Acessibilidade / Reportar erro

Effect of association of imagery and physical practice on children’s motor learning

Efeito da associação de prática imagética e física na aprendizagem motora em crianças

Abstract

Imagery training has been shown to induce motor learning in adults, but similar evidence in children is scarce. In this experiment, we aimed to evaluate the effect of association between imagery and physical practice compared to pure physical practice in the learning of a manual task in 9-10 year-old children. The task consisted of transporting a block and fitting it into a support with speed and steadiness, assessing movement time to complete the “reaching” and “transport” task components. The children were assigned to one of three groups: (a) physical practice (PHYS) (240 trials), (b) combination (COMB) of imagery (180 trials) and physical (60 trials) practice, and (c) control (CON), associating visual rotation (180 trials) and physical practice (60 trials). Performance was evaluated immediately and 24 h after practice. Results indicated that the PHYS group achieved a persistent performance gain in the “transport”, but not in the “reaching” task component, while the COMB group achieved persistent performance gains in both movement components; no significant differences were found for the CON group. Our results suggest that imagery training improves the task mental representation in children, while physical practice provides sensory feedback on the performed movements. As a conclusion, the results suggest that combination of imagery and physical practice can be more effective than pure physical practice for children’s motor learning.

Key words
Child; Learning; Motor activity; Motor skills

Resumo

Imagética dos próprios movimentos tem mostrado induzir aprendizagem motora em adultos, porém são escassas evidências similares em crianças. O objetivo deste experimento foi avaliar o efeito da associação entre prática imagética e prática física em comparação com prática física pura em uma habilidade manual em crianças de 9-10 anos de idade. A tarefa consistiu em transportar um bloco e encaixá-lo em um suporte com rapidez e estabilidade, avaliando-se o tempo de movimento para completar os componentes de “alcance” e “transporte”. As crianças foram distribuídas em três grupos: (a) prática física (FIS) (240 tentativas), (b) combinação (COMB) de prática imagética (180 tentativas) e prática física (60 tentativas), e (c) controle (CON), com associação de rotação visual (180 tentativas) e prática física (60 tentativas). O desempenho foi avaliado imediatamente e 24 h após a prática. Os resultados indicaram que o grupo FIS obteve ganho persistente de desempenho no componente “transporte”, porém não no componente “alcance”, enquanto o grupo COMB alcançou ganho persistente de desempenho em ambos os componentes de movimento; não foram encontradas diferenças significantes para o grupo CON. Estes resultados sugerem que a prática imagética aprimora a representação mental da tarefa motora em crianças, enquanto que a prática física oferece informação de feedback sensorial sobre o os movimentos realizados. Como conclusão, nossos resultados sugerem que a combinação de prática física e imagética é um procedimento que pode ser mais efetivo do que prática física pura para aprendizagem motora em crianças.

Palavras-chave
Atividade Motora; Aprendizagem; Criança; Destreza motora

INTRODUCTION

Imagery practice consists of the repeated mental rehearsal of movements themselves, without being physically executed. It has been proposed that imagery practice allows the formation of an internal model that is more flexible and independent of the effector11 Wohldmann EL, Healy AF, Bourne LE. A mental practice superiority effect: less retroactive interference and more transfer than physical practice. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2008;34(4):823-33.. One of the theories that support learning through imagery practice proposes that imagery helps in motor concentration and programming, facilitating the execution of specific movements22 Jackson PL, Lafleur MF, Malouin F, Richards C, Doyon J. Potential role of mental practice using motor imagery in neurologic rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2001;82(8):1133-41.. In addition, there is evidence that when imagery and physical practices are combined, neuroplasticity is stimulated, inducing cortical reorganization33 Allami N, Brovelli A, Hamzaoui el M, Regragui F, Paulignan Y, Boussaoud D. Neurophysiological correlates of visuo-motor learning through mental and physical practice. Neuropsychologia 2014;55:6-14.,44 Butler AJ, Page SJ. Mental practice with motor imagery: evidence for motor recovery and cortical reorganization after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2006;87(12 Suppl 2):S2-11..

There is evidence that in imagery practice, good simulation of effective movements is obtained, given the similarity between the time spent to execute and imagine a given movement55 Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D. Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(1):105-13.

6 Gentili R, Han CE, Schweighofer N, Papaxanthis C. Motor learning without doing: trial-by-trial improvement in motor performance during mental training. J Neurophysiol 2010;104(2):774-83.
-77 Papaxanthis C, Pozzo T, Skoura X, Schieppati M. Does order and timing in performance of imagined and actual movements affect the motor imagery process? The duration of walking and writing task. Behav Brain Res 2002;134(1-2):209-15., as well as by similar autonomic responses between imagined and executed movement55 Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D. Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(1):105-13.,88 Decety J, Jeannerod M, Durozard D, Baverel G. Central activation of autonomic effectors during mental simulation of motor actions in man. J Physiol 1993;461:549-63.. Studies with imagery techniques of high temporal and spatial resolutions99 Cicinelli P, Marconi B, Zaccagnini M, Pasqualetti P, Filippi MM, Rossini PM. Imagery-induced cortical excitability changes in stroke: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cereb Cortex 2006;16(2):247-53.,1010 Porro CA, Francescato MP, Cettolo V, Diamond ME, Baraldi P, Zuiani C, et al. Primary motor and sensory cortex activation during motor performance and motor imagery: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 1996;16(23):768-898. have shown that the primary motor cortex is activated during imagination, although to a lesser degree than when movements are actually performed. However, more recent data have pointed out that the neural structures involved in imagery practice and physical practice are different. When compared, there is greater activation of the pre-motor cortex and supplementary motor area during motor training, whereas the visual cortex is more activated during imagery training. That is, the combined practice activates both motor and visual areas1111 Nyberg L, Eriksson J, Larsson A, Marklund P. Learning by doing versus learning by thinking: An fMRI study of motor and mental training. Neuropsychologia 2006;44(5):7117.,1212 Olsson CJ, Jonsson B, Nyberg L. Learning by doing and learning by thinking: an FMRI study of combining motor and mental training. Front Hum Neurosci 2008;2:5.. These findings suggest that individuals engaged in imagery practice may strengthen neural pathways relevant to the visuomotor regulation of movements. Thus, imagery practice could favor the acquisition of motor skills33 Allami N, Brovelli A, Hamzaoui el M, Regragui F, Paulignan Y, Boussaoud D. Neurophysiological correlates of visuo-motor learning through mental and physical practice. Neuropsychologia 2014;55:6-14.,1212 Olsson CJ, Jonsson B, Nyberg L. Learning by doing and learning by thinking: an FMRI study of combining motor and mental training. Front Hum Neurosci 2008;2:5.,1313 Feltz DL, Landers DM. The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: a meta-analysis. J Sport Psychol 1983;5(1):25-57..

Although imagery practice is widely used for sports training1313 Feltz DL, Landers DM. The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: a meta-analysis. J Sport Psychol 1983;5(1):25-57.

14 Millard M, Mahoney C, Wardrop J. A preliminary study of mental and physical practice on the kayak wet exit skill. Percept Mot Skills 2001;92(3 Pt 2):977-84.
-1515 Gomes TVB, Ugrinowitsch H, Coelho HUR, Marinho NFS, Fonseca FS, Benda N. Efeitos do posicionamento e quantidade de prática mental na aprendizagem do arremesso do dardo de salão. Motriz: J Phys Ed 2012;18(2):273-9. and for rehabilitation in adults22 Jackson PL, Lafleur MF, Malouin F, Richards C, Doyon J. Potential role of mental practice using motor imagery in neurologic rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2001;82(8):1133-41.,44 Butler AJ, Page SJ. Mental practice with motor imagery: evidence for motor recovery and cortical reorganization after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2006;87(12 Suppl 2):S2-11.,1616 Dickstein R, Deutsch JE. Motor imagery in physical therapist practice. Phys Ther 2007;87(7):942-53., its use in children is still limited1717 Gabbard C. Studying action representation in children via motor imagery. Brain Cogn 2009;71(3):234-9.. Cognitive functions considered important for this type of practice, such as operational memory, attention and abstraction may not be fully developed in childhood due to the immaturity of some brain areas1818 Best JR, Miller PH, Jones LL. Executive Functions after Age 5: Changes and Correlates. Dev Rev 2009;29(3):180-200.. Evidence has suggested that onset of motor imagery is around the age of 5 years1919 Frick A, Daum MM, Wilson M, Wilkening F. Effects of action on children's and adults' mental imagery. J Exp Child Psychol 2009;104(1):34-51.. Between 7 and 8 years of age, it has been shown that children are able to perform imaginative training2020 Caeyenberghs K, Wilson PH, van Roon D, Swinnen SP, Smits-Engelsman BC. Increasing convergence between imagined and executed movement across development: evidence for the emergence of movement representations. Dev Sci 2009;12(3):474-83., because this control is consolidated, allowing the prediction of changes of their bodies and their kinematics, and thus, the anticipation of trajectories of movements and detection of deviations from what has been planned2020 Caeyenberghs K, Wilson PH, van Roon D, Swinnen SP, Smits-Engelsman BC. Increasing convergence between imagined and executed movement across development: evidence for the emergence of movement representations. Dev Sci 2009;12(3):474-83.. In addition, motor imagery depends on the interaction of neural networks, which include the posterior parietal cortex, the pre-motor cortex, and the cerebellum. These structures, involved in motor planning and predictive control, develop gradually throughout child development, with rapid differentiation between 6 and 10 years of age, which has implications for the development of anticipatory control2020 Caeyenberghs K, Wilson PH, van Roon D, Swinnen SP, Smits-Engelsman BC. Increasing convergence between imagined and executed movement across development: evidence for the emergence of movement representations. Dev Sci 2009;12(3):474-83.

21 Smits-Engelsman BC, Wilson PH. Age-related changes in motor imagery from early childhood to adulthood: probing the internal representation of speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Hum Mov Sci 2013;32(5):1151-62.

22 Skoura X, Vinter A, Papaxanthis C. Mentally simulated motor actions in children. Dev Neuropsychol 2009;34(3):356-67.
-2323 Spruijt S, van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B. Current insights in the development of children's motor imagery ability. Front Psychol 2015;6:787.. A recent study has suggested that children aged 9-10 years are able to acquire and retain learning through imagery practice2323 Spruijt S, van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B. Current insights in the development of children's motor imagery ability. Front Psychol 2015;6:787.. In addition, recent results have indicated that the association of imagery and physical practice may lead to learning effects superior to pure physical practice in adolescents with neural lesions2424 Cabral-Sequeira AS, Coelho DB, Teixeira LA. Motor imagery training promotes motor learning in adolescents with cerebral palsy: comparison between left and right hemiparesis. Exp Brain Res 2016;234(6):1515-24..

To improve understanding of how motor imagery can be used in children’s motor learning, further studies should assess the effectiveness of imagery practice. In particular, current literature lacks information on the effect of the association between imagery practice and physical practice in the learning of motor skills in children. The aim of this experiment was to compare the effect of the combination of imagery practice and physical practice in relation to gains obtained with pure physical practice in a manual ability in children aged 9-10 years. The performance indicator in the task was the speed of execution, with movement time per task component as the primary outcome. We hypothesized that combination of imagery with physical practice induces motor learning gains superior to pure physical practice.

METHODOLOGICAL PROCEDURES

Participants

Eighteen children of both sexes aged 9-10 years (M = 9.33, SD = 0.49), right handed, healthy, without history of neurological diseases, delayed neuropsychomotor development or learning disorders were selected. To participate, children demonstrated to be able to imagine themselves doing the experimental task. For this purpose, a Portuguese-language version of a children’s specific imagery questionnaire, the Movement Imagery Questionnaire for Children, was used2525 Martini R, Carter MJ, Yoxon E, Cumming J, Ste-Marie DM. Development and validation of the Movement Imagery Questionnaire for Children (MIQ-C). Psychol Sport Exerc 2016;22:190–201.. Parents were informed about the objectives and procedures of the research and agreed to the participation of their children in the study by signing a free and informed consent form. The experimental protocol was approved by the Local Research Ethics Committee (University of São Paulo, CAAE No. 51887915.2.0000.5391).

Equipment and task

Procedures were individually performed with constant supervision by the evaluator in a closed environment, where the child remained seated in a chair in front of a table, starting with the palm of the right hand downwards resting on the starting point located 20 cm to the right side regarding the sagittal axis. The manual practice skill was based on the same as that used by Allami5. The skill consisted of holding a plastic block (dimensions: 60 x 20 x 45 mm, weight: 80 g) with the index and thumb fingers and inserting it into a support as soon as possible. The block was located on the midline the participant’s body, 30 cm away from the thorax. Half the block surface was gray, the other half white with black marks, which coincided with the marks of the support. Participants held the block on its gray side and carefully placed it inside the support. To make the task more difficult, a marble was placed unstable in a shallow hole made in the block surface. This characteristic required stability when transporting the block. In addition, two sticks were glued to the sides of the block to enforce children to hold it in the region appropriate to the task execution (Figure 1). Angles of block orientation other than 0o required its rotation during approach to the fitting position. Evaluations were recorded by means of a commercial digital camera (Sony), with acquisition frequency of 60 Hz.

Figure 1
Representation of the task components: (1) initial position, (2) gripping the block and (3) fitting the block into the support.

Experimental design and procedures

Participants were randomized into three groups (n = 6 each). Each group received a different training combination, totaling 240 trials. In the physical practice group (PHYS), children performed the experimental task in an exclusively physical way. In the combined imagery-physical practice group (COMB), a block of 180 trials was initially performed through imagery, which was immediately followed by a block of 60 physical trials. A greater proportion of imagery practice was offered because this distribution has been shown to lead to superior learning outcomes in adults55 Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D. Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(1):105-13.. In the control group (CON), children imagined a task of visual rotation in 180 trials and performed 60 physical practices of the same task as the other groups. In this case, the evaluator demonstrated the block rotation, and afterwards children were asked to imagine the same rotation of the block, but without associated manual movements. At each rotation, when the segment was aligned with a mark in a circle, children should indicate the end of the imagery trial by raising the right index finger. This condition aimed to determine the effects of imagery that may be related to non-motor phenomena, such as attention55 Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D. Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(1):105-13..

For practice, verbal instruction was associated with the demonstration of movements by the evaluator, with movement performed in front of the participant. Subsequently, the participant physically performed five trials to get familiarized with the task, with a single block orientation. In this way, children have learned to place their right thumb and index fingers on the block surface (pinch-like), and to adjust the movement speed to successfully insert the block into the support, without letting the marble fall. The practice step was performed in a single session of 240 trials in approximately 50 min. In physical practice, after verbal instruction, demonstration and familiarization with the task, participants were instructed to close their eyes while the evaluator positioned the block on the table surface, with spatial orientation of the support fixed at zero degree, while the block orientation was altered by the evaluator in a pseudo-random manner at -22°, 0°, 45° and 56° each trial. The verbal command “go” was used as the signal to start the test: the children opened their eyes to reach and grasp the block with a pinch-like grip (between thumb and forefinger), carry it and correctly insert it into the support (the marks in the block should correspond to those of the support). Then, children returned to the starting position and closed their eyes, waiting for the next trial. The block orientation was pseudo-randomly varied between trials. Participants were instructed not to speak or perform any other movement during the experiment. The goal of movement speed associated with manual steadiness in the block transportation was reinforced every 10 trials. No feedback was given to the children about their performance.

In imagery practice, children remained seated in their initial position with their right hand resting on the starting point. They were guided to imagine the sensations that they would have when performing the movements. The instruction was as follows: “Close your eyes and imagine yourself sitting in front of a table, performing the movement that I just demonstrated and that you also performed. Imagine you grasping the block, lifting it and taking it to the support, as fast as possible and without dropping the marble. Imagine this movement, thinking of all the sensations it provides”. The sequence of procedures was equivalent to that of physical practice, with pseudo-random variation of the initial block orientation among trials in both imagery practice and physical practice. After imagining making the complete fit of the block into the support, the participant was requested to elevate his right index finger, signaling to the evaluator the end of the movement imagination. Inter-trial intervals were approximately 10 s, and intervals of 1 min. every 60 trials were allowed.

Data analysis

Performance was assessed by quantifying the average movement time of 3 physical trials in the following periods: before practice (pre-test), immediately after the end of physical practice (post-test), and 24 h after practice (retention). Movement time was measured based on images, considering the following visually detectable events: movement time of the “reach” component, starting with the first frame with perceptible displacement of the right hand at the beginning of the reach, and as the end of this component, the contact of the index finger and thumb with the block, movement time of the “transport” component, having as a starting point the detection of the first vertical displacement of the block, considering as the end of this component the visual detection of the fit of the block into the support. Trials in which the marble was dropped during the execution of the motor task were excluded from the analysis and immediately retried.

The analysis of the imagery questionnaire was done in a segmented way for each imagery modality: internal visual, external visual and kinesthetic. This analysis was done using ANOVA of non-repeated measures for the single group factor. Movement time data were separately analyzed for the reach and transport components. Primary outcome analyses for movement time were performed using two-way ANOVA, 3 (group) x 3 (phase: pre-test x post-test x retention), with repeated measures in the second factor. Post hoc comparisons were made using the Newman-Keuls test. Significance level of 5% was adopted in all analyses.

RESULTS

The percentage of retried trials during physical practice due to falling marbles was 6.67% in the PHYS group and 5.83% in the COMB group. For reliability assessment, the analysis of 10% of transport component trials was repeated after an interval of 3 weeks. This evaluation indicated that in 94% of trials, movement times were coincident between the two evaluation moments, indicating good measurement reliability. The imagery capacity analysis values between groups for each modality are presented in Table 1. The imagery capacity analysis indicated that there were no significant differences between groups, with F values (2, 15) < 0.3 and p values > 0.8 among analyses in the three imagery modalities.

Table 1
Mean and standard deviations in the MIQ-C score for each group in each imagery modality.

Analysis of variance for both movement times, for the reach and transport components, indicated significant interaction between group and test factors: reach, F (4, 30) = 2.71, p = 0.05; transport, F (4, 30) = 7.02, p = 0.001. Post-hoc comparisons for the reach component indicated that for the COMB group, smaller values were found in post-test and retention in relation to pre-test, without significant differences between tests performed after practice. For the PHYS group, lower values were found in post-test compared to pre-test, but only a trend of lower times in retention compared to pre-test (p = 0.07), with no significant differences between tests performed after practice. Post-hoc comparisons for the transport component indicated lower values in post-test and retention compared to pre-test for both experimental groups, PHYS and COMB, without significant differences between tests performed after practice. No significant differences were found between tests for the CON group and no differences between groups in each test in both movement components were found (Figure 2).

Figure 2
Comparison of movement times (standard deviation in vertical bars) for the components (A) reach and (B) transport between groups (PHYS, physical practice; COMB, combination of imagery-physical practice; CON, control) in each test. *represents significant difference in relation to the respective pre-test

DISCUSSION

The present study compared the effect of association between imagery and physical practice with pure physical practice in the acquisition of a manual motor skill in children aged 9-10 years. We hypothesized that combined physical-imagery practice induces motor learning gains superior to pure physical practice. The results partially confirmed our hypothesis, since better retention of the “reach” component was observed in the COMB group compared to the PHYS group, as the mean MT values in both movement components tended to be lower in the COMB group than in the PHYS group. These results indicate a trend toward better learning by combining physical and mental practice compared to pure physical practice. On the other hand, both experimental groups, COMB and PHYS, obtained significant retention in the transport component, suggesting learning similarity between experimental groups for this component. The performance similarity of the CON group between tests indicates that the observed performance gains were not due to evaluation trials or experimental procedures. These results suggest that children aged 9-10 years can benefit from the combination of physical and imagery practice.

The performance improvement presented by the COMB group may be due to the fact that the combined practice reinforced two forms of representation. It is possible that the representation improved, regardless of the muscular activation of the effector limb developed by the imagery practice, and then with the execution and sensory feedback of the physical practice, different neural networks involved in both types of practice have been activated55 Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D. Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(1):105-13.,1212 Olsson CJ, Jonsson B, Nyberg L. Learning by doing and learning by thinking: an FMRI study of combining motor and mental training. Front Hum Neurosci 2008;2:5.. The less potent effect of imagery practice on children in our results compared to previous studies with adults33 Allami N, Brovelli A, Hamzaoui el M, Regragui F, Paulignan Y, Boussaoud D. Neurophysiological correlates of visuo-motor learning through mental and physical practice. Neuropsychologia 2014;55:6-14.,55 Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D. Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(1):105-13. could be explained by the fact that children have some cognitive functions considered important for this type of practice not yet fully developed, such as operational memory, attention and abstraction1818 Best JR, Miller PH, Jones LL. Executive Functions after Age 5: Changes and Correlates. Dev Rev 2009;29(3):180-200.. Previous studies involving imagery practice differ in the types of tasks used, ages, characteristics of children, designs and methods used1414 Millard M, Mahoney C, Wardrop J. A preliminary study of mental and physical practice on the kayak wet exit skill. Percept Mot Skills 2001;92(3 Pt 2):977-84.,2626 Schuster C, Hilfiker R, Amft O, Scheidhauer A, Andrews B, Butler J, et al. Best practice for motor imagery: a systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines. BMC Med 2011;9:75.. Investigations with combined practice have presented interesting results and interpretations on the benefits of imagery practice. More than verifying whether isolated imagery practice promotes learning or not, the use of combined practice allows us verifying how much imagery practice influences learning33 Allami N, Brovelli A, Hamzaoui el M, Regragui F, Paulignan Y, Boussaoud D. Neurophysiological correlates of visuo-motor learning through mental and physical practice. Neuropsychologia 2014;55:6-14.,55 Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D. Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(1):105-13.. In one of the few studies that evaluated intervention with mental practice in children, Taktek et al.2727 Taktek K, Salmini A, Rigal RA. The Effects of Mental Imagery on the Learning and Transfer of a Discrete Motor Task by Young Children. J Ment Imagery 2008;28(3-4):87-120. found, through the task of throwing a ball, that combined physical and mental practice provided equivalence in performance in the retention test and superiority in the transfer test in tasks when compared to pure physical practice. These results were attributed to the fact that combined practice allowed the formation of a more flexible internal task model, with greater potential for adaptation to a new motor task. Regarding long-lasting effects, a finding of interest was the persistence of performance gain by practice after 24 h in the COMB group. Previous studies also verified persistence of effects of imagery practice after a night of sleep2828 Debarnot U, Creveaux T, Collet C, Gemignani A, Massarelli R, Doyon J, et al. Sleep-related improvements in motor learning following mental practice. Brain Cogn 2009;69(2):398-405.. Taken together, these results support the notion that imagery practice can induce stable performance gains in children when associated with physical practice.

Evidence indicates that isolated imagery practice is inferior to physical and combined practices, being more effective only compared to the absence of practice1515 Gomes TVB, Ugrinowitsch H, Coelho HUR, Marinho NFS, Fonseca FS, Benda N. Efeitos do posicionamento e quantidade de prática mental na aprendizagem do arremesso do dardo de salão. Motriz: J Phys Ed 2012;18(2):273-9.. In addition, physical practice seems to be essential for learning a new skill, since it is related to the perception the learner has about his performance2525 Martini R, Carter MJ, Yoxon E, Cumming J, Ste-Marie DM. Development and validation of the Movement Imagery Questionnaire for Children (MIQ-C). Psychol Sport Exerc 2016;22:190–201.. When performing the skill physically, the learner obtains information about the achievement of the task that only physical practice allows through the action-perception interaction. In the case of isolated imagery practice, there is no feedback derived from movements, which can prevent motor learning gains observed when imagery and physical practice are combined1515 Gomes TVB, Ugrinowitsch H, Coelho HUR, Marinho NFS, Fonseca FS, Benda N. Efeitos do posicionamento e quantidade de prática mental na aprendizagem do arremesso do dardo de salão. Motriz: J Phys Ed 2012;18(2):273-9.. Studies that tested the combined practice proportionally55 Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D. Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(1):105-13.,1515 Gomes TVB, Ugrinowitsch H, Coelho HUR, Marinho NFS, Fonseca FS, Benda N. Efeitos do posicionamento e quantidade de prática mental na aprendizagem do arremesso do dardo de salão. Motriz: J Phys Ed 2012;18(2):273-9. or with greater proportion of imagery practice (75%) compared to physical practice (25%)55 Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D. Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(1):105-13., led to the conclusion that imagery practice plays an important role in performance gain, while combined practice with lower imagery practice rate (25%) seems to be less effective in inducing performance improvements55 Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D. Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(1):105-13..

CONCLUSION

The results of this experiment indicate the positive role of the association of imagery and physical practice in children motor learning, with superior retention of performance gains in the “reach” component and equivalent in the “transport” component compared to pure physical practice. Thus, the results suggest that the combination of physical and imagery practice could be an advantageous procedure for promoting motor learning in children aged 9-10 years. However, some caution is needed with the generalization of findings revealed in this study due to the limited number of children evaluated. Once the potential use of imagery practice to promote motor learning in children aged 9-10 years has been evidenced, in future studies a higher number of children should be evaluated.

Ethical approval

  • Ethical approval was obtained from the Human Research Ethics Committee of the School of Physical Education and Sport of the University of São Paulo, CAAE No. 51887915.2.0000.5391, in accordance with the standards set by the Declaration of Helsinki.

Funding

  • This research was funded by National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq) for the award of Productivity scholarship (LAT #137844/2017-4).

REFERENCES

  • 1
    Wohldmann EL, Healy AF, Bourne LE. A mental practice superiority effect: less retroactive interference and more transfer than physical practice. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2008;34(4):823-33.
  • 2
    Jackson PL, Lafleur MF, Malouin F, Richards C, Doyon J. Potential role of mental practice using motor imagery in neurologic rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2001;82(8):1133-41.
  • 3
    Allami N, Brovelli A, Hamzaoui el M, Regragui F, Paulignan Y, Boussaoud D. Neurophysiological correlates of visuo-motor learning through mental and physical practice. Neuropsychologia 2014;55:6-14.
  • 4
    Butler AJ, Page SJ. Mental practice with motor imagery: evidence for motor recovery and cortical reorganization after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2006;87(12 Suppl 2):S2-11.
  • 5
    Allami N, Paulignan Y, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D. Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp Brain Res 2008;184(1):105-13.
  • 6
    Gentili R, Han CE, Schweighofer N, Papaxanthis C. Motor learning without doing: trial-by-trial improvement in motor performance during mental training. J Neurophysiol 2010;104(2):774-83.
  • 7
    Papaxanthis C, Pozzo T, Skoura X, Schieppati M. Does order and timing in performance of imagined and actual movements affect the motor imagery process? The duration of walking and writing task. Behav Brain Res 2002;134(1-2):209-15.
  • 8
    Decety J, Jeannerod M, Durozard D, Baverel G. Central activation of autonomic effectors during mental simulation of motor actions in man. J Physiol 1993;461:549-63.
  • 9
    Cicinelli P, Marconi B, Zaccagnini M, Pasqualetti P, Filippi MM, Rossini PM. Imagery-induced cortical excitability changes in stroke: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cereb Cortex 2006;16(2):247-53.
  • 10
    Porro CA, Francescato MP, Cettolo V, Diamond ME, Baraldi P, Zuiani C, et al. Primary motor and sensory cortex activation during motor performance and motor imagery: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 1996;16(23):768-898.
  • 11
    Nyberg L, Eriksson J, Larsson A, Marklund P. Learning by doing versus learning by thinking: An fMRI study of motor and mental training. Neuropsychologia 2006;44(5):7117.
  • 12
    Olsson CJ, Jonsson B, Nyberg L. Learning by doing and learning by thinking: an FMRI study of combining motor and mental training. Front Hum Neurosci 2008;2:5.
  • 13
    Feltz DL, Landers DM. The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: a meta-analysis. J Sport Psychol 1983;5(1):25-57.
  • 14
    Millard M, Mahoney C, Wardrop J. A preliminary study of mental and physical practice on the kayak wet exit skill. Percept Mot Skills 2001;92(3 Pt 2):977-84.
  • 15
    Gomes TVB, Ugrinowitsch H, Coelho HUR, Marinho NFS, Fonseca FS, Benda N. Efeitos do posicionamento e quantidade de prática mental na aprendizagem do arremesso do dardo de salão. Motriz: J Phys Ed 2012;18(2):273-9.
  • 16
    Dickstein R, Deutsch JE. Motor imagery in physical therapist practice. Phys Ther 2007;87(7):942-53.
  • 17
    Gabbard C. Studying action representation in children via motor imagery. Brain Cogn 2009;71(3):234-9.
  • 18
    Best JR, Miller PH, Jones LL. Executive Functions after Age 5: Changes and Correlates. Dev Rev 2009;29(3):180-200.
  • 19
    Frick A, Daum MM, Wilson M, Wilkening F. Effects of action on children's and adults' mental imagery. J Exp Child Psychol 2009;104(1):34-51.
  • 20
    Caeyenberghs K, Wilson PH, van Roon D, Swinnen SP, Smits-Engelsman BC. Increasing convergence between imagined and executed movement across development: evidence for the emergence of movement representations. Dev Sci 2009;12(3):474-83.
  • 21
    Smits-Engelsman BC, Wilson PH. Age-related changes in motor imagery from early childhood to adulthood: probing the internal representation of speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Hum Mov Sci 2013;32(5):1151-62.
  • 22
    Skoura X, Vinter A, Papaxanthis C. Mentally simulated motor actions in children. Dev Neuropsychol 2009;34(3):356-67.
  • 23
    Spruijt S, van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B. Current insights in the development of children's motor imagery ability. Front Psychol 2015;6:787.
  • 24
    Cabral-Sequeira AS, Coelho DB, Teixeira LA. Motor imagery training promotes motor learning in adolescents with cerebral palsy: comparison between left and right hemiparesis. Exp Brain Res 2016;234(6):1515-24.
  • 25
    Martini R, Carter MJ, Yoxon E, Cumming J, Ste-Marie DM. Development and validation of the Movement Imagery Questionnaire for Children (MIQ-C). Psychol Sport Exerc 2016;22:190–201.
  • 26
    Schuster C, Hilfiker R, Amft O, Scheidhauer A, Andrews B, Butler J, et al. Best practice for motor imagery: a systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines. BMC Med 2011;9:75.
  • 27
    Taktek K, Salmini A, Rigal RA. The Effects of Mental Imagery on the Learning and Transfer of a Discrete Motor Task by Young Children. J Ment Imagery 2008;28(3-4):87-120.
  • 28
    Debarnot U, Creveaux T, Collet C, Gemignani A, Massarelli R, Doyon J, et al. Sleep-related improvements in motor learning following mental practice. Brain Cogn 2009;69(2):398-405.

Publication Dates

  • Publication in this collection
    Sep-Oct 2018

History

  • Received
    12 July 2017
  • Accepted
    10 May 2018
Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina, Campus Universitário Trindade, Centro de Desportos - RBCDH, Zip postal: 88040-900 - Florianópolis, SC. Brasil, Fone/fax : (55 48) 3721-8562/(55 48) 3721-6348 - Florianópolis - SC - Brazil
E-mail: rbcdh@contato.ufsc.br