Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

VILLAGE ORNAMENTS: FAMILIARIZATION AND PETS AS ART(IFACTS) IN AMAZONIA1 1 This article was made possible by reviews, comments, and suggestions from various partners on the two occasions when the preliminary results were presented. I especially would like to thank Carlos Sautchuk, Guilherme Sá, Ciméa Bevilaqua, Jean Segata, Bernardo Lewgoy, Caetano Sordi, Lady Selma Albernaz, Geraldo Andrello, Clarice Cohn, and Marcos Lanna. Marcy Norton sent me some of her texts, which were fundamental to the arguments developed herein.

ENFEITES DE ALDEIA: FAMILIARIZAÇÃO E MASCOTES COMO ARTE(FATOS) NA AMAZÔNIA

Abstract

The objective of this article is to discuss some reasons the Karitiana (Rondônia, Brazil) evoke to explain their ever-present desire to maintain familiarized or domesticated animals in their villages. Based on the ethnography of the relationships among the Karitiana and these animals, this paper enters into dialogue with the hypotheses formulated to explore the Amazonian people's fondness for the company of non-human species. It also provides insights for rethinking these debates, advocating that Indians are particularly looking for beauty represented by the diversity of animals and by the arts of domestication, just like the aesthetics of conviviality as proposed by Joanna Overing. This aesthetic dimension of human-animal relations seems to be overlooked by theorists of domestication or familiarization because they consider these phenomena to be more techniques or technologies than arts. Renewed perspectives on human-animal relations can be opened by addressing the "arts of domestication" and avoiding an a priori opposition between technique and art.

Keywords:
human-animal relations; domesticity; familiarization; art; aesthetics

Resumo

Este artigo discute o desejo sempre presente, entre os Karitiana (Rondônia), de ter animais domesticados ou familiarizados em suas aldeias. A partir da etnografia de seus animais de criação, dialoga com hipóteses formuladas para investigar o gosto dos povos indígenas pela companhia de seres não humanos, e defende que os povos indígenas procuram especialmente a beleza, representada pela diversidade de animais e pelas artes envolvidas na sua familiarização e na realização de um bem viver doméstico, que se traduzem em uma estética da convivialidade (Joanna OveringOVERING, Joanna; PASSES, Alan (eds.). 2000. The anthropology of love and anger: the aesthetics of conviviality in native South America. London: Routledge .). Esta dimensão estética da relação com animais parece ter sido esquecida diante de trabalhos que abordam a familiarização e/ou a domesticação de animais como técnicas ou tecnologias e não como artes. Novas perspectivas para a análise da relação entre humanos e não humanos podem ser abertas com o tratamento das "artes da domesticação", evitando-se oposição apriorística entre técnica e arte.

Palavras-chave:
relações humano-animal; domesticidade; familiarização; arte; estética

A arte de viver é simplesmente a arte de conviver [The art of living is simply the art of living together] Mário Quintana

The main village of the Karitiana people, Kyõwã (also known as Aldeia Central [Central Village] or Aldeia Nova [New Village]), is inhabited by a large number of animals that live with the human inhabitants on a long-term and continuous basis. Dogs, chickens, ducks, horses, parrots and macaws, coatis and monkeys, sometimes trumpeteers and even tapirs and rabbits (and at times even snakes, electric eels, and other animals) can be found everywhere, moving about in the spaces between and within houses, pestering people as they go about their daily tasks or just wandering as they hunt for food or shade in the scorching heat of the Amazonian afternoon. In this aspect, Kyõwã is no different from most indigenous villages in Brazil and neighboring countries, some of which have even more familiar or domesticated animals; this clearly shows the interest that the native populations of the South American lowlands have in these non-human companions.

In this article I attempt to discuss some of the reasons the Karitiana use to explain their ever-present desire to accumulate more and more of these companion animals. The motives the Karitiana use to explain this love for the variety of animals living in their villages can, in my opinion, help reopen the debate about the ubiquitous presence of these creatures among Amazonian indigenous populations. This may be accomplished by no longer starting from generalized hypotheses (as we shall see below), but giving voice to native explanations for the phenomenon, a task that still remains to be done for the vast majority of these populations. The Karitiana's rationalizations offer perfectly intelligible explanations which are very relevant to the habit of (and the enjoyment of) accumulating domesticated animals which live among humans. They apply even to us, modern Westerners, once we understand the idea (a discussion of which concludes this reflection and is intended to open up new fields of research about the relationship between humans and non-humans) that living together is an art.

* * *

The Karitiana (Yjxa) speak the Tupi language, Arikém branch (the only language in this family), and traditionally inhabit the valleys of the Candeias, Jamari, and Jaci-Parana Rivers, major tributaries of the right bank of the Madeira River in the northern portion of what is currently the state of Rondônia. Today, the group has a registered area of approximately 89,000 hectares, a square of reasonably preserved forest bordered on the south and east by the Bom Futuro National Forest (FLONA), and on the north by several cattle ranches and large rural properties, many of which belong to prominent figures in the state's politics and economy. The demarcation of this indigenous land left out vast portions of traditional Karitiana territory in the Candeias River region, an area the group has been fighting to obtain for nearly twenty years and is awaiting an anthropological study for identification and demarcation, which is currently in progress.

At this time there are around 315 Karitiana (Sesai/2014) living in five villages. One of them, Kyõwã (New Village, or Central Village) is located within the Karitiana Indigenous Territory and has been continuously inhabited for almost 50 years. It is where most of the population's institutional structures are located: the school, clinic, FUNAI quarters, housing for non-Indian teachers, and covered meeting space, among others (this was where I conducted most of my field work between 2003 and 2012). Two other villages were established outside the indigenous territory, as part of the Karitiana's efforts to recover old parts of their traditional lands which still lay outside the territory: Byyjyty ot'soop aky ("Byyjyty's Hair") or the Candeias River Village, situated on the banks of this river and built by the traditional healer [pajé] Cizino in 2002 (today, home to approximately 30 residents), and Mywyno (the name of the Preto river), a village built by a significant leader at the northern boundary of the demarcated area within a nearby cattle ranch. A fourth small village, Bom Samaritano, was erected in 2011 about five kilometers from Aldeia Nova, and is occupied by a few families; more recently, in 2014, part of the group created a village on the banks of the Caracol River, in the far southwest of the indigenous territory. Many Karitiana also live (on a temporary or permanent basis) in cities in the region, particularly Porto Velho and Cacoal.

My work (especially Vander Velden 2012a_____. 2012a. Inquietas companhias: sobre os animais de criação entre os Karitiana. São Paulo: Alameda.) has sought to investigate the relationship between the Karitiana and the animals they raise (as they call them), which we will call domesticated animals, companion animals, or pets. These are not limited to animals which are traditionally obtained in the forest; generally, juveniles are captured after adult animals are killed in hunts, and then become domesticated (or tamed) in what is almost always forced coexistence with humans, and abound in all indigenous villages in the South American lowlands (coatis, capuchin monkeys, jacamins, macaws, and parrots are the most common among the Karitiana). Animals introduced by whites after contact also feature among these animals, and are fully incorporated into modern Karitiana everyday life (dogs, cats, horses, donkeys, oxen, pigs, chickens, ducks, and even rabbits), and are also extremely numerous in most indigenous villages throughout Brazil.2 2 I wish to note the Karitiana language, like most Amerindian languages, does not have a term for the (semantic) category animal. If a comparison must be provided, speakers offer the term himo to refer to animals. This term, however, falls far short in the scope and fixed nature of the concept of animal, since it designates prey animals for hunting, i.e. those that are or may be food (himo is the same word used for meat); consequently, only hunted animals are himo, which gives the term a contextual application. For example, if a spider monkey is killed in the forest (which we would call "wild"), it is himo, but a spider monkey raised as a pet in the village (and which will never be slaughtered) is not. As can be seen, seeing the category animal as a monolithic entity can create more problems than solutions (Derrida 2002). Therefore, in the interest of economy here I will use the suggestion by Tim Ingold (2000) which attempts to avoid dissociating humans and animals by recognizing that this dichotomous opposition does not consider the multiple modes of relationship between various human and non-human collectives.

I have been attempting to understand how the incorporation of animals taken from the forest may have orchestrated the adoption of new species of beings, and also highlighting the differences between these groups. There are important differences, especially with regards to recognizing the origin of these beings: the Karitiana say that introduced or exotic animals "have no history", unlike the native animals who have their origins explained in narratives we can describe as mythical, known as stories or stories from the old times. I do not intend to dwell here on these issues, but instead to focus on one of the aspects of this mechanism of "keeping" (perhaps rather than "capturing", I think) beings and enabling permanent coexistence between humans and non-humans in the everyday life of the Karitiana villages.

* * *

Why raise animals in the village? This issue has been addressed by some Americanist ethnologists who were encouraged by the bountiful and obvious symbolic rendering of xerimbabos (a Tupi word designating animals that live together with humans in the indigenous villages; in broad terms, it would roughly correspond to "pet" or "companion") living in the communities in which they worked. Disagreeing with previous authors who maintained that familiarized animals are raised to provide meat in societies which depend on the uncertainties of hunting, Philippe Erikson (1987ERIKSON, Philippe. 1987. "De l'apprivoisement à l'approvisionnement: chasse, alliance et familiarization en Amazonie amérindienne." Techniques et Cultures, 9 (n.s.): 105-140.; see also 1997_____. 1997. "On native american conservation and the status of Amazonian pets." Current Anthropology, 38(3): 445-446.) used extensive ethnographic evidence to argue that the indigenous lowland societies in South America adopt young animals as a way of restoring the "natural balance," using care (the feminine) to cancel out or compensate for the violent and destructive effects of the hunt (the masculine).

In a later text, Erikson (2000_____. 2000. "The social significance of pet keeping among Amazonian Indians." In: A. Podbersceck; E. S. Paul & J. Serpell (eds.), Companion animals and us. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 7-26.) retains a "psychologizing" perspective by suggesting that the adoption of animals is forced by a conceptual discomfort; in the author's words, "pets serve as an intellectual counterweight to prey" provoked by the death of a being. In this way, adoption continues to be a way of counterbalancing the effects of the hunt, understood as aggression against another in the context of the rivalries with the spirit-owners of the hunt (Erikson 2012: 16). This creates a "dishonest alliance" that keeping pets seeks to ameliorate, since this can cause danger for humans. This is criticized by Philippe Descola (1998b_____. 1998b. "Estrutura ou sentimento: a relação com o animal na Amazônia." Mana, 4(1): 23-45., 1999_____. 1999. "Des proies bienveillantes. Le traitement du gibier dans la chasse amazonienne." In: F.Héritier (org.), De la violence II. Paris: Editions Odile Jacob. pp. 19-44.), who maintains that the "discomfort" Erikson alludes to is much more characteristic of Western sensibilities with regard to taking the life of an animal, and does not do justice to the symbolic complexity of the relations between humans and animals in Amazonia. Descola (1994DESCOLA, Philippe. 1994. In the society of nature: a native ecology in Amazonia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press., 1998b_____. 1998b. "Estrutura ou sentimento: a relação com o animal na Amazônia." Mana, 4(1): 23-45.), like Patrick Menget (1988MENGET, Patrick. 1988. "Notes sur l'adoption chez les Txicáo du Brésil Central." Anthropologie et Sociétés, 12(2): 63-72.), Carlos Fausto (1999FAUSTO, Carlos. 1999. "Of enemies and pets: warfare and shamanism in Amazonia." American Ethnologist, 26(4): 933-956.), and Fernando Santos-Granero (2009SANTOS-GRANERO, Fernando. 2009. Vital enemies: slavery, predation, and the Amerindian political economy of life. Austin: University of Texas Press.), prefers approaches that seek to position the raising of wild pets3 3 I use "wild pets" to refer to animals brought from the forest and familiarized in the villages; these differ from animals that are normally called "pets", a term corresponding to species or varieties of domesticated animals that have been undergoing a process of co-evolution alongside humans for milennia. These "wild pets" approach those animals Christian Talin (2000) calls "new companion animals" (nouvelles animaux de compagnie) in the Western world which are not domesticated, strictly speaking, and mostly remain wild (iguanas, spiders, ferrets, martens, snakes, etc.). into the broader cosmological contexts, focusing the need for alterity and exteriority to reproduce the socius, reinforcing the homology between the familiarization of animals and the adoption of captured children as opposed to the analogy between hunting and cannibalistic predation (real or metaphorical) of enemies; all of this, finally, is the expression of the global position between consanguinity and affinity. The structural formula is summarized by Descola (1998b: 37):

"hunting : pet keeping: : enemies : captive children : : affines : consanguines"

All these authors assume a complementarity between "keeping pets" and hunting (like the capture of people and wartime aggression), which seems to position the entire process as a "man's subject." Taylor (2000TAYLOR, Anne Christine. 2000. "Le sexe de la proie: répresentations jivaro du lien de parenté". L'Homme, 154-155: 309-334.; 2001_____. 2001. "Wifes, pets and affines: marriage among the Jivaro". In: L. Rival & N. Whitehead (eds.). Beyond the visible and the material: the amerindianization of society in the work of Peter Rivière. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 45-56.) offers an alternative scenario by definitively incorporating women, suggesting that for the Jivaro, the analogy connects the "taming" of women through marriage and taming of forest animals; this suggestion continues by maintaining that caring for the animals is a female task since, structurally, women and pets are equivalent. In another article, this same author maintains that among the Jivaro, wild pets are "orphans," whose "protection" must be guaranteed by women in the sequence of violent events entailed in the hunt (Taylor 2000: 324). More recently, Cormier (2003CORMIER, Loretta. 2003. Kinship with monkeys: the Guajá foragers of Eastern Amazonia. New York: Columbia University Press.: 112ss), in a specific study on the pets (primarily monkeys) among the Avá-Guajá, referenced these positions, defending "the importance of raising pets in the production of feminine identities" (Cormier 2003: 113). Although she recognizes that several of the explanations posited by previous works can be applied to pets among the Guajá, this author maintains that the relationship between pets and women should be emphasized: "(...) I believe that the maternal relationship is the key to understanding pet keeping among the Guajá"; and, continuing: "(...) In their beliefs, pet keeping among the Guajá is best described as a reproduction of maternage (mothering)" (Cormier 2003: 114; maternage, sensu Haudricourt 1986HAUDRICOURT, André. 1986. "Note sur le statut familial des animaux". L'Homme 99, XXVI (3): 119-120.).

I do not intend to resolve this question here. However, the material from the Karitiana seems to indicate that the keeping of animals should be seen as a practice pertinent to the set of family relations, which involves the actions and intentions of women, men, and children: after all, speaking of children (considering the Karitiana insist that "a dog is like a child") implies talking about maternity and paternity. More than this, the incorporation of pets seems to be a subject that concerns the entire village as a whole, as a type of community policy project which may on the one hand be powered by the families that individually care for their pets, and on the other hand seems to be part of a symbolic economy that reflects the desire and an image of an entire collective.

* * *

The practice of taking in the young of various species from the forest reflects the ever-present desire also to continue incorporating introduced animals into daily life: the Karitiana always express their intention to "get another dog", "have more chickens", "raise pigs" or "raise cattle". The kept animals, therefore, always come from outside, from the exterior of the villages, whether they come from the forest or the city. Additionally, the historical narratives of contact with whites, which were mainly collected by Liliam Moser (1993MOSER, Lilian. 1993. Os Karitiana e a colonização recente em Rondônia. Porto Velho: Universiade Federal de Rondônia, monograph. and 1997_____. 1997. Os Karitiana no processo de desenvolvimento de Rondônia nas décadas de 1950 a 1990. Recife: UFPE, master's thesis.), in many cases highlight the fact that the Karitiana immediately began to ask for and to steal animals, particularly dogs and chickens, from the rubber tappers and other agents of colonization who sporadically reached the region starting in the middle of the nineteenth century and arrived on a larger scale starting in the 1940s and 1950s. There was and still is an active search for different recently-arrived animals.4 4 These must have been quite common events in the history of the initial contacts between indigenous populations and the colonizers, but we know little about these events, mostly because of the virtual inexistence of documentation. One of the few documented cases involved the group contacted in the Javari River valley (eastern Amazonas, probable the Matis) who insistently asked FUNAI employees for puppies. The following text is from the Revista de Atualidade Indígena: "Stray dogs began to be part of the relationship of the traditional gifts used to attract reluctant and isolated Indians. The sertanistas from FUNAI were surprised with strange requests for dogs when last year they made contact for the first time with an as-yet unidentified tribal group on the banks of the Ituí River in Amazonas. Since first contact the Indians showed a desire to receive more than the axes, machetes, cooking pots, and scissors that the sertanistas offered them. (...). They wanted dogs (...). In the two subsequent encounters, they again asked for more dogs, forcing the attracting team to round up the stray dogs they found in neighboring villages, and buying some (...)" (FUNAI 1978: 18). Alex Golub (2005) has reflected about the tremendous impact that the presence of dogs in the first expeditions throughout the highlands of New Guinea had on the native populations of that region, and on the way in which relations between these people and the foreign explorers were organized.

When asked why they adopt animals, the Karitiana provide several explanations. Valter, for example, told me once that "people kill the mother of the animals in the forest, then you have to get the young to bring up, they can't be left alone. They have to be brought up like a child," reasoning that evokes the suggestion by Erikson (1987ERIKSON, Philippe. 1987. "De l'apprivoisement à l'approvisionnement: chasse, alliance et familiarization en Amazonie amérindienne." Techniques et Cultures, 9 (n.s.): 105-140., 2000) that the adoption of pets serves as compensation for the assault inflicted upon animals through hunting. Nevertheless, most of the other statements I collected point in another direction, one summarized by the conclusion of a conversation with Antônio Paulo, Elivar, Meireles, and Arnaldo, in the Casa do Índio:

Women like to raise animals, it makes them happy to raise them, it is good for women. Children also like [to do it]. Men don't like it, it's difficult. That's why men get the animal from the forest to raise. People like to catch animals in the forest just to see them, to raise them.5 5 I note that here, as in other citations of statements from my Karitiana interlocutors, I chose to standardize the language.

There are two important things here. First, the desire to collect animals in the forest is driven by the women, with the men only doing it to serve them; this is consistent with countless reports from hunters who claim to raise animals only because "the woman [wife] wants them" or "child [children] asks [for them]." Second, the reason for raising animals is pleasure, "it makes them happy," "it is good for women" - which is indeed aesthetic - "just to see," which also appears in the phrase by the same Antônio Paulo, a great storyteller: "the Karitiana like to raise animals." And this pleasure is not only female, or childish.

It described macaws and parrots, but also chickens, the mare that belonged to the vice-chief John, and the rabbits that Valter had in 2006: they "adorned the village." The mere presence of the mare was said to serve as "embellishment," or to "decorate the yard" of the house. Valter, responding to my question, said that his three rabbits, which were kept in a large wood-and-mesh cage in front of the family home and fed with greens from a small vegetable garden (Valter is a small entrepreneur: he plants and sells coffee and oranges, and is also a pastor at one of the village's evangelical churches), "just serve as decoration for the house." The notion of decoration or adornment (pojatĩ) is used to refer to the body paint and adornments made of feathers used by the Karitiana, but also describes various bird feathers, which are defined as their decorations. The myth of Ombygmo chronicles how birds came to have differently colored plumage, by painting themselves with different substances from the broken body of this unfortunate man: "the bird took the colors and became different, he painted himself to decorate himself," the story concludes. In this way, painting is embellishment, and marks the origin of diversity, which in this situation is chromatic. Therefore, the enjoyment of raising animals, besides pure pleasure, also seems to be a nod to variety, which exists in the forest but can and should be reproduced in the village.

Although in some case the Karitiana may place some small collars or adornments on their dogs and cats (some improvised, like one of the cats belonging to Renato, in the city, which wears a Karitiana bracelet around its neck), they do not adorn their animals per se, in the way that the Pirahã (Gonçalves 2001GONÇALVES, Marco Antônio. 2001. O mundo inacabado: ação e criação em uma cosmologia Amazônica. Etnografia Pirahã. Rio de Janeiro: Editora da UFRJ. : 368), the Karajá (Ferreira 1983FERREIRA, Dante Martins. 1983. "Um estudo da etnozoologia Karajá: o exemplo das máscaras de Aruanã." In: B. Ribeiro et al. (eds.), O artesão tradicional e seu papel na sociedade contemporânea. Rio de Janeiro: Funarte/Instituto Nacional do Folclore. pp. 213-232.: 226),6 6 And this was also done by the groups living in villages on the Madeira and Tapajós Rivers (the Tupinambarana and Tapajó) in the eighteenth century, according to a report from the Jesuit João Daniel (apud Cypriano 2007: 125, free translation): "they greatly prize the pendants and medallions with images of the saints; but it is for the beauty of them and not for the respect and devotion they hold; and so often they use them to adorn their monkeys and puppies, tying them around their necks (...)." and the Matis (Erikson 2012_____. 2012. "Animais demais...os xerimbabos no espaço doméstico matis (Amazonas)." Anuário Antropológico, 2011/II: 15-32.) do, particularly with beads. In our case, the animals themselves are the adornments. We can remember in passing what Loretta Cormier (2003CORMIER, Loretta. 2003. Kinship with monkeys: the Guajá foragers of Eastern Amazonia. New York: Columbia University Press.: 115-116) said about the keeping of monkeys among the Guajá people as body art: "monkeys can be considered a type of body art that projects the image of fertility, and therefore of sexual attractiveness...." There, monkeys are women's ornaments, just as for the Karitiana they are ornaments for the village.

Dogs, however, do not adorn the village space- I never heard this statement apply to them. At least not at first glance.7 7 They are not adornments, but not because they are ugly, dirty or sick. Even if the Karitiana abhor the odor of the dogs, I never heard them say the dogs were ugly. Perhaps their ugliness offends the eyes of non-Indian observers, who are accustomed to the careful and detailed treatment these pets get in the contemporary world. Furthermore, according to Golub (2005: 8), "[E]uropean judgments of indigenous dogs had historically been quite negative." However, the statement remains that it is the women who are principally responsible for adopting these animals into a residence. The men, in turn, provide an additional reason to keep dogs, beyond the simple pleasure of raising them: for hunting. If dogs are like children - for both men and women (and this is always the answer when the Karitiana are asked about the relationship they have with dogs and other animals they keep) - they are also companions and helpers (cf. Vander Velden 2015_____. 2015. "Como se faz um cachorro caçador entre os Karitiana (Rondônia)." Trabalho apresentado na XI Reunión de Antropología del Mercosur. Montevideo: FHCE/UDELAR (unpublished).). Cizino Karitiana summarizes:

Dogs are just like us, because they help people, they kill animals in the hunt for people, they eat the flesh and bones. They help, and kill pigs, agoutis. That's why dogs are not eaten, they are our friends, friends [with] four feet.

It is said that the dog is a "companion in the forest," and that is why they are raised: "to scare away angry beasts in the forest; it is dangerous to walk in the forest without a dog." Besides the enjoyment of keeping dogs, they are an important helper, as Meireles points out: "dogs help us a lot, as hunters, and at night, when animals come near the house, the dog faces it, it lets us know..." Highlighting another function of the dog, as a guardian and a protector, Meireles adds: "the dog keeps a watch at night, if people come he warns us. He's company, he always accompanies us. "

Dogs do not adorn the village,8 8 Among the Pitaguary in Ceará, dogs decorate the village, especially beautiful and healthy ones; like other animals reared in homes, particularly birds in cages and certain lizards (iguanas and tegus), dogs are animaux decoratifs that beautify the home environment (Kagan 2015: 138-140). The Kaingang of Toldo Chimbangue (in Santa Catarina) refuse to eat the chickens they keep, since they "decorate the yard" (Stefanuto 2015: 41). but enter into the social universe of the Karitiana when they are adults under the guise of companionship, it can be said, of work9 9 The equines that lived in the village, a mare and two donkeys (the latter two having died in 2009), also seem (or seemed) to be subsumed in the universe of work: Epitácio told me that the donkeys were used to transport cultivated crops from the clearings and the products extracted from the forests to the village. John, who was the owner of the mare, told me that he hooked up a small wagon (which I saw alongside his house) to the animal to drive the family to more distant clearings, which I never saw; only once I witnessed Junio, the son of Irene (John's wife) saddle and mount the mare to visit a distant clearing, and the use of the horse for riding seems unusual (most of the Karitiana do not know how to ride). This infrequent "use" seems to suggest to me that equines are not (and were not) in fact considered working animals, and the everyday treatment they receive seem to indicate firstly that they are (or were) kept at home, or closely, though they were not referred to as companions or helpers. Furthermore, they "adorn the village." : as protectors, and especially as hunters, which are eminently male obligations. This is why many Karitiana men say they only appreciate dogs that know how to hunt, and even when they respond to frequent requests from their wives they often refuse animals that do not demonstrate the ability or propensity to "kill prey;" other men state they do not even enjoy the company of dogs during the hunt. But perhaps the fact that dogs do not decorate the village when they are adults, and quite to the contrary, are filthy, dirty, disorderly, and incestuous (and are associated with the Devil among evangelical believers who frequent the churches that exist in Kyõwã10 10 This was in my last year in the field; I know the religious configuration of the village has changed slightly since that time, but I do not have precise data. ) explains why that they are the only animals many Karitiana refuse to keep, or have refused to keep in recent years. Maybe it is better to state that they say they refuse to keep dogs.11 11 If there are many dogs, the Karitiana state that today there are few which are effective hunters. This would explain why many houses do not have dogs: several men stated they only liked dogs that know how to hunt, and since these are rare, they do not keep any dogs. I cannot say to what extent this shortage of hunting dogs is real, but it can be suggested that training dogs to stalk and kill prey is a domain which is increasingly restricted to older and more experienced people, and consequently it has become more difficult to obtain these specialized dogs.

The filth of dogs is a common argument: Valdomiro says he does not have dogs because he has small children at home, who are in close contact with the animals - "they [would] keep touching and biting the dog, I don't like it. When the children grow I'm going to get a dog. My wife wants to keep a dog, but I don't." Meanwhile, while Marcelo wanted to have dogs, his wife Milane did not allow it since she does not like them. Furthermore, dogs are said to be "odorous" (that is to say, smelly), and they transmit opira, a pathological condition that can be described as "a strong or bad smell" that also seems to be applied to skin problems: a dog that is pirento (a Portuguese-Karitiana pidgin term) is mangy, hairless, and smelly. Gumercindo adds another reason that is increasingly alluded to in refusing to keep dogs at home:

I don't have a dog, I just like hunting dogs to kill prey, since they help people, but just keeping [dogs] [without any utilitarian purpose?] I don't like, no, they smell [stink]. I kept hunting dogs in the past, but you feel so bad when they die, [because] dogs kill prey and help people, and dogs don't live very long either, since dangerous prey kills many dogs.

The emergence of a new sensibility (cf. Thomas 2001THOMAS, Keith. 2001. O homem e o mundo natural: mudanças de atitude em relação às plantas e aos animais (1500-1800). São Paulo: Cia. das Letras.) can be detected in this statement with regard to the suffering involved in the death of a dog, especially an animal trained for hunting. Here, much more is at stake than the loss of an efficient companion in chasing animals in the forest, coupled with a feeling of anger (pa'ira) caused by the aggression against the kept animal, which forces the hunter to "pay the dog" (that is, avenge it) by slaughtering the offending animal; anger is a disagreeable feeling in the Karitiana worldview, with strong connotations of insanity, emotional disarray, and a very strong potential for violence (see Araújo 2014ARAÚJO, Íris Morais. 2014. Osikirip: os 'especiais' Karitiana e a noção de pessoa ameríndia. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Doctoral thesis.), and so bringing the dogs to the forest has been avoided. This would explain why the refusal to have dogs seems virtually restricted to adult men; women and children never speak of not keeping dogs, and cases like Milane, who doesn't want dogs in her home, are rare.

But there is another share of sensitivity found among the men's explanations, one which women do not mention about not keeping dogs. As Antônio Paulo states:

I don't want to get another dog, because they suffer so much from hunger, they get beat up. I don't like to see dogs suffer. I don't want to, but my wife gets them anyway. It's women and children who get them, who always want to get [dogs].

Antônio José, his son, agrees:

I don't want any more dogs, I don't have any more, they all died. People have to leave the village, so the dog stays alone, hungry, nobody feeds it, it suffers a lot.

"When they are kept, they suffer a lot from being kept" is a common phrase. Antônio Paulo asserts that he doesn't like to see the animals suffer, and Cizino says that the common habit of leaving dogs alone in the village without food or care whenever families travel to the city is "ugly." This leads to acts that could read as mercy, such as the care the healer Cizino gave to a kitten that had its leg broken during a beating in the city. Actions such as these, however, are much more common among women and children, who in turn do not refuse the dogs, and seem to want more and more animals. They also suffer when dogs are killed during the hunt (especially from attacks by coatis) and when dogs are abused in the village or in the city, but this does not seem to be a reason to stop women and children from gathering them around. As Cormier (2003CORMIER, Loretta. 2003. Kinship with monkeys: the Guajá foragers of Eastern Amazonia. New York: Columbia University Press.) suggests, something in the female identity must be strongly linked to raising animals.

* * *

Therefore, as the village ornaments we have kept animals that serve the aesthetics of everyone: this is also noted by Stephen Hugh-Jones (apud Serpell 1996SERPELL, James. 1996. In the company of animals: a study of human-animal relationships. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press .: 64) regarding the Barasana, among whom caring for pets is primarily leisure:

These people simply take advantage of the day to care for and feed the animals they raise. The animals are a strong source of discussion and entertainment, and are considered an integral part of the community.

Long before the British anthropologist, the French missionary Charles de Rochefort, who lived among the Karib in the Lesser Antilles, described them as:

Great Lovers of divertissements and recreation...[T]o that purpose they take a pleasure in keeping and teaching a great number of Parrots and Paraquitos (cited in Norton 2013NORTON, Marcy. 2013. "Going to the birds: animals as things and beings in early modernity." In: Paula Findlen (ed.), Early modern things: objects and their histories, 1500-1800. London: Routledge . pp. 53-83.: 66, author's translation).

Further reflection on the aesthetic dimension of raising animals is in order, including how to connect the dogs to the other kept animals which are said to decorate the village. The importance among Amazonian lowland peoples of the creation or ownership of beauty as one of the fundamental attributes of humanity, to be pursued actively and daily by individuals, is indeed well known: the ability to produce beautiful things is a crystallization of the manifestation of human creativity, knowledge, and productivity, and of the pleasure and joy involved in daily activities. Beauty, care, attention to detail, and perfection in the arts of doing and the art of the everyday life (Overing 1999_____. 1999. "Elogio do cotidiano: a confiança e a arte da vida social em uma comunidade amazônica." Mana, 5(1): 81-107.) are always sought, because they are indicators of the agency, and productive capacities of people: in short, they are signs of joy, harmony, and continuous production of social behavior itself (Overing 1991 and 1999; Overing & Passes 2000; van Velthem 1998VAN VELTHEM, Lúcia H.. 1998. A pele de Tuluperê: uma etnografia dos trançados Wayana. Belém: Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi/Funtec., 2003_____. 2003. O belo é a fera: a estética da produção e da predação entre os Wayana. Lisboa: Museu Nacional de Etnologia/Assírio & Alvim.; Lagrou 2007LAGROU, Els. 2007. A fluidez da forma: arte, alteridade e agência em uma sociedade amazônica (Kaxinawa, Acre). Rio de Janeiro: Topbooks/CAPES/PPGSA-UFRJ., 2009, Santos-Granero & Mentore 2008SANTOS-GRANERO, Fernando; MENTORE, George (eds.). 2008. "Special issue in honor of Prof. Joanna Overing. In the world and about the world: Amerindian modes of knowledge." Revista de Antropologia, 49 (1): 11-16.).

Can the arts of domestication be considered to produce beauty? Aristóteles Barcelos Neto (2002BARCELOS NETO, Aristóteles. 2002. A arte dos sonhos: uma iconografia ameríndia. Lisboa; Museu Nacional de Etnologia/Assírio & Alvim.), in his discussion of the relationship between the Wauja people and hyperbolic beings called apapaatai, makes some interesting contributions in this regard. According to this author, the images (masks and drawings on paper) of the apapaatai awaken "intense emotion" of an aesthetic nature in the Wauja, which is founded on the fact that closeness to these beings is found under the signs of "enmity/friendship, danger, mistrust, and forced reciprocity." He continues:

Among the Wauja, the scale of the monstrosity of the apapaatai ​​becomes 'domesticated' through art, which provides a less-'dangerous' contact with the 'supernatural' beings. In this case, aesthetic pleasure is associated exactly with the pleasure of a controlled approach (...). This is because the aesthetic pleasure awakened by the representations of the apapaatai could be the pleasure of a virtual 'domestication' of monstrosity (...) (Barcelos Neto 2002: 175-176, my translation).

Domestication, by reducing the harmful powers of the apapaatai through iconographic fixation, is a process that provides aesthetic pleasure: joy and beauty contribute to the domestication of monstrosity, and are the main fruits of this activity (Barcelos Neto 2006: 305-306_____. 2006. "De divinações xamânicas e acusações de feitiçaria: imagens Wauja da agência letal." Mana, 12(2): 285-313.). Is the same not true with the familiarization/domestication of animals, transforming the uncultivated and unsociable young animal taken from the forest, or the unknown animal from the city, into a fully social being that pleases individuals because of its agentive capacities and the colorful, beautiful, and pleasant variety that its presence lends the villages? Els Lagrou (2009_____. 2009. Arte indígena no Brasil. Belo Horizonte: Editora C/Arte.: 56, my translation, emphasis added) also considers a similar possibility:

Obtaining and preparing materials from the outside into materials that constitute the very group identity follows the same logic, whether this is the incorporation of people, of the agentive qualities or abilities of people (soul, singing, name), or of objects [And, I would also add, of non-human beings or persons]. These elements that are won from -or negotiated from- outside must be pacified, familiarized. This process of transforming what is external into something internal has eminently aesthetic characteristics.

While the dogs themselves may not exactly adorn the village (because of their characteristics associated with uncleanliness and certain antisocial habits), can we not say that they contribute to creating harmony and joy in the community, which in turn does make them lovely, by demonstrating the human creativity and productivity seen in the transformation from puppies to companion dogs? After all, the "wise dogs" ("cachorro sabido", in Portuguese), a term used to refer to certain animals that are good hunters or show a particular appreciation for human company, and have learned to demonstrate these tendencies especially through observing behavioral ethics (such as not stealing food or entering the homes of others, for example) are very much appreciated by the Karitiana.

The act of domestication,12 12 Here I use the term domestication in the broad sense, borrowing the term from the ethnologists who discuss art as the domestication of potentially dangerous beings and agencies in the villages (their miniaturization, one might say, to human scales; with regard to miniaturization or smaller-scale models as a form of ownership, see Levi-Strauss 1997 [1962]: chapter 2). Being faithful to the technical terminology, I obviously am referring to the familiarization (taming) of kept animals and not their domestication, which is generally held to not be present in the South American lowlands (Descola 1998a); in any case, recent reflections (Cassidy & Mullin 2007) have urged us to rethink the current notions of domestication to approach cases of closeness that are not usually considered as such (for example, marine fish farms, "wild domestication," eating together, mutualism, etc.) in an effort to reconsider human-animal relations as a whole. In a certain sense, even these ethnologists with whom I dispute should employ the idea of art as familiarization (or taming), since the set of relationships with these beings/agencies is a permanent process and refers to specific individuals, and not the "species" of beings or powers. Still, perhaps the opposition between domestication and familiarization/taming even may lose its meaning, when domestication is understood not as a finished process, but as an ongoing and continuous effort to create the interspecies bond (Haraway 2008; Fijn 2011). Similarly, captives (as well as the animals taken from the forest) gradually are familiarized by their captors when they are captured (Fausto 1999): there are no "domesticated peoples" (as a state or condition), so to speak, although a recent study by Fernando Santos-Granero (2009) suggests the contrary. therefore, seems to be beautiful in itself and ends up creating more beauty: a village populated by different species living in harmony, highlighting the humans' ability to produce variety, harmony, peace, and friendliness. In his ethnography of the forms of constituting subjects and subjectivities from subjugation (which is required for full and satisfactory coexistence) among the Urarina people in Peru, Harry Walker (2013WALKER, Harry. 2013. Under a watchful eye: self, power, and intimacy in Amazonia. Berkeley: University of California Press .: 192-195) shows that the most extreme form of shamanistic control among this people of the Peruvian Amazon is the domestication of small bowl-shaped stones called egaando, which are powerful beings because they hold many small darts used in mystical attacks. The egaando can be familiarized and thus subjugated by the shamans - placing them "at their service," so to speak - in a laborious process that the author describes as "where practices of taming and raising reach their highest level as an art form" (Walker 2013: 192, emphasis mine), although the author does not develop this comparison. Familiarization is the art of making companions, human and non-human, artifacts and animals: after all, "pets are nothing if not good company" (Walker 2013: 208).

If "the beauty is the beast" (cf. Van Velthen 2003_____. 2003. O belo é a fera: a estética da produção e da predação entre os Wayana. Lisboa: Museu Nacional de Etnologia/Assírio & Alvim.), beauty can also be the art of getting ferocious beings used to living together, and considering their diversity, making a certain space decorated and consequently beautiful. Since the notions of beautiful and aesthetically pleasing are a fertile field with an infinite variety of fruit (cf. Descola 1996_____. 1996. "Constructing natures: symbolic ecology and social practice." In: P. Descola & G. Pálsson (editors), Nature and society: anthropological perspectives. London: Routledge. pp. 82-102.: 166-167), beauty also resides in a village filled with beings of different natures living in harmony: an additional aspect of what Barcelos Neto (2002: 263, emphasis in the original) called "animals as art."

* * *

Therefore, the dogs also adorn the villages in their own way: as dedicated workers and efficient companions. Throughout Amazonia, their training depends on a set of technical operations (cf. Vander Velden 2012a_____. 2012a. Inquietas companhias: sobre os animais de criação entre os Karitiana. São Paulo: Alameda. and 2015_____. 2015. "Como se faz um cachorro caçador entre os Karitiana (Rondônia)." Trabalho apresentado na XI Reunión de Antropología del Mercosur. Montevideo: FHCE/UDELAR (unpublished).). But there is another important dimension to the position of these beings. As companions, in the sense of helpers, dogs are restricted to the male domain, defined by the hunter's activity; but for the women, they continue to be something else, as the Karitiana say, they are "like children." Not only dogs, but kept animals in general are defined by the Karitiana as being "like children." In this sense, the Karitiana resemble the Matis according to Philippe Erikson (2012_____. 2012. "Animais demais...os xerimbabos no espaço doméstico matis (Amazonas)." Anuário Antropológico, 2011/II: 15-32.: 29), for whom "the purpose of familiarization is precisely to make 'children.'" Taking animals from the forest or the city, therefore, obeys other reasons besides just the aesthetic pleasure of taming/domesticating and creating friendship. There are other dimensions at play, such as membership, companionship (in the case of hunting dogs), and even wealth, particularly in the case of livestock, although this can be extended to other species which may eventually serve a consuming market outside the village, whether as pets, crafts that use feathers, teeth, claws, skins, bones, or other fragments of animal bodies which are found in abundance.

However, it seems to me that all these reasons to collect more and more animals are connected to the theme of decoration and the production of beauty. Indeed, the relationship of companionship and mutual help (if we can use this term, cf. Lestel 2007LESTEL, Dominique. 2007. Les amis de mes amis. Paris: Edition du Seuil.) between humans and dogs reflects the good relations that fully-fledged humans must maintain between each other for the continuing production of peaceful and productive conviviality, which in itself is also beautiful for this reason (Overing 1991OVERING, Joanna. 1991. "A estética da produção: o senso de comunidade entre os Cubeo e os Piaroa." Revista de Antropologia, 34: 7-33. ). Similarly, the demand for kept animals as wealth, which is evident in the case of the Karitiana's desire to introduce cattle raising into the villages, as I have written elsewhere (Vander Velden 2011_____. 2011. "Inveja do gado: o fazendeiro como figura de poder e desejo entre os Karitiana." Anuário Antropológico, 2010-1: 55-76.), also operates in the sense of the production of beauty, since in the Karitiana view wealth (as it is seen in the farmers who own herds of cattle within the indigenous territory) is essential for the production of health, and in turn is expressed in the beauty of perfectly functioning healthy bodies and high community morale (as I showed in Vander Velden 2004VANDER VELDEN, Felipe. 2004. Por onde o sangue circula: os Karitiana e a intervenção biomédica. Campinas: Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Master's thesis.).

With respect to the association between kept animals and children - which is expressed in phrases like "dogs are like children" and detected in various daily practices and rituals among the Karitiana (described in Vander Velden 2012a_____. 2012a. Inquietas companhias: sobre os animais de criação entre os Karitiana. São Paulo: Alameda.) - the subject of adornment and beauty is revealed with even greater clarity. This is because the children of the Karitiana are considered to adorn the village: a beautiful community is a community full of children. The village of Kyõwã is called "child village" (one of the possible translations, since õwã translates as "child" in Karitiana) not only because it is a new village (or it was, when it was named), but because it is a space with many children: it is even said that the name stems from the fact that the village "is as beautiful as a child's smile.". It is no mistake to say that considering the abundance of young inhabitants as a way to beautify a village arises in good part from a spectacular13 13 The Karitiana numbered only 45 in the late 1960s; today they are more than 350 in number, having increased almost seven-fold over the course of 40 years. demographic recovery, of which the Karitiana boast: consequently, the numerous children show victory in the fight against extinction and the persistence of an entire people and their culture. This topic certainly has not escaped the notice of other indigenous peoples, who mention how beautiful villages full of children are, precisely because along with the children comes population growth (cf. Cohn 2000COHN, Clarice. 2000. A criança indígena: a concepção Xikrin de infância e aprendizado. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Master's dissertation.: 13; Müller 2002MÜLLER, Regina Polo. 2002. "As crianças no processo de recuperação demográfica dos Asurini do Xingu." In: A. Lopes da Silva; A. V. S. Macedo & A. Nunes (orgs.), Crianças indígenas: ensaios antropológicos. São Paulo: Global/MARI/Fapesp. pp. 188-209).). "Children represent joy [...], a house without children is a sad house," claim the Iny-Karajá, according to Sofia Scartezini (2015SCARTEZINI, Sofia. 2015. Crianças Iny: entre o kube e a escola. Brasília: DAN/UnB, course monograph (unpublished).: 36).

The bond between animals and children becomes even closer when we realize that it is particularly the offspring of animals, whether these are captured in the forest or brought from town, that effectively receive care and affection from people, especially the women and children. Baby birds and dogs are kept in straw baskets and fed by hand, are set on laps, and like human children sleep in the company of their owners; the young mammals collected in the forest tend to be more reluctant and are kept in cages or on chains, but at least in the Karitiana view this is a far cry from abuse or neglect. Just like human children, the young of introduced domestic animals (such as puppies and chicks) - and the wild pets, which remain "eternal children" (Anderson, 2003ANDERSON, Patricia. 2003. "A bird in the house: an anthropological perspective on companion parrots." Society & Animals, 11(4): 393-418.) for their entire lives - certainly help to adorn the villages. When they become adults, however, the treatment of these beings changes appreciably. In most cases they are treated indifferently, as in the case of chickens; monkeys and coatis can be beaten for their antics, and macaws and parrots are only sought out when their feathers are needed for crafts, although the Karitiana enjoy playing with parakeets and parrots, which always seem to seek out human companionship. They are never eaten, however, and only rarely are returned to the wild, even if they become troublesome or aggressive.14 14 Returning kept animals to the wild was recorded among other groups such as the Awá-Guajá (Uirá Garcia, personal communication) and the Wayapi (Igor Scaramuzzi, personal communication), but not among the Karitiana.

None of these changes, however, is as radical as what occurs with dogs. Although they are loved and pampered when puppies, adult dogs suffer from hunger, abandonment, illness, and unreasonable violence, particularly when they do not demonstrate an aptitude for hunting (anyone who has ever spent time among Indians is familiar with the notorious "village dogs"). In a previous work (Vander Velden 2012a_____. 2012a. Inquietas companhias: sobre os animais de criação entre os Karitiana. São Paulo: Alameda.), I suggested that this transformation in treatment is the result not only of associations between dogs and the devil (Kida hu~j hu~j), but particularly the perception of their life cycle, which is true to a greater or lesser extent for all animals that coexist with humans. Like children, puppies require permanent care and attention; fully-grown adults need to show the ability to care for themselves, control themselves, be productive, and share responsibility for comprising the community; as the Karitiana say, they have to know how to "take care of themselves."

The dog, which is an uncomfortable hinge between the worlds of nature and culture (as discussed in Lévi-Strauss 1997 [1962]LÉVI-STRAUSS, Claude. 1997 [1962]. O pensamento selvagem. Campinas: Papirus.), subverts this need at every turn: with their pernicious habits of stealing food, attacking people, eating feces, and copulate with kin, dogs evoke the fragility of the human condition, trapped in the arduous task of producing beauty in a universe where violence, disorder, disease, and death swirl about, the "everyday tragedies" that nevertheless are a necessary part of life in the forest (Kohn 2013KOHN, Eduardo. 2013. How forests think: toward an anthropology beyond the human. Berkeley: University of California Press.). And let us not forget that dogs are jaguars, even if they are domesticated. Their name in the domestic indigenous language, obaky by'edna, translates as "kept jaguars" or "house jaguars." The danger of the great Amazonian predator is concealed in each and every dog, but keeping them controlled and making them polite and useful are practices that illustrate human mastery in domesticating the wild powers of the non-human universe. Some dose of violence will doubtless be involved, which is why dogs are beaten: to be reminded of their duties as adults, as adult (not human) people who must be productive and controlled. In the polished words of Herbert Baldus, as he observed the way in which the Tapirapé treated their dogs: "after their childhood full of pampering, life becomes canine for the dog" (Baldus 1970: 183BALDUS, Herbert. 1970. Tapirapé, tribo Tupí no Brasil Central. São Paulo: Cia. Editora Nacional.; my translation, emphasis added).

* * *

The domestication and even familiarization of animals has been addressed in most works as a technique/technology or a set of techniques (cf. Clutton-Brock 1990CLUTTON-BROCK, J. (ed.). 1990. The walking larder: patterns of domestication, pastorialism, and predation. London: Routledge.) rather than an art. Some authors have dedicated themselves to defending practices of dealing with domestic species as an art - notably those related to controlling animals which are intended to perform more complex tasks like training for shows or competition, such as equines (Patton 2003PATTON, Paul. 2003. "Language, power, and the training of horses." In: C. Wolfe (ed.), Zoontologies: the question of the animal. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press . pp. 83-99.; Cassidy 2007CASSIDY, Rebecca. 2007. Horse people: thoroughbred culture in Lexington and Newmarket. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.) and dogs (Haraway 2008HARAWAY, Donna. 2008. When species meet. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.). Nevertheless, raising animal species or individuals in the company of human beings and groups - whether they are domesticated or familiarized - is generally understood to be a set of techniques or technologies designed to conquer the "characteristics" of the animal (Pavão 2015PAVÃO, Luna Castro. 2015. 'O que é que cavalo sabe': um estudo antropológico sobre o vínculo animal-humano na equoterapia. São Carlos: Universidade Federal de São Carlos, Master's thesis.), which are epitomized in the animal's refusal to obey human designs, a refusal of the technique itself, which is understood as a human/cultural predicate as opposed to the nature of the animal, and the animal's desire to enjoy its natural freedom far from humans.

Maybe this equation relates to the way that in the modern West we consider the relationship between art and technique, which are understood to be opposing and contradictory terms (Benjamin 1985BENJAMIN, Walter. 1985. Magia e técnica, arte e política - obras escolhidas, vol. I. São Paulo: Brasiliense.): in this sense, domestication, with its usual association with themes of power, control, and domination (cf. Patton 2003PATTON, Paul. 2003. "Language, power, and the training of horses." In: C. Wolfe (ed.), Zoontologies: the question of the animal. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press . pp. 83-99.; Cassidy & Mullin 2007CASSIDY, Rebecca & MULLIN, Molly (eds.). 2007. Where the wild things are now: domestication reconsidered. Oxford: Berg.), cannot be associated with art and its traditional ties with freedom, autonomy, and emancipation. However, the domestication (or familiarization) of animals considered as a form of art is not foreign at certain times in the history of cohabitation between humans and non-humans in the modern West.

In her study of exotic animals in eighteenth-century France, Louise Robbins (2002ROBBINS, Louise. 2002. Elephant slaves and pampered parrots: exotic animals in eighteenth-century Paris. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press .) looks at menageries, the precursors of zoological institutions, a type of "living curio cabinet" designed to accommodate and acclimate foreign species which were brought to Europe in increasing numbers by travelers and colonial merchants starting in the fifteenth century. In fact, Robbins argues that menageries, besides being expressions of royal and colonial power - and the animals, besides being appreciated in themselves as ornaments for their beauty, exotic nature, or curious habits and morphological characteristics (Robbins 2002: 26; 44) - also expressed the human ability to control and teach animals, since different species were kept in the same environment and cages were only used sporadically (Robbins 2002: 79). In this way, the French kings, as well as the nobility, large-scale animal dealers, naturalists, and even ordinary people could express power and wisdom in managing the variety (going far beyond exotic) that lived at liberty in the spaces which were generally built especially to house this type of fauna.15 15 Consequently, the menageries of French kings resembled the collections (which included living exotic animals) assembled by Chinese emperors in palaces which were constructed especially for this use; Marshall Sahlins notes that "[T]he synthesis of diversity and conquest made these imperial retreats [the palaces] perfect microcosms. They represented the whole world as the work of the Emperor and within his power" (Sahlins 1988: 22). It is no wonder that there were many attempts (theoretical and practical) to domesticate these new species brought to Europe in order to broaden the menu of species which were subjected to peaceful coexistence with human society (Robbins 2002: 29-31; 35-36; 193-198).

In a brilliant but little-known article, Alfred Gell (1988GELL, Alfred. 1988. "Technology and magic." Anthropology Today, 4(2): 6-9. ) argues against the separation of techniques/technologies and arts, treating the latter as modalities of technologies which he calls technologies of enchantment. Gell (1988: 7), who in this article is also interested in the relationship between humans and animals, suggests a link between domesticating animals and what he calls technologies of reproduction, namely kinship: we, humans, domesticate animals because we first domesticated ourselves. The same techniques that we use to make humans (kinship and marriage) were later employed to domesticate animals. Nevertheless, the technologies of enchantment as arts can also be considered fundamental in the process of domestication, since they act on the passions and work "to enchant the other person[s] and cause [them] to perceive social reality in a way favorable to the social interests of the enchanter" (Gell 1988: 7); the same workings that apply in the "domestication" (or, in other words, enchantment) of human minds through the arts can apply to the domestication of non-human minds.16 16 The Pied Piper of Hamelin from the European folk tale may be an extreme example of this analogy. Art and technique are not opposites, after all, and if domestication is a technique, the arts also are: the opposition between technique and magic (which is in all technology as its "ideal" form) would lack basis (Gell 1988: 6) and the author's intent is to indicate that the technology of enchantment exists and that "[...] it has to be considered, not as a separate province, i.e.. 'Art' - opposed to technology - but as a technology in itself" (Gell 1988: 7).17 17 For Alfred Gell, all art is a technology for capturing, captivating, and holding the viewer and keeping him or her under control. The classic example of the Zande hunting trap, which is discussed by Gell (1996), points in this direction: the indiscernibility between technical objects and artistic objects in the modes of capturing random agencies. Is it possible that domestication is exactly that: the capture of random agencies?

More recently, Tim Ingold (2000INGOLD, Tim. 2000. The perception of the environment: essays in livelihood, dwelling and skill. London: Routledge .) has argued that the modern split between art and technique was unknown in Greek and Roman cultures, where both comprised the same skill craftsmen employed to manufacture durable objects. The separation between art/aesthetic and technique/technology in the contemporary world, according to Ingold, results from the opposition between mind and body that characterizes Western thinking, and is developed in other complex dichotomies such as work/signification, mechanical/semiotic, sensible/intelligible, and so on. Nevertheless, Feeley-Harnik's study (2007FEELEY-HARNIK, Gillian. 2007. "'An experiment on a giant scale': Darwin and the domestication of pigeons." In: R. Cassidy & M. Mullin (eds.), Where the wild things are now: domestication reconsidered. Oxford: Berg. pp. 147-182.) of pigeon breeders in eighteenth-century England suggests the mutual implication between art and technique in the relationship between weavers and birds, as if both activities - which were simultaneously artistic and technical - were considered under the same symbolic key:

The weavers' birds and plants alike were clearly tied to artisanal ideals. They were an aesthetic expression of ideals of craftsmanship associated with silk weaving (...)" (Feeley-Harnik 2007: 165).

And the idea of design in the sense of planning or redesigning the living beings, according to Feeley-Harnik (2007: 174), drove the London weavers' passion for pigeons. Nevertheless, one could argue that the aesthetic pleasure offered by the pigeons was found not only in the development of new varieties, but also in the creation and maintenance of living spaces and domesticity, since the birds not only lived inside but were ideally kept separate from the "wild" varieties of pigeons (Columba livia). The intense pleasure in companionship between humans and non-humans is simultaneously associated with both technology/technique and art/aesthetics in nineteenth century Britain: beauty and technique in breeding animals, and Lévi-Strauss (1997 [1962]) seems to have been correct when he considered the permanence of the savage mind in certain niches of the modern world, most notably among those who live intimately with animals at work, such as circus performers or zookeepers.

Furthermore, this rapprochement between animals and objects does not seem foreign in the Amazonian landscape in general, and to the Karitiana in particular. In fact, the mythology of this group is lavish in what I have called artefactual animals: creatures that were made by demiurge-creators from objects, or from a combination of inert material things. Jaguars were carved from cedarwood, and agoutis and peccaries were made from the shells of Brazils and termite mounds; Ora, the mischievous and evil brother of the creator Botyj, made the aquatic creatures from trunks, leaves, and branches that he found floating in a river. He also made woodpeckers by sticking a stone axe in the mouths of a group of men, and the deer by putting a broken bow into the head of another, while the anacondas came from a painted piece of wood. There was, therefore, an art in the creation or manufacture of the beings that inhabit the forest today, with many of them made from material objects, some of which were worked, decorated, or combined, while others were simply available to the creators to be converted into living creatures. Therefore, from the beginning animals have been manufactured, made, like the range of artifacts made by humans; what the Karitiana do today is manufacture animals, from the art of adorning their villages and through raising/socializing/domesticating these various animals that they bring to their villages and make efforts to live with.

* * *

Reconsidering domestication from the notion of sharing meals, coevolution, and companionship (Haraway 2008HARAWAY, Donna. 2008. When species meet. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.; Cassidy & Mullin 2007CASSIDY, Rebecca & MULLIN, Molly (eds.). 2007. Where the wild things are now: domestication reconsidered. Oxford: Berg.) - and moving away from ideas that allude to power, domination, and control - can help us understand domestication as an art. Among us today we consider that there is an "art of living" or that "living together is an art," and a difficult, laborious, and risky art at that. Haraway (2008: 205-246) highlights the authentic and intense aesthetic pleasure involved in living with dogs (and other animals), but develops a strong warning in the way in which aesthetic categories (intrinsically linked to categories of originality, prestige, rarity, and purity) shape contemporary practices of breeding to develop or improve domesticated animal breeds, especially canines (Haraway 2008: 95-132).

I should warn here that I am specifically addressing domestication as an art as well as a technique, since I maintain that both are not in opposition: from the process of raising and overseeing coexistence between humans and animals (and among different animals) as an activity directed towards the search for aesthetic pleasure.18 18 Bechelany (2012) argues that Indian hunting practices are simultaneously signified as both technique and aesthetic, art and technology, creativity and repetition, freedom and determination; in this way, efficiency and beauty cannot be separated in the activities of Amazonian hunters, nor the precision of pure enjoyment, the economy of profit and expenditure. Uirá Garcia (2010) suggests a "poetics of predation" by advocating a similar idea among the Awá-Guajá. If hunting can be both technique and art, is there a reason why domestication/familiarization - obviously, for different reasons - cannot also be? It is interesting that Henry Walter Bates asked "what arts the old woman used" (quoted in Norton 2013: 68, emphasis mine) when referring to "an old Indian woman's" success in taming what the naturalist described as an "intractable green parrot." And not so much from the animals as "objects" of art in themselves, whose beauty alone is able to abduct agencies (according to Gell 1998_____. 1998. Art and agency: an anthropological theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.) and power networks of actors or to produce aesthetic pleasure by its mere presence. Consequently, I am not especially concerned with certain domestic species valued for their intrinsic beauty, for example, which becomes an important (and sometimes definitive) criterion in the reproductive selection of individuals and ultimately guides the very activity that brings together humans and animals, such as professional breeding of purebred dogs and horses, champion bulls and ornamental birds and fish (cf. Cassidy 2007CASSIDY, Rebecca & MULLIN, Molly (eds.). 2007. Where the wild things are now: domestication reconsidered. Oxford: Berg., on horses; Anderson 2003ANDERSON, Patricia. 2003. "A bird in the house: an anthropological perspective on companion parrots." Society & Animals, 11(4): 393-418.: 406-408, on birds; Leal 2014LEAL, Natacha. 2014. Nomes aos bois: zebus e zebuzeiros em uma pecuária brasileira de elite. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Doctoral thesis., on bulls), beyond of course the consumption of these species with the goal of ornamenting homes and collections.19 19 Not to mention, of course, that many of these animals are further embellished by their owners or keepers with special collars and even clothes that mimic human clothing. The practice of decorating family pets has been discussed above. I also do not address beauty as a criterion behind the illegal trafficking of wild animals, where the relationships between the search for rare and beautiful species20 20 And here, exoticism and exteriority play a central role and are, in a sense, near the interest of the Karitiana through the integration of non-human beings into their daily lives, whether these animals are brought from the forest, or incorporated in the world of the whites and brought from the cities. Of course the Karitiana, like other Amazonian indigenous peoples, appreciate various animals for their beauty; this is particularly the case in birds, whose feathers are essential for making the most beautiful indigenous artifacts, featherwork, which are highly valued inside as well as outside the villages (Reina & Kensinger 1991). When dressed in their headdresses and feathered gauntlets the Indians often describe themselves as "beautiful," and bird feathers are described in literature as being native "jewels," objects that are actively sought, have great value, and deserve special care such as storage in special containers (cf. Giannini 1991; Vander Velden 2012b). For quite some time, bird feathers were appreciated in Western women's fashion like jewels or body decorations (Schindler 2001; Kirsch, 2006 on bird-of-paradise feathers). Peter Beysen (2013: 226-227, n. 4) constructs intriguing suggestions around the equivalents (via patterns on the skin) made between snakes and jewels or decorations of the female body in Ashaninka thought and in Western painting. and the language of mining or mineral prospecting leads to the capture of birds, snakes, and insects with coloring like jewels or ornaments.21 21 This association between beauty and wealth does not escape the indigenous peoples: Hans Staden (1999 [1557]: 101; 113) stated in the sixteenth century that the brilliant feathers of various birds which were jealously guarded by the Tupi in the coastal Portuguese Americas were their "riches": "their treasures are feathers. He who has many of them is rich (...)." It is very interesting that André Thevet at a certain point viewed sixteenth-century Tupi society through the eyes of European royalty, which is why Cunhambebe's feather headdress becomes his crown (Mason 1994: 10). Nor am I interested in the form of contemporary art called bioart that uses animals (and plants) to produce aesthetic impact, and in many cases borders on cruelty and the exploitation of sentient and non-sentient beings (Kirksey & Helmreich 2010KIRKSEY, S. Eben; HELMREICH, Stefan. 2010. "The emergence of multispecies ethnography". Cultural Anthropology, 25(4): 545-576.; Kac 2013KAC, Eduardo. 2013. Telepresença e bioarte: humanos, coelhos e robôs em rede. São Paulo: Edusp.).

These cases point to a difference (although certainly not an incompatibility or a mutual exclusivity) between the Karitiana and many but certainly not all non-Indians in the criteria that define beauty and pleasure in the relationship between humans and animals, and a definition of animals as adornment. Indeed, we have seen that the Karitiana collect all kinds of animals that can become accustomed to living with humans in order to decorate their villages. In other words, beauty also seems to lie in diversity and in the very art of domestication/taming, and in raising and living together with these numerous non-human children (as well as the human ones) in the villages, decorating these and making them beautiful. The Karitiana report various attempts to tame different species of animals, from tayras, tapirs, and bush dogs to snakes and electric eels; there seems to be a taste for and a certain enjoyment that is simply derived from the presence of these beings in human spaces from the processes of getting them accustomed to a multi-species coexistence. In any case, I think that the exploration by Joanna Overing (1999_____. 1999. "Elogio do cotidiano: a confiança e a arte da vida social em uma comunidade amazônica." Mana, 5(1): 81-107.) into what is called the art of the everyday maintains that this does not imply opposition of the individual and the collective: thus, the animal is an ornament in itself, in its body (the colorful feathers of birds, for example) and in the aesthetically pleasing effect produced by taming/domestication and harmonious coexistence. In the same way, if caring for children (watching over them, feeding them, pampering them) is among the most important arts of social living, since it produces full-fledged people (cf. McCallum 2001MCCALLUM, Cecilia. 2001. How real people are made: gender and sociality in Amazonia. Oxford: Berg .), the same can be said of kept animals, which after all are produced by similar processes or arts.

Among non-Indians, in many contexts there is also a search for ornaments (ornamental fish and birds, purebred dogs, cats, and horses, for example) that seems to be at play,22 22 This common search for the ornamental can be further evidence for the thesis that the acclimatization of animals to human company meets a need which is specifically human (Serpell 1996). Note that in the contemporary West the search for diversity appears in what are known as "new pets" (Talinn 2000): iguanas, pythons, amphibians, spiders, rats, pigs, foxes, etc. Perhaps the diffusion of this habit (along with the constant discussions and experiences of domesticating various new animal species and getting others used to captivity) is related to the ideas of bringing increasingly diverse beings into coexistence with humans. among other factors. But for us the ornamental is closely linked to plastic beauty (and to the other symbolic characteristics associated with the different animals that are investigated: strength, vitality, strangeness, docility, and so forth), as well as rarity and exoticism. The most valuable and expensive species (or their by-products) are those which are the rarest, most endangered, most difficult to find and capture (cf. RENCTAS 2001RENCTAS. 2001. 1o. Relatório Nacional sobre o Tráfico de Animais Silvestres. Brasília: RENCTAS.). Here, beauty does not lie in variety and conviviality, but in exoticism and rarity.23 23 According to RENCTAS (2001), the feathers of some birds are artificially colored to more closely resemble more expensive and more valuable species in the market for wild animals. This is cruel logic that yet again emulates the indigenous practice of tapiragem, in which technical processes that today still remain obscure are applied to the plucked bodies of live birds so their feathers grow back in different colors. This also emulates the sixteenth-century Tupi practice recorded by Jean de Léry of coloring white chicken feathers with turmeric to make them resemble the feathers of the scarlet ibis, which were quite appreciated by these Indians and used to make their main feather adornments (see Vander Velden 2012b). All these procedures change the color of the plumage; in one of them, however, the goal is to produce different feathers, while in the other the goal is to produce a different animal. Many origin myths addressing the variety of birds (including those of the Karitiana) focus on the feathers acquiring their coloration from various substances. So would painting animals be so strange to the Indians after all? Would creating diversity be strange?

In conclusion, it is possible to understand how for the Karitiana, a mistreated, skinny, and dirty dog can be understood to adorn the village. We are not discussing a specific individual animal that is used as an ornament, but all of them together, all the beings that cohabit the villages. I do not wish to say that a miserable, mangy and emaciated dog itself adorns the village, although this is not impossible, since aesthetic appreciation (of what is beautiful) may not be universally or transculturally identical (Overing 2001_____. 2001. "Against the motion (1) - 1993 debate: aesthetics is a cross-cultural category." In: T. Ingold (ed.), Key debates in anthropology. London: Routledge. pp. 260-266.; also Overing 1991, about the Piaroa, for whom beauty is evaluated morally). However, I maintain that what is beautiful and adorns the village is the diversity of beings, and above all the beauty involved in the familiarization process itself: beauty is producing coexistence, beauty is producing kinship, beauty is producing domesticity from the wild. Beauty and enjoyment. Beauty lies in having a village full of different beings living in peace and harmony. Fellowship can be a source of aesthetic pleasure, as well as power and joy (cf. Walker 2013WALKER, Harry. 2013. Under a watchful eye: self, power, and intimacy in Amazonia. Berkeley: University of California Press .).24 24 The notion of "ecological aesthetics" discussed by Nading (2012) from the suggestions of Gregory Bateson can be usefully applied here in exploring the aesthetic pleasure we extract from our daily "entanglement" with the creatures who live with us and share our world, in the continuous "becoming" that is life in flux (Ingold 2011). Ecological aesthetics (different from environmental aesthetics, which is more concerned with ordering and control of the natural world) "privileges a relational knowledge of life" (Nading 2012: 577) and invests in the marvel and the beauty that we derive from our relationship with the world and with others. These ideas can also illuminate a reflection on domestication/familiarization as an art or source of aesthetic fruition. Along these lines, in a future work I hope to test Michel Foucault's ideas about the "aesthetics of existence," which consist of considering the changes themselves in thought knowledge about experiences. Could this then involve using knowledge to modify other species? Does the consideration (also by Alfred Gell) of domestication as being preceded by self-domestication - man taming himself before domesticating animals and plants (Leach 2003) - not address precisely this? As Anna Tsing states (2005TSING, Anna. 2005. Friction: an ethnography of global connection. Princeton: Princeton University Press.: 167-169), "the pleasures of biodiversity" are valued by people the world over, and it is not true that "nature appreciation is an idea only in the privileged West."

Keith Thomas (2001THOMAS, Keith. 2001. O homem e o mundo natural: mudanças de atitude em relação às plantas e aos animais (1500-1800). São Paulo: Cia. das Letras.: 133) showed that it was in the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries, a period of great discoveries and European explorations of the Americas, that the presence of pets within European homes became established; among the most significant of these animals were monkeys and parrots from the New World, which were very famous and prized (Françozo 2014FRANÇOZO, Mariana. 2014. De Olinda a Holanda: o gabinete de curiosidades de Nassau. Campinas: Editora da Unicamp.). Marcy Norton (2013NORTON, Marcy. 2013. "Going to the birds: animals as things and beings in early modernity." In: Paula Findlen (ed.), Early modern things: objects and their histories, 1500-1800. London: Routledge . pp. 53-83.: 71-77) furthers this suggestion: she believes it is undeniable that the practices and techniques - and, hence, the set of relations (adoption, familiarity, intimacy, and parental care) - related to animal taming in the Americas had a decisive impact on the phenomenon of pet keeping in Europe. In this way, the native peoples of the Americas seem to have stimulated the habit of keeping pets in European homes at the beginning of the modern era, and of treating them dearly with care and affection: as members of the family. Norton also argues that the Europeans did not adopt only American species, but also the mode of relationship between humans and animal pets. Perhaps these same people also taught the Old World the value of the beauty and diversity of the animals not only in themselves, but when harmonic coexistence is maintained among different species including humans; the menageries, as the precursors of contemporary zoos, may speak to this argument, since it was the discovery of the Americas that populated Europe with countless monkeys and parrots. But, after all, are zoos imperial devices (Acampora 2005ACAMPORA, Ralph. 2005. "Zoos and eyes: contesting captivity and seeking successor practices." Society and Animals, 13(1): 69-88.; Rago 2008RAGO, Margareth. 2008. "Michel Foucault e o zoológico do rei." In: A. Veiga-Neto, A. de Souza Filho & D. Muniz de Albuquerque Júnior (orgs.), Cartografias de Foucault. Belo Horizonte: Autêntica. pp. 253-267.)? Perhaps. Yet they may also have been our way of dealing with the concentration of natural diversity around us without focusing on the deeper efforts towards coexistence: we manage the diversity of animals by keeping them in cages outside our homes, while the Indians insist on making them thrive as they roam free and within domestic spaces. But was this and will this always be the way? Does our fascination with the biblical paradise of Genesis (or Noah's Ark, for that matter) not stem precisely from a fascination with the peaceful coexistence between us and other non-humans? And for this reason, does it not suggest a similar consideration about the aesthetic value of living together with difference? Is this not the sign of the Fall, the loss of the singular beauty of paradise, where men and all types of animals spoke to each other?

References

  • ACAMPORA, Ralph. 2005. "Zoos and eyes: contesting captivity and seeking successor practices." Society and Animals, 13(1): 69-88.
  • ANDERSON, Patricia. 2003. "A bird in the house: an anthropological perspective on companion parrots." Society & Animals, 11(4): 393-418.
  • ARAÚJO, Íris Morais. 2014. Osikirip: os 'especiais' Karitiana e a noção de pessoa ameríndia. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Doctoral thesis.
  • BALDUS, Herbert. 1970. Tapirapé, tribo Tupí no Brasil Central. São Paulo: Cia. Editora Nacional.
  • BARCELOS NETO, Aristóteles. 2002. A arte dos sonhos: uma iconografia ameríndia. Lisboa; Museu Nacional de Etnologia/Assírio & Alvim.
  • _____. 2006. "De divinações xamânicas e acusações de feitiçaria: imagens Wauja da agência letal." Mana, 12(2): 285-313.
  • BECHELANY, Fabiano. 2012. Figuras da captura: a atividade cinegética na etnologia indígena. Master's thesis, Universidade de Brasília.
  • BELAUNDE, Luisa Elvira. 2001. Viviendo bien: género y fertilidad entre los Airo-Pai de la Amazonía Peruana. Lima: CAAAP/BCRP.
  • BENJAMIN, Walter. 1985. Magia e técnica, arte e política - obras escolhidas, vol. I. São Paulo: Brasiliense.
  • BEYSEN, Peter. 2013. "Kempiro. A arte gráfica dos traços fortes entre os Ashaninka do Oeste Amazônico." In: C. Severi & E. Lagrou (orgs.), Quimeras em diálogo: grafismo e figuração na arte indígena. Rio de Janeiro: 7Letras. pp. 223-245.
  • CASSIDY, Rebecca. 2007. Horse people: thoroughbred culture in Lexington and Newmarket. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
  • CASSIDY, Rebecca & MULLIN, Molly (eds.). 2007. Where the wild things are now: domestication reconsidered. Oxford: Berg.
  • CLUTTON-BROCK, J. (ed.). 1990. The walking larder: patterns of domestication, pastorialism, and predation. London: Routledge.
  • COHN, Clarice. 2000. A criança indígena: a concepção Xikrin de infância e aprendizado. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Master's dissertation.
  • CORMIER, Loretta. 2003. Kinship with monkeys: the Guajá foragers of Eastern Amazonia. New York: Columbia University Press.
  • CYPRIANO, Doris C. de Araujo. 2007. "Almas, corpos e especiarias: a expansão colonial nos rios Tapajós e Madeira." Pesquisas - Instituto Anchietano de Pesquisas, Antropologia, 65: 1-170.
  • DERRIDA, Jacques. 2002. O animal que logo sou (a seguir). São Paulo: Edunesp.
  • DESCOLA, Philippe. 1994. In the society of nature: a native ecology in Amazonia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • _____. 1996. "Constructing natures: symbolic ecology and social practice." In: P. Descola & G. Pálsson (editors), Nature and society: anthropological perspectives. London: Routledge. pp. 82-102.
  • _____. 1998a. "Pourquoi les indiens d'Amazonie n'ont-ils pás domestique le pecari? Généalogie des objets et anthropologie de l'objectivation." In: P. Lemmonier & B. Latour (orgs.), De la préhistoire a les missiles balistiques: l'intelligence sociale des techniques. Paris: Éditions de la Découverte. pp. 329-344.
  • _____. 1998b. "Estrutura ou sentimento: a relação com o animal na Amazônia." Mana, 4(1): 23-45.
  • _____. 1999. "Des proies bienveillantes. Le traitement du gibier dans la chasse amazonienne." In: F.Héritier (org.), De la violence II. Paris: Editions Odile Jacob. pp. 19-44.
  • ERIKSON, Philippe. 1987. "De l'apprivoisement à l'approvisionnement: chasse, alliance et familiarization en Amazonie amérindienne." Techniques et Cultures, 9 (n.s.): 105-140.
  • _____. 1997. "On native american conservation and the status of Amazonian pets." Current Anthropology, 38(3): 445-446.
  • _____. 2000. "The social significance of pet keeping among Amazonian Indians." In: A. Podbersceck; E. S. Paul & J. Serpell (eds.), Companion animals and us. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 7-26.
  • _____. 2012. "Animais demais...os xerimbabos no espaço doméstico matis (Amazonas)." Anuário Antropológico, 2011/II: 15-32.
  • FAUSTO, Carlos. 1999. "Of enemies and pets: warfare and shamanism in Amazonia." American Ethnologist, 26(4): 933-956.
  • FEELEY-HARNIK, Gillian. 2007. "'An experiment on a giant scale': Darwin and the domestication of pigeons." In: R. Cassidy & M. Mullin (eds.), Where the wild things are now: domestication reconsidered. Oxford: Berg. pp. 147-182.
  • FERREIRA, Dante Martins. 1983. "Um estudo da etnozoologia Karajá: o exemplo das máscaras de Aruanã." In: B. Ribeiro et al. (eds.), O artesão tradicional e seu papel na sociedade contemporânea. Rio de Janeiro: Funarte/Instituto Nacional do Folclore. pp. 213-232.
  • FIJN, Natasha. 2011. Living with herds: human-animal coexistence in Mongolia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press .
  • FRANÇOZO, Mariana. 2014. De Olinda a Holanda: o gabinete de curiosidades de Nassau. Campinas: Editora da Unicamp.
  • GARCIA, Uirá. 2010. Karawara: a caça e o mundo dos Awá-Guajá. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Doctoral thesis.
  • GELL, Alfred. 1988. "Technology and magic." Anthropology Today, 4(2): 6-9.
  • _____. 1996. "Vogel's net: traps as artworks and artworks as traps." Journal of Material Culture, 1(1): 15-38.
  • _____. 1998. Art and agency: an anthropological theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • GIANNINI, Isabelle. 1991. A ave resgatada: a impossibilidade da leveza do ser. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Master's thesis.
  • GOLUB, Alex. 2005. "Shooting snowy was the toughest job I ever had: the role of dogs in first contact." Paper presented at the conference Fashioning anthropology: papers in honor of Gail Kelly. Portland: Reed College, April 2005 (Unpublished).
  • GONÇALVES, Marco Antônio. 2001. O mundo inacabado: ação e criação em uma cosmologia Amazônica. Etnografia Pirahã. Rio de Janeiro: Editora da UFRJ.
  • HARAWAY, Donna. 2008. When species meet. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
  • HAUDRICOURT, André. 1986. "Note sur le statut familial des animaux". L'Homme 99, XXVI (3): 119-120.
  • INGOLD, Tim. 2000. The perception of the environment: essays in livelihood, dwelling and skill. London: Routledge .
  • _____. 2011. Being alive: essays on movement, knowledge, and description. London: Routledge.
  • KAC, Eduardo. 2013. Telepresença e bioarte: humanos, coelhos e robôs em rede. São Paulo: Edusp.
  • KAGAN, Cinthia Moreira de Carvalho. 2015. Les indiens Pitaguary et leurs chiens: une communauté hybride? Paris: Université Sorbonne Nouvelle - Paris III/IHEAL, Doctoral thesis.
  • KIRKSEY, S. Eben; HELMREICH, Stefan. 2010. "The emergence of multispecies ethnography". Cultural Anthropology, 25(4): 545-576.
  • KIRSCH, Stuart. 2006. Reverse anthropology: indigenous analysis of social and environmental relations in New Guinea. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
  • KOHN, Eduardo. 2013. How forests think: toward an anthropology beyond the human. Berkeley: University of California Press.
  • LAGROU, Els. 2007. A fluidez da forma: arte, alteridade e agência em uma sociedade amazônica (Kaxinawa, Acre). Rio de Janeiro: Topbooks/CAPES/PPGSA-UFRJ.
  • _____. 2009. Arte indígena no Brasil. Belo Horizonte: Editora C/Arte.
  • LEACH, Helen. 2003. "Human domestication reconsidered." Current Anthropology , 44(3): 349-360.
  • LEAL, Natacha. 2014. Nomes aos bois: zebus e zebuzeiros em uma pecuária brasileira de elite. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Doctoral thesis.
  • LESTEL, Dominique. 2007. Les amis de mes amis. Paris: Edition du Seuil.
  • LÉVI-STRAUSS, Claude. 1997 [1962]. O pensamento selvagem. Campinas: Papirus.
  • MASON, Peter. 1994. "From presentation to representation: Americana in Europe." Journal of the History of Collections, 6(1): 1-20.
  • MCCALLUM, Cecilia. 2001. How real people are made: gender and sociality in Amazonia. Oxford: Berg .
  • MENGET, Patrick. 1988. "Notes sur l'adoption chez les Txicáo du Brésil Central." Anthropologie et Sociétés, 12(2): 63-72.
  • MOSER, Lilian. 1993. Os Karitiana e a colonização recente em Rondônia. Porto Velho: Universiade Federal de Rondônia, monograph.
  • _____. 1997. Os Karitiana no processo de desenvolvimento de Rondônia nas décadas de 1950 a 1990. Recife: UFPE, master's thesis.
  • MÜLLER, Regina Polo. 2002. "As crianças no processo de recuperação demográfica dos Asurini do Xingu." In: A. Lopes da Silva; A. V. S. Macedo & A. Nunes (orgs.), Crianças indígenas: ensaios antropológicos. São Paulo: Global/MARI/Fapesp. pp. 188-209).
  • NADING, Alex. 2012. "Dengue mosquitoes are single mothers: biopolitics meets ecological aesthetics in Nicaraguan Community Health Work." Cultural Anthropology , 27(4): 572-596.
  • NORTON, Marcy. 2013. "Going to the birds: animals as things and beings in early modernity." In: Paula Findlen (ed.), Early modern things: objects and their histories, 1500-1800. London: Routledge . pp. 53-83.
  • OVERING, Joanna. 1991. "A estética da produção: o senso de comunidade entre os Cubeo e os Piaroa." Revista de Antropologia, 34: 7-33.
  • _____. 1999. "Elogio do cotidiano: a confiança e a arte da vida social em uma comunidade amazônica." Mana, 5(1): 81-107.
  • _____. 2001. "Against the motion (1) - 1993 debate: aesthetics is a cross-cultural category." In: T. Ingold (ed.), Key debates in anthropology. London: Routledge. pp. 260-266.
  • OVERING, Joanna; PASSES, Alan (eds.). 2000. The anthropology of love and anger: the aesthetics of conviviality in native South America. London: Routledge .
  • PATTON, Paul. 2003. "Language, power, and the training of horses." In: C. Wolfe (ed.), Zoontologies: the question of the animal. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press . pp. 83-99.
  • PAVÃO, Luna Castro. 2015. 'O que é que cavalo sabe': um estudo antropológico sobre o vínculo animal-humano na equoterapia. São Carlos: Universidade Federal de São Carlos, Master's thesis.
  • PEREIRA, Luis Fernando. 2008. Legislação ambiental e indigenista: uma aproximação ao direito socioambiental no Brasil. São Paulo: Iepé.
  • RAGO, Margareth. 2008. "Michel Foucault e o zoológico do rei." In: A. Veiga-Neto, A. de Souza Filho & D. Muniz de Albuquerque Júnior (orgs.), Cartografias de Foucault. Belo Horizonte: Autêntica. pp. 253-267.
  • REINA, Ruben; KENSINGER, Kenneth (eds.). 1991. The gift of birds: featherwork of native South American peoples. Philadelphia: The University Museum of Archaeology and Anthropology, University of Pennsylvania.
  • RENCTAS. 2001. 1o. Relatório Nacional sobre o Tráfico de Animais Silvestres. Brasília: RENCTAS.
  • ROBBINS, Louise. 2002. Elephant slaves and pampered parrots: exotic animals in eighteenth-century Paris. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press .
  • SAHLINS, Marshall. 1988. "Cosmologies of capitalism: the Trans-Pacific sector of 'the World System.'" Proceedings of the British Academy, LXXIV: 1-51.
  • SANTOS-GRANERO, Fernando. 2009. Vital enemies: slavery, predation, and the Amerindian political economy of life. Austin: University of Texas Press.
  • SANTOS-GRANERO, Fernando; MENTORE, George (eds.). 2008. "Special issue in honor of Prof. Joanna Overing. In the world and about the world: Amerindian modes of knowledge." Revista de Antropologia, 49 (1): 11-16.
  • SCARTEZINI, Sofia. 2015. Crianças Iny: entre o kube e a escola. Brasília: DAN/UnB, course monograph (unpublished).
  • SCHINDLER, H.. 2001. "Plumas como enfeites da moda." História, Ciências, Saúde - Manguinhos, VIII (suplemento): 1089-1108.
  • SERPELL, James. 1996. In the company of animals: a study of human-animal relationships. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press .
  • STADEN, Hans. 1999 [1557]. Hans Staden: primeiros registros escritos e ilustrados sobre o Brasil e seus habitantes. São Paulo: Editora Terceiro Nome.
  • STEFANUTO, Miriam Rodeguero. 2015. Trabalho calado: uma etnografia dos índios Kaingang nos frigoríficos do oeste de Santa Catarina. São Carlos: PPGAS/UFSCar, Assessment report (unpublished).
  • TALIN, Christian. 2000. Anthropologie de l'animal de compagnie: l'animal, autre figure de l'altérité. Paris: L'Atelier de l'Archer.
  • TAYLOR, Anne Christine. 2000. "Le sexe de la proie: répresentations jivaro du lien de parenté". L'Homme, 154-155: 309-334.
  • _____. 2001. "Wifes, pets and affines: marriage among the Jivaro". In: L. Rival & N. Whitehead (eds.). Beyond the visible and the material: the amerindianization of society in the work of Peter Rivière. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 45-56.
  • THOMAS, Keith. 2001. O homem e o mundo natural: mudanças de atitude em relação às plantas e aos animais (1500-1800). São Paulo: Cia. das Letras.
  • TSING, Anna. 2005. Friction: an ethnography of global connection. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
  • VANDER VELDEN, Felipe. 2004. Por onde o sangue circula: os Karitiana e a intervenção biomédica. Campinas: Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Master's thesis.
  • _____. 2011. "Inveja do gado: o fazendeiro como figura de poder e desejo entre os Karitiana." Anuário Antropológico, 2010-1: 55-76.
  • _____. 2012a. Inquietas companhias: sobre os animais de criação entre os Karitiana. São Paulo: Alameda.
  • _____. 2012b. "As galinhas incontáveis: Tupis, europeus e aves domésticas na conquista do Brasil." Journal de la Société des Américanistes, 98: 97-140.
  • _____. 2015. "Como se faz um cachorro caçador entre os Karitiana (Rondônia)." Trabalho apresentado na XI Reunión de Antropología del Mercosur. Montevideo: FHCE/UDELAR (unpublished).
  • VAN VELTHEM, Lúcia H.. 1998. A pele de Tuluperê: uma etnografia dos trançados Wayana. Belém: Museu Paraense Emílio Goeldi/Funtec.
  • _____. 2003. O belo é a fera: a estética da produção e da predação entre os Wayana. Lisboa: Museu Nacional de Etnologia/Assírio & Alvim.
  • WALKER, Harry. 2013. Under a watchful eye: self, power, and intimacy in Amazonia. Berkeley: University of California Press .
  • Translated by Tracy Miyake
  • 1
    This article was made possible by reviews, comments, and suggestions from various partners on the two occasions when the preliminary results were presented. I especially would like to thank Carlos Sautchuk, Guilherme Sá, Ciméa Bevilaqua, Jean Segata, Bernardo Lewgoy, Caetano Sordi, Lady Selma Albernaz, Geraldo Andrello, Clarice Cohn, and Marcos Lanna. Marcy Norton sent me some of her texts, which were fundamental to the arguments developed herein.
  • 2
    I wish to note the Karitiana language, like most Amerindian languages, does not have a term for the (semantic) category animal. If a comparison must be provided, speakers offer the term himo to refer to animals. This term, however, falls far short in the scope and fixed nature of the concept of animal, since it designates prey animals for hunting, i.e. those that are or may be food (himo is the same word used for meat); consequently, only hunted animals are himo, which gives the term a contextual application. For example, if a spider monkey is killed in the forest (which we would call "wild"), it is himo, but a spider monkey raised as a pet in the village (and which will never be slaughtered) is not. As can be seen, seeing the category animal as a monolithic entity can create more problems than solutions (Derrida 2002DERRIDA, Jacques. 2002. O animal que logo sou (a seguir). São Paulo: Edunesp.). Therefore, in the interest of economy here I will use the suggestion by Tim Ingold (2000) which attempts to avoid dissociating humans and animals by recognizing that this dichotomous opposition does not consider the multiple modes of relationship between various human and non-human collectives.
  • 3
    I use "wild pets" to refer to animals brought from the forest and familiarized in the villages; these differ from animals that are normally called "pets", a term corresponding to species or varieties of domesticated animals that have been undergoing a process of co-evolution alongside humans for milennia. These "wild pets" approach those animals Christian Talin (2000TALIN, Christian. 2000. Anthropologie de l'animal de compagnie: l'animal, autre figure de l'altérité. Paris: L'Atelier de l'Archer.) calls "new companion animals" (nouvelles animaux de compagnie) in the Western world which are not domesticated, strictly speaking, and mostly remain wild (iguanas, spiders, ferrets, martens, snakes, etc.).
  • 4
    These must have been quite common events in the history of the initial contacts between indigenous populations and the colonizers, but we know little about these events, mostly because of the virtual inexistence of documentation. One of the few documented cases involved the group contacted in the Javari River valley (eastern Amazonas, probable the Matis) who insistently asked FUNAI employees for puppies. The following text is from the Revista de Atualidade Indígena: "Stray dogs began to be part of the relationship of the traditional gifts used to attract reluctant and isolated Indians. The sertanistas from FUNAI were surprised with strange requests for dogs when last year they made contact for the first time with an as-yet unidentified tribal group on the banks of the Ituí River in Amazonas. Since first contact the Indians showed a desire to receive more than the axes, machetes, cooking pots, and scissors that the sertanistas offered them. (...). They wanted dogs (...). In the two subsequent encounters, they again asked for more dogs, forcing the attracting team to round up the stray dogs they found in neighboring villages, and buying some (...)" (FUNAI 1978: 18). Alex Golub (2005GOLUB, Alex. 2005. "Shooting snowy was the toughest job I ever had: the role of dogs in first contact." Paper presented at the conference Fashioning anthropology: papers in honor of Gail Kelly. Portland: Reed College, April 2005 (Unpublished).) has reflected about the tremendous impact that the presence of dogs in the first expeditions throughout the highlands of New Guinea had on the native populations of that region, and on the way in which relations between these people and the foreign explorers were organized.
  • 5
    I note that here, as in other citations of statements from my Karitiana interlocutors, I chose to standardize the language.
  • 6
    And this was also done by the groups living in villages on the Madeira and Tapajós Rivers (the Tupinambarana and Tapajó) in the eighteenth century, according to a report from the Jesuit João Daniel (apud Cypriano 2007: 125CYPRIANO, Doris C. de Araujo. 2007. "Almas, corpos e especiarias: a expansão colonial nos rios Tapajós e Madeira." Pesquisas - Instituto Anchietano de Pesquisas, Antropologia, 65: 1-170., free translation): "they greatly prize the pendants and medallions with images of the saints; but it is for the beauty of them and not for the respect and devotion they hold; and so often they use them to adorn their monkeys and puppies, tying them around their necks (...)."
  • 7
    They are not adornments, but not because they are ugly, dirty or sick. Even if the Karitiana abhor the odor of the dogs, I never heard them say the dogs were ugly. Perhaps their ugliness offends the eyes of non-Indian observers, who are accustomed to the careful and detailed treatment these pets get in the contemporary world. Furthermore, according to Golub (2005: 8), "[E]uropean judgments of indigenous dogs had historically been quite negative."
  • 8
    Among the Pitaguary in Ceará, dogs decorate the village, especially beautiful and healthy ones; like other animals reared in homes, particularly birds in cages and certain lizards (iguanas and tegus), dogs are animaux decoratifs that beautify the home environment (Kagan 2015: 138-140KAGAN, Cinthia Moreira de Carvalho. 2015. Les indiens Pitaguary et leurs chiens: une communauté hybride? Paris: Université Sorbonne Nouvelle - Paris III/IHEAL, Doctoral thesis.). The Kaingang of Toldo Chimbangue (in Santa Catarina) refuse to eat the chickens they keep, since they "decorate the yard" (Stefanuto 2015: 41STEFANUTO, Miriam Rodeguero. 2015. Trabalho calado: uma etnografia dos índios Kaingang nos frigoríficos do oeste de Santa Catarina. São Carlos: PPGAS/UFSCar, Assessment report (unpublished).).
  • 9
    The equines that lived in the village, a mare and two donkeys (the latter two having died in 2009), also seem (or seemed) to be subsumed in the universe of work: Epitácio told me that the donkeys were used to transport cultivated crops from the clearings and the products extracted from the forests to the village. John, who was the owner of the mare, told me that he hooked up a small wagon (which I saw alongside his house) to the animal to drive the family to more distant clearings, which I never saw; only once I witnessed Junio, the son of Irene (John's wife) saddle and mount the mare to visit a distant clearing, and the use of the horse for riding seems unusual (most of the Karitiana do not know how to ride). This infrequent "use" seems to suggest to me that equines are not (and were not) in fact considered working animals, and the everyday treatment they receive seem to indicate firstly that they are (or were) kept at home, or closely, though they were not referred to as companions or helpers. Furthermore, they "adorn the village."
  • 10
    This was in my last year in the field; I know the religious configuration of the village has changed slightly since that time, but I do not have precise data.
  • 11
    If there are many dogs, the Karitiana state that today there are few which are effective hunters. This would explain why many houses do not have dogs: several men stated they only liked dogs that know how to hunt, and since these are rare, they do not keep any dogs. I cannot say to what extent this shortage of hunting dogs is real, but it can be suggested that training dogs to stalk and kill prey is a domain which is increasingly restricted to older and more experienced people, and consequently it has become more difficult to obtain these specialized dogs.
  • 12
    Here I use the term domestication in the broad sense, borrowing the term from the ethnologists who discuss art as the domestication of potentially dangerous beings and agencies in the villages (their miniaturization, one might say, to human scales; with regard to miniaturization or smaller-scale models as a form of ownership, see Levi-Strauss 1997 [1962]: chapter 2). Being faithful to the technical terminology, I obviously am referring to the familiarization (taming) of kept animals and not their domestication, which is generally held to not be present in the South American lowlands (Descola 1998a_____. 1998a. "Pourquoi les indiens d'Amazonie n'ont-ils pás domestique le pecari? Généalogie des objets et anthropologie de l'objectivation." In: P. Lemmonier & B. Latour (orgs.), De la préhistoire a les missiles balistiques: l'intelligence sociale des techniques. Paris: Éditions de la Découverte. pp. 329-344.); in any case, recent reflections (Cassidy & Mullin 2007) have urged us to rethink the current notions of domestication to approach cases of closeness that are not usually considered as such (for example, marine fish farms, "wild domestication," eating together, mutualism, etc.) in an effort to reconsider human-animal relations as a whole. In a certain sense, even these ethnologists with whom I dispute should employ the idea of art as familiarization (or taming), since the set of relationships with these beings/agencies is a permanent process and refers to specific individuals, and not the "species" of beings or powers. Still, perhaps the opposition between domestication and familiarization/taming even may lose its meaning, when domestication is understood not as a finished process, but as an ongoing and continuous effort to create the interspecies bond (Haraway 2008; Fijn 2011FIJN, Natasha. 2011. Living with herds: human-animal coexistence in Mongolia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press .). Similarly, captives (as well as the animals taken from the forest) gradually are familiarized by their captors when they are captured (Fausto 1999): there are no "domesticated peoples" (as a state or condition), so to speak, although a recent study by Fernando Santos-Granero (2009) suggests the contrary.
  • 13
    The Karitiana numbered only 45 in the late 1960s; today they are more than 350 in number, having increased almost seven-fold over the course of 40 years.
  • 14
    Returning kept animals to the wild was recorded among other groups such as the Awá-Guajá (Uirá Garcia, personal communication) and the Wayapi (Igor Scaramuzzi, personal communication), but not among the Karitiana.
  • 15
    Consequently, the menageries of French kings resembled the collections (which included living exotic animals) assembled by Chinese emperors in palaces which were constructed especially for this use; Marshall Sahlins notes that "[T]he synthesis of diversity and conquest made these imperial retreats [the palaces] perfect microcosms. They represented the whole world as the work of the Emperor and within his power" (Sahlins 1988: 22SAHLINS, Marshall. 1988. "Cosmologies of capitalism: the Trans-Pacific sector of 'the World System.'" Proceedings of the British Academy, LXXIV: 1-51.).
  • 16
    The Pied Piper of Hamelin from the European folk tale may be an extreme example of this analogy.
  • 17
    For Alfred Gell, all art is a technology for capturing, captivating, and holding the viewer and keeping him or her under control. The classic example of the Zande hunting trap, which is discussed by Gell (1996_____. 1996. "Vogel's net: traps as artworks and artworks as traps." Journal of Material Culture, 1(1): 15-38.), points in this direction: the indiscernibility between technical objects and artistic objects in the modes of capturing random agencies. Is it possible that domestication is exactly that: the capture of random agencies?
  • 18
    Bechelany (2012BECHELANY, Fabiano. 2012. Figuras da captura: a atividade cinegética na etnologia indígena. Master's thesis, Universidade de Brasília. ) argues that Indian hunting practices are simultaneously signified as both technique and aesthetic, art and technology, creativity and repetition, freedom and determination; in this way, efficiency and beauty cannot be separated in the activities of Amazonian hunters, nor the precision of pure enjoyment, the economy of profit and expenditure. Uirá Garcia (2010GARCIA, Uirá. 2010. Karawara: a caça e o mundo dos Awá-Guajá. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Doctoral thesis.) suggests a "poetics of predation" by advocating a similar idea among the Awá-Guajá. If hunting can be both technique and art, is there a reason why domestication/familiarization - obviously, for different reasons - cannot also be? It is interesting that Henry Walter Bates asked "what arts the old woman used" (quoted in Norton 2013: 68, emphasis mine) when referring to "an old Indian woman's" success in taming what the naturalist described as an "intractable green parrot."
  • 19
    Not to mention, of course, that many of these animals are further embellished by their owners or keepers with special collars and even clothes that mimic human clothing. The practice of decorating family pets has been discussed above.
  • 20
    And here, exoticism and exteriority play a central role and are, in a sense, near the interest of the Karitiana through the integration of non-human beings into their daily lives, whether these animals are brought from the forest, or incorporated in the world of the whites and brought from the cities. Of course the Karitiana, like other Amazonian indigenous peoples, appreciate various animals for their beauty; this is particularly the case in birds, whose feathers are essential for making the most beautiful indigenous artifacts, featherwork, which are highly valued inside as well as outside the villages (Reina & Kensinger 1991REINA, Ruben; KENSINGER, Kenneth (eds.). 1991. The gift of birds: featherwork of native South American peoples. Philadelphia: The University Museum of Archaeology and Anthropology, University of Pennsylvania.). When dressed in their headdresses and feathered gauntlets the Indians often describe themselves as "beautiful," and bird feathers are described in literature as being native "jewels," objects that are actively sought, have great value, and deserve special care such as storage in special containers (cf. Giannini 1991GIANNINI, Isabelle. 1991. A ave resgatada: a impossibilidade da leveza do ser. São Paulo: Universidade de São Paulo, Master's thesis.; Vander Velden 2012b_____. 2012b. "As galinhas incontáveis: Tupis, europeus e aves domésticas na conquista do Brasil." Journal de la Société des Américanistes, 98: 97-140.). For quite some time, bird feathers were appreciated in Western women's fashion like jewels or body decorations (Schindler 2001SCHINDLER, H.. 2001. "Plumas como enfeites da moda." História, Ciências, Saúde - Manguinhos, VIII (suplemento): 1089-1108.; Kirsch, 2006KIRSCH, Stuart. 2006. Reverse anthropology: indigenous analysis of social and environmental relations in New Guinea. Stanford: Stanford University Press. on bird-of-paradise feathers). Peter Beysen (2013: 226-227, n. 4BEYSEN, Peter. 2013. "Kempiro. A arte gráfica dos traços fortes entre os Ashaninka do Oeste Amazônico." In: C. Severi & E. Lagrou (orgs.), Quimeras em diálogo: grafismo e figuração na arte indígena. Rio de Janeiro: 7Letras. pp. 223-245.) constructs intriguing suggestions around the equivalents (via patterns on the skin) made between snakes and jewels or decorations of the female body in Ashaninka thought and in Western painting.
  • 21
    This association between beauty and wealth does not escape the indigenous peoples: Hans Staden (1999STADEN, Hans. 1999 [1557]. Hans Staden: primeiros registros escritos e ilustrados sobre o Brasil e seus habitantes. São Paulo: Editora Terceiro Nome. [1557]: 101; 113) stated in the sixteenth century that the brilliant feathers of various birds which were jealously guarded by the Tupi in the coastal Portuguese Americas were their "riches": "their treasures are feathers. He who has many of them is rich (...)." It is very interesting that André Thevet at a certain point viewed sixteenth-century Tupi society through the eyes of European royalty, which is why Cunhambebe's feather headdress becomes his crown (Mason 1994: 10MASON, Peter. 1994. "From presentation to representation: Americana in Europe." Journal of the History of Collections, 6(1): 1-20.).
  • 22
    This common search for the ornamental can be further evidence for the thesis that the acclimatization of animals to human company meets a need which is specifically human (Serpell 1996). Note that in the contemporary West the search for diversity appears in what are known as "new pets" (Talinn 2000TALIN, Christian. 2000. Anthropologie de l'animal de compagnie: l'animal, autre figure de l'altérité. Paris: L'Atelier de l'Archer.): iguanas, pythons, amphibians, spiders, rats, pigs, foxes, etc. Perhaps the diffusion of this habit (along with the constant discussions and experiences of domesticating various new animal species and getting others used to captivity) is related to the ideas of bringing increasingly diverse beings into coexistence with humans.
  • 23
    According to RENCTAS (2001RENCTAS. 2001. 1o. Relatório Nacional sobre o Tráfico de Animais Silvestres. Brasília: RENCTAS.), the feathers of some birds are artificially colored to more closely resemble more expensive and more valuable species in the market for wild animals. This is cruel logic that yet again emulates the indigenous practice of tapiragem, in which technical processes that today still remain obscure are applied to the plucked bodies of live birds so their feathers grow back in different colors. This also emulates the sixteenth-century Tupi practice recorded by Jean de Léry of coloring white chicken feathers with turmeric to make them resemble the feathers of the scarlet ibis, which were quite appreciated by these Indians and used to make their main feather adornments (see Vander Velden 2012b_____. 2012b. "As galinhas incontáveis: Tupis, europeus e aves domésticas na conquista do Brasil." Journal de la Société des Américanistes, 98: 97-140.). All these procedures change the color of the plumage; in one of them, however, the goal is to produce different feathers, while in the other the goal is to produce a different animal. Many origin myths addressing the variety of birds (including those of the Karitiana) focus on the feathers acquiring their coloration from various substances. So would painting animals be so strange to the Indians after all? Would creating diversity be strange?
  • 24
    The notion of "ecological aesthetics" discussed by Nading (2012NADING, Alex. 2012. "Dengue mosquitoes are single mothers: biopolitics meets ecological aesthetics in Nicaraguan Community Health Work." Cultural Anthropology , 27(4): 572-596.) from the suggestions of Gregory Bateson can be usefully applied here in exploring the aesthetic pleasure we extract from our daily "entanglement" with the creatures who live with us and share our world, in the continuous "becoming" that is life in flux (Ingold 2011_____. 2011. Being alive: essays on movement, knowledge, and description. London: Routledge.). Ecological aesthetics (different from environmental aesthetics, which is more concerned with ordering and control of the natural world) "privileges a relational knowledge of life" (Nading 2012: 577NADING, Alex. 2012. "Dengue mosquitoes are single mothers: biopolitics meets ecological aesthetics in Nicaraguan Community Health Work." Cultural Anthropology , 27(4): 572-596.) and invests in the marvel and the beauty that we derive from our relationship with the world and with others. These ideas can also illuminate a reflection on domestication/familiarization as an art or source of aesthetic fruition. Along these lines, in a future work I hope to test Michel Foucault's ideas about the "aesthetics of existence," which consist of considering the changes themselves in thought knowledge about experiences. Could this then involve using knowledge to modify other species? Does the consideration (also by Alfred Gell) of domestication as being preceded by self-domestication - man taming himself before domesticating animals and plants (Leach 2003LEACH, Helen. 2003. "Human domestication reconsidered." Current Anthropology , 44(3): 349-360.) - not address precisely this?

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Dec 2016

History

  • Received
    11 Dec 2015
  • Accepted
    15 July 2016
Associação Brasileira de Antropologia (ABA) Caixa Postal 04491, 70904-970 Brasília - DF / Brasil, Tel./ Fax 55 61 3307-3754 - Brasília - DF - Brazil
E-mail: vibrant.aba@gmail.com