Paulo - SP - Brazil
E-mail: revfisio@usp.br